UNDERWATER BEHAVIOUR OF THE HAWAIIAN SPINNER DOLPHIN AND THE ATLANTIC SPOTTED DOLPHIN MAGDALENA BŁASZAK

Similar documents
The reaction of Southern resident orca to sensitive frequencies produced by nearby vessels

Increased Number of Whistles of Bottlenose Dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, Arising from Interaction with People

DOLPHIN RESEARCH CENTER Acoustics

Marine Mammals and Sound

Measurements of the low frequency components of active and passive sounds produced by dolphins

Unit 2. Lesson 2. Sound Production and Reception

ACOUSTIC ASSESSMENT OF POPULATIONS OF COMMON DOLPHIN DELPHINUS DELPHIS IN CONJUNCTION WITH SEISMIC SURVEYING

Term Paper. Midterm Exam

MBA Education. For non profit use only.

Basic Hearing and Echolocation Mechanisms of Marine Mammals: Measured Auditory Evoked Potential and Behavioral Experiments FY 2008

Chapter 17 Sound Sound and Hearing. Properties of Sound Waves 1/20/2017. Pearson Prentice Hall Physical Science: Concepts in Action

Auditory Weighting Functions and Frequency-Dependent Effects of Sound in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)

High Frequency Acoustic Recording Package Annual Data Summary Report March 14, 2009 March 26, 2010 SOCAL Site N

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

Sonar induced temporary hearing loss in dolphins

Acoustic and Visual Survey of Cetaceans at Palmyra Atoll

Seismic testing and the impacts of high intensity sound on whales. Lindy Weilgart Department of Biology Dalhousie University Halifax, Nova Scotia

Sound and its characteristics. The decibel scale. Structure and function of the ear. Békésy s theory. Molecular basis of hair cell function.

Transfer of Sound Energy through Vibrations

ACOUSTIC VOCALIZATIONS OF DOLPHINS AND EFFECTS OF ANTHROPOGENIC NOISE

Denise L. Herzing b) Department of Biological Sciences, Florida Atlantic University, 777 Glades Road, Boca Raton, Florida 33431

Course evaluation submission:

Dalkeith High School Level 4 Physics. Waves and Sound

Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005

Remote Monitoring of Dolphins and Whales in the High Naval Activity Areas in Hawaiian Waters

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Technical Discussion HUSHCORE Acoustical Products & Systems

GRAY WHALE. Text source: The Marine Mammal Center

Differences in oscillatory whistles produced by spinner (Stenella longirostris) and pantropical spotted (Stenella attenuata) dolphins

whether or not the fundamental is actually present.

Report on the research activities with Orcinus orca in Loro Parque

The rapidly increasing pressure of human activity in coastal and pelagic marine environments has led to

Auditory Weighting Functions and Frequency-Dependent Effects of Sound in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)

Ecological Constraints on Sound Production in Marine Animals: the Importance of Listening

Topics in Linguistic Theory: Laboratory Phonology Spring 2007

Sequential structure analysis in the vocal repertoire of the Southern Resident Killer Whales orcinus orca

Auditory Weighting Functions and Frequency-Dependent Effects of Sound in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops Truncatus)

Linguistic Phonetics. Basic Audition. Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions.

Listening to wild bottlenose dolphins

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 1. Laboratory session: Measuring thresholds

Underwater Acoustics: Webinar Series for the International Regulatory Community

New Approaches to Studying Auditory Processing in Marine Mammals

Answer to DG Environment request on scientific information concerning impact of sonar activities on cetacean populations

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Echo-Processing Procedure in Bottlenose Dolphins

Prelude Envelope and temporal fine. What's all the fuss? Modulating a wave. Decomposing waveforms. The psychophysics of cochlear

Hearing Lectures. Acoustics of Speech and Hearing. Auditory Lighthouse. Facts about Timbre. Analysis of Complex Sounds

Chapter 3. Sounds, Signals, and Studio Acoustics

DolphinWatch: Dolphins in the Chesapeake Bay. Amber Fandel Faculty Research Assistant

