STRESS AND AROUSAL CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS IN SMOKERS AND NON-SMOKERS WORKING DAY AND NIGHT SHIFTS

Similar documents
Acute pharmacodynamic tolerance to the of cigarette smoking. subjective effects. Psychopharmacology. Psychopharmacology (1994) 116:93-97

Stress and arousal in sedative and stimulant. cigarette smokers. Psychopharmacology. Psychopharmacology (1992) 107: Springer-Verlag 1992

Cigarette Smoking and Abstinence: Comparative Effects Upon Cognitive Task Performance and Mood State over 24 Hours

Bi-directional Relationship Between Poor Sleep and Work-related Stress: Management through transformational leadership and work organization

Nesbitt s Paradox resolved? Stress and arousal m odulation during cigarette sm oking

Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up

Management of Tobacco Dependence. Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur

Trait anxiety and nicotine dependence in adolescents A report from the DANDY study

Minimal conditions for the creation of a unit relationship: the social bond between birthdaymates

Session 1: Days 1-3. Session 4: Days Session 2: Days 4-7. Session 5: Days Session 3: Days Day 21: Quit Day!

Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours

ASLEF. More than. just a union. Rostering Best Practice THE TRAIN DRIVERS UNION

Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings. Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005

Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision

Occupational stress and gender: a cross-cultural study

Subjects Motivations

Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia

COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE

Reference document. Night work

HSE information sheet. Guidance for managing shiftwork and fatigue offshore. Offshore Information Sheet No. 7/2008

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION. Randomized Comparative Trial of Nicotine Polacrilex, a Transdermal Patch, Nasal Spray, and an Inhaler

Tobacco Dependence in Youths Les jeunes et la dépendance au tabac. Joseph R. DiFranza, M.D. University of Massachusetts Medical School

On the Making of an Experience: The E ects of Breaking and Combining Experiences on their Overall Evaluation

Alterations in self-perceptions following childhood onset of spinal cord injury

Fatigue. Based on information from FAA briefing prepared by Thomas E. Nesthus, Ph.D.

An experimental test of an alcohol expectancy challenge in mixed gender groups of young heavy drinkers

When are you too tired to be safe?

Special Populations: Guidelines for Pregnant Smokers

SMOKING CESSATION WORKSHOP. Dr Mark Palayew December

Impression formation and cooperative behavior

The Hidden Dangers of Fatigue

NICOTINE PHARMACOLOGY and PRINCIPLES of ADDICTION. 3 rd of 3 Prep for Session 1

Journal of Pharmaceutical and Scientific Innovation

Parents Guide to Teenagers and Sleep. Copyright Child Mind Institute

#11 Circadian Rhythm & Shift work. By : Dewi Hardiningtyas, ST., MT., MBA. Industrial Engineering Dept. University of Brawijaya

Preventing Relapse among Problem Gamblers using a Minimal Intervention

Trait motivational correlates of athleticism

BASIC VOLUME. Elements of Drug Dependence Treatment

The data box. Multiple ways of assessment

Facts. Sleepiness or Fatigue Causes the Following:

If treatment for tobacco addiction was evidence-based, what would it look like? Robert West University College London YORK, November 2005

Smoking Cessation Counselling

Addressing the elephant in the (bed)room: Sleep and fatigue as a shiftworker

Level 1 l Pre-intermediate / Intermediate

Evaluating framing e ects

Smoking Cessation Counselling

Depression and anxiety as risk factors for failure in smoking cessation attempt among personnel of Tartu University Hospital

Let s Sleep On It: Developing a Healthy Sleep Pattern. The Presenter. Session Overview

SUBJECTIVE EXPERIMENTS ON RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN INDOOR ENVIRONMENT AND AROUSAL STATE AND BETWEEN AROUSAL STATE AND WORK PERFORMANCE

Lucid dreaming: associations with internal locus of control, need for cognition and creativity

A Reliability Study of The Turkish Version of The Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire

Reported Stress and Its Relationship to Tobacco Use among U.S. Military Personnel

SCHRES1520. Schizophrenia Research 000 (2001) 000±000. Affective reactivity of language and right-ear advantage in schizophrenia

Let s Sleep On It: Developing a Healthy Sleep Pattern. Session Overview. Quote. Sleep is the best meditation.

