Individual differences in difficulty quitting smoking

Similar documents
Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up

A comparison of the abuse liability and dependence potential of nicotine patch, gum, spray and inhaler

TWIN VALLEY BEHAVIORAL HEALTHCARE CLINICAL GUIDELINES FOR MANAGEMENT OF SMOKING CESSATION

Evaluation of the mood and physical symptoms scale (MPSS) to assess cigarette withdrawal

Integrating Tobacco Cessation into Practice

Is there any way you might be better off if you quit? What happens when you think about it? What do you imagine will happen if you don t change?

15 INSTRUCTOR GUIDELINES

ASSISTING PATIENTS with QUITTING EFFECTS OF CLINICIAN INTERVENTIONS. The 5 A s. The 5 A s (cont d)

EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTIONS TO HELP PATIENTS QUIT TOBACCO

Addiction and Dependence

Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings. Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005

BASIC VOLUME. Elements of Drug Dependence Treatment

Alcoholism has been demonstrated to have a genetic component, especially among men.

Session 1: Days 1-3. Session 4: Days Session 2: Days 4-7. Session 5: Days Session 3: Days Day 21: Quit Day!

Trait anxiety and nicotine dependence in adolescents A report from the DANDY study

Proactive peer support by telephone to help women quit smoking

VOLUME B. Elements of Psychological Treatment

Smoking and Smoking Cessation in England 2011: Findings from the Smoking Toolkit Study

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION. Randomized Comparative Trial of Nicotine Polacrilex, a Transdermal Patch, Nasal Spray, and an Inhaler

Manual of Smoking Cessation

Outcome Milestones in Smoking Cessation: An Organizational Framework. Deborah M. Scharf. BA, McGill University, 2000

Examples of what to say when intervening with smoking clients. Do you smoke cigarettes or tobacco at all, or have you ever smoked regularly?

Why Tobacco Cessation?

Tobacco Withdrawal in Women and Menstrual Cycle Phase

Treatment Outcomes from the TDC: A Look at Smoking Cessation Among Patients with Co- Occurring Disorders

Executive Summary. Context. Guideline Origins

Difference Between Men and Women in Drug Use Disorders

ASK Ask ASK about tobacco use ADVISE HANDOUT

RECOMMENDATIONS FOR HEALTH CARE PROVIDERS

Brief Report Time-Varying Smoking Abstinence Predicts Lower Depressive Symptoms Following Smoking Cessation Treatment

Evaluating Smokers Reactions to Advertising for New Lower Nicotine Quest Cigarettes

UMASS TOBACCO TREATMENT SPECIALIST CORE TRAINING

16851 Mount Wolfe Road Caledon ON L7E 3P or 1 (855)

Disclosure 8/27/2014. Smoking and Substance Abuse in Individuals. ADHD and Drug Abuse

Modifications to Labeling of Nicotine Replacement Therapy Products for Over-the-Counter

Tobacco Dependence in Youths Les jeunes et la dépendance au tabac. Joseph R. DiFranza, M.D. University of Massachusetts Medical School

Stage Based Interventions for Tobacco Cessation

Examining the relationship between daily changes in support and smoking around a selfset. quit date. Urte Scholz. University of Zurich

Smoking and mental health A joint report by the Royal College of Physicians and the Royal College of Psychiatrists Endorsed by:

The 5A's are practice guidelines on tobacco use prevention and cessation treatment (4):

Practical advice on smoking cessation: Patients with long-term conditions

Brief Intervention for Smoking Cessation. National Training Programme

5. Offer pharmacotherapy to all smokers who are attempting to quit, unless contraindicated.

SACTob Position Statement on Nicotine and Its Regulation in Tobacco and Non-Tobacco Products.

