INTERACTIONAL CHALLENGES TO THE CONSISTENT ENACTMENT OF THERAPY FOR LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENTS IN APHASIA

Similar documents
Critical Review: Group Therapy for Post-Stroke Aphasia Rehabilitation

Case Study 1 Aimee Burns, Stephanie Kramer, Courtney Scholl, Diona Ysaac

How difficult is it? How well Adults with Aphasia Perceive Task Demands

MANUAL FOR THE LIDCOMBE PROGRAM OF EARLY STUTTERING INTERVENTION 2008 CONTENTS

INTRODUCTION. Evidence standards for justifiable evidence claims, June 2016

Maximizing Generalization Effects of Semantic Feature Analysis

The detection and management of pain in patients with dementia in acute care settings: development of a decision tool: Research protocol.

Critical Evaluation of the Beach Center Family Quality of Life Scale (FQOL-Scale)

LIHS Mini Master Class

One week program of activities. Aimed at AS-Level Psychology students. Takes place in July, after AS-Level exams

INTRODUCTION Use of images in multiple-choice tasks is common in linguistic comprehension

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

VIEWS AND EXPERIENCES OF DEAF TEACHERS ON INCLUSIVE EDUCATION AND DISABILITY

1. Before starting the second session, quickly examine total on short form BDI; note

CurriculumVitae. Regina Jokel. Business Address: (1) Baycrest Centre (2) Graduate Department

The image part with relationship ID rid3 was not found in the file. Susan Cochrane, M.A., CCC SLP, BRFS Sheryl R. Gottwald, Ph.D.

Establishing long-term fluency goals when working with adults who stutter

Tasks of Executive Control TEC. Interpretive Report. Developed by Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Robert M. Roth, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1

(77, 72, 74, 75, and 81).

Lecture 5 Conducting Interviews and Focus Groups

Abstract. Key words: bias, culture, Five-Factor Model, language, NEO-PI-R, NEO-PI-3, personality, South Africa

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010.

SLP is a scientific field. SLPs are clinical scientists. Why EBP? Why EBP? 12/12/2013. Disclosures

Performance Improvement Project Implementation & Submission Tool

2.1 Participants 122 stroke patients with aphasia and 25 healthy controls (Table 1). <insert Table 1>

Speak Out! Sam Trychin, Ph.D. Copyright 1990, Revised Edition, Another Book in the Living With Hearing Loss series

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast

RECOVERY OF LINGUISTIC DEFICITS IN STROKE PATIENTS; A THREE- YEAR-FOLLOW UP STUDY.

SFHPT25 Explain the rationale for systemic approaches

Effective Intentions: The Power of Conscious Will

Hearing aid dispenser approval process review Introduction Hearing aid dispenser data transfer... 6

Mental Health Awareness Training for Prison Officers: Evaluation of a Training Video

GCE. Psychology. Mark Scheme for January Advanced Subsidiary GCE Unit G541: Psychological Investigations. Oxford Cambridge and RSA Examinations

A p p e n d i c e s. Appendix A: Using the Curriculum

Research, Evidence and Practice: Recursive Relationships. Arlene Vetere, PhD

Ministry of Children and Youth Services. Follow-up to VFM Section 3.01, 2013 Annual Report RECOMMENDATION STATUS OVERVIEW

City, University of London Institutional Repository

Constructing a Three-Part Instrument for Emotional Intelligence, Social Intelligence and Learning Behavior

PHYSICAL MEDICINE AND REHABILITATION CSHCN SERVICES PROGRAM PROVIDER MANUAL

WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One

Critical Review: The Effectiveness of Constraint-Induced Language Therapy in a Distributive Format

Process of a neuropsychological assessment

Empirical approaches to family assessment. Journal of Family Therapy, 22 (2):

Online Courses for Parents and Professionals Who Want to Know More About Children who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing

The Personal Profile System 2800 Series Research Report

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Deaf-blindness

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

Conference: Atypical Interaction: Conversation Analysis and Communication Impairments. Venue: The Edge, 34 Endcliffe Crescent, Sheffield S10 3ED

CHAPTER 4 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

An Evaluation of Companion Pets with Elderly Psychiatric Patients Elaine Haughie

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi

Effects of Response Elaboration Training on Increased Length and Complexity of Utterances with Two Participants with Fluent Aphasia

CATS IN SHORTS. Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions. Holger Ursin March Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen.

In this chapter we discuss validity issues for quantitative research and for qualitative research.

The Essex Study Optimised classroom acoustics for all

Appreciative systems

Introduction Persons with aphasia who are trained to generate abstract words (e.g., justice) in a specific context-category (e.g.