Bio-Rhythms. Biorhythms. Presented by: Dr. Magdy Akladios 1. What is a Biorhythm. Biorhythms Theory. SENG/ INDH 5334: Human Factors Engineering

Smoking Cessation. lyondellbasell.com

Commercial Vehicle Drivers Hours of Service Module 1 Overview

London Pathway Evaluation

Differences in Social 1. Running head: DIFFERENCES IN SOCIAL SUPPORT AS A RISK FACTOR

Locally Enhanced Service for Stopping Smoking

QUIT TODAY. It s EASIER than you think. DON T LET TOBACCO CONTROL YOUR LIFE. WE CAN HELP.

Smoking and Smoking Cessation in England 2011: Findings from the Smoking Toolkit Study

Dr Alex Bartle. Director Sleep Well Clinic

Hull s Adult Health and Lifestyle Survey: Summary

Heavy drinking among college students is in uenced by anxiety sensitivity, gender, and contexts for alcohol use

To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors

Smoking Reduction is the First Step Towards Smoking Cessation

NIGHT FIT AT SEAWAY HEAVY LIFTING. Improving the quality of sleep, health and safety at the Oleg Strashnov KM HUMAN FACTORS ENGINEERING

Fatigue Risk in the Workplace. Who is NSC? Agenda. National Safety Council Causes & Effects Survey. Risk Factors. Goals & Activities

A comparison of e ects measured with isotonic and isometric recording: II. Concentration-e ect curves for physiological antagonists

Psychological Sleep Services Sleep Assessment

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch

Fatigue Management. Sample Only

FATIGUE MANAGEMENT & MITIGATION

Treatment Outcomes from the TDC: A Look at Smoking Cessation Among Patients with Co- Occurring Disorders

1 Report on Sleep Deprivation Trial

SMOKE-FREE FOR LIFE 2ND FLOOR 4TH FLOOR

A good night s sleep

RELAPSE PREVENTION: AN EVIDENCE-BASED REVIEW. David C. Hodgins University of Calgary October

TOBACCO CESSATION SUPPORT PROGRAMME

Clearing the Air: What You Need to Know and Do to Prepare to Quit Smoking. Getting Ready to Quit Course

Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems.

Week 4 Psychology. Theory of mind is an individual s understanding that they and others think, feel, perceive, and have private experiences.

Effects of nicotine gum on repeated administration of the Stroop test

Sleep. Information booklet. RDaSH. Adult Mental Health Services

Smoking Cessation Counselling

Shiftwork, sleep, fatigue and time of day: studies of a change from 8-h to 12-h shifts and single vehicle accidents

Characterization of Tobacco Withdrawal: Physiological and Subjective Effects

ROLL-YOUR-OWN CIGARETTES AS A RISK FACTOR FOR TOBACCO DEPENDENCE IN NEW ZEALAND

Northern Tobacco Use Monitoring Survey Northwest Territories Report. Health and Social Services

Indepen dent Study On Program Effective ness

Smoking Cessation Counselling

Impact of brief motivational smoking cessation intervention: a short study

Evidence base, treatment policy and coverage in England. Ann McNeill

An Experimental Comparison of the Fairness Models by Bolton and Ockenfels and by Fehr and Schmidt

Gender bias in risk aversion: evidence from multiple choice exams

th Ave NE Suite F Bellevue, WA Phone: (425) Fax: (425) Excessive Daytime Sleepiness

Sleep Problems and Smoking Cessation in Women

Getting a Great Nights Sleep. Dr. Michael Long ND BSc, Dr. Katie McKeown ND BSc

COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY

Transcription:

STRESS MEDICINE, VOL. 13: 91±97 (1997) STRESS AND AROUSAL CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS IN SMOKERS AND NON-SMOKERS WORKING DAY AND NIGHT SHIFTS M. E. E. JONES 1, Bsc AND A.C. PARROTT 1, BSc, PhD, CPsychol, FBPs S 1 Department of Psychology, University of East London, London, U.K. ABSTRACT Twenty smokers and twenty non-smokers completed a brief Mood State Questionnaire every 2 hours, over one day shift and one night shift. The subjects comprised male police o cers and factory workers, aged 23±57 years. Cigarette smokers reported signi cantly higher stress levels than non-smokers on both day and night shifts p < 0:05. This con rms previous ndings that smokers are often more stressed than non-smokers. Stress levels varied over time within each shift p < 0:001, but the circadian patterns did not di er between smokers and non-smokers. Thus smoking did not alter circadian mood rhythms, nor did it facilitate stress control. Self-rated levels of arousal showed the archetypal inverted-u pattern over time in both smokers and non-smokers. There was no di erence in mean arousal levels between subgroups, indicating that cigarettes did not lead to greater alertness. The shift time interaction was signi cant for both stress p < 0:001 and arousal p < 0:003, indicating di erent circadian rhythms during the day shift and the night shift. Finally, while smokers consumed slightly more cigarettes during the night shift than day shift (22.3, 19.4 respectively, p < 0:05), mean stress and arousal levels did not di er between shifts. The implications of these ndings for smoking behaviour are discussed. # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Stress Med., Vol. 13, 91±97 (1997) No. of Figures: 2. No. of Tables: 1. No. of Refs: 25. KEY WORDS Ð cigarette; nicotine; smoking; mood; stress; arousal; circadian rhythm; shift work Cigarette smoking has been found to produce a number of diverse psychophysiological changes. These include increased physiological arousal, feelings of alertness, faster information processing and improved vigilance. 1 ±3 The act of smoking also reduces feelings of stress and aids relaxation. 4 ±7 Typically, an inverted-u function has been found for self-rated feelings of arousal, with levels peaking around midday and decreasing over the late afternoon and evening. Arousal levels fall to their lowest during the night, which can make it di cult for nightshift workers to maintain optimal alertness. 8 Parrott and Joyce 9 found that self-rated feelings of anxiety/stress decreased over the day in both smokers and non-smokers. While average stress levels did not di er between smokers and non-smokers, smokers started the day with higher stress than nonsmokers, but ended with slightly Correspondence to: M. E. E. Jones, Department of Psychology, University of East London, London E15 4LZ, UK. Tel: (44) 0181 5907722 Fax: (44) 0181 8493697 lower stress levels. Complex circadian patterns were obtained for self-rated feelings of arousal. The physiological functions served by cigarette smoking have so far only been studied for day workers, not night workers. It is for this reason that the present study was undertaken. The basic aim was to investigate whether smoking subserves similar psychological functions during the day and night. Self-rated feelings of stress and arousal were monitored in shift workers over one working day shift and one working night shift, while the number of cigarettes smoked was also monitored. It was predicted that smoking would be more frequent during night shifts and that this would be accompanied by greater mood modulation. METHODS Subjects Subjects were all males from a variety of ethnic backgrounds; they comprised manual workers from a local factory and police o cers. All subjects CCC 0748±8386/97/020091±07$17.50 # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

