The Bystander Effect in Non-Emergency Situations: Influence of Gender and Group Size

Similar documents
REPLICATIONS AND REFINEMENTS

Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo

II. Personal Qualities and Prosocial Behavior: Why Do Some People Help More Than Others?

David F. Urschler, Julia Fischer, Andreas Kastenmüller, Peter Fischer

sample Contents Content Guidance Questions & Answers AS exam-style questions A-level exam-style questions

Gender Differences in Prosocial Behaviour

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

Lose Weight & Inches Without Struggling With Exercise Or Boxed Food

EXPERIMENTAL METHODS 1/15/18. Experimental Designs. Experiments Uncover Causation. Experiments examining behavior in a lab setting

ENERGY CONSERVATION BEHAVIOR 1. Promoting Energy Conservation Behavior in Public Settings: The Influence of Social Norms and Personal Responsibility

Published online: 23 Jul 2014.

The Effects of Moral Licensing on High-Cost and Low-Cost Helping Behaviors

Lecturer: Dr. P. Mamle Tetteh, Department of Sociology Contact Information:

Self-Handicapping Variables and Students' Performance

21: Stigmatized Conditions: Responsibility and Blame. 1. A hurricane seriously harms many people

Factors that affect interpersonal attraction:

Reducing Children s False Identification Rates in Lineup Procedures

Ones Way of Thinking. Every day, people make decisions that determine where the next step we take in life will

Wishing Tree Project: Encouraging Growth in Guerilla Art Participation through a Quasi- Experiment

Attitudes Toward Mental Disorders Among College Students

SOCI 323 Social Psychology

David O Malley, Ph.D., LISW Case Western Reserve University Cleveland, Ohio

Infrastructure Wellness and Productivity Initiative: Sit-Stand Desk Trial Interim Results

Lecture 12. Prosocial Behavior. Copyright Allyn and Bacon 2005

PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR. What is Prosocial Behavior? Prosocial Behavior is voluntary behavior that is carried out to benefit another person

The effect of leadership in volunteer dilemmas

Understanding the Relationship of Self-Reports of Rape Myth Acceptance, Bystander Behavior, Bystander Intervention Efforts for Violence Prevention

University of Groningen

USCLAP Project Submission Risk Factors for Teen Alcohol Use 12/22/17. Abstract

Get Back! The Use of Personal Space Among College Students

Sexual Assault Prevention: The Effects of a Priming Intervention on the Likelihood of Intervening

Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1

HOW DO YOU REALLY KEEP YOUR KIDS SAFE FROM ADDICTION?

BRIEF REPORT OPTIMISTIC BIAS IN ADOLESCENT AND ADULT SMOKERS AND NONSMOKERS

Introduction to Wellness Recovery Action Plan (WRAP) Amanda Berg, AA Peer-Led Recovery Workshop Coordinator

Making Your Treatment Work Long-Term

Revisiting the issue of safety in numbers: The likelihood of receiving help from a group

Unit 5: Individual and. Group Behaviour

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals

VOLUNTEER WELCOME DESK - SITE COVERAGE

Chapter 18: Categorical data

What You Need to Know When Your Loved One is Diagnosed with Alzheimer s or Dementia

Best Practice Principles for teaching Orientation and Mobility skills to a person who is Deafblind in Australia

A Powerful Way to Understand People An introduction of the DISC concept By Robert A. Rohm, Ph.D. Everyone is not like you!

