Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: an event-related brain potential study

Similar documents
Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Material-speci c neural correlates of memory retrieval

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

The face-sensitive N170 encodes social category information Jonathan B. Freeman, Nalini Ambady and Phillip J. Holcomb

Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

The face-sensitive N170 encodes social category information Jonathan B. Freeman a, Nalini Ambady a and Phillip J. Holcomb a

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier

Task-switching and memory retrieval processing: Electrophysiological evidence.

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Functional Dissociation of the Left and Right Fusiform Gyrus in Self-Face Recognition

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Cogniţie, Creier, Comportament / Cognition, Brain, Behavior Vol. IX(3), , 2005 Romanian Association of Cognitive Sciences

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Visual self-recognition in patients with schizophrenia

Neurophysiological correlates of memory illusion in both encoding and retrieval phases

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Event-related potentials and time course of the other-race face classi cation advantage

The time-course of intermodal binding between seeing and hearing affective information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Introduction

Is my voice just a familiar voice? An electrophysiological study

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

An event-related potential study on the early processing of crying faces Hendriks, M.C.P.; van Boxtel, Geert; Vingerhoets, Ad

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Neuropsychologia xxx (2009) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: Direct versus indirect memory tasks

Gender difference in N170 elicited under oddball task

ERP Correlates of Identity Negative Priming

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

Familiar-face recognition and comparison: source analysis of scalp-recorded event-related potentials

Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Tilburg University. Published in: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. Publication date: Link to publication

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

Discovering Processing Stages by combining EEG with Hidden Markov Models

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces?

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

PROOF COVER SHEET. For detailed guidance on how to check your proofs, please see

Perceptual asynchrony: motion leads color

The Mechanism of Valence-Space Metaphors: ERP Evidence for Affective Word Processing

Interaction of facial expressions and familiarity: ERP evidence

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society

Address for correspondence: School of Psychological Sciences, Zochonis Building, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, M139PL, Manchester, UK

Title change detection system in the visu

The Spatial-verbal Difference in the N-back Task: An ERP Study

The Central Nervous System

In what way does the parietal ERP old new effect index recollection?

Neurophysiology & EEG

Event-related brain potentials distinguish processing stages involved in face perception and recognition

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014)

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Outline. Biological Psychology: Research Methods. Dr. Katherine Mickley Steinmetz

Biological Psychology

Reward positivity is elicited by monetary reward in the absence of response choice Sergio Varona-Moya a,b, Joaquín Morís b,c and David Luque b,d

Frontal steady-state potential changes predict long-term recognition memory performance

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm

ERP Correlates of Feedback and Reward Processing in the Presence and Absence of Response Choice

ERP evidence for task modulations on face perceptual processing at different spatial scales

International Journal of Psychophysiology

Sensitivity of Electrophysiological Activity from Medial Frontal Cortex to Utilitarian and Performance Feedback

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions

Generalization Effect of Conditioned Sadness: An Innovation of the Approach to Elicit Implicit Sadness

Electrophysiological indices of memory for temporal order in early childhood: implications for the development of recollection

The relationship between electrophysiological correlates of recollection and amount of information retrieved

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

The effects of covert attention and stimulus complexity on the P3 response during an auditory continuous performance task

For Non-Commercial Academic Use Only

Supporting Information

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures

Sun, D; Chan, CCH; Hu, Y; Wang, Z; Lee, TMC

Early brain potentials link repetition blindness, priming and novelty detection

Social class affects neural empathic responses

Working Memory Load for Faces Modulates P300, N170, and N250r


Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention.