92 M. E. E. JONES AND A. C. PARROTT routinely worked rotating shifts with scheduled rotation. None were paid for participation. Of the 45 returned questionnaires, ve were omitted due to incomplete data. The remaining 40 subjects comprised two subgroups: non-smokers (N ˆ 20: age range 23±53 years) who had never smoked, or not smoked for over 1 year, and smokers (N ˆ 20: age range 27±46 years) who currently smoked between 15 and 40 cigarettes a day (mean ˆ 20:0 cigarettes per day). Assessment measures The Mood State Questionnaire was derived from the Short Adjective Checklist (SACL 10 ). Two questions were taken from the SACL stress factor (questions 1 and 2), while two were taken from the SACL arousal factor (questions 3 and 4). The resulting mood questionnaire 11 is summarized below: Tense strongly slightly neither slightly strongly Relaxed Nervous strongly slightly neither slightly strongly Calm Energetic strongly slightly neither slightly strongly Tired Alert strongly slightly neither slightly strongly Drowsy Each question was scored from 1 to 5. The rst two questions were combined to produce the overall stress score 2 ˆ low; 10 ˆ high, while the two latter questions generated the arousal score 2 ˆ low; 10 ˆ high. In addition, the Smoking Motivation Questionnaire 5;6 was also completed. Procedure All subjects were given an instruction booklet, a set of Mood State Questionnaires 4 and the Smoking Motivation Questionnaire. The Mood State Questionnaire was to be completed at the following times: 12 midnight, 2.00 am, 4.00 am, 6.00 am, 8.00 am, 10.00 am, 12 noon, 2.00 pm, 4.00 pm, 6.00 pm, 8.00 pm, and 10.00 pm. If asleep at any times, subjects ticked the `asleep' box. Smokers also indicated how many cigarettes had been smoked within each 2-hour time period. This procedure was carried out over one working day shift and one working night shift, when they would not be consuming alcohol. Subjects were asked to follow this procedure on the same working day for each shift. Treatment of results A three-way split-plot ANOVA was performed on these data, with one between-subjects factor, group (smoker, non-smoker) and two withinsubjects factors, shift (day, night) and time (eight time periods). RESULTS With self-rated feelings of stress, signi cant ANOVA main e ects were found with groups p < 0:05, time p < 0:001 and shift time interaction p < 0:001. Feelings of stress were signi cantly higher in smokers than non-smokers and varied over time (Table 1; Fig. 1). The signi cant shift time interaction showed that patterns of stress changes over time di ered between each shift. Although stress levels were slightly higher when working nights, this di erence was not signi cant p ˆ 0:104, two-tail. With self-rated feelings of arousal, signi cant ANOVA e ects were found for time p < 0:001, and shift time interaction p < 0:05. Arousal levels therefore di ered over time, while these circadian patterns of change also di ered between shifts (Table 1; Fig. 2). Arousal levels peaked higher when working nights compared to days, but also showed a steeper decline later in the shift. Signi cantly more cigarettes were smoked during night shifts mean ˆ 22:3 compared with day shifts mean ˆ 19:4; p < 0:009. Table 1 Ð ANOVA stress and arousal ndings (two-tailed probability values) Stress Arousal F-value df p-value F-value df p-value Groups (smoker/non-smoker) 4.53 1;38 0.040 1.61 1;38 0.212 Shift (day/night) 2.78 1;38 0.104 0.39 1;38 0.536 Time (8 time periods) 9.81 7;266 0.001 52.58 7;266 0.001 Groups Shift 0.89 1;38 0.352 0.55 1;38 0.465 Groups Time 0.40 7;266 0.902 1.93 7;266 0.065 Shift Time 6.51 7;266 0.001 3.14 7;266 0.003 Groups Shift Time 1.01 7;266 0.389 0.88 7;266 0.525