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Lecturer: Dr. Adote Anum, Dept. of Psychology Contact Information:

Animal Services Officers Physical Fitness Assessment

COMMITMENT &SOLUTIONS UNPARALLELED. Assessing Human Visual Inspection for Acceptance Testing: An Attribute Agreement Analysis Case Study

Unraveling the Mystery of Knee Pain #7: Clinical Applications

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Social Psychology (Soc 220) Data Analysis Exercise Spring 2004

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective

Dr. Gigi Osler Inaugural address Dr. Gigi Osler President Canadian Medical Association Winnipeg, Man. August 22, 2018 Check against delivery

Epidemiology of adolescent and young adult hospital utilization for alcohol and drug use, poisoning, and suicide attempts in the United States

Situationism. Consider a few of the famous experiments that kicked things off:

Data Analysis. A cross-tabulation allows the researcher to see the relationships between the values of two different variables

Ranking: Perceptions of Tied Ranks and Equal Intervals on a Modified Visual Analog Scale

Analysis and Interpretation of Data Part 1

From empathy to apathy: The bystander effect revisited

STRESS LEVELS AND ALCOHOL USE AMONG UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS: A QUANTITATIVE STUDY. Noemi Alsup California State University, Long Beach May 20, 2014

Section 6: Analysing Relationships Between Variables

Conformity ASCH S STUDY 12/8/2010 ASCH S STUDY. Social Psychology (581, ) Others Focus on Social Influences on Our Behavior

The Effect of Choice-Making Opportunities during Activity Schedules on Task Engagement of Adults with Autism

Everyone asks and answers questions of causation.

Similarity and Helping Behavior on the Web: The Impact of the Convergence of Surnames Between a Solicitor and a Subject in a Request Made by 1

What you should know before you collect data. BAE 815 (Fall 2017) Dr. Zifei Liu

Related KidsHealth Links

The Professional Helper s Resilience Pathways to Resilience III Halifax, Canada, 2015

By: Denise Lord, Abbey Moore, Jeffery Coffman, Lauren Lofgren, Giovanni Ellzey

Social Psychology 220

Chapter 19: Categorical outcomes: chi-square and loglinear analysis

Copyright The British Psychological Society. Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

MULTIPLE LINEAR REGRESSION 24.1 INTRODUCTION AND OBJECTIVES OBJECTIVES

Health Consciousness of Siena Students

The Presence of Others, Prosocial Traits, Machiavellianism

REPLICATION: GOING GREEN TO BE SEEN 1. Replication of: Griskevicius, Tybur, & Van den Bergh, (2010), Going Green to be seen: Status,

Political Science 15, Winter 2014 Final Review

PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE?

Framework for Ethical Decision Making: How Various Types of Unethical Clothing Production Have Different Impacts on People

Red Ribbon Week Your Future is Key, so Stay Drug Free. October History

Evaluating framing e ects

Hunger & Homelessness Awareness Week November 11-19, Media Outreach Toolkit

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction

Welcome participants, introduce the expert, and briefly outline today s session.

SOCI 323 Social Psychology

Instrumental Variables I (cont.)

c) Potential sources of error in social cognition A Basic Tilt in Social Thought, Situation-Specific Sources of Error in Social Cognition

Walking Beats. Joyce Ma. March 2002

Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Time for Break: Understanding Information Workers Sedentary Behavior Through a Break Prompting System

NHS Training for Physiotherapy Support Workers. Workbook 3 Balance re-education

Taxonomy of Helping Behaviors

Colorado Resilience Collaborative: Research for Preventing Identity-Based Violence

Stereotype threat for the scholar-athlete in the classroom

Trauma Introduction to Trauma-Informed Care and The Neurosequential Model

The UCD community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters!

What is Beautiful is Good Online: Physical Attractiveness, Social Interactions and Perceived Social Desirability on Facebook Abstract

Does anxiety cause some difficulty for a young person you know well? What challenges does this cause for the young person in the family or school?

WHY LEARN SELF DEFENSE?

Transcription:

Modern Psychological Studies Volume 23 Number 2 2018 The Bystander Effect in Non-Emergency Situations: Influence of Gender and Group Size Amanda Cox University of Southern Indiana, aecox@eagles.usi.edu Aimee Adam University of Southern Indiana, asadam@usi.edu Follow this and additional works at: https://scholar.utc.edu/mps Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Cox, Amanda and Adam, Aimee (2018) "The Bystander Effect in Non-Emergency Situations: Influence of Gender and Group Size," Modern Psychological Studies: Vol. 23 : No. 2, Article 3. Available at: https://scholar.utc.edu/mps/vol23/iss2/3 This articles is brought to you for free and open access by the Journals, Magazines, and Newsletters at UTC Scholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in Modern Psychological Studies by an authorized editor of UTC Scholar. For more information, please contact scholar@utc.edu.

