Jeffry A. Simpson, W. Andrew Collins, and Jessica E. Salvatore University of Minnesota. Introduction. An Organizational Perspective on Development

Similar documents
Developmental Antecedents of Emotion in Romantic Relationships. Jeffry A. Simpson. W. Andrew Collins. SiSi Tran. Katherine C.

Shared and Distinctive Origins and Correlates of Adult Attachment Representations: The Developmental Organization of Romantic Functioning

Attachment Working Models Twist Memories of Relationship Events

Running Head: COGNITIVE VULNERABILITY AND ATTACHMENT. Cognitive Vulnerability and Attachment. Nathan L. Williams University of Arkansas

CUA. National Catholic School of Social Service Washington, DC Fax

Attachment and relationships: Milestones and future directions

The impact of attachment insecurity on emotion regulation

Supporting Families to Build Secure Attachment Relationships : Comments on Benoit, Dozier, and Egeland

Running head: ATTACHMENT STYLES AND DIFFERENT PERSONALITY TRAITS

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Child Dev Perspect. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 March 01.

Predictors of young adults representations of and behavior in their current romantic relationship: Prospective tests of the prototype hypothesis

Secretary: Mrs. Judy Manners x Office: 280 BSB

A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY. Kenneth Lee Raby

Adult Attachment Theory and Affective Reactivity and Regulation. Paula R. Pietromonaco University of Massachusetts, Amherst

FINAL DRAFT 3/30/01. Running head: RELATIONSHIP COHERENCE

Attachment disorders: classroom strategies to identify cases, limit emotional outbursts and

PSYCHOSOCIAL DEVELOPMENT IN INFANCY

Interpersonal Biofeedback Training with Couples

Scoring Adults Secure Base Use And Support

The Enduring Predictive Significance of Early Maternal Sensitivity: Social and Academic Competence Through Age 32 Years

Moderating Processes in the Link between Early Caregiving and. Adult Individual and Romantic Functioning:

Silent ACEs: The Epidemic of Attachment and Developmental Trauma

The Effect of Attachment and Sternberg s Triangular Theory of Love on Relationship Satisfaction

Counseling and Psychotherapy Theory. Week 4. Psychodynamic Approach II : Object Relations/Attachment Theory

Observational Research

AAA. Report #10 A Profile of Adolescent and Adult Siblings. - Principal Investigators Marsha Mailick Seltzer, Ph.D.

Attachment, Commitment, and Relationship Maintenance

Resilience in Individuals and Communities

Identity Development. Kate C. McLean Western Washington University. Moin Syed University of Minnesota

TERMINOLOGY INSECURITY? APPLICATION OF ATTACHMENT THEORY TO THE HUMAN-COMPANION ANIMAL BOND, AND REVIEW OF ITS EFFECTS

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN LIFE SATISFACTION AND ATTACHMENT STYLES WITH PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING OF CHILDREN ON COVERED BY BEHZISTEY IN TEHRAN

Emotion Regulation in Intimate Relationships

Focus of Today s Presentation. Partners in Healing Model. Partners in Healing: Background. Data Collection Tools. Research Design

understanding ATTACHMENT Torben Bergland, MD Associate Director General Conference of Seventh-day Adventists Health Ministries Department

= add definition here. Definition Slide

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

CUA. National Catholic School of Social Service Washington, DC Fax

Attachment Styles as Moderator Between Stress and Emotional Eating in Adolescent Girls: A Research Proposal

What s the Main Issue Here?

Written Assignment 3. Chapters covering Human Development, Personality and Motivation and Emotion. Corresponds with Exam 3

EFT for Individuals. NY EFT Summit 2012

Regulation Processes in Romantic Relationships

THE RELATIONSHIPS THAT SHAPE US THE EFFECT OF INTERACTIONS ON DEVELOPMENT KEELY J. HOPE, PH.D., LMHC, NCC EASTERN WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY

A longitudinal study of conflict in new parents: The role of attachment

Prorelationship Maintenance Behaviors: The Joint Roles of Attachment and Commitment

Test Reactivity: Does the Measurement of Identity Serve as an Impetus for Identity Exploration?