Temporal resolution of the Risso s dolphin, Grampus griseus, auditory system

Whistle characteristics of common dolphins (Delphinus sp.) in the Hauraki Gulf, New Zealand

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17

MSFD and MEDCIS contribution

Whose Line Sound is it Anyway? Identifying the Vocalizer on Underwater Video by Localizing with a Hydrophone Array

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency

Auditory Masking Patterns in Bottlenose Dolphins from Anthropogenic and Natural Sound Sources

Underwater hearing in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus): Expansion and interpretation of existing data

Essential feature. Who are cochlear implants for? People with little or no hearing. substitute for faulty or missing inner hair

College of Medicine Dept. of Medical physics Physics of ear and hearing /CH

THE MECHANICS OF HEARING

Remote Monitoring of Dolphins and Whales in the High Naval Activity Areas in Hawaiian Waters

Speech Spectra and Spectrograms

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Animal Senses 9/2/16

An active unpleasantness control system for indoor noise based on auditory masking

Week 2 Systems (& a bit more about db)

Noise has been defined as a sound without agreeable quality or as unwanted or undesirable sound

Sound Workshop. What is sound Longitudinal Waves Frequency and pitch Hearing ranges Sounds in solids, liquids and gases Sound in a vacuum

Computational Perception /785. Auditory Scene Analysis

Results of Nature Foundation Marine Mammal Monitoring Project Jan-May 2011

Hearing the Universal Language: Music and Cochlear Implants

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency. (Some slides are adapted from Zhiyao Duan s course slides on Computer Audition and Its Applications in Music)

Stefanie Werner Federal Environment Agency (UBA) Stralsund, January 24th, 2012

SUBJECT: Physics TEACHER: Mr. S. Campbell DATE: 15/1/2017 GRADE: DURATION: 1 wk GENERAL TOPIC: The Physics Of Hearing

Ambiguity in the recognition of phonetic vowels when using a bone conduction microphone

Underwater Acoustics: Webinar Series for the International Regulatory Community

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Reference: Mark S. Sanders and Ernest J. McCormick. Human Factors Engineering and Design. McGRAW-HILL, 7 TH Edition. NOISE

How high-frequency do children hear?

Speech (Sound) Processing

Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, Auditory system. http:

Zones of impact around icebreakers affecting beluga whales in the Beaufort Sea

Auditory studies on harbour porpoises in relation to offshore wind turbines

Advanced otoacoustic emission detection techniques and clinical diagnostics applications

Electrophysiological Techniques for Sea Lion Population-level Audiometry

Marine Mammals in Scottish Waters

Chapter 1: Introduction to digital audio

9.3 Sound The frequency of sound Frequency and pitch pitch Most sound has more than one frequency The frequency spectrum

Marine Protected Species Permi:ng: In-water construc8on and noise impact assessment

Criteria and Thresholds for U.S. Navy Acoustic and Explosive Effects Analysis

DIAGNOSTIC TECHNIQUE OF ABNORMALITIES IN BALL BEARINGS WITH AN ULTRASONIC METHOD

2/25/2013. Context Effect on Suprasegmental Cues. Supresegmental Cues. Pitch Contour Identification (PCI) Context Effect with Cochlear Implants

An acoustic and behavioral analysis of the southern resident killer. whales of British Columbia: How does gender and age affect behavior

ULTRASOUND IMAGING EE 472 F2018. Prof. Yasser Mostafa Kadah

What Do Whales Feel?

SoundRecover2 the first adaptive frequency compression algorithm More audibility of high frequency sounds

AUDL GS08 and GAV1: 2013 Final exam page 1/13. You must complete all sections. Label all graphs. Show your work!

