Electrophysiological evidence of enhanced distractibility in ADHD children

Similar documents
Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Title change detection system in the visu

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

Event-related brain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries

Atypical processing of prosodic changes in natural speech stimuli in school-age children with Asperger syndrome

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm

Negative emotional context enhances auditory novelty processing

Developmental Course of Auditory Event-Related Potentials in Autism

AUDITORY ERPS IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL COORDINATION DISORDER

Activation of the auditory pre-attentive change detection system by tone repetitions with fast stimulation rate

An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials

Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

When Loading Working Memory Reduces Distraction: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence from an Auditory Visual Distraction Paradigm

RAPID COMMUNICATION Scalp-Recorded Optical Signals Make Sound Processing in the Auditory Cortex Visible

1. Department of clinical neurology, Tbilisi State Medical University, Tbilisi, Georgia.

Early Occurrence Of Auditory Change Detection In The Human Brain

ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage

Stefan Debener a,b, *, Scott Makeig c, Arnaud Delorme c, Andreas K. Engel a,b. Research report

When distraction is not distracting: A behavioral and ERP study on distraction in ADHD

What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3

Material-speci c neural correlates of memory retrieval

International Journal of Neurology Research

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Hypersensitivity to acoustic change in children with autism: Electrophysiological evidence of left frontal cortex dysfunctioning

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fmri and ERP Results

Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures

Separate memory-related processing for auditory frequency and patterns

Activation of brain attention systems in individuals with symptoms of ADHD

Effects of involuntary auditory attention on visual task performance and brain activity

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential


Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Temporal integration: intentional sound discrimination does not modulate stimulus-driven processes in auditory event synthesis

Auditory sensory memory in 2-year-old children: an event-related potential study

No Behavioral or ERP Evidence for a Developmental Lag in Visual Working Memory Capacity or Filtering in Adolescents and Adults with ADHD

Do children with ADHD and/or PDD-NOS differ in reactivity of alpha/theta ERD/ERS to manipulations of cognitive load and stimulus relevance?

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY OF UNIMODAL AND AUDIOVISUAL SPEECH PERCEPTION

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

Cognition. Post-error slowing: An orienting account. Wim Notebaert a, *, Femke Houtman a, Filip Van Opstal a, Wim Gevers b, Wim Fias a, Tom Verguts a

Author s Accepted Manuscript

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Primitive intelligence in the auditory cortex

Theta Oscillation Related to the Auditory Discrimination Process in Mismatch Negativity: Oddball versus Control Paradigm

Effects of discrepancy between imagined and perceived sounds on the N2 component of the event-related potential

Differentiation of conversive sensory loss and malingering by P300 in a modified oddball task

Visual Event-Related Potentials to Moving Stimuli: Normative Data

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Effects of dynamic rotation on event-related brain potentials

International Journal of Psychophysiology

The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential

Development of infant mismatch responses to auditory pattern changes between 2 and 4 months old

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA

Implicit, Intuitive, and Explicit Knowledge of Abstract Regularities in a Sound Sequence: An Event-related Brain Potential Study

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory

The effects of covert attention and stimulus complexity on the P3 response during an auditory continuous performance task

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

D. Debatisse, E. Fornari, E. Pralong, P. Maeder, H Foroglou, M.H Tetreault, J.G Villemure. NCH-UNN and Neuroradiology Dpt. CHUV Lausanne Switzerland

Erratum to Age-related change in neural processing of time-dependent stimulus features [Cognit. Brain Res. 25 (2005) ]

Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces

The mind s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control in internal attention shifting

Supporting Information

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Synchronous cortical gamma-band activity in task-relevant cognition

The N1 hypothesis and irrelevant sound: evidence from token set size effects

Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: an event-related brain potential study

Apathy is often observed after stroke and is defined as

The psychophysiology of selective attention and working memory in children with PPDNOS and/or ADHD Gomarus, Henriette Karin

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

An Electrophysiological Examination of ADHD- Associated Symptoms and Selective Attention in Adults

Timing & Schizophrenia. Deana Davalos Colorado State University

Shirley Y. Hill, Stuart Steinhauer, Lisa Lowers, and Jeannette Locke. ERP Procedure

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Brain Maturation - it covers three decades: Considerations of the development of ADHD. Professor Robert D. Oades,

The maturation of interference suppression and response inhibition: ERP analysis of a cued Go/Nogo task.