BYSTANDER EFFECT 1 Abstract Although research on the bystander effect spans 50 years, the influence of some variables on prosocial behavior are still unclear. In a field experiment, I tried to replicate the basic bystander effect in three non-emergency situations, and studied the impact of bystander and victim gender on helping behavior. I successfully replicated the basic bystander effect; bystanders who were alone were significantly more likely to help than bystanders with one or more than one companion. In addition, when people noticed the need for help, a male and a female confederate were helped equally as often. Finally, women were more likely to help both confederates than men, but that men were more likely to help the female confederate than the male confederate. Keywords: bystander effect; gender; prosocial behavior; helping; non-emergency

BYSTANDER EFFECT 2 The Bystander Effect in Non-Emergency Situations: Influence of Gender and Group Size Prosocial behavior is behavior that primarily benefits another, rather than oneself (Batson & Powell, 2003). Although there are many influences on prosocial behavior, of particular interest to many researchers is the number of other people present who could act prosocially, but likely won t. The probability of a bystander helping someone in need is inversely related to the number of bystanders present (Aronson, Wilson, Akert, & Sommers, 2016). This bystander effect has been documented in many well-known experiments (e.g., Darley & Latané, 1968; Darley, Teger, & Lewis, 1973; Latané & Darley, 1968). However, there are many other variables that influence bystander intervention, including whether or not the situation is an emergency (a danger is posed to a victim or bystanders, or situations in which a villain has infringed upon the rights of others and prompt action is necessary; Latané & Nida, 1981), and the sex of both the person in need and the bystander. In this study, I conducted research to replicate the basic bystander effect in non-emergency situations (situations in which help is needed, but there is no danger to victims or other bystanders, and no one s rights are being infringed upon), and also examined the effect of bystander and victim gender on intervention. In an early review of all psychology experiments published over a ten-year span that examined the effect of group size on helping behavior, Latané and Nida (1981), found that in 48 of 56 experiments, more people helped when alone compared to when there were others present, confirming the bystander effect. Overall, 53% of those tested with others present exhibited helping behavior while 75% of those alone helped (Latané & Nida). More recently, in a review of the bystander literature over the past 50 years, including 53 articles with 7,000 participants, Fischer and colleagues (2011) also found that the more bystanders present lead to a larger bystander effect, but also that this effect was smaller in emergency situations.

BYSTANDER EFFECT 3 The influence of gender of both the bystander and the person in need of help on the bystander effect is less clear. According to social-role theory of sex differences, men are primed to help in heroic ways, while women are primed to help in more nurturing ways (Eagly & Crowley, 1986). Although Latané and Nida (1981) looked at the gender of bystanders in their review of the literature, they found no clear gender differences in helping behavior. Metaanalyses investigating sex differences in helping behavior find that men help more often than women (Eagly & Crowley; Fischer et al., 2011); however, this may be more particularly true when the situation is defined as an emergency, allowing men to act in more heroic ways (Eagly & Crowley). On the other hand, in everyday situations, gender differences in helping behavior may be less likely. If men exhibit heroic and chivalrous helping behavior, according to the social-role theory of gender and helping, then one would think that women would receive more help than men. Although earlier researchers found that women were helped more often than men (Howard & Crano, 1974; Latané & Dabbs, 1975), in their review, Latané and Nida (1981) found that in some experiments women were helped more, while in other experiments women and men were helped equally. Although Eagly and Crowley (1986) found that women were helped more often than men, supporting a social-role view of prosocial behavior, more recently Fisher et al. (2011) found that women were not helped more than men. Again, the type of situation probably interacts with helping behavior, but this has not been well-addressed. It is likely that women are more likely to receive help in emergency situations (likely from male bystanders), but similarly, gender differences in receiving help may disappear in non-emergency situations. In the current study, I conducted research to replicate previous findings regarding the overall effect of bystander group size on helping behavior in three non-emergency situations, and