Contingency Management with Adolescents and Their Families

Increase in common cold symptoms and mouth ulcers following

Northwick Park Mental Health Centre Smoking Cessation Report October Plan. Act. Study. Introduction

Addictive Properties of Caffeine. Roland R. Griffiths, Ph.D. Departments of Psychiatry and Neuroscience Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine

Combined Alcohol and Drug Abuse Problems. Edward Gottheil, Section Editor

Sociology 3308: Sociology of Emotions. Prof. J. S. Kenney. Overheads Class 5-6: The Psychology of Emotions:

Comprehensive Smoking Cessation Programs

Best practice for brief tobacco cessation interventions. Hayden McRobbie The Dragon Institute for Innovation

TRENDS IN SUBSTANCE USE AND ASSOCIATED HEALTH PROBLEMS

Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision

Clearing the Air: What You Need to Know and Do to Prepare to Quit Smoking. Getting Ready to Quit Course

Attachment A Brighton & Hove City Council

How to think AND feel about nicotine and the people who use it

BASIC SKILLS FOR WORKING WITH SMOKERS

0 questions at random and keep in order

Phenomenology/Phenomenography

Northern Tobacco Use Monitoring Survey Northwest Territories Report. Health and Social Services

Nicotine Reduction: Harm Reduction not Prohibition

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Available online at Addictive Behaviors xx (2008) xxx xxx

Manuscripts submitted to Nicotine & Tobacco Research. Nicotine & Tobacco Research Advance Access published September 25, 2014

Tailoring Tobacco Treatment Services based on Psychological Factors

Behavioral Patterns of Tobacco Addiction of Students at Some Universities in HCMC Viet Nam

Relapse Prevention. Jennifer S. B. Moran, MA, TTS MFMER slide-1

Note: Staff who work in case management programs should attend the AIDS Institute training, "Addressing Prevention in HIV Case Management.

ASSISTING PATIENTS with QUITTING

A randomised trial of glucose tablets to aid smoking cessation

The Quit Clinic As an Anti-smoking Advocacy Tool

5. is the process of moving from the specific to the general. a. Deduction

Epidemiology of Hardcore Smoking: The Need to Advance the Field

Update on Medications for Tobacco Cessation

Tobacco Research at NIDA: Neuroscience, Treatment and Regulatory-related studies

Initiation of Smoking and Other Addictive Behaviors: Understanding the Process

BASIC SKILLS FOR WORKING WITH SMOKERS

Treatment Outcomes of a Tailored Smoking Cessation Programme for Individuals Accessing Addiction Treatment Services

STRESS AND AROUSAL CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS IN SMOKERS AND NON-SMOKERS WORKING DAY AND NIGHT SHIFTS

Choosing Life: Empowerment, Action, Results! CLEAR Menu Sessions. Substance Use Risk 5: Drugs, Alcohol, and HIV

Fundamentals of Brief Cessation Counseling Approaches

Smoking Cessation: Nurse Interventions and Effective Programs

SMOKING CESSATION IS HARD

Project TEACH Addressing Tobacco Treatment for Pregnant Women Jan Blalock, Ph.D.

What recovery means: Independent living. Control of symptoms. Active remission of substance use. Competitive employment

Tobacco Dependence Screening & Treatment

CANDIS. A Marijuana Treatment Program for Youth and Adults SCOPE AND SEQUENCE. An Evidence-Based Program from

the high CVD risk smoker

Psychology 205, Revelle, Fall 2014 Research Methods in Psychology Mid-Term. Name:

Nicotine Management and Smoke Free

PHD THESIS STUDY ON THE USE OF THE PHARMACOLOGIC THERAPY WITH BUPROPION ON TOBACCO ABSTINENCE FOR SMOKERS WITH HIGH NICOTINE DEPENDENCE

Smoking cessation: the value of working together. Dr Donita Baird and Dr Sarah L White

Tobacco Cessation For Providers. Betty Murphy Health Promotion Program Naval Hospital Rota

Smoking Cessation Counseling Strategies

Moving Toward Wellness: Smoking Cessation

Tobacco Use & Multiple Risk Factors:

Smoking Cessation. Disclosures. Thank You. None

Researchers are studying the link between genes and addiction to learn how to better prevent and treat this potentially deadly illness.