Beware misleading cues: Perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex

International Journal of Psychophysiology

Emotional context enhances auditory novelty processing: behavioural and electrophysiological evidence

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Ear and Hearing (In press, 2005)

Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness and REM sleep

Neurocognitive and Behavioral Indexes for Identifying the Amnestic Subtypes of Mild Cognitive Impairment

The EEG Analysis of Auditory Emotional Stimuli Perception in TBI Patients with Different SCG Score

Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception

ERROR PROCESSING IN CLINICAL POPULATIONS IN CONTRAST TO ADHD J.J.VAN DER MEERE

Reward positivity is elicited by monetary reward in the absence of response choice Sergio Varona-Moya a,b, Joaquín Morís b,c and David Luque b,d

Left-hemisphere dominance for processing of vowels: a whole-scalp neuromagnetic study

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA

The processes underlying the recovery from a brain stroke

Transcription:

Neuroscience Letters 374 (2005) 212 217 Electrophysiological evidence of enhanced distractibility in ADHD children V. Gumenyuk a,, O. Korzyukov a,b, C. Escera c,m.hämäläinen d, M. Huotilainen a,e,f, T. Häyrinen d, H. Oksanen d,r.näätänen a,e, L. von Wendt d, K. Alho g a Cognitive Brain Research Unit, Department of Psychology, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland b Department of Psychiatry, Yale University and VA-Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven, CT, USA c Cognitive Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain d Department of Child Neurology, Hospital for Children and Adolescents, Helsinki University Central Hospital, Helsinki, Finland e Helsinki Brain Research Centre, Helsinki, Finland f Collegium of Advanced Studies, University of Helsinki, Finland g Department of Psychology, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland Received 28 June 2004; received in revised form 14 October 2004; accepted 21 October 2004 Abstract Abnormal involuntary attention leading to enhanced distractibility may account for different behavioral and cognitive problems in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). This was investigated in the present experiment by recording event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to distracting novel sounds during performance of a visual discrimination task. The overall performance in the visual task was less accurate in the ADHD children than in the control children, and the ADHD children had a higher number of omitted responses following novel sounds. In both groups, the distracting novel sounds elicited a biphasic P3a ERP component and a subsequent frontal Late Negativity (LN). The early phase of P3a (180 240 ms) had significantly smaller amplitudes over the fronto-central left-hemisphere recording sites in the ADHD children than in the control group presumably due to an overlapping enhanced left-hemisphere dominant negative ERP component elicited in the ADHD group. Moreover, the late phase of P3a (300 350 ms) was significantly larger over the left parietal scalp areas in the ADHD children than in the controls. The LN had a smaller amplitude and shorter latency over the frontal scalp in the ADHD group than in the controls. In conclusion, the ERP and behavioral effects caused by the novel sounds reveal deficient control of involuntary attention in ADHD children that may underlie their abnormal distractibility. 2004 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; Distraction; Children; Orienting; P3a; Reorienting negativity (LN/RON) Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a multidimensional disorder that has its onset in childhood and that is characterized by persistent problems of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity [2,15]. In ADHD patients, difficulties with sustained attention tasks [11] can be caused by different alternatives, one being abnormal distractibility. Therefore, the present study compared involuntary attention and distractibility in ADHD and Corresponding author. Tel.: +358 9 191 29460; fax: +358 9 191 29450. E-mail address: valentina.gumenyuk@helsinki.fi (V. Gumenyuk). healthy children, as indicated by distraction of visual task performance by task-irrelevant novel sounds and by eventrelated brain potentials (ERPs) to these sounds. Highly deviant, task-irrelevant auditory and visual stimuli elicit a P3 component of ERP called the P3a or novelty P3 [5,21]. The P3a is maximal over the fronto-central scalp, and therefore, it can be separated from the parietally maximal P300 or P3b response to auditory and visual target stimuli. The P3a has been suggested to be associated with the orienting response [5,6,13,20] and with detection and evaluation of novelty [7]. Association of P3a with involuntary attention 0304-3940/$ see front matter 2004 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2004.10.081