Sound localization psychophysics

Transcription:

UNDERWATER BEHAVIOUR OF THE HAWAIIAN SPINNER DOLPHIN AND THE ATLANTIC SPOTTED DOLPHIN MAGDALENA BŁASZAK Adam Mickiewicz University ul. Umultowska 85, Poznań, Poland boksiub@o2.pl To understand the behaviour of dolphins a specific language of these animals must be decoded. Dolphins acoustic emissions can be classified into two general categories: tonal whistles and pulsed sounds or clicks. Spinner whistles are frequency-modulated sounds with a fundamental component usually below 20 [khz], while the echolocation signals have bimodal frequency spectra with a low-frequency peak between 40 and 50 [khz] and a high frequency peak between 110 and 130 [khz]. Because of the fact that the signal of 100 kc/sec is much stronger absorbed by water than that of 20 kc/sec, at some longer distances higher harmonics are not audible for dolphins. Therefore, these harmonics probably do not play a significant role in long distance conversation. Another great problem is the noise produced by human activity that can impede the communication between dolphins or even prevent echolocation. INTRODUCTION Dolphins communication is the transmission of information from one animal to another by means of sound, visible signs or behaviour, touch, or a combination of these means. The existence, nature and extent of dolphins communication have long been a subject of speculation and have in modern times stimulated much scientific investigation. Dolphins communication is based on the sound, which has several advantages over other means. It fades quickly, leaving no trace of the communicator s location, and it can vary in pitch, duration, volume, and repetition, thus permitting a development of special codes. Sound may also be used in many settings, conditions, and situations in which other means are less feasible, such as across various distances, in darkness [2]. Hawaiian spinner dolphins (stanella longirostris) [Fig.1] are island-associated dolphins (around the Philippines in the western Pacific Ocean, French Polynesia in the South Pacific, Kwajalein Atoll in the Marshall Islands and waters of Hawaiian Islands [Fig.2]). The representatives of this species are often seen resting and travelling in numbers from 100 to

300, though groups of over 1000 in offshore regions have been reported. Large groups can be divided into smaller sub-groups according to age, sex and behavioural activity. Fig. 1. Hawaiian spinner dolphins Fig. 2. Hawaiian shore They differ from Spotted dolphins (stanella frontalis) [Fig.3] and live in the vicinity the Bahama Islands [Fig.4]. This archipelago in the tropical West Atlantic east of Florida is surrounded by deep water and relatively shallow coastal areas (up to 15 [m]). The Hawaiian spinner dolphins are found only in the warm tropical and sub-tropical waters of the Atlantic Ocean, in both northern and southern hemispheres, although they are more abundant in the northern one. The representatives of the species form groups ranging from 5 to 15 in numbers, although groups of a hundred often have also been spotted. Generally, a family pod can number a maximum of about fifty. Till now approximately two hundred spotted dolphins have been identified and observed in a variety of behavioural context in the vicinity of the Bahamas Island [5].

Fig. 3. Atlantic spotted dolphins Fig. 4. Bahamas Island shore 1. DOLPHINS COMMUNICATION SIGNALS 1.A. WHISTLES Dolphin acoustic emissions can be classified into two general categories: tonal whistles and pulsed sounds or clicks. The whistles produced by the Hawaiian spinner dolphins are characterised by the whistle repertoire i.e. the types of whistle and the frequency of occurrence of each type of whistle, i.e. the whistle usage [2]. Dolphin whistles have been characterised in terms of their instantaneous frequency as a function of time. The basic property of a vocal cord sound source is its periodicity expressed by the voice fundamental frequency. A voice source is further characterised by the spectrum envelope being a specification of the amplitudes of the source harmonics as a function of their frequency [1]. The source spectrum envelope reflects personal characteristics of the speaking (whistling) dolphin and depends on the voice range and intensity. A sound spectrogram [Fig.5] is a visual representation of sound, the abscissa corresponds to time and the axis of ordinates gives the frequency (or pitch). The frequency is measured in Hertz (Hz), or cycles per second; and in kilohertz (khz) or thousands of cycles per second. The relative intensity of the sound at any particular time is indicated by a scale of intensity of the colour of the spectrogram. The colours black or blue correspond to the low amplitude of the frequency, while the red one to high amplitudes.