Transcription:

COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: an event-related brain potential study Jie Sui a,ying Zhu a and Shihui Han a,b a Department of Psychology, Peking University and b Learning and Cognition Laboratory,Capital Normal University, Beijing, PR China Correspondence and requests for reprints to Professor Shihui Han, PhD, Department of Psychology, Peking University, 5 Yiheyuan Road, Beijing1871, PR China Tel: + 81 275 18; fax: + 81 27181; e-mail: shan@pku.edu.cn Sponsorship: This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Project 2252, 281 and 2741) and the China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (Grant No. 2414). Received 2 November 25; revised 4 January 2; accepted 4 January 2 This study investigated whether neural mechanisms of self-face recognition are modulated by attention by recording event-related brain potentials associated with self-face recognition. Participants identi ed head orientations of self-faces and familiar or unfamiliar other faces presented brie y at the center of the visual eld. Eventrelated brain potentials to self-faces and other faces were recorded when self-faces and familiar or unfamiliar other faces were either task relevant (attended) or irrelevant (unattended) in separate blocks of trials.we found that early face-speci c event-related brain potential components such as the N17 and vertex positive potential did not di er between self-faces and other faces. Relative to familiar faces, however, self-faces induced an increased positivity over the frontocentral area at 22^7 ms. The increased positivity to self-faces relative to familiar faces between 5 and 7 ms was reduced in the attended relative to the unattended conditions, which arose from the fact that the amplitude to familiar faces during this time window was increased in the attended relative to the unattended conditions, whereas the event-related brain potential amplitude to self-faces was not in uenced by attention. The event-related brain potential results suggest an automatic process of self-face recognition in human brains that occurs after face structure encoding and is independent of task relevance. NeuroReport 17:42^ 427 c 2 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Keywords: attention, event-related potential, face recognition, self Introduction Human beings are extremely good at recognition of human faces that provide affective and other social information. For example, humans respond faster to self-faces than to other faces, and this self-face advantage has been demonstrated over either familiar or unfamiliar faces and is evident even when face stimuli are presented in unfamiliar views [1,2]. It has been suggested that recognition of self-faces is an indicator of higher-order self-awareness and involves neural substrates beyond those involved in general face recognition and exists only in higher primates [ ]. While neuropsychological studies suggest that self-face recognition is dominated by the right hemisphere ([4,5,7,8], but see []), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) studies have identified neural activities in the right superior frontal gyrus linked to self-face recognition [1,11]. The anterior cingulate, left prefrontal, superior temporal, and inferior parietal cortex also show stronger activation to the morphed version of self-faces than to unknown faces or partners faces [,12]. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) have also been used to examine the neural mechanisms underlying self-face recognition. It has been shown that selffaces elicited a positivity peaking at 4 ms after stimulus onset (P) relative to unfamiliar faces [1]. As self-faces appeared less (%) than unfamiliar faces (%) in the study by Ninomiya et al. [1], the enhanced P to self-faces might arise from the difference in stimulus probability. Passively viewing self-faces produced a positivity at 2 ms (P2) with smaller amplitude relative to passively viewing a famous face (the president s face) [14]. As familiarity of selffaces and famous faces may not be equally matched (e.g. participants saw themselves in mirrors more frequently than they saw the president in the media), the results of Caharel et al. [14] may index the processing of face familiarity rather than self-specific processing. This study examined the temporal aspects of the neural mechanisms of self-face recognition while controlling the possible flaws of the previous work. We recorded ERPs to self-faces and other faces shown with equal probability. In addition, to match familiarity of self-faces and other faces, we took pictures from the participants in this study and their classmates or roommates, whom the participants saw every day. We also investigated whether the neural mechanisms underlying self-face recognition are modulated by attention by comparing the ERP differences between selfface and other-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions. Previous work required participants to passively view self-faces [1,14] and thus reported the self-face effect 5-45 c Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Vol 17 No 4 2 March 2 42