Perceived relative harm of e cigarettes and attitudes towards their availability, advertising and use: A longitudinal survey

Transcription:

British Joumal of Addiction (1991) 86, 555-558 Individual differences in difficulty quitting smoking PETR HAJEK ICRF Health Behaviour Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, 101 Denmark Hill, London SES 8AP, UK Abstract This essay comments on the main approaches to studying individual differences in ability to quit smoking, and suggests complementing them with a concept of 'difficulty of quitting'. Several areas of research into determinants of ability to quit smoking are discussed, including measures of dependence, reactivity to nicotine, severity of withdrawal symptoms, endurance of discomfort, and psychological well being. A number of correlates of inability to quit may have a common denominator in stress and neuroticism. If these are the primary obstacles to quitting, changes in the composition of the smoking population and in the image of smoking can be expected which could lead to smoking losing some of its appeal for potential new recruits. Introduction After the second world war, the majority of the male population of Great Britain and the USA smoked. Smoking was a 'normal' male behaviour and as such it did not invite the types of question which are emerging now. In the 'normal people smoke' days, one would probably be more interested in examining the pathology of non-smokers. (Incidentally, when smoking prevalence was at these high levels, the peculiarity of non-smoking was emphasized by the fact that neither Hitler nor Mussolini smoked, while Stalin, Roosevelt and Churchill did.) When there was no reason not to smoke, the most interesting behavioural difference between smokers, the ability to quit, was almost totally invisible. Currently in Western developed countries most smokers want to quit. The question of why some succeed (with varying degrees of subjective difficulty), while others do not, is increasing in importance. If current trends continue, smoking in developed countries may become primarily the domain of those who cannot quit. If the hypotheses at the end of this paper are justified, difficulty of quitting will emerge as being determined primarily by variables pertaining to psychological health. Smoking will then adjoin the field of drug abuse, in as much as some kind of psychosocial pathology will be seen as an almost inevitable accompaniment of regular use. The 'normal people don't smoke' era will dawn. Approaches to studying individual differences in ability to quit smoking Two main contexts in which individual differences in quitting smoking have been discussed so far are the concept of dependence, and theories of relapse. They are associated with different models of determinants of success or failure in quitting smoking. In a sense, inability to quit constitutes the core of the concept of dependence. Under this view, the assertion that inability to quit is determined by degree of dependence is a tautology; dependence and inability to quit being essentially synonymous. Current work on dependence however often concentrates on mapping its hypothetical biological 555