Fig. 5. The spectrogram of the dolphin s whistle Spinner whistles are frequency-modulated sounds with a fundamental component usually below 20 [khz] (which is an upper limit of human audibility) and harmonics up to 62 [khz]. Durations of whistles usually vary between 0.1and 1.5 [s] but nearly all cluster between 0.2 and 0.7 [s]. The first harmonic is the most important because it transmits maximum energy and probably contains the majority of information. Frequency as a function of time is also referred to as the whistle contour [3]. The dolphins produce a large number of different contours that, all together, comprise the whistle repertoire of a given species and maybe even of a population. The spinner dolphin s whistles analysed were recorded in the waters of the Hawaiian Archipelago. They are characterised by [Fig.6]: beginning frequency, end frequency, maximum frequency, minimum frequency, peak frequency, duration, peak time, centre time, number of turns, loops and steps. Probably various configurations of these parameters carry different pieces of information. Fig. 6. Parameters of each whistle contour Less then 3% of the whistles have a fundamental maximum frequency between 22 and 24 [khz], while for approximately 95% of the whistles it is below 22 [khz]. In addition, less then 1% of the whistles have minimum frequencies below 2 [khz]. That is why the whistles

can be recorded by a DAT recorder which has the upper frequency limit of 24 [khz] (sampling rate of 48 [khz]) [3]. The spinner dolphin whistle contours can also be classified into six general categories proposed by (Whitlou W. L. Au, after Driscoll and Bazua-Duran). The constant frequency contour [Fig.7] is that in which the frequency changes 1000 [Hz] or less in the duration of the whistle and in which the frequency span ( height ) is less then one quarter of the duration ( length ) of the whistle. The frequency rarely remains constant in the whole duration of the whistle. The upsweep contour [Fig.8] is that in which the frequency is mainly ascending. If there are any inflection points, the descending frequency part comprises less than half of the frequency span of the whistle. The downsweep contour [Fig.9] is the one in which the frequency is mainly descending. If there are any inflection points, the ascending frequency part comprises less than half of the frequency span of the whistle. The concave contour [Fig.10]is the one with at least one inflection point prior to which the frequency is mainly descending and then mainly ascending. The ascending and descending parts comprise more than half of the frequency span of the whistle. The convex contour [Fig.11] is that with at least one inflection point prior to which the frequency is mainly ascending and than mainly descending. The ascending and ascending parts comprise more than half of the frequency span of whistle. The sine contour [Fig.12] is that with at least two inflection points where the frequency is first mainly ascending, then mainly descending, and so forth, or vice versa. At least three of the ascending and descending parts comprise more than half of the frequency span of the whistle [3]. Hawaiian spinner dolphins emit a great amount of upsweep whistles (47%). The convex contours comprise 20% of the whistles and 13% are downsweep whistles. It is sometimes difficult to assign a whistle contour to either the upsweep or downsweep category and the convex or concave categories. More interestingly, Wang (1995) has found a linear relationship between the maximum fundamental frequency of whistles and the dolphin body length: Maximum Frequency = 21.452 3.2537 * Body Length (1) Fig. 7. Schematic constant frequency contour examples

Fig. 8. Schematic upsweep contour examples Fig. 9. Schematic downsweep contour examples Fig. 10. Schematic concave contour examples Fig. 11. Schematic convex contour examples