V. Gumenyuk et al. / Neuroscience Letters 374 (2005) 212 217 213 switching is evidenced by the fact that P3a-eliciting novel sounds also distract visual task performance in adults [6] and children [9]. The P3a to novel sounds has two phases: an early phase (ep3a) peaking at 200 250 ms and a late phase (lp3a) peaking at 300 350 ms from sound onset [6]. In adults, the ep3a has its maximum amplitude over the central scalp, whereas the lp3a is distributed widely and has its maximum over the frontal scalp. According to scalp current density analysis [25], magnetoencephalography [1], and studies in patients with local brain lesions [14], the ep3a is generated in the auditory and temporo-parietal cortices, while the lp3a gets a major contribution from the prefrontal cortex [13,25]. Thus, multiple brain areas are involved in involuntary attention and auditory novelty detection. This appears to be true also in children. The scalp distributions of the ep3a and lp3a to novel sounds in children aged over 11 years are similar to those in adults [9]. However, in 7 10-year-olds, the lp3a has its maximum over the centro-parietal scalp suggesting that the auditory attention networks are not yet fully developed at this age [9]. The P3a to distracting sounds is followed by a reorienting negativity (RON) component in adult ERPs [19] and the Late Negativity (LN) in children [9]. RON and LN have maximum amplitudes over fronto-central scalp areas at 400 700 ms after onset of a distracting sound and may be associated with the prefrontal brain mechanisms involved in reorienting attention back to task performance after a distracting event [9,19]. Previous studies found no differences between ADHD and control children in P3a responses to novel visual stimuli [10,11]. However, Kemner et al. s [11] data suggest smaller P3a responses over the parietal scalp to task-irrelevant novel sounds in ADHD children than in healthy controls, although this effect did not reach statistical significance. In contrast, Kilpeläinen et al. [12] found enhanced frontal P3 deflections (presumably the P3a) in distractible children in comparison with non-distractible children in response to infrequent target tones occurring among frequent tones. In the present study, we investigated these issues further by recording ERPs to repeating tones and novel sounds from ADHD children and healthy controls while they concentrated on a demanding visual task. Eleven 8 10-year-old ADHD and 10 age-matched healthy children were studied. One ADHD child was excluded due to a very poor performance (less than 50% hits) in the visual discrimination task (see below). The remaining ADHD group and the control group consisted of 10 subjects each (mean ages 8.7 and 8.6 years., respectively; one girl in each group). The ADHD children were recruited from the Department of Child Neurology of the Helsinki University Central Hospital. All of them had a thorough multidisciplinary diagnostic assessment and all fulfilled the DSM-IV [2] criteria for ADHD and had no other comorbidity disorders. Those ADHD children who were on medication were withdrawn from it at least a day before the experiment. The general intellectual ability of the clinical group was assessed with Wechsler s [22,23] intelligence scales. All children in the ADHD group had their verbal, performance, and full-scale intelligence quotients (IQ) within the normal range 90 126 (means: 108, 100, and 104, respectively). The ADHD children were rewarded with a ticket to a self-chosen movie (according to the protocol). The control children were recruited from ordinary primary schools (none of the control children were in gifted programs at school) and were rewarded for their participation with 6D. None of the control children had any diagnosed psychological or neurological problems. All of them were in the ageappropriate grade at school. All children had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision and normal hearing. Written informed consents were obtained from all parents of children participating in the study. The Ethics Committee of the Hospital for Children and Adolescents approved the protocol of the present study for the ADHD children and The Ethical Committee of the Department of Psychology, University of Helsinki, approved the protocol of the present study for the control group. The children were presented with auditory-visual stimulus pairs at a constant rate of one pair in every 1.7 s. Each sound had a duration of 200 ms and was delivered binaurally through headphones at 50 db SPL. The sound was either a sinusoidal tone of 600 Hz (P = 0.80; or 560 tones) or one of the complex environmental novel sounds presented randomly (P = 0.20) with the exception that at least 3 tones occurred between any two successive novel sounds. The novel sounds were drawn from a pool of 140 different environmental sounds, such as those produced by a hammer, rain, and car horn. Each novel sound was presented only once during the experiment. The visual stimuli were 32 different white-line drawings (16 animals and 16 non-animals) extending 9 cm horizontally and vertically, presented for 300 ms, and starting at 300 ms after the onset of the preceding sound. The drawings were displayed in a random order in the center of a black computer screen located approximately 130 cm from the subject s eyes. The children were instructed to ignore the sounds, to focus their gaze on a white fixation cross (1 cm 1 cm) continuously presented at the center of the screen, and to respond as accurately as possible by pressing one response button with their right thumb to an animal image appearing on the screen and another button with their left thumb to a non-animal image. The children s motivation to perform the task was indicated by their genuine interest to their hit rate reported to them after each block. The electroencephalogram (EEG; 0.1 100 Hz, sampling rate 250 Hz) was recorded from frontal (Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, and F8), central (C3, Cz, C4), temporal (T3, T4, T5, T6), parietal (P3, Pz, P4), and occipital (O1, O2) scalp sites and from the left and right mastoids (LM and RM, respectively). Voltage changes caused by eye movements and blinks were monitored with recordings from the forehead sites (Fp1, Fp2) and from additional electrodes placed at the left and right canthi. The common reference electrode was placed at the tip of the nose. ERPs were obtained separately for the tones and novel sounds by averaging EEG epochs over a 900 ms pe-