Toward An Understanding of Bullying Within the Puerto Rican Cultural, Social, and School Context

Implementing Lean Six Sigma in Organizations. Dhr. B.A. Lameijer

thoughts, wants, or needs, they might get frustrated and engage in behaviors that are potentially

CHAPTER 10 Educational Psychology: Motivating Students to Learn

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Memory. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs

birds were used. Every two weeks during treatment, all thirty-two items were probed. A final probe was collected six to seven weeks post-treatment.

Report of Recovery Star Research Seminar

PUPILS WITH AUTISM UNIT 14 THE SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL CURRICULUM: SOCIAL ASSISTANCE FOR THE PUPIL ON THE AUTISM SPECTRUM

WORKING WITH INTERPRETERS GUIDELINES FOR MENTAL HEALTH PROFESSIONALS. Table of Contents. 1- Introduction

Occupational Therapy (OTHR)

Improving business performance with emotional intelligence. Genos emotional intelligence products and services overview

Towards an instrument for measuring sensemaking and an assessment of its theoretical features

A Randomized- Controlled Trial of Foundations for Literacy

Counselling and Self- Awareness. Mr. Lema, Isaac Clinical Psychologist (MSc) 11 th November 2015

Attention STAR Providers: Physical, Occupational, and Speech Therapy Benefits for All Ages to Change for Texas Medicaid September 1, 2017

An International Study of the Reliability and Validity of Leadership/Impact (L/I)

CONSTITUENT VOICE FISCAL YEAR 2016 RESULTS REPORT

How Do We Assess Students in the Interpreting Examinations?

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

An Introduction to the CBS Health Cognitive Assessment

ISSN X Journal of Educational and Social Research Vol. 2 (8) October 2012

Multi-agency collaboration and service provision in the early years

A Mixed-Model Approach to Studying Treatment Outcomes

Intensive Training Program Overview

@sssftnhs. sssft.nhs.uk

The Cochrane Collaboration

SRP Component 1. PART II: Session-by-Session Guide

Trainer Assessment Report Guidelines (TARG) 2019 Approved : December Contents

Speech perception in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer s type (DAT) Mitchell S. Sommers Department of Psychology Washington University

CLINICAL DEMENTIA RATING SUMMARY

Cognitive Therapy Rating Scale (CTRS)

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE

Observation and Assessment. Narratives

AN ANALYSIS ON VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY OF TEST ITEMS IN PRE-NATIONAL EXAMINATION TEST SMPN 14 PONTIANAK

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT

Initially published in The EMDRIA Newsletter, 12/2003, Vol. 8 #4, pp11-13 Updated 5/31/04

Lecturer: Dr. Adote Anum, Dept. of Psychology Contact Information:

Study protocol. Version 1 (06 April 2011) Ethics ref: R&D ref: UK CRC portfolio ID:

THE RESEARCH ENTERPRISE IN PSYCHOLOGY

GCAP Final Project Letter of Intent. Intervening with Autism: Linking Theory to Practice: Margaret R Davidson

Transcription:

INTERACTIONAL CHALLENGES TO THE CONSISTENT ENACTMENT OF THERAPY FOR LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENTS IN APHASIA INTRODUCTION Task or information-processing demands (e.g. Wallach and Miller 1988) are the challenges that essentially set in motion the language and cognitive processing work through which the goals of aphasia language therapy are achieved (Robson and Horton, in press). Therapists operationalise goals by introducing stimuli, getting the client to respond, responding contingently themselves, and then moving on to the next stimulus (McTear and King 1991). Byng and Black (1995: 311) argue that therapists contingent responses modulating the therapy task could be one of the critical aspects in the process of therapy. As Simmons- Mackie et al (1999) point out, clients receive feedback from therapists, but therapists also modify their treatment in response to the feedback they receive from clients. It has also been suggested (Horton and Byng 2002) that therapists increased awareness of consistency in implementing interventions could lead to more efficient and possibly more effective therapy. A study aimed at developing a systematic and explicit descriptive framework for aphasia language therapy examined therapy between experienced clinicians and people with aphasia in day-to-day practice. One question addressed in this study concerned the type of evidence which might provide explicit definitions of techniques such as those described in the literature as cueing, facilitation and feedback in other words those techniques implicated in the modulation and consistent enactment of therapy tasks. METHODS Participants Fourteen therapists and thirteen people with aphasia formed fifteen therapist-aphasic person dyads. Therapists were working in the UK and registered with the professional body, had at least three experience of aphasia therapy, and were members of the British Aphasiology Society. Participants with aphasia were at least one month post onset of a left CVA, neurologically stable, and with evidence of moderate-mild expressive aphasia. People with significant cognitive or comprehension difficulties, hearing impairment or concurrent psychiatric difficulties were excluded. Participating therapists had a mean of 11 experience of working with people with aphasia. There were ten people with non-fluent aphasia and three with fluent aphasia, described as having severe or neologistic jargon. Range of time since onset of aphasia is shown in Table 1. TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE Time known to each other ranged from 0.25 [one session] to 48. This is partly indicative of the different types of location represented. The highest end of the range (D1) which anecdotal evidence suggests is unusually long is the only example here of privately funded therapy. TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE Table 2 shows data associated with location of sessions and length of time working together for each dyad. It should be noted that two participants with aphasia (D2/3A and D10/11A) worked with more than one therapist, and one therapist (D9/10T) worked with two of the participants with aphasia. 1