Fig. 12. Schematic sine contour examples 2.B. CLICKS Dolphins use short, broadband clicks with peak energies between 40 and 130 [khz] to echolocate. Burst pulses are produced alone, in bouts or associated with whistles. Spinner dolphins produce burst pulses that have on average approximately 30 clicks per train. Spotted dolphin burst pulses have on average about 100 clicks per train. The number of clicks in a burst pulse displayed an approximately bimodal distribution that distinguished burst pulses into low quantity (<seventy clicks) and high quantity (>seventy clicks) click trains [4]. Spotted dolphins produce significantly more high quantity burst pulses than spinner dolphins. Peak and centre frequencies for spinner dolphin burst is on average 32,3 and 40,1 [khz]. Spectral energy distribution in clicks doesn t show any relationship to either the number of clicks in a train or the interclick interval. Approximately 80% of the total energy in burst pulses is above the upper limit of human audibility (20 [khz]) for both species. Only a minority of signals have peak frequencies below this limit. Click trains are sometimes produced that begin with long interclick intervals (10-100 [ms]) and end with very short ones (1,5-9 [ms]) [5]. These are often observed when animals are foraging and presumably echolocating on prey. Experiments with free-swimming bottlenose dolphins (tursiops truncates) [Fig.13.] show that an echoprocessing lag time between 15 and 45 [ms] is always associated with successive clicks produced by animals echolocating on targets further than 0,4 [m] away [8]. As animals close in on a target (<0,4 [m]), interclick intervals as low as 2,5 [ms] are observed [8]. There is always a gradual progression towards shorter click intervals as dolphin approaches a target. Fig. 13. Bottlenose dolphins (tursiops truncates)

The echolocation signals of Atlantic spotted dolphins have bi-modal frequency spectra [Fig.14.] with a low-frequency peak between 40 and 50 [khz] and a high frequency peak between 110 and 130 [khz] [5]. The low frequency peak is dominant when the signal source level is low and the high-frequency peak dominate when the source level is high. Measurements from stationary dolphins in captivity show that echolocation clicks are emitted in a directional beam and signals measured off-axis are distorted with respect to the signals measured along the major axis of the beam. The amplitude of the echoes returning to the Fig. 14. Schematic bi-modal spectra of the echolocation signal dolphins increases as the distance decreases, suggesting that the dolphins prefer receiving echoes with a high signal-to-noise. The interclick intervals are always greater than the twoway travel time from the animals to the target and back. Spotted dolphins produce relatively high-amplitude signals with a maximum source level of about 223 [db] re 1 µpa although most of the source levels are between 200 and 210 [db] re 1 µpa [5]. 3. PROPAGATION LOSS The transmission loss associated with any given length of ray path in water will be specifically designated as a propagation loss. The propagation loss between two points is, in general, a function of frequency. For each frequency the ratio of the intensity of sinusoidal acoustic waves measured at the source of sound to that measured at a certain distance from the sound is calculated. Fig.15 presents the propagation loss plotted against a logarithmic scale of distance for different frequencies. The practical working equation for nominal propagation loss, to be used whenever specific values of the range exponent and of the range coefficient are not available: where (Nw)o = 2 log S + ( 0,20f + 0,00015f² ) S + 60 [db] (2) (Nw)o = the nominal value of the propagation loss of sea water, based on statistical averages, likely to be found at any time or place between a given index point and a given observation point [db] S = the range from the effective centre of the source [kyd] F = the frequency of the wave [kc/sec]

Fig. 15. Propagation loss as a function of range, with frequency [khz] as a parameter It shows that for distances under 1000 yards (600 [m]) little variation in the average value of propagation loss is to be expected as the frequency of the wave is changed. The propagation loss level also indicates that acoustic signals in water can be transmitted over longer distances at frequencies close to 20 kc/sec than those close to 100 kc/sec [7]. The signal of 100 kc/sec is much stronger absorbed by water, approximately 110 [db] per 2000 yards (1200[m]), than the signal of 20 kc/sec, approximately 70 [db] over the same distance. This fact can bring a partial answer to the question whether the higher harmonics in the dolphins sound spectrum are important in their communication. They could be important in a short distance conversation, but they may not play any role in the dolphins' calling. 4. NOISE IN THE SEA There is no such thing as absolutely quiet water. Even if it was possible to isolate a sample of water completely from all external influence there would still be minute movements of the molecules, due to thermal agitation, accompanied by the release of acoustic energy. This molecular agitation, which is proportional to the absolute temperature of the water, is referred to as thermal noise. Many of the sounds present in the waters of the ocean are produced without relevant purpose. They may result from natural phenomena or from the activities of marine creatures. They may also result from man's activity undertaken for reasons other than their generation. The sounds of these different types are often made up of many components whose magnitudes can in time in a random manner, independently of one another. They sometimes exhibit more or less regularly recurrent variations which may be identified with some repetitive process. Such sounds are generally referred to as noise. Many of the sounds present in the waters of the ocean originate from human activity. The most common of these sounds are, of course, those produced by ships. Ships noises in the water somewhat resemble the noise of general traffic on the shore. This sound might be classified as ambient noise and reaches a level as high as 90 db // 1 watt/cm² for the frequency from 0.1 to 10 kc/sec [6]. In harbours in busy industrial areas there is always a certain amount of noise which gets into the water from machinery operating on barges, on