SUI ETAL. only in an unattended condition. A recent positron emission tomography (PET) study examined attentional effects on neural representation of self-faces by comparing regional cerebral blood flow between the conditions when active or passive self-face recognition was required [15]. The researchers found increased activity in the prefrontal cortex and right anterior cingulate in the active relative to the passive viewing condition. As stimulus familiarity was not matched between self-faces and other faces, it is unclear whether the neural activity observed reflected the enhanced recognition of self-faces or familiar faces per se. The PET results also lacked temporal information of self-face recognition because of the long latency of PET signals. It has been proposed that self-face processing demands less attention resources than that of other faces [2]. We assessed this proposal by having participants identify head orientations of self-faces or other faces in separate blocks of trials, and thus self-faces and other faces could be task relevant (attended) or task irrelevant (unattended). Self-specific processing was identified by comparing ERPs to self-faces and familiar faces. The process of face familiarity was examined by comparing ERPs to familiar and unfamiliar other faces. Materials and methods Study participants Eighteen healthy participants (eight men, 1 women, aged between 2 and 2 years; mean 24.772.4 years) took part in this study as paid volunteers. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were right handed. Three participants were excluded from data analysis because of excessive artifacts during electroencephalogram (EEG) recording. Informed consent was obtained from all participants before the study. This study was approved by a local ethics committee. Stimuli and procedure Each participant was presented with images of self-face, one familiar other face, and one unfamiliar other face matched for sex and age. Thus, stimulus probability of self-face, familiar, and unfamiliar faces was identical. The familiar face stimuli were taken from the classmates or roommates of the participants, whom the participants had known for at least years. Ten face pictures of each participant, with a neutral facial expression, were taken using a digital camera. Participants heads were oriented to the left (from 451 1) in five pictures and to the right in the others. All images were calibrated in luminance and contrast and were converted into jpg format. Each face stimulus was presented in color and subtended a visual angle of.1.1 at a viewing distance of 12 cm. The stimuli were presented on a black background of a 21-inch color monitor. Each trial began with the presentation of a fixation cross for ms, which was followed by a blank screen for 1 ms. A face image was then displayed for ms, overlapping the fixation and followed by a blank screen for 7 ms. Twelve blocks of 4 trials were included. In four blocks of trials, participants identified head orientations of self-faces, familiar faces, or unfamiliar faces by pressing the left or right buttons using the left and right index fingers while ignoring other faces. The order of the tasks was counterbalanced across participants. Instructions emphasized both accuracy and response speed. Electrophysiological data recording and analysis The EEG was recorded from electrodes placed at 1 2 standard positions and five other pairs of nonstandard sites. Recordings were made with respect to the left and right mastoid references. The electrode impedance was kept below 5 ko. The EEG was amplified by using a bandpass of.1 75 Hz (1/2 amplitude cutoffs), digitized at 25 Hz/ channel. The vertical electrooculogram was monitored from two electrodes placed above and below the right eye. The horizontal electrooculogram was recorded from electrodes placed about 1.5 cm lateral to the left and right external canthi. ERPs were averaged offline using a computer program that extracted epochs of EEG beginning 2 ms before stimulus onset and continuing for 1 ms. Trials containing eye blinks, eye movement deflections exceeding 75 mv at any electrode, or incorrect behavioral responses were excluded from the ERP averages. The baseline for ERP measurements was the mean voltage of a 2-ms prestimulus interval and the latency was measured relative to the stimulus onset. Reaction times and response accuracy to different faces were compared using a paired t-test. The mean ERP amplitudes were submitted to repeated-measures analyses of variance with face owner (self, familiar, unfamiliar faces), attention (task relevant vs. irrelevant), and hemisphere (electrodes over the left vs. right hemispheres) as independent variables. Results Behavioral data A main effect of face owner on reaction times [F(2,28)¼5.44, Po.5] was observed. Responses to self-faces (551 ms) were faster than those to familiar faces (5 ms) [t(14)¼4.4, Po.1] and unfamiliar faces (588 ms) [t(14)¼2.28, Po.5]. Reaction times, however, did not differ between the familiar and unfamiliar faces [t(14)¼.4, P4.5]. The response accuracy did not differ among the three conditions [%, %, and 8%, to self-faces, familiar faces, and unfamiliar faces, respectively, F(2, 28)¼.5, P4.5]. Electrophysiological data Figure 1 illustrates the grand-average ERPs to familiar and unfamiliar faces, and self-faces. ERPs to both self-faces and other faces were characterized by a negativity peaking at 148 and 188 ms (N17), which was followed by a negativegoing wave at 22 and ms (posterior N2). A positivity at 148 188 ms [vertex positive potential (VPP)] at middle, central, and frontal sites and a negative-going component at 22 ms (anterior N2) were observed. A long-latency positive component at 7 ms was evident over the frontal, central, and parietal areas. The analyses of variance of the mean ERP amplitudes did not show any significant effects before ms. To examine the ERP effects of face familiarity, we compared ERPs elicited by familiar and unfamiliar faces. A main effect of attention at 7 ms at FCz, Cz, CPz, Pz, FC FC4, C C4, CP CP4, and P P4 [F(1,14)¼5. 7.2, all Po.5] was observed; familiar and unfamiliar other faces elicited a long-latency positivity with larger amplitudes in the attended than in the unattended conditions. Neither the main effect of face owner (familiar vs. unfamiliar faces) nor the interaction between attention and face owner was significant (P4.5). 424 Vol17 No4 2March2