214 V. Gumenyuk et al. / Neuroscience Letters 374 (2005) 212 217 riod starting 100 ms before each sound onset. These EEG epochs were digitally band-pass filtered at 1 30 Hz. Epochs with extracerebral artifacts exceeding ±100 V at any electrode were excluded from averaging. Also, the epochs for the first four stimuli of each block and the epochs for any tone occurring right after a novel sound were excluded. For each child, averaged ERPs to tones and novel sounds consisted of at least 380 and 90 acceptable EEG epochs, respectively. Because the children were instructed to perform the visual discrimination task with an accuracy instruction (stress on performance accuracy, not on speed, cf. [17]), changes in the rates of correct responses ( hits ) and incorrect responses (wrong button presses and response omissions) caused by the task-irrelevant novel sounds were used as major behavioral indexes distractibility. A correct button press given 200 1500 ms after visual stimulus onset was classified as a hit. Within-group difference of means rates of hit, wrong and missed responses, and hit reaction times (RTs) were analyzed with t-tests, while betweengroup difference of those were analyzed with Mann Whitney U-test (depended variables: tone versus novel). The P3a and LN amplitudes were measured from ERP difference waves obtained by subtracting ERPs to tones from those to novel sounds [6,9]. Their amplitudes were measured as mean voltages over fixed latency windows determined after visual inspection of the grand-average ERP difference waves at Fz and Cz electrodes. The mean amplitudes of the ep3a and lp3a were measured over 180 250 and 300 350 ms from sound onset, respectively, in relation to the mean difference-wave amplitude during the 100 ms prestimulus baseline. The LN amplitudes, in turn, were measured over two latency windows: 420 490 and 490 560 ms. Between-group differences in the P3a and LN amplitudes were examined with ANOVAs and Newman Keuls post hoc tests. Greenhouse Geisser corrections were used for the P-values when appropriate. ANOVAs for the ep3a and lp3a, included electrode factors Frontality (frontal F3, Fz, F4 versus central C3, Cz, C4 versus parietal P3, Pz, P4 electrodes) and Laterality (left-hemisphere F3, C3, P3 versus midline Fz, Cz, Pz versus right-hemisphere F4, C4, P4 electrodes). Table 1 shows mean RTs, and the mean rates of hits, wrong responses, and response omissions in the visual task for the ADHD and control groups. In both groups, the distraction effect in the visual task caused by novel sounds was significant: An occurrence of a novel sound decreased the number of correct responses to the following visual stimulus, in comparison with visual stimuli preceded by a tone both in the ADHD and control groups (t(9) = 2.54; P < 0.03; t(9) = 2.96; P < 0.01, respectively). Both groups showed longer RTs after novel sounds than after tones, but this effect reached significance only in the controls (t(9) = 3.79; P < 0.004). A Mann Whitney U-test for the hit rates yielded a significant between-group differences in the visual-task accuracy both for the visual stimuli preceded by a tone (U(11) = 2.9; P < 0.002) and for those preceded by a novel sound (U(14) = 2.7; P < 0.005), indicating that ADHD chil- Table 1 Mean (standard deviations in parentheses) RTs, hit rates, wrong-response and miss rates of the ADHD and control children in performing the visual discrimination task after an occurrence of a task-irrelevant tone vs. novel sound Performance Stimulus ADHD mean (S.D.) Controls mean (S.D.) RT (ms) Tone 476 (78) 498 (122) Novel 488 (86) 553 (142) Hit rate (%) Tone 65 (13) 81 (8) Novel 62 (12) 78 (10) Wrong-response (%) Tone 13 (5) 6 (3) Novel 12 (6) 9 (3) Miss-response (%) Tone 22 (9) 12 (10) Novel 26 (9) 13 (10) dren had an overall smaller percentage of correct responses than the controls. A Mann Whitney U-test for the rate of missed responses indicated that an occurrence of a novel sound (in comparison with an occurrence of a tone) enhanced significantly more the number of response omissions in the ADHD children than in the controls (U(17) = 2.49; P < 0.01), this effect of the preceding sound on the number of omissions being 4% in the ADHD group but only 1% in the control group (see Table 1). No significant group difference or effect of preceding stimulus was found for the number of wrong responses. Fig. 1 displays grand-average ERPs elicited by the auditory-visual stimulus pairs. In the ADHD and control groups, tones elicited only a small auditory N1 component (peak around 100 ms from sound onset at the Cz electrode), presumably due to a relative immaturity of the auditory cortex [18]. A P3a response was elicited by novel sounds in each age group (Fig. 1). Fig. 2 shows the grand-average ERP difference waves of the P3a and LN responses to the novel sounds for the ADHD and control groups. Consistent with our previous studies in healthy children [9], the P3a had a biphasic structure. As seen in Fig. 2, the ep3a was largest over the fronto-central scalp in both groups, whereas the lp3a had a wider distribution over the midline scalp areas. An ANOVA for the ep3a amplitudes at F3, FZ, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, and P4 electrodes indicated a significant Group Frontality Laterality interaction (F(4,72) = 4.64; P < 0.01). Subsequent Newman Keuls tests showed that this was due to an amplitude difference between the groups over the frontocentral left-hemisphere for the ep3a: as seen in Figs. 2 and 3a, the ADHD children had smaller ep3a amplitudes than did the controls (mean amplitude measured at 180 250 ms: 0.9 versus 3.35 V at F3, and 0.9 versus 3.27 V at C3, respectively; P < 0.04 for both comparisons). As seen in Fig. 2 (e.g., the F3 electrode), this effect might be caused by an overlapping negative component peaking around 240 ms in ADHD children and reducing their ep3a amplitudes over the left frontal scalp. The left-frontal scalp distribution of this possible negative component is seen in Fig. 3b showing a