piers, or even on shore at some distance from the water s edge. This noise can impede the communication between dolphins or even prevent echolocation, which might be a great problem for dolphins, which echolocate to find food and to survive. 5. CONCLUSION The paper presents an outline of the communication and echolocation signals of the Hawaiian spinner dolphins and the Bahamas spotted dolphins. The signals of these two types play different and important roles in the dolphins society. On the one hand, marine mammals ears physically resemble land mammals ears. Therefore, since many forms of hearing loss are based on physical structure, hearing damage may occur by similar mechanism in both land and marine mammals ears. On the other hand, the sea has never been silent. The ocean is by nature an environment of relatively high noise level. The main natural sound sources include seismic, volcanic, wind, and even biotic. Dolphins evolved ears that function well within this context of high natural ambient noise. This may mean that they have developed a tougher inner ears less susceptible to hearing loss. Dolphins may be more resistant than many land mammals to temporary threshold shifts, but they may also be subject to hearing diseases [6]. Although, the subject of communication has been treated in a number of articles written by many researchers such as Whitlow W. L. Au from the Institute of Marine Biology (Hawaii), Denise L. Herzing from the Department of Biological Sciences (Florida) and Carmen Bazua-Duran from the Department of Oceanography (Hawaii), no answers to many basic questions have been given yet. We still do not know whether dolphins in fact are able to change the harmonic structure of their whistles and how burst pulses function in their communication. We can only speculate on the significance of these differences. Our surface behavioural observations are too limited in detail to be able to match many signals to a specific behaviour. For a species that lives in large groups the coordinate behaviour at all times is crucial to maintain communication between all members of the group. REFERENCES [1] Fant, G. (1960), Acoustic Theory of Speech Production, Mouton, The Hague, 16-18. [2] The new Encyclopedia Britannica (1768), MICROPEDIA, vol.1, 418-420. [3] Au, W. W. L., Bazua-Duran, C. (2002). The whistle of Hawaiian spinner dolphins, J.Acoust.Soc.Am, 112, 3064-3972. [4] Lammers, M. O., Au, W. W. L. (2003). The broadband social acoustic signaling behavior of spinner and spotted dolphins, J.Acoust.Soc.Am, 114, 1629-1639. [5] Au, W. W. L., Herzing, D. L. (2003). Echolocation signals of wild Atlantic spotted dolphin (stanella frontalis), J.Acoust.Soc.Am, 113, 598-604. [6] Ketten, D. R., Marine mammal auditory systems: a summary of audiometric and anatomical data and its implications for underwater acoustic impacts, www.solcomhouse.com/auditory.htm. [7] Horton, J. W., Fundamentals of sonar (1957), United States Naval Institute, Annapolis, Maryland, 57-86. [8] Evans, W. E., and Powell, B. A. (1967). Discrimination of different metallic plates by an echolocating delphinid in Animal Sonar Systems: Biology and Bionics edited by R. G. Busnel (Laboratorie de Physiologie Acoustic, Jouy-en-Josas, France), 363-382.