SELF-FACE RECOGNITION AND ATTENTION (a) Amplitude (µv) Amplitude (µv) 12 Cz P8 VPP N17 Attended familiar Attended unfamiliar Unattended familiar Unattended unfamiliar N2 Attended familiar Attended self Unattended familiar Unattended self 2 2 4 8 ms 2 2 4 Fig. 1 (a) Grand-average ERPs elicited by familiar and unfamiliar faces recorded at Cz and P8. (b) Grand-average ERPs elicited by self-faces and familiar other faces recorded at Cz and P8. ERP, event-related potentials; VPP, vertex positive potential;, long-latency positive component. (b) 12 Cz P8 VPP N17 N2 8 ms (a) 4 ms 4 ms 5 ms 5 ms ms ms 7 ms (b) +15. µv 7 ms +4. µv Fig. 2 Voltage topographies of the di erence wave obtained by subtracting event-related potentials to self-faces from those to familiar other faces. (a) Self-faces minus familiar faces in the attended condition. (b) Selffaces minus familiar faces in the unattended condition; the di erence waves in both attended and unattended conditions showed a frontocentral scalp distribution. To examine the self-specific ERP effects, ERPs to self-faces were compared with those to familiar faces. A significant main effect of face owner at 22 7 ms at FCz, Cz, CPz, Pz, FC FC4, C C4, CP CP4, and P P4 [F(1,14)¼.5 54.4, all Po.5] was found, indicating that the long-latency positivity was of larger amplitudes to self-faces than to familiar faces. A reliable main effect of attention at 22 7 ms at FCz, Cz, CPz, Pz, FC FC4, C C4, CP CP4, and P P4 [F(1,14)¼5.4 1.14, all Po.5] was observed, suggesting that the long-latency positivity was of larger amplitudes in the attended than in the unattended conditions. A reliable interaction was also noted between face owner and attention at 5 7 ms at Cz, CPz, Pz, and CP CP4 [F(2,28)¼4.27 7.2, all Po.5]; the self-face ERP effect during this time window was reduced in the attended relative to the unattended condition. This interaction stemmed from the fact that the mean amplitude to familiar faces was increased in the attended relative to the unattended conditions at Cz, CPz, Pz, and CP CP4 [F(1,14)¼.75 2.55, all Po.5], whereas attention did not influence the amplitude to self-faces (P4.5). The voltage topographies of the difference waves obtained by subtracting ERPs to familiar faces from those to self-faces showed that the enhanced positivity in association with selfface recognition had a focus over the frontocentral scalp sites in both attended and unattended conditions (Fig. 2). Discussion Consistent with previous observations [1,2], our behavioral data showed that responses were faster to self-faces than to unfamiliar or familiar other faces, suggesting either more salient or earlier representation of self-faces [2]. Our ERP results showed, first, that both self-faces and other faces Vol17 No4 2March2 425