V. Gumenyuk et al. / Neuroscience Letters 374 (2005) 212 217 215 Fig. 1. Grand-average ERPs elicited by tones (solid line) and novel sounds (dashed line) at selected electrodes in the ADHD and control children. Note that the auditory ERPs are followed and partially overlapped by the ERPs to the subsequent visual stimulus (visual-stimulus onset indicated by vertical dashed line). distribution map for the amplitude difference between the grand-average novel-minus-tone ERP difference waves of the ADHD and control group at 200 250 ms from sound onset. An ANOVA for the lp3a revealed a significant Group Frontality Laterality interaction (F(4,72) = 2.56; P < 0.04). According to the subsequent Newman Keuls tests, the ADHD children had significantly larger lp3a amplitudes at the left and midline parietal scalp sites (4.8 versus 3 Vat P3, P < 0.03; 6.7 versus 4.6 V at Pz; P < 0.01). This effect is seen in Figs. 2 and 3a. Following the P3a, a frontal LN was observed in both groups (Figs. 2 and 3a). The LN appeared to be smaller in amplitude and to peak earlier in the ADHD group than in the controls. Therefore, the LN amplitudes were measured at frontal electrode sites (F8, F4, Fz, F3, F7) as mean voltages over two time windows, i.e., over 420 490 and 490 560 ms from sound onset. The earlier of these windows covers the peak latency of the LN in the grand-average ERP difference waves of the ADHD group and the later window covers the peak latency of the LN of the control group. An ANOVA for these LN amplitudes including factors Group, Laterality (F8 versus F4 versus Fz versus F3 versus F7) and Time Window (420 490 ms versus 490 560 ms) revealed a significant Group Laterality Time Window interaction (F(4,72) = 6.18; P < 0.002) caused by the fact that over the frontal midline, the LN was larger in amplitude in the ADHD group than in the controls during the earlier time window (mean amplitudes 2.8 V versus 1.2 V, respectively; Newman Keuls test: P < 0.001) but smaller in the ADHD group than in the controls during the later time window (mean amplitude 0.01 V versus 1.7 V, respectively; P < 0.001). Fig. 2. Grand-average ERP difference waves obtained by subtracting the ERPs to tones from those to novel sounds. The P3a elicited by novel sounds consists of two phases: the ep3a and lp3a, which are followed by the LN.