556 Fetr Hajek causes such as nicotine tolerance and withdrawal. Variables such as endurance of discomfort or neuroticism are usually not part of the agenda. There is also a tendency to treat dependence as if it were the same as level of intake of nicotine. Thus questionnaire measures of dependence which show only small and inconsistent relationships with various indices of difficulty of quitting (Hughes et al, 1990) tend to be considered as validated if they relate satisfactorily to biochemical measures of nicotine intake. Theories of relapse are another area where individual differences in ability to quit smoking have been examined. As a single observable event, relapse invites enquiry into its immediate causes rather than into some general individual vulnerability, and the currently prevailing approaches emphasize situational and cognitive factors, rather than more enduring dispositions (Sutton, 1989). A strong theoretical perspective can limit the field of enquiry, and affect the interpretation of data. See for instance the literature on self-efficacy. Measures of self-efficacy taken post-treatment correlate with relapse in the near future, but pre-treatment measures in general do not (Sutton, 1989). Thus the post-treatment correlations may be caused by subjects accurately supposing that the degree of difficulty which they have experienced so far is indicative of the degree of difficulty to come. Yet the findings tend to be interpreted as showing that selfefficacy, rather than enduring difficulty of not smoking, determines relapse. Perhaps the relevant individual differences should be considered in terms of a concept such as 'difficulty of quitting, which would be less theoryladen. While the question of causes of dependence tends to implicate biological factors, and the question of causes of relapse suggests immediate antecedents, asking about causes of difficulty of quitting (or difficulty not smoking) could lead to a more open enquiry. It could also help communication, as there is currently no simple term for general use in this context. For instance, infiuences on an attempt to stop smoking of factors such as stress or depression might be better described as affecting difficulty of quitting rather than dependence or probability of relapse. Difficulty of quitting could be seen as encompassing both outcome of an attempt to stop smoking, and the accompanying subjective experience. To operationalize the term, e.g. as a subjective rating, would narrow it, but it could serve other useful functions. Most data on individual differences in quitting smoking come from studies of predictors of treatment outcome and of relapse, and from comparisons of smokers with ex-smokers. This usually leads to smokers being dichotomised into those who quit and those who did not. This dichotomy may obscure some facets of the problem. There may be advantages to complementing the perspective of success and failure with difficulty of quitting as a quantitative subjective variable. The concept of difficulty of quitting smoking (whether in a broad or narrow sense) may provide a complementary frame of reference for considering relevant individual differences. For instance, thinking about quitting smoking in terms of difficulty of quitting invites questions such as how smokers differ in the types of difficulty they encounter (e.g. in their withdrawal phenomenology); what causes these differences; how they are related to outcome in quitting smoking; whether and how difficulties vary at different stages of abstinence; how is difficulty of stopping smoking related to difficulty of staying quit, etc. One question of great importance is the stability of 'difficulty of quitting'. The concept of dependence implies that it is a more stable characteristic than according to current theories of relapse or stages of the smoking career. More research is needed to provide the answers. Further questions on difficulty of quitting suggest themselves. If it is a relatively stable characteristic, would it be an indicator of some general susceptibility to dependency ('addictive personality')? Drug users and alcoholics are very likely to smoke cigarettes, but so are prisoners, schizophrenics, and Spanish physicians. The relevant question is whether drug users and alcoholics find quitting tobacco more difficult than other comparable smokers. Some possible sources of individual differences in ability to quit smoking One of the main determinants of stopping smoking is obviously motivation to quit. The term 'ability to quit' is meant to imply concern with differences between individuals assumed to be matched for their motivation. Future research can be expected to continue to tackle a number of issues at various levels of proximity to quitting behaviour, ranging from differences in nicotine pharmacodynamics to situational influences. It may be that ability to quit will emerge as having a complex set of determinants, but it is also possible that several areas will be found to be interrelated and the problem simplified. The