SUI ETAL. elicited a posterior N17 and an anterior VPP. In addition, we found that the amplitudes of the N17 and VPP did not differ between familiar and unfamiliar faces. The results are consistent with previous research [1 18] and indicate that the N17 and VPP mainly reflect the process of structural encoding of face stimuli [1]. We showed further that the N17 and the VPP did not differ between self-faces and other faces, suggesting that self-faces could not be distinguished from other faces at the early stage of face structural encoding. We found, however, that a long-latency positivity over the frontocentral area was involved in dissociating self-faces from other faces. The long-latency positivity over the frontocentral area at 22 7 ms was of larger amplitude to self-faces than to familiar faces. In addition, these effects were evident when self-faces and other faces were both task relevant and irrelevant. In contrast, the long-latency positivity did not differ between familiar and unfamiliar faces. By controlling well factors such as target probability and face familiarity, our results indicate that the increased long-latency positivity over the frontocentral area reflects enhanced neural activity associated with self-face recognition. In addition, our ERP results indicate that self-faces could be distinguished from other faces as early as 22 ms after sensory stimulation. The self-face ERP effect was obtained in the task of identifying head orientation rather than face owners, which implies that the ERP index of selfface recognition may occur even when participants are not required to perform an explicit face-recognition task. Prior fmri studies have shown that the anterior brain structures such as the frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate are engaged in self-face recognition [,11,12,15]. In agreement with the previous results, the self-face ERP effect observed in the current study was evident mainly over the frontocentral areas. The increased long-latency positivity may reflect the enhanced self-face recognition compared with other-face recognition regardless of whether the other faces were familiar or unfamiliar. Alternatively, as studies of social psychology suggest that perception of individuals may induce the spontaneous activation of traits and attitudes associated with the perceived individuals [2,21], the self-face ERP effect may reflect automatically generated representation of one s personal traits or other episodic information that arose from the perception of self-faces. Consistent with this analysis, prior fmri studies have shown evidence that the medial prefrontal cortex shows increased activity associated with judgment of self-descriptive traits relative to other-descriptive traits [22,2]. Whatever the case, our ERP results indicate that self-awareness induced by self-faces can occur as early as 22 ms after stimulus onset over the frontocentral area. More interestingly, we found that the increased positivity at 22 5 ms linked to self-faces relative to familiar faces did not differ when the face stimuli were task relevant and irrelevant. The interaction between face owner and attention at 5 7 ms reflected that the ERP amplitudes to familiar and unfamiliar faces (but not to self-faces) were increased in the attended more than unattended conditions. Taken together, these ERP results suggest that, relative to familiarface recognition, self-face recognition was less influenced by task relevance. Our ERP findings support the proposal that self-face recognition demands less attentional resources relative to other-face recognition [2]. The differential attentional effects on self-face and other-face recognition further indicate that self-face recognition is underpinned by unique mechanisms compared with otherface recognition. Previous ERP studies show evidence that familiar faces can be distinguished from unfamiliar faces as early as 25 [24] or ms [1] after stimulus onset. The current experiment, however, did not find any difference in ERPs between familiar and unfamiliar faces. As previous work used only familiar and unfamiliar faces [1,24] or only self-faces and unfamiliar faces [14], the ERP results of these works mainly reflected the neural activity associated with the process of face familiarity. In the current experiment, self-faces were intermixed with familiar and unfamiliar faces and could not be distinguished from other faces by face familiarity. As a result, mainly the mechanisms of self-face recognition were activated to distinguish self-faces from other faces. It follows that familiar-face recognition was inhibited and thus the familiar and unfamiliar other faces were difficult to distinguish from each other. It appears that the involvement of selffaces with other faces may greatly influence the discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar faces. Conclusion We found that self-face recognition was indexed by a longlatency positivity at 22 7 ms and the ERP correlates of self-face recognition were not influenced by the task relevance of self-faces. The results provided evidence for an automatic process of self-face recognition in human brains that occurs after face structure encoding. References 1. Keenan JP, McCutcheon B, Sanders G, Freund S, Gallup GG, Pascual- Leone A. Left hand advantage in a self-face recognition task. Neuropsychologia 1; 7:1421 1425. 2. Tong F, Nakayama K. Robust representations for faces: evidence from visual search. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1; 25:11 15.. Gallup GG. Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science 17; 17:8 87. 4. Keenan JP, Wheeler MA, Gallup GG, Pascual-Leone A. Self-recognition and the right prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 2; 4:8 44. 5. Keenan JP, Nelson AM, Pascual-Leone A. Self-recognition and the right hemisphere. Nature 21; 4:5.. Kircher TTJ, Senior C, Phillips ML, Rabe-Hesketh S, Benson PJ, Bullmore ET, et al. Recognizing one s own face. Cognition 21; 78:B1 B15. 7. Breen N, Caine D, Coltheart M. Mirrored-self misidentification: two case of focal onset dementia. Neurocase 21; 7:2 254. 8. Keenan JP, Gallup GG, Falk D. The face in the mirror: the search for the origins of consciousness. New York: HarperCollins/Ecco; 2.. Turk DJ, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM, Funnel MG, Gazzaniga MS, Macrae CM. Mike or me? Self-recognition in a split-brain patient. Nat Neurosci 22; 5:841 842. 1. Platek SM, Gallup GG. Self-face recognition is affected by schizotypal personality traits. Schizophr Res 22; 57:11 15. 11. Platek SM, Keenan JP, Gallup GG, Mohamed FB. Where am I? The neurological correlates of self and other. Cogn Brain Res 24; 1:114 122. 12. Kircher T, Senior C, Phillips ML, Benson PJ, Bullmore ET, Brummer M, et al. Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self-processing: effect of faces and words. Cogn Brain Res 2; 1:1 144. 1. Ninomiya H, Onitsuka T, Chen CH, Sato E, Tashiro N. P in response to the subject s own face. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 18; 52:51 522. 14. Caharel S, Poiroux S, Bernard C, Thibaut F, Lalonde R, Rebai M. ERPs associated with familiarity and degree of familiarity during face recognition. Int J Neurosci 22; 112:14 1512. 15. Sugiura M, Kawashima R, Nakamura K, Okada K, Kato T, Nakamura A, et al. Passive and active recognition of one s own face. Neuroimage 2; 11: 48. 1. Eimer M. Event-related brain potentials distinguish processing stages involved in face perception and recognition. Clin Neurophysiol 2; 111:4 75. 42 Vol17 No4 2March2