216 V. Gumenyuk et al. / Neuroscience Letters 374 (2005) 212 217 Fig. 3. (a) P3a and LN scalp-distribution maps for each group of children. The maps show average voltages of novel-minus-tone ERP difference waves (see Fig. 2) at the latency ranges of the ep3a, lp3a, and the earlier and later phases of LN. (b) Topographic maps of the ERP difference between the ADHD and control groups (ADHD minus control) suggesting abnormal brain activity in the left-hemisphere of the ADHD children peaking around 240 ms. In conclusion, the present performance data are in accordance with our previous studies [9] by showing that novel sounds distract children s performance in a visual discrimination task as indicated by decreased hit rates and increased RTs after an occurrence of a novel sound in comparison with occurrence of a repeating tone. Moreover, larger number of response omissions after a novel sound in the ADHD group than in the control group revealed higher distractibility in the ADHD group. Abnormal involuntary attention in the ADHD children was also supported by the ERPs to the distracting novel sounds. These sounds elicited a biphasic P3a response with an early phase (ep3a) that had significantly smaller amplitude over the left-hemisphere in the ADHD children than in the control group. Thus, reduced ep3a amplitudes in the ADHD were presumably caused by a left-hemisphere dominant negative ERP component evoked by the novel sounds in the ADHD group but not in the control group or evoked with much smaller amplitudes in the latter group. Alternatively, the abnormal left-hemisphere activity in the ADHD children around 240 ms from novel-sound onset might be caused by a genuine reduction in the activity of one of the left-hemisphere generator sources contributing to the P3a response at the latencies between the ep3a and lp3a peaks (cf. [25]). For example, this reduction might be associated with a deficit in controlling verbalized encoding of these distracting environmental sounds that may be based on automatic stimulus identification and classification [6]. Moreover, the lp3a showed enhanced amplitudes in the ADHD group in comparison with the controls in parietal electrode sites especially over the lefthemisphere. These findings are in agreement with previous findings showing differences between ADHD and controls at left-hemisphere sites to auditory stimulus changes in ADHD patients compared with control subjects [16]. Interestingly, Gomot et al. [8] reported in 5 9-year-old autistic children concentrating on watching a silent movie atypical activity elicited by frequency changes in a repeating sound. This activity occurred at the latency of the mismatch negativity (MMN) and was localized with scalp current density analysis to the left frontal cortex. Their study together with the present results suggests left-hemisphere abnormality that may underlie various attentional problems in children. Finally, in the present study, the lp3a was followed by the LN component that had a smaller amplitude and shorter latency in the ADHD children than in the controls. In healthy children, the LN has been interpreted to correspond the reorienting negativity (RON) observed in adults and proposed to be generated by prefrontal mechanisms involved in reorienting of attention back to the distracted task performance [3,9,19,24]. Therefore, the reduced LN in the ADHD chil-