Emotionally Focused. EFT with individuals. Dr. Yolanda Von Hockauf. Per il Progresso nella Pratica e nella Ricerca in Psicoterapia

Running Head: VISUAL SCHEDULES FOR STUDENTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

The Importance of Attachment in Adolescence. Lesley Bell Therapeutic Social Worker

Reading: Allan Schore: Affect Regulation and Origin of the Self Chapter1 "Introduction" Affect Regulation and the Origin of the Self"

Using Attachment Theory in School Social Work

Community based services for children and adults with learning disabilities

Dimensions of Attachment and Commitment Across the Transition to Parenthood. Honors Research Thesis

PERSON PERCEPTION AND INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION

Mental Health Strategy. Easy Read

Advanced Code of Influence. Book 10

The Relationship Between Attachment, Couple Conflict, and Recovery From Conflict

The goal of this Study Guide is to facilitate for students of teenage years a better understanding of issues such as, Addiction, LGBTQ Community,

The Action Is In the Interaction

Attachment Styles, View of Self and Negative Affect

Family and Individual Difference Predictors of Trait Aspects of Negative Interpersonal Behaviors During Emerging Adulthood

10/9/2018. Ways to Measure Variables. Three Common Types of Measures. Scales of Measurement

ABSTRACT PARTNER POSITIVE BEHAVIOR AND RECIPIENT SATISFACTION IN MILD TO MODERATELY ABUSIVE COUPLES AS MODERATED BYATTACHMENT STYLE

Personal Disclosure Statement and Notice of Practices

(Specification A) PSYA1 (JAN09PSYA101) General Certificate of Education Advanced Subsidiary Examination January 2009

Center for Child Counseling

Frontal EEG Asymmetry and Regulation during Childhood

Hammen Publications since 2000

Infancy. Caregiver Responses to Infant s Crying 1. Predictable transformation of infant distress to comfort (B); Type B

Basic Needs and Well-Being: A Self-Determination Theory View

COUNSELLING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY. COSCA s DESCRIPTION

MENTAL HEALTH FIRST AID TRAINING

First Semester. 2. Describe two connections between adult attachment style and adult relationships.

Emotional Development

Attachment, Affect, Anxiety & Addiction. Brian G. Lengfelder LCPC, CAADC, CCJP, SAP, MAC, CSAT, CMAT, ACRPS

I Fear no Evil for Thou Art with Me: Mental health and the sense of God as an attachment figure

Relationship Questionnaire

Working The Companion Animal in the Context of New Interpersonal Relationships

PSYCHODYNAMIC THEORY PART I: HISTORICAL UNDERPINNINGS 571-NCSSS

Mental Health is for Everyone

Developmental Psychology

Enhancing Support for Special Populations through Understanding Neurodiversity

Developmental, Individual-Differences, Relationship-Based Model

How to Raise Emotionally Healthy Children - Webinar Handout/Outline

Day #3 of 8 Types of Attachment & Communication. Roster: Handouts:

The Needs of Young People who have lost a Sibling or Parent to Cancer.

Slide 1. Slide 2. Slide 3 Similar observations in all subsets of the disorder. Personality Disorders. General Symptoms. Chapter 9

Autism & intellectual disabilities. How to deal with confusing concepts

Attachment and Positive Engagement Following Relationship Conflict

ABSTRACT. Mandy K. Mount, Ph.D., Professor Mary Ann Hoffman, Ph.D. Department of Counseling and Personnel Services

The Goodness of Fit in Attachment

Trauma and Children s Ability to Learn and Develop. Dr. Katrina A. Korb. Department of Educational Foundations, University of Jos

12 The biology of love

A-TIP Acute -Traumatic Incident Procedures Roy Kiessling, LISW, ACSW

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby

Class 2 Self-states: Regulated-integrated vs Dysregulated-dissociated (Affect is at the core of self-states)

Eating Disorder Support Services

Personality. Development of Personality

The Effect of Adult Attachment Style on Couples. Relationship Satisfaction

What s Trauma All About

Transcription:

The Impact of Early Interpersonal Experience on Adult Romantic Relationship Functioning: Recent Findings From the Minnesota Longitudinal Study of Risk and Adaptation Current Directions in Psychological Science 20(6) 355 359 The Author(s) 2011 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0963721411418468 http://cdps.sagepub.com Jeffry A. Simpson, W. Andrew Collins, and Jessica E. Salvatore University of Minnesota Abstract Adopting an organizational view on social development, we have investigated how interpersonal experiences early in life predict how well individuals will resolve relationship conflicts, recover from conflicts, and have stable, satisfying relationships with their romantic partners in early adulthood. We have also identified specific interpersonal experiences during middle childhood and adolescence that mediate the connection between how individuals regulated their emotions with their parents very early in life and how they do so as young adults in their romantic relationships. We discuss the many advantages of adopting an organizational view on social development. Keywords early experience, romantic relationships, conflict resolution, conflict recovery, weak-link status Introduction One of the core ideas underlying many theories of personality is that early social experiences leave an indelible imprint on people, including how they behave in later relationships. This premise is a cornerstone of several major theories, including those of Freud (1940), Erikson (1963), and Bowlby (1969, 1973). For the past several years, we have investigated how certain interpersonal experiences early in life are systematically linked to how people think, feel, and behave in their adult romantic relationships two decades later. Our goal has been to identify and understand how early attachment-relevant experiences some of which occurred before individuals could form memories of these events affect the functioning of adult romantic relationships and the interpersonal pathways through which transmission patterns might occur. We have also examined the significant role that romantic partners assume in buffering certain people from the vulnerabilities posed by their early negative relationship histories. Our studies have generated three key insights: (a) Certain early experiences exert small but apparently lasting effects on how most people think, feel, and behave in their adult romantic relationships years later; (b) There appear to be specific interpersonal pathways through which early-life experiences impact adult romantic relationship functioning; and (c) Some romantic partners are able to buffer individuals with early-life vulnerabilities, such as insecure attachment histories, from experiencing negative relationship outcomes in adulthood. An Organizational Perspective on Development Our research has been guided by an organizational perspective on social development that has four basic principles (Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005). First, the meaning of behavior depends on how it fits with other behaviors in a specific social context. For example, disengaging from conflict with a romantic partner when it is appropriate to do so ought to protect individuals from the deleterious effects of further conflict (Gottman, 1994), whereas failure to disengage especially when continued conflict is futile should harm future relationship functioning (Gottman & Levenson, 1999). Second, the way that individuals regulate their emotions in adult relationships should be associated with how they regulated their emotions with their caregivers earlier in life (Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986; Thompson, 2008). Synchronous and supportive Corresponding Author: Jeffry A. Simpson, Department of Psychology, 75 E. River Road, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455-0344 E-mail: simps108@umn.edu