Individual differences in difficulty quitting smoking 557 following comments concern only some of the areas of potential importance. The most explored predictors of ability to quit so far have been nicotine intake and previous experience of abstinence. They are often put together under the label of dependence (Fagerstrom & Schneider, 1989; West & Russell, 1985). Overall, the usual unexciting finding is that heavy smokers who say that they generally cannot manage without cigarettes find quitting on a particular occasion more difficult. However, the correlations involved are typically rather small. New measures of dependence (in sense of predictors of ability to quit) which incorporate some of the areas covered below as well as some attitudinal and motivational factors are likely to emerge in future. Reactivity to nicotine. Individual differences in reactivity to nicotine may influence difficulty in stopping smoking. For instance, subjective and heart rate reactions to the first cigarette after 24 hour abstinence seem to be related to withdrawal severity (West & Russell, 1988). At a behavioural level, individual differences in responses to nicotine are being explored for example by studies of long-term users of nicotine chewing gum (Hajek et al., 1988) or by newly emerging research on very light smokers (Shiffman, 1989; Shiffman et al., 1990; Hajek & West, 1989). Stable very light smokers are rare (the first tentative longitudinal data are only now emerging), and they seem to obtain similar nicotine exposure per cigarette as other smokers. Looking at how they react to nicotine, and what protects them from escalation to 'normal' heavier smoking, may contribute to explanations of individual differences in ability to quit smoking. Severity of withdrawal. Withdrawal discomfort is generally considered a major obstacle to quitting. Treatments, especially pharmacological ones, are often aimed explicitly at alleviating withdrawal. Measures of withdrawal discomfort seem to be close to directly measuring difficulty of quitting. Yet, for a while it seemed that severity of withdrawal was unrelated to outcome of an attempt to quit smoking (Hughes et al, 1990). Recent studies showed that the two may be related, although not closely (West et al, 1989; Hajek & West, in press), and more prospective studies of such relationships are needed. 'Withdrawal discomfort' is a composite phenomenon and contributions of its various parts need to be explored, together with their causes. Subjective ratings of withdrawal are probably a mixture of objective neurophysiological changes, and the way an individual handles the discomfort which they cause. If we were able to disentangle these two components, the second could well emerge as being at least as important for continuing abstinence as the first. Endurance of discomfort. In drug abuse and alcoholism, much speculative attention has been given to the concept of self-control, but most research data available seem to concern a so far rather unproductive issue of locus of control (Szara, 1986). The general public seems quite sure that quitting smoking is a question of'willpower'. Lack of willpower in this context apparently stands for giving in easily when the struggle gets unpleasant. Low endurance of discomfort seems to be an important part of the picture. There is a possible new opening in exploring this association in the finding that breathholding endurance may predict short-term outcome of an attempt at quitting smoking (Hajek et al, 1987). Breath holding has been shown to be a valid measure of endurance of physical discomfort (Hajek, 1989). Resisting withdrawal discomfort and holding one's breath or maintaining a hand grip might involve similar subjective criteria of what is unbearable and when to stop trying. If endurance of discomfort is related to sensitivity to withdrawal and to likelihood of relapse, it could be relevant to other addictions as well. Psychological well being. Success in quitting smoking seems to be related to 'psychosocial assets' (Ockene et al, 1982) such as good psychological health and supportive family background. The level and appraisal of stress (Ockene et al, 1982; Cohen, 1986), coping skills and strategies (Kamarck & Lichtenstein, 1988), and psychiatric morbidity (Hughes et al, 1986) seem to be important. More data are needed on most of these issues, but there are persuasive theoretical arguments for these connections. A number of theories of smoking which focus on the association between smoking and stress, imply a positive correlation between difficulty of quitting and stress or neuroticism (e.g. Ockene et al, 1981; Grunberg & Baum, 1985; Pomerlau, 1986; Warburton, 1990). The simplest hypothetical connection would be based on the assumption that smoking helps coping with negative states. In that case, more stressed and neurotic smokers should appreciate smoking more, smoke more often, and find quitting more difficult. This is even before any physical addiction is considered.