BYSTANDER EFFECT 4 to clarify the effects of both bystander and victim gender on helping behavior. The first hypothesis was that the fewer witnesses to a situation in need of help, the more likely those witnesses would be to assist (Darley & Latané, 1968; Darley & Teger, 1973; Fischer et al., 2011; Latané & Darley, 1968; Latané & Nida, 1981). The second hypothesis was that women and men would be equally likely to receive help, based on the most recent meta-analysis (Fisher et al., 2011), and because these were non-emergency situations. Finally, because it is unclear whether men or women are likely to help more often in non-emergency situations, this will also be examined. The research question examined in the current study was whether one gender would exhibit helping behavior more frequently than the other in non-emergency situations. Method Design The study took place on a midsized, mid-western college campus after IRB approval. I conducted a naturalistic field experiment with a quasi-experimental between-subjects design. The independent variables consisted of the group size a bystander was in when they were observed (alone, in a group of two, or a group containing more than two), the perceived gender of the bystander (male or female), and the sex of the person who was in need of the help (male or female). Those observed while alone had no one near them within approximately 20 feet. The dependent variable was how helpful the person was. Bystanders were categorized as not seeming to notice the person in need of help, noticing but not helping, or making an effort to help. Making an effort to help consisted of an observed person helping by picking up papers, opening a door, or picking up thumbtacks. This design is similar to that used by Levine, Martinez, Erase, and Sorenson (1994).

BYSTANDER EFFECT 5 I and a male confederate conducted 192 trials, including observations of 76 individuals by themselves, 62 individuals who were in a group of two and 54 individuals who had two or more companions. People who had at least one free hand were chosen to be observed. This precaution was taken to exclude any person who might not have helped because their hands were full. In the alone condition, we observed 44 women and 32 men. In the group of two condition, we observed 34 women and 27 men. In the larger group condition, we observed 33 women and 21 men. Procedure All trials were conducted by myself, a female, or a male confederate of the same age and ethnicity. We dressed similarly in casual clothes, such as jeans and a t-shirt. The study took place during weekday business hours during the fall semester on campus. Most trials took place between 12:00 and 1:00 PM, when students tended to have more of a break on campus, and we usually collected data for 30 minutes to an hour at a time. Three different scenarios were created to test helping behavior. All scenarios took place in different buildings on campus. In the first scenario we accidentally dropped 10 loose-leafed papers in front of a person or group of people walking in the opposite direction. We dropped the papers once an observed person, in the desired group size, walking in the opposite direction, came within 10-15 feet in front of us. The second scenario consisted of the researcher standing in front of a bulletin board, attempting to put up flyers. Once an observed person or people in the desired group size came within 10-15 feet of us, we accidentally dropped precisely 15 thumb tacks that we were using to put up the flyers. The third scenario consisted of the researcher with full arms approaching a door in need of another to open the door. Once an individual, a group of two, or a group of more than two coming in the opposite direction came within 10-15 feet of us, we would approach the same door

BYSTANDER EFFECT 6 with full arms. Three scenarios were created so that we didn t replicate the same trial so many times that people guessed the purpose of the study. While one researcher created the helping situation, the other researcher stood approximately 20-30 feet away, and observed the trial and took notes. This distance guaranteed those that were observed would not look to the confederate for help. Results My first hypothesis was that the fewer witnesses to a potential emergency, the more likely those witnesses would be to help. The quasi-independent variable was group size (alone, in a group of two, or a group containing more than two). Helping behavior was also classified into three categories, including not seeming to notice the person in need of help, noticing but not helping, or making an effort to help. To test this hypothesis, a chi-square test of independence was conducted using SPSS. Group size was significantly related to helping behavior, χ²(4) = 16.94, p =.002, φ =.297. The smaller the group a person was in, the more likely they were to engage in helping behavior (see Table 1). However, it was very apparent that in the larger group, people were much less likely to notice a need for help. I also wanted to see whether there was still a difference in behavior when those who did not appear to notice the situation (N = 34) were excluded from analyses. The results of a second chi-square were still significant, χ²(2) = 7.34, p =.026, φ =.216. This hypothesis was supported. The larger the group, the less likely people were to help, even when they noticed that there was a need for help. My second hypothesis was that because these were non-emergency situations, women and men would receive help equally as often. The quasi-independent variable was the sex of the researcher, male or female, and the dependent variable was again the target person s helping behavior. To test this hypothesis, a chi-square was again calculated. The relationship between