356 Simpson et al. relationships with early caregivers are the first context in which good, functional emotion-regulation skills are learned (Sroufe et al., 2005). Indeed, attachment security, which is a strong indicator of good interaction synchrony and effective emotion regulation in early childhood (Schore, 2005), predicts better emotion-regulation skills later in life (Thompson, 2008). Third, mental representations (working models) of the self and relationship partners formed early in life guide interaction patterns in their later relationships, including romantic ones (Roisman, Collins, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2005; Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986). According to Bowlby (1973), the quality of caregiving provided by early caregivers is a template for what later relationships will be like, which in turn affects how people think, feel, and behave in their later relationships. Fourth, experiences in early relationships with parents and in later relationships with adult romantic partners should jointly influence what happens later in development (Carlson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2004; Sroufe, Egeland, & Kreutzer, 1990). For example, positive relationship experiences encountered later in life (such as becoming involved with a committed, well-adjusted partner) can counteract negative relationship experiences earlier in life (such as experiencing less responsive or less consistent caregiving during childhood; see Sroufe et al., 2005). Moreover, romantic partners may buffer developmentally vulnerable individuals (such as those who have insecure attachment histories) from experiencing bad outcomes in their romantic relationships (Rönkä, Oravala, & Pulkkinen, 2002; Tran & Simpson, 2009). The Minnesota Longitudinal Study of Risk and Adaptation In our research, we have tested how specific early interpersonal experiences are systematically linked to adult romanticrelationship functioning by following a longitudinal sample of participants from birth into adulthood. The sample comes from the Minnesota Longitudinal Study of Risk and Adaptation (MLSRA; Sroufe et al., 2005). In 1976 and 1977, new mothers receiving free prenatal services in Minneapolis public health clinics were recruited for the study. Their firstborn children (whom we call targets ) became the primary focus of the study. From birth, targets (N = 174) have been assessed at regular intervals at each stage of development using multimethod measures (e.g., interviews, questionnaires, teachers and parents ratings, behavioral observations). Our research has focused on approximately 75 targets and their romantic partners who were involved in an established romantic relationship when targets were 20 to 21 years old. Assessments were made at several pivotal points of social development points at which targets were transitioning to new roles and exploring how to balance autonomy and interdependence with significant people in their lives, especially parents, peers, close friends, and romantic partners. As children, targets were observed in myriad social situations to assess how well they regulated (managed) their emotions in different stressful interpersonal contexts. For example, when they were 12 and 18 months old, targets were videotaped in the Strange Situation (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978), a stressful lab procedure that involves separations and reunions with caregivers (targets mothers). Securely attached children use their caregivers as a source of comfort to reduce negative affect in the Strange Situation, which allows them to engage in other life tasks, such as exploring the environment. Insecurely attached children do not use and cannot rely on their caregivers to reduce negative affect. Consequently, their attachment systems stay activated, and they remain distressed throughout the Strange Situation procedure. When targets were 2 years old, they and their mothers completed a series of videotaped tasks during which mothers tried to teach targets (i.e., their own children) skills that were above the children s capabilities. When targets were in elementary school (ages 6 8), they were rated by their classroom teachers in terms of how socially competent they were in organized classroom situations compared to their classmates. When targets were age 16, they completed an interview during which they described the nature and quality of their relationships with their best friends, including the security of their relationships and how conflicts were resolved. When targets were 20 to 21 years old, they and their romantic partners came to our lab and completed a videotaped conflict-resolution task, which was immediately followed by a conflict-recovery task (see below). Each of these developmental measures assessed how well each target managed (regulated) her or his emotions with significant others at different points of social development in different types of stressful situations. When targets were 23 years old, we assessed whether they were still dating the same romantic partners. Emotion regulation during conflict In an initial study examining emotion regulation during relationship conflict, we (Simpson, Collins, Tran, & Haydon, 2007) found that if targets had had an insecure attachment relationship with their mothers at 12 months (assessed in the Strange Situation), they reported and behaviorally expressed more negative emotions when trying to resolve major relationship conflicts with their romantic partners at age 20 to 21. However, this lingering effect of early attachment was mediated by targets degree of social competence in elementary school (rated by their grade-school teachers) and the qualities of their relationships with their best friend at age 16 (e.g., the degree to which targets felt they could share any personal feelings with their best friend, how much they trusted and could count on their best friend, etc.). This mediation pattern, which fit the data better than several other possible models, is shown in Figure 1. These findings reflect one interpersonal pathway through which infant attachment security versus insecurity is probabilistically related to the quality of emotion regulation in adult romantic relationships 20 years later.