558 Petr Hajek After quitting, tobacco withdrawal may magnify the now exposed original discomfort, perhaps proportionally to the relief smoking used to provide, and future stress may evoke conditioned urge to smoke. Such arguments seem to provide a common denominator of a number of correlates of quitting, including psychological well being, smoke intake, withdrawal severity, and endurance of discomfort. (The sum total of discomfort associated with abstinence, and endurance of discomfort, could also be considered as independent contributors in a twofactor model of ability to quit smoking.) They present at best only a rough picture in need of much data, but they lead to a variety of testable hypotheses. To return to the theme of the introduction, if stress and neuroticism prevent quitting, the composition of the smoking population in countries where people want to quit may be shifting accordingly; and the image of smoking in the eyes of children could eventually change from a grown-up, rebellious or daring activity to a sign of inadequacy. In this scenario a new approach for anti-smoking campaigns could help to accelerate this process, and we may yet see the beginning of the end to the tobacco epidemic, when potential new recruits start to lose interest in cigarettes. Actmowledgement The support of the Imperial Cancer Research.Fund is gratefully acknowledged. References COHEN, S. (1986) Contrasting the Hassles Scale and the Perceived Stress Scale: Who's really measuring appraised stress?, American Psychologist, 41, pp. 716-718. FAGERSTROM, K.-O. & SCHNEIDER, N. (1989) Measuring nicotine dependence: A review of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire, Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, pp. 159-181. GRUNDBERG, N. & BAUM, A. (1985) Biological commonalities of stress and substance abuse, in: S. SHIFFMAN & T. A. WILLS (Eds) Coping and Substance Use, pp. 25-62 (Orlando, Academic Press). HAJEK, P., BELCHER, M. & STAPLETON, J. (1987) Breathholding endurance as a predictor of success in smoking cessation. Addictive Behaviors, 12, pp. 285-288. HAJEK, P., JACKSON, P. & BELCHER, M. (1988) Long-term use of nicotine chewing gum. Occurrence, determinants, and effect on weight gsan. Journal of American Medical Association, 260, pp. 1593-1596. HAJEK, P. (1989) Breath holding and success in stopping smoking. What does breath holding measure?. The International Journal of Addictions, 24, pp. 633-639. HAJEK, P. & WEST, R. (1989) Occasional smoking: nicotine intake, determinants, and stability over time. The Welcome Trust Grant 030735/Z/89. HAJEK, P. & WEST, R. (in press) On the relationship between withdrawal discomfort in abstaining smokers and relapse. Smoking and Health 1990 (Proceedings of the 7th World Conference). HUGHES, J., HATSUKAMI, D., MITCHELL, J. & DAHLGREN, L. (1986) Prevalence of smoking among psychiatric outpatients, American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, pp. 993-997. HUGHES, J., HIGGINS, S. & HATSUKAMI, D. (1990) Effects of abstinence from tobacco. A critical review, in: KOZLOWSKI et al. (Eds) Research Advances in Alcohol and Drug Problems, Vol. 10, pp. 317-198 (Plenum Publishing Corporation). KAMARCK, T. W. & LICHTENSTEIN, E. (1988) Program adherence and coping strategies as predictors of success in a smoking treatment program. Health Psychology, 7, pp. 557-574. OCKENE, J., NUTALL, R., BENFARI, R., HURWITZ, L & OCKENE, I. (1981) A psychosocial model of smoking cessation and maintenance of cessation. Preventive Medicine, 10, pp. 623-638. OCKENE, J., BENFARI, R., NUTTALL, R., HURWITZ, I. & OCKENE, I. (1982) Relationship of psychosocial factors to smoking behavior change in an intervention program. Preventive Medicine, U, pp. 13-28. POMERLEAU, O. (1986) The 'Why' of tobacco dependence: underlying reinforcing mechanisms in nicotine selfadministration, in: J. K. OCKENE (Ed.) The pharmacologic treatment of tobacco dependence: Proceedings of the World Congress, pp. 32-47 (Cambridge, MA, ISSBP). SHIFFMAN, S. (1989) Tobacco 'chippers' individual differences in tobacco dependence, Psychopharmacology, 97, pp. 539-547. SHIFFMAN, S., FISCHER, L., ZETTLER-SEGAL, M. & BE- NOWITZ, N. (1990) Nicotine explosure among nondependent smokers. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, pp. 333-336. SUTTON, S. (1989) Relapse following smoking cessation: a critical review of current theory and research, in: M. GOSSOP (Ed.) Relapse and addictive behaviour, pp. 41-72 (London, Routledge). SZARA, S. (Ed.) (1986) Neurobiology of behavioral control in drug abuse, NIDA Research Monograph 74, Rockville, MD. WARBURTON, D. M. (1990) Psychopharmacological aspects of nicotine, in: S. WONNACOTT, M. RUSSELL & L R. STOLERMAN (Eds) Nicotine psychopharmacology. Molecular, cellular, and behavioural aspects, pp. 77-111 (Oxford, Oxford University Press). WEST, R. & RUSSELL, M. (1985) Pre-abstinence smoke intake and smoking motivation as predictors of severity of cigarette withdrawal symptoms, Psychopharmacology, 87, pp. 334-336. WEST, R. & RUSSELL, M. (1988) Loss of acute nicotine tolerance and severity of cigarette withdrawal, Psychopharmacology, 94, pp. 563-565. WEST, R., HAJEK, P. & BELCHER, M. (1989) Severity of withdrawal symptoms as a predictor of outcome of an attempt to quit smoking. Psychological Medicine, 19, pp. 981-985.