BYSTANDER EFFECT 7 the sex of the researcher and helping behavior approached significance, χ²(2) = 5.86, p =.054, φ =.176. However, this appeared to be due primarily to differences in noticing a need for help fewer people appeared to notice the female researcher in need of help across conditions (see Table 2). When those who did not notice the need for help were removed from the analysis, there was no difference in whether the male or female research was helped more often, χ²(1) =.24, p =.621, φ =.039. This hypothesis was mostly supported. When people noticed the need for help, they were not more likely to help the female researcher more than the male researcher. My research question was whether one gender would exhibit helping behavior more frequently. The quasi-independent variable was the perceived gender of the helper (male or female) while the dependent variable was helping behavior. Helping behavior was categorized the same way as it was in the first two hypotheses. To test this hypothesis a chi-square was again used to compare the frequency of helping behavior between women and men. There was one trial in which the gender of the observed person could not be determined, and that trial was excluded from the analysis. Overall, there was a marginally significant relationship between bystander gender and helping behavior, χ²(2)= 5.95, p =.051, φ =.176. Women helped more often than men (see Table 3), but there did not appear to be a difference between men and women in whether they noticed the situation. When those who did not notice were excluded from analysis, the relationship was significant, χ²(1) = 5.94, p =.015, φ =.195. Overall, women were more likely to help than men. However, I examined only people who did notice and who actually helped the experimenter to see if there was an interaction between the sex of the researcher and the gender of the person who helped. Of the men who helped (N = 24), they were more likely to help the female researcher (N = 18) than the male researcher (N = 6), while

BYSTANDER EFFECT 8 women were fairly equally likely to help either the female researcher (N = 22) or the male researcher (N = 29), χ²(1) = 6.66, p =.010, φ =.287. Discussion There are many variables that affect whether a person helps another. Among these variables are group size, gender of the observed, and gender of the person in need of help. These variables were the focus of the current study. I wanted to replicate the basic bystander effect; that the more people around, the less likely any one person is to help, particularly in nonemergency situations. My first hypothesis was supported; college students who were alone were more likely to assist the researcher in need than those with companions. This was even true after excluding from analysis the people who did not seem to notice the need for help. This finding supports the basic bystander effect. The difference in noticing need based on group size also lends support for Latané and Darley s bystander intervention model (1970), in which the first step to bystander intervention is noticing the need for intervention. When in larger groups, people are more involved with each other, and possibly less aware of their surroundings. In the current study, people who noticed a need for help were still more likely to help if they were alone, compared to with companions. My second hypothesis, that women and men would receive the same amount of help in non-emergency situations, was mostly supported. When only taking into account those who appeared to notice the need for help, both I and my male confederate were equally likely to receive help, which replicates the overall findings of the most recent meta-analysis on this topic (Fischer et al., 2011). Because these scenarios involved non-emergency helping situations, it is still unclear whether women will in fact receive more help in emergency situations, but the results of my study suggest that gender differences in who receives help depend on the type of