Data production and analysis Each dyad was asked to contribute three sessions to the study. In all forty one videotapes, varying in length from about thirty minutes to just over an hour formed the data set. The interactions between participants and the social processes of therapy entailed in those interactions were studied through the systematic observation of videotapes. The goal was to construct a data-driven descriptive framework accounting for the organisation of every interaction in the corpus (Mehan 1979: 1, 20). Data were analysed and evidence constructed using approaches which acknowledge the need to construct a big picture framework (Spradley 1980) as well as examining the detail of moment-by-moment interaction (Mehan 1979), supported by the methodology and specific techniques of Conversational Analysis (CA) (e.g. Sacks et al 1974). RESULTS A descriptive framework for therapy sessions The methods described above were used to develop a broad descriptive framework for the structure of therapy sessions. This domain structure was developed through close attention to turn-taking and sequence organisation, lexical choice, pauses and timing, materials and other artefacts. The framework is outlined in Figure 1. FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE The framework of domains is not prescriptive and does not describe an ideal session. This paper focuses on issues arising from the domain Doing therapy tasks, which in this study was demonstrably the main business of sessions. Domains have a number of key features, each of which has one or more types of representation. Table 3 gives an overview of the feature Task management. TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE Overall management of tasks concerns the ways therapists manage tasks across sessions as a whole which tasks are deployed, when they are introduced or curtailed and so on. Task management is also typified at a moment-by-moment level in Enacting tasks, with associated Task demands and their associated dimensions. Both types of Task management feature are implicated in notions of modulation and consistency. Modulation or inconsistency? The deployment of tasks and routines Task demands are often modulated from session-to-session. Within sessions tasks of different types or perceived levels of difficulty follow each other in orderly ways. Therapists also make responsive local judgements about introducing unpredicted subtasks in order to steer the main task in the right direction. Tasks often have associated responding routines, but in actual practice these routines were frequently flaunted. Usually it was the therapist who controlled changes, but there were instances where the aphasic person demonstrably resisted a proposed routine. It s getting a bit easy The notion of task demands often arose when therapists mentioned harder or a tricky one. Therapists were rarely explicit about reasons for a task or item being harder or tricky. However, there were examples of therapists clearly stating how they proposed to modulate the task. In D8(1) a three second time delay between stimulus presentation and response was introduced. The therapist s count-down soon became highly variable however. 2

She occasionally reached towards the aphasic person to try and attract his attention to the count, a move which he generally ignored. In D11(1) the therapist stated that she would withhold feedback until the aphasic person had carried out his own self-evaluation. This tactic was quickly undermined by the aphasic person looking to the therapist for feedback as soon as he responded. The timing of her feedback was highly predictable when he did get it right her positive evaluation was immediate. The suggestion is that even the split-second absence of a therapist response signalled that there was a problem. Therapist follow-up patterns become quickly established and the aphasic person is quickly attuned and very sensitive to changes in that pattern. Precision or clarity of responses Aphasic participants exhibited their own standards of precision, as evidenced by, for example, repeated self-initiated attempts at target words. They also clearly elicited help from the therapist to make necessary corrections. Requirements for precision or clarity in certain types of task were by no means always explicit, often being decided on a case-by-case basis. Therapist follow-up of the aphasic person s response while not overtly corrective may indicate a norm to be aspired to (Simmons-Mackie et al 1999: 224). In D8 (2) for example this occurred once in task-set one (9%); seven times in task-set two (28%); twice in task-set three (15%). Clinicians demonstrably worked assiduously to achieve the correct response (see van Kleeck and Richardson 1986). However, therapists adjusted their elicitations in ways that sometimes seemed at odds with the apparent goals of therapy. There were several lengthy sequences of semantically-related repair work which failed to elicit the required response, which the therapist concluded with phonetic fragments (e.g. unreleased phonemes) which functioned as phonemic cues to elicit the required response. Take your time Time allowed for the response to emerge, or before the therapist intervened varied greatly across the data. This variation is clearly due to many different factors. The aphasic person sometimes took an active role in claiming processing time ( seizing the floor : Sacks 1992: Vol II, Part VII, Lecture 13, 497). This enabled him/her to hold off therapist interventions, but led to widely variable within-task timings between elicitation and response. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Task modulation occurs in numerous ways in aphasia language therapy. Therapists clearly strive to maintain the integrity of therapy goals, although the drive to achieve the response may compromise the process through which they are achieved. Various interactive phenomena, such as the person with aphasia soliciting feedback, capitalising on predictable feedback patterns, seizing (and retaining) the floor, or declining to use various task-related procedures may affect the consistent implementation of tasks. It is suggested that close attention to technique-in-interaction has the potential to improve the consistency and hence efficiency and effectiveness of therapy. 3