SELF-FACE RECOGNITION AND ATTENTION 17. Mnatsakanian EV, Tarkka IM. Matching of familiar faces and abstract patterns: behavioral and high-resolution ERP study. Int J Psychophysiol 2; 47:217 227. 18. Rossion B, Campanella S, Gomez CM, Delinte A, Debatisse B, Liard L, et al. Task modulation of brain activity related to familiar and unfamiliar face processing: an ERP study. Clin Neurophysiol 1; 11:44 42. 1. Bruce V, Young AW. Understanding face recognition. Br J Psychol 18; 77:5 27. 2. Bargh JA, Chen M, Burrows L. Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. J Pers Soc Psychol 1; 71:2 244. 21. Todorov A, Uleman JS. Spontaneous trait inferences are bound to actors faces: evidence from a false recognition paradigm. J Pers Soc Psychol 22; 8:151 15. 22. Craik FIM, Moroz TM, Moscovitch M, Stuss DT, Winocur G, Tulving E, et al. In search of the self: a positron emission tomography study. Psychol Sci 1; 1:2 4. 2. Kelley W, Macrae C, Wyland C, Caglar S, Inati S, Heatherton T. Finding the self? An event-related fmri study. J Cogn Neurosci 22; 18:21 25. 24. Bentin S, Deouell LY. Structural encoding and identification in face processing: ERP evidence for separate mechanisms. Cogn Neuropsychol 2; 17:5 54. Vol17 No4 2March2 427