Early Interpersonal Experience and Adult Romantic Relationship Functioning 357 Peer Competence (age 6-8) Friendship Security (age 16) Infant Attachment (age 1) Reported/Displayed Negative Emotion in the Romantic Relationship (age 20 21) Fig. 1. The best-fitting mediation model showing the interpersonal pathway through which attachment security versus insecurity at age 1 is associated with greater reported and displayed negative emotions in adult romantic relationships at age 20 to 21. Recovering from conflict In a second study, we (Salvatore, Kuo, Steele, Simpson, & Collins, 2011) examined whether and how early interpersonal experience is related to the way in which individuals recover from romantic-relationship conflicts. Conflict recovery refers to how quickly, well, and completely individuals can emotionally and behaviorally shift from a negative interaction (e.g., discussing a major relationship problem) in order to achieve another dyadic goal (e.g., discussing topics on which both partners agree). Recovering from conflict is likely to involve a different set of skills and abilities than resolving a conflict constructively and fairly (Salvatore et al., 2011). Targets who had been securely attached at 12 to 18 months rebounded from conflicts with their romantic partners better at age 20 to 21, controlling for how difficult the conflicts were. Their romantic partners also recovered better if targets had been securely attached earlier in life. Moreover, having a romantic partner who recovered better from conflict was associated with greater relationship satisfaction and more positive relationship emotions. Finally, targets who had been insecurely attached as infants were more likely to still be with their partners 2 years later (at age 23) if their partners displayed better conflict recovery at age 20 to 21. Emotionally well-regulated romantic partners, therefore, can protect individuals who have insecure attachment histories from certain romantic relationship difficulties in adulthood. Becoming the weak-link partner In a third study, we (Oriña et al., 2011) investigated whether being treated poorly by others earlier in life leads individuals to protect themselves by becoming the less-committed/lessinvested partner in their adult romantic relationships (i.e., the weak-link partner; Attridge, Berscheid, & Simpson, 1995). The weak-link partner in each relationship was defined as the one who scored relatively lower on commitment. We found that targets who either received less support from their mothers during challenging teaching tasks at age 2 or had more difficulty resolving conflicts with their best friends at age 16 were more likely to become the weak-link partners that is, the less committed/less invested partners within their romantic relationships at age 20 to 21. Furthermore, the less committed the weak-link partner was in each relationship compared to other weak links in the sample, as well as the larger the difference in commitment between the weak-link partner and the strong-link partner within each relationship, the more romantic couples behaved in an overtly hostile manner during their conflict discussions at age 20 to 21. In all three studies, the longitudinal effects we described remained statistically significant when several measures of current romantic relationship quality and functioning were statistically controlled. These effects, therefore, are not attributable to the quality of the current relationship. Advantages of an Organizational Perspective Mental representations (working models) of past relationships tend to be carried forward into new relationships, which can change existing representations, depending on how an individual is treated in later relationships (Bowlby, 1973; Carlson et al., 2004). By adopting an organizational view on development, one can generate unique predictions about an individual s future relationship functioning based on both his or her past and current functioning (Sroufe et al., 2005). For example, individuals can arrive at the same relationship outcome (e.g., they can have the same level of satisfaction) from different starting points; they can also arrive at different relationship outcomes from the same starting point. Cast another way, people who have different developmental histories can exhibit the same adaptation at one point in time but different adaptations at later points of development (Sroufe et al., 1990). These differences can be predictable to the extent that an individual s current relationship functioning partially reflects his or her cumulative developmental history up to the current relationship. Consider a concrete example. John and Tom both feel and display strong negative affect whenever they interact with their current romantic partners. John has a secure attachment history, but he recently discovered that his partner has been cheating on him. John and his partner are trying to repair their damaged relationship in therapy, but the emotional strains of

358 Simpson et al. the betrayal continue to be reflected in John s negative feelings and turbulent interactions with his partner. Tom also recently learned of his partner s infidelity, and he too is in therapy with her, working to repair the relationship. Tom, however, has an insecure attachment history. Tom, in other words, is carrying a lot of emotional baggage from his earlier insecure, unsupportive relationships into his current one. When John s and Tom s relationships are viewed now (at a single time point), they look very similar. Both are having problems with their current partners. A non-developmental perspective might predict that John and Tom will have fairly similar relationship trajectories and outcomes in the future. Very different predictions emerge, however, when John s and Tom s current adaptations are considered in combination with their divergent developmental histories. If John s therapy is successful and he is able to trust his partner again, his relationship should show better and more rapid improvement in the future, based on his secure working models. Tom s relationship, in contrast, is likely to show poorer and slower improvement even if therapy is helpful, given his insecure working models. Future Directions and Conclusions There are several important directions in which future research should head. We need to learn more about how early social experiences accumulate across development to affect adult relationship outcomes, including non-romantic ones. We still do not know whether early social experiences occurring at specific time points (e.g., during the first few years of life) are uniquely and independently linked to specific types of adult relationship outcomes. We also are just beginning to understand how current relationship variables statistically interact with representations of relationships earlier in life to influence adult relationship experiences and outcomes (see Salvatore et al., 2011). Future research also needs to determine whether and how specific Gene Early Environment interactions are associated with specific adult relationship outcomes. In conclusion, an organizational perspective highlights the coherence of social behavior in