V. Gumenyuk et al. / Neuroscience Letters 374 (2005) 212 217 217 dren might be associated with a prefrontal cortical dysfunction [4] leading to problems in reorienting attention back to the distracted task performance. This problem in reorienting attention is indicated in the present study by enhanced number of omitted responses in the visual task after a novel sound. The decrement in the LN peak latency observed in the ADHD children in relation to the controls might, in turn, be associated with a higher degree of impulsivity, which is a symptom of ADHD [2,15]. In summary, the present study suggests that ERPs recorded to the task-irrelevant novel stimuli distracting the children s performance provide us with evidence supporting the observations of attentional and behavioral deficits in ADHD. Although, the present study should be considered as preliminary, given the relatively small size of the two groups, the present results suggest that a multimethodological approach combining neuropsychological, behavioral and electrophysiological measures of distractibility and involuntary orienting of attention improve our understanding of the brain behavior relationship in ADHD. Acknowledgements The authors thank Ms. Kirsi Järvinen for her help in recruiting the control children, as well as Ms. Leena Wallendahr and Mr. Timo Saarinen for assistance with the electrophysiological recording. V. Gumenyuk was supported by the Academy of Finland grant no. 55606. R. Näätänen and O. Korzyukov were supported by the Academy of Finland grants nos. 77322 and 102316. C. Escera was supported by the grant no. PM99-0167 of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Technology. References [1] K. Alho, I. Winkler, C. Escera, M. Huotilainen, J. Virtanen, I.P. Jääskeläinen, E. Pekkonen, R.J. Ilmoniemi, Processing of novel sounds and frequency changes in the human auditory cortex: magnetoencephalographic recordings, Psychophysiology 35 (1998) 211 224. [2] American Psychiatric Association, DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth ed., American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, 1994. [3] S. Berti, E. Schröger, A comparison of auditory and visual distraction effects: behavioral and event-related indices, Cogn. Brain Res. 10 (2001) 265 273. [4] G. Bush, J.A. Frazier, S.L. Rauch, L.J. Seidman, P.J. Whalen, M.A. Jenike, B.R. Rosen, J. Biederman, Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fmri and the Counting Stroop, Biol. Psychiatry 45 (1999) 1542 1552. [5] E. Courchesne, S.A. Hillyard, R. Galambos, Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man, Clin. Neurophysiol. 39 (1975) 131 143. [6] C. Escera, K. Alho, E. Schröger, I. Winkler, Involuntary attention and distractibility as evaluated with Event-related brain potentials, Audiol. Neurootol. 