STRESS AND AROUSAL IN SMOKERS AND NON-SMOKERS 93 Fig. 1

94 M. E. E. JONES AND A. C. PARROTT Fig. 2

STRESS AND AROUSAL IN SMOKERS AND NON-SMOKERS 95 DISCUSSION Smokers reported signi cantly higher stress levels than non-smokers on both day and night shifts ( p < 0:05; Fig. 1). This agrees with a number of previous studies where smokers have been found to experience higher levels of daily stress than nonsmokers. In the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey involving over 9000 subjects, current smokers were signi cantly more stressed than either non-smokers or former smokers. 12;13 Robertson 14 similarly found that heavy smokers experienced higher levels of anxiety, neuroticism and depression than nonsmokers. Cohen and Williamson 15 also reported higher levels of self-rated stress in current smokers than non-smokers. Furthermore in a longitudinal study of smokers attempting to quit, those who managed to stop smoking reported decreasing levels of stress over time, whereas those who failed in their cessation attempt reported consistently high stress levels throughout the study. 16 In a brief overview, West 17 concluded: `Smokers do not present as less anxious than nonsmokers. Indeed, in surveys they emerge as signi cantly more anxious overall'. The relationship between smoking and stress has been a subject of controversy for many years. 18;19 The two main explanations proposed for this relationship are the nicotine resource model and the deprivation reversal model. The nicotine resource model suggests that nicotine is a useful resource, used by smokers to control their daily moods. 20 The deprivation reversal model suggests that smoking has no real bene cial functions and that the positive feelings which accompany the act of smoking re ect the reversal of the deleterious mood e ects of nicotine deprivation. 21 In a development of the latter model, Parrott 9;22 suggests that smoking is a direct pharmacological cause of stress. According to this explanation, smokers routinely experience a source of stress not experienced by non-smokers: acute nicotine depletion. They su er from nicotine withdrawal when they have not smoked recently, and smoking then restores their normal moods. This model explains not only why smokers are more stressed than nonsmokers, but also why former smokers become less stressed when they quit. 9;22 The comparative pro les of mood change over the day for smokers and non-smokers have been assessed in only one previous study. Parrott and Joyce 9 reported a signi cant group by time interaction, with smokers starting the day with higher stress than non-smokers but ending the day with slightly lower stress. It was hypothesized that the high initial stress re ected overnight nicotine depletion, while the steeper stress decrease over the day re ected the gradual accumulation of nicotine. 9 One central aim of the current investigation was therefore to replicate this earlier study. However, the current ndings are ambivalent in this regard. The subgroup by time interaction was non-signi cant here F ˆ 0:044; Table 1, whereas it was signi cant earlier. Thus in the current study, smokers started the day with higher stress levels than non-smokers (5.2 compared with 3.9), while by the end of the day stress levels were similar for both groups (each 3.0; Fig. 1). This general pattern of higher stress during the rst part of the day, then lower stress levels later in the day,also agrees with Thayer, 23 who has noted: `Tense energy mood is usually present during the rst third of the day, while later tense energy is replaced by tense-tiredness' (p. 53). During the night shift a di erent circadian mood pattern was evident, with high stress levels throughout the whole period from 10.00 pm to 6.00 am (Fig. 1). This is the time of day when circadian arousal levels are normally at their lowest. 8 It may be that since night shift working is unnatural, it is therefore also stressful. However, it should be noted that stress levels during the night shift were not signi cantly di erent from those during the day shift, although they were slightly higher (5.4 at night, compared to 4.8 during the day; p ˆ 0:104, two-tail). As with the day shift, cigarette smokers during the night shift reported higher stress levels than non-smokers. However, unlike the day shift, this subgroup di erence remained consistent throughout the whole time period. Thus smokers commenced the night shift more stressed than non-smokers, reported higher peak stress levels during the middle of the night and remained more stressed in the early morning (Fig. 1). This consistent mood di erence between groups shows that increasing plasma nicotine levels (found with repeated cigarette smoking 24 ) were not having any bene cial e ects (contrasting with our earlier ndings, 9 also see above paragraph). Thus cigarette smoking was not providing a means for mood modulation at night. Arousal rhythms demonstrated an inverted-u pattern over time, in both smokers and nonsmokers (ANOVA time e ect: p < 0:001). This inverted-u arousal pattern is typical of studies where subjects rate their feelings of alertness over