BYSTANDER EFFECT 9 situation (emergency or non-emergency). On the other hand, it is unclear why there was a difference in the number of people who noticed the female vs. male researcher s need for help. This could have been due to the specific researchers in the study; some aspect of the male confederate may simply have been more noticeable. Future research could be conducted with multiple male and female confederates. Past research on whether one gender exhibits helping behavior more frequently has been unclear, at least in non-emergency situations. I sought to answer whether men or women would help more in non-emergency situations. I found that women who noticed the need for help were actually significantly more likely to help than were men who noticed, at least in these particular situations. However, when researcher sex and participant gender were examined together, it was clear that men were more likely to help the female researcher than the male researcher, which may support a social-role theory of sex differences regarding prosocial behavior. There were several limitations of my study. As a result of conducting the experiment on a college campus, most people observed were around the same age. Older adults (or children) may have responded differently. A college campus is also an environment where everyone is usually busy and in a hurry. This might have lowered the number of people who helped, or who noticed the need for help, because they were too busy. In this study, each condition (papers, thumbtacks or full arms) was conducted on a different day, although in different buildings on campus, and this introduces history as a potential threat to internal validity. We started collecting data before the 2016 presidential election, and ended after the election. Specific events such as these could have affected people s helping behavior. Future researchers can easily address these limitations by better counterbalancing the order of their trials.

BYSTANDER EFFECT 10 Overall, this study adds to the literature about the circumstances in which people will help one another. Although many people recognize the bystander effect, it is not simply the number of bystanders in a situation that will determine their behavior. My research suggests that not only the gender of both the person in need for help and of the bystanders matters, but also that behavior will also depend on whether the situation is or is not an emergency. Although older research found that men were overall more likely to help (Eagly & Crowley; Fischer et al., 2011), in my study, women were actually more helpful. Although it is likely that this is because these were non-emergency situations, this study should help change the idea that men are more helpful.

BYSTANDER EFFECT 11 References Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., Akert, R. M., & Sommers, S. R. (2016). Social Psychology, (9th ed.). Pearson. Batson, C. D., & Powell, A. A. (2003). Altruism and prosocial behavior. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Psychology, Vol. 5: Personality and social psychology (pp. 463 484). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8(4, Pt.1), 377-383. Darley, J. M., Teger, A. I., & Lewis, L. D. (1973). Do groups always inhibit individuals' responses to potential emergencies? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 26(3), 395-399. Eagly, A. H., & Crowley, M. (1986). Gender and helping behavior: A meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological bulletin, 100(3), 283. Fischer, P., Krueger, J. I., Greitemeyer, T., Vogrincic, C., Kastenmüller, A., Frey, D.,... & Kainbacher, M. (2011). The bystander-effect: A meta-analytic review on bystander intervention in dangerous and non-dangerous emergencies. Psychological bulletin, 137(4), 517-537. Howard, W., & Crano, W. D. (1974). Effects of sex, conversation, location, and size of observer group on bystander intervention in a high risk situation. Sociometry, 37(4), 491-507. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2786423 Latané, B., & Dabbs, J. M. (1975). Sex, group size, and helping in three cities. Sociometry, 38(2), 180-194. Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1968). Group inhibition of bystander intervention in emergencies.

BYSTANDER EFFECT 12 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10(3), 215-221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0026570 Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why doesn t he help? Englewood Cliffs, NJ : Prentice Hall. Latané, B., & Nida, S. (1981). Ten years of research on group size and helping. Psychological Bulletin, 89(2), 308-324. Levine, R. V., Martinez, T. S., Brase, G., & Sorenson, K. (1994). Helping in 36 U.S. cities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(1), 69-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.1.69

BYSTANDER EFFECT 13 Appendix Table 1 Crosstabulation of Group Size and Helping Behavior Size Not notice Notice not help Help Total Alone 8 29 39 76 1 companion 9 28 25 62 2+ companions 17 26 11 54 Total 34 83 75 192 Table 2 Crosstabulation of Researcher Sex and Helping Behavior Researcher Not notice Notice not help Help Total Female 25 41 40 106 Male 9 42 35 86 Total 34 83 75 192 Table 3 Crosstabulation of Gender of Person Observed and Helping Behavior Gender Not notice Notice not help Help Total Female 20 40 51 111 Male 14 42 24 80 Total 34 82 75 191