TABLES Table 1 Aphasic person participants: gender, age and time since onset Aphasic person Gender Age () Time since onset of aphasia () D1A F 53 60 D6A F 67 8 D7A F 60 18 D2/3A (with two different therapists) F 50 24 D9A F 63 9 D13A F 59 8 D15A F 85 3 D14A F 83 24 N = 8 Women Mean age = 65 Range = 53-85 D8A M 63 7 D5A M 64 19 D10/11A M 40 5 D12A M 75 28 D4A M 59 24 N = 5 Men Mean age = 53 Range = 40-75 All participants (N = 13) Mean age = 63.15 Range = 40-85 Mean time since onset = 19.25 Range = 3-60 Mean time since onset = 16.6 Range = 5-28 Mean time since onset = 18.23 Range = 3-60 4

Table 2 Therapists and people with aphasia: time working together and location Therapist code Aphasic person code Location of videotaped sessions Time working together () D1T D1A Domiciliary (private) 48 D2T D2/3A Out-patient 6 D3T 24 D4T D4A 4 D5T D5A Out-patient 1 D6T D6A Out-patient 7 Domiciliary D7T D7A Out-patient 0.75 D8T D8A In-patient rehabilitation 0.75 D9/10T D9A clinic 0.75 D10/11A 0.25 D11T 1 D12T D12A Out-patient aphasia 4 D13T D13A Out-patient 5 D14T D14A Domiciliary 9 D15T D15A Out-patient 2.75 14 therapists 13 people with aphasia 11 different individual locations (3 different types of location) Mean = 7.62 Range = 0.25 48 5

Table 3 Doing therapy tasks Task management types, sub-types and dimensions Features Types Sub-types and dimensions B. Task management Overall management of tasks when are tasks introduced? what tasks are introduced and enacted? which tasks follow each other? when are tasks curtailed, prolonged or repeated? Enacting tasks Task demands Dimensions associated with task-related responses Dimensions associated with stimulus items Dimensions associated with enacting routines Response mode Response content Responding routines Precision Item types and combinations Numbers of items Item clarity Content and manner of therapist Elicitations Time and timing in Enacting routines Elicitation-response follow-up Responding process FIGURES 3. Doing therapy tasks 1. The Settling down period 2. Opening up the business Task introductions Task management Enacting tasks Response management 4. The Closing down period Figure 1 Domains a descriptive framework for therapy sessions 6

References Byng, S and Black, M (1995) What makes a therapy? Some parameters of therapeutic intervention in aphasia. European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 30, 3, 303-316. Mehan, H (1979) Learning Lessons. Social Organisation in the Classroom. Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. McTear, MF and King, F (1991) Miscommunication in Clinical Contexts: The Speech Therapy Interview. (Chapter 10, pp 195-214). In: Coupland, N, Giles, H and Wiemann, JM (Eds) Miscommunication and Problematic Talk. London: Sage. Robson, J and Horton, S (in press) Replicating therapy more than just more of the same. Chapter 10, in: Byng, S, Duchan, J and Pound, C (Eds). The Aphasia Therapy File Volume 2. London: Psychology Press Sacks, H (1992) Two floor-seizure techniques: Appositional expletives and Uh. Lecture 13, Part VII, Fall 1971 (pp 495-498). In: Jefferson, G (Ed) Lectures on Conversation, Volume II. Oxford: Blackwell. Sacks, H, Schegloff, EA and Jefferson, G (1974) A Simplest Systematics for the Organisation of Turn-Taking in Conversation. Language, 50, 4, 696-735. Simmons-Mackie, N, Damico, JS and Damico, HL (1999) A Qualitative Study of Feedback in Aphasia Treatment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 218-230. Spradley, J (1980) Participant Observation. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Wallach, GP and Miller, L (1988) Language Intervention and Academic Success. Boston: Little, Brown and Co. Van Kleek, A and Richardson, A (1986) What s in an error? Using children s responses as language teaching opportunities. Journal of the National Student Speech- Language-Hearing Association, 14, 25-50. 7