96 M. E. E. JONES AND A. C. PARROTT the day. 8;11. This inverted-u pattern was found on both shifts (Fig. 2), although, somewhat surprisingly, peak arousal was higher at night than day, but then declined more steeply as the morning approached (shift by time interaction: p < 0:003; Fig. 2). There were no di erences between subject groups in mean arousal levels, nor were any of the subgroup interaction terms signi cant. Thus there were no di erences between smokers and nonsmokers in their pro les of self-rated feelings of arousal. Again, this agrees with the many studies where current smokers and non-smokers report similar average levels of arousal/alertness (see review by Wesnes and Parrott 2 ). There was a slight, but statistically signi cant increase in the number of cigarettes smoked at night (22.3 compared to 19.4 during the day, p < 0:023). This con rmed our prediction that more cigarettes would be smoked during the night shift, since it was expected to be more tiring and stressful than the day shift. However, it was also expected that any increase in cigarette consumption would be accompanied by a greater degree of arousal modulation and stress modulation. But as noted earlier, there was no di erence in the amount of stress or arousal change between shifts. However, it may be that the numbers of cigarettes consumed during the day and night were too similar for any di erential psychological e ects to be evident. Overall, therefore, the reason for the greater cigarette consumption at night needs to be further investigated. It might be related to a greater opportunity for smoking breaks at night, or the less demanding work pro les typically found. 25 The present study has raised a number of issues for further investigation. The degree of adaptation to the shift period was not recorded in this study. Therefore, the shift groups may have included a range of subjects, some of whom were well adapted (ie already working more than 1 week on that shift pattern) and others who were poorly adjusted (ie new to that shift schedule). Further studies might investigate this factor and compare well adjusted and poorly adjusted workers. Another topic for study could be the use of cigarettes by smokers during the process of shift adaptation. Finally, the e ects of nicotine deprivation and smoking cessation during shifts need to be studied. Does the night shift constitute a particularly di cult period to go without cigarettes, or is smoking cessation equally di cult during the night and day? ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We would like to thank Letchworth and Berkhamsted Police Stations and the Nalcanco Factory, Milton Keynes for allowing their employees to take part in this study. REFERENCES 1. Golding, J. and Mangan, G. L. Arousing and dearousing e ects of cigarette smoking under conditions of stress and mild sensory isolation. Psychophysiology, 1982; 19(4): 449±452. 2. Wesnes, K. and Parrott, A. C. Smoking, nicotine and human performance. In: Factors A ecting Human Performance, Vol. II. Smith, A. and Jones, D. (Eds) Academic Press, New York, 1992. 3. Wesnes, K. and Warburton, D. M. Smoking, nicotine and human performance. Pharmacol. Therapeut. 1983; 21: 189±208. 4. Dunn, W. L. Experimental methods and conceptual models as applied to the study of motivation in cigarette smoking. In Smoking Behaviour Motives and Incentives. Dunn, W. L. (Ed.) Winston, Washington DC, 1973. 5. Parrott, A. C. Individual di erences in stress and arousal during cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology, (1994); 115: 389±396. 6. Russell, M. A. H., Peto, J. and Patel, V. A. The classi cation of smoking by a factorial structure of motives. J. Roy. Statist. Soc. 1974; 137: 313±346. 7. Silverstein. Cigarette smoking, nicotine addiction and relaxation. J. Personal. and Soc. Psychol. 1982; 42: 946±950. 8. Folkard, S. Diurnal variation. In: Stress and Fatigue in Human Performance. Hockey, R. (Ed.) Wiley, Chichester, 1983, pp. 245±272. 9. Parrott, A. C. and Joyce, C. Stress and arousal rhythms in cigarette smokers, deprived smokers and nonsmokers. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 1993; 8: 21±28. 10. Mackay, C. J., Cox, T., Burrows, G. and Lazzenni, T. An inventory for the measurement of selfreported stress and arousal. Brit. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol., 1978; 17: 283±284. 11. Parrott, A. C. Cigarette smoking: E ects upon selfrated stress and arousal over the day. Addict. Behav. 1993; 18: 389±395. 12. Cox, D. B., Blaxter, M. et al. The Health and Lifestyle Survey. The Health Promotion Trust, London, 1987. 13. West, R. J. Bene cial e ects of nicotine: Fact or ction? Addiction 1993; 88: 589±590. 14. Robertson, J. A. Coping, social support and drinking among young married couples. In: Addiction Controversies. Warburton, D. (Ed.) Harwood, London, 1990, pp. 296±313.

M. E. E. JONES AND A. C. PARROTT 97 15. Cohen, S. and Williamson, G. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. In: The Social Psychology of Health. Spacapan, S. and Oskamp, S. (Eds) Sage, California, 1988, pp. 31±68. 16. Cohen, S. and Lichtenstein, E. Perceived stress, quitting smoking and smoking relapse. Health Psychol. 1990; 9: 466±478. 17. West, R. J. The nicotine replacement paradox in smoking cessation: How does nicotine gum really work? Brit. J. Addict. 1992; 87: 165±167. 18. Gilbert, D. G. and Wesler, R. Emotion, anxiety and smoking. In: Smoking and Human Behaviour. Ney, T. and Gale, A. (Eds) Wiley, Chichester, 1989. 19. Pomerleau, O. F. and Pomerleau, C. S. Research on stress and smoking: Progress and problems. Brit. J. Addict. 1991; 86: 599±604. 20. Warburton, D. M. The puzzle of nicotine use. In: The Pharmacology of Addiction. Lader, M. (Ed.) Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1988. 21. Schachter, S. Pharmacological and psychological determinants of smoking. In: Smoking, Behaviour, Physiological and Psychological In uences. Thornton, R. E. (Ed.) Churchill-Livingstone, Edinburgh, 1978. 22. Parrott, A. C. Does cigarette smoking increase stress? Addiction 1994; 89: 142±144. 23. Thayer, R. E. The Biopsychology of Mood and Arousal. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1989. 24. Benowitz, N. L. Pharmacokinetic considerations in understanding nicotine dependence. In: The Biology of Nicotine Dependence. Ciba Foundation Report 152, Wiley, Chichester, 1990, pp. 186±209. 25. Grandjean, E. P., Baschera, P., Martin, E. and Weber, A. The e ects of various conditions on subjective states and critical icker fusion frequency. In: Vigilance. Mackie, R. R. (Ed.) Plenum, New York, 1977, pp. 331±339.