5 (2000) 151 166. [7] D. Friedman, Y.M. Cycowicz, H. Gaeta, The novelty P3: an eventrelated potential (ERP) sign of the brain s evaluation of novelty, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25 (2001) 355 373. [8] M. Gomot, M.H. Giard, J.L. Adrien, C. Barthelemy, N. Bruneau, Hypersensitivity to acoustic change in children with autism: electrophysiological evidence of left frontal cortex dysfunction, Psychophysiology 39 (2002) 577 584. [9] V. Gumenyuk, O. Korzyukov, K. Alho, C. Escera, R. Näätänen, Effects of auditory distraction on electrophysiological brain activity and performance in children aged 8 13 years, Psychophysiology 41 (2004) 30 36. [10] L.M. Jonkman, C. Kemner, H.S. Koelega, G. Camfferman, R.J. vd Gaag, J.K. Buitelaar, H. van Engeland, M.N. Verbaten, Event-related potentials and performance of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: children and normal controls in auditory and visual selective attention tasks, Biol. Psychiatry 41 (1997) 595 611. [11] C. Kemner, M.N. Verbaten, H.S. Koelega, J.K. Buitelaar, R.J. van der Gaag, G. Camfferman, H. van Engeland, Event-related brain potentials in children with attention-deficit and hyperactivity disorder: effects of stimulus deviancy and task relevance in the visual and auditory modality, Biol. Psychiatry 40 (1996) 522 534. [12] R. Kilpeläinen, L. Luoma, E. Herrgard, H. Yppärilä, J. Partanen, J. Karhu, Persistent frontal P300 brain potential suggests abnormal processing of auditory information in distractible children, NeuroReport 10 (1999) 3405 3410. [13] R.T. Knight, Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man, Clin. Neurophysiol. 59 (1984) 9 20. [14] R.T. Knight, D. Scabini, D.L. Woods, C.C. Clayworth, Contributions of temporal parietal junction to the human auditory P3, Brain Res. 502 (1989) 109 116. [15] A.E. Morgan, G.W. Hynd, C.A. Riccio, J. Hall, Validity of DSM-IV ADHD predominantly inattentive and combined types: relationship to previous DSM diagnoses/subtype differences, J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 35 (1996) 325 333. [16] R.D. Oades, Frontal, temporal and lateralized brain function in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a psychophysiological and neuropsychological viewpoint on development, Behav. Brain Res. 94 (1998) 83 95. [17] A. Pfefferbaum, J. Ford, R. Johnson Jr., B. Wenegrat, B.S. Kopell, Manipulation of P3 latency: speed vs. accuracy instructions, Clin. Neurophysiol. 55 (1983) 188 197. [18] C.W. Ponton, J.J. Eggermont, B. Kwong, M. Don, Maturation of human central auditory system activity: evidence from multichannel evoked potentials, Clin. Neurophysiol. 111 (2000) 220 236. [19] E. Schröger, C. Wolff, Attentional orienting and reorienting is indicated by human event-related brain potentials, NeuroReport 9 (1998) 3355 3358. [20] E.N. Sokolov, J.A. Sprinks, R. Näätänen, H. Lyytinen, The Orienting Response in Information Processing, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, London, NJ, 2002. [21] N.K. Squires, K.C. Squires, S.A. Hillyard, Two varieties of longlatency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli, Clin. Neurophysiol. 38 (1975) 387 401. [22] D. Wechsler, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, revised, Psykologien kustannus, Helsinki, 1984 (in Finnish). [23] D. Wechsler, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third ed., Psykologien kustannus, Helsinki, 1999 (in Finnish). [24] N. Wetzel, S. Berti, A. Widmann, E. Schroger, Distraction and reorientation in children: a behavioral and ERP study, NeuroReport 15 (2004) 1355 1358. [25] E. Yago, C. Escera, K. Alho, M.-H. Giard, J.M. Serra-Grabulosa, Spatiotemporal dynamics of the auditory novelty-p3 event-related brain potential, Cogn. Brain Res. 16 (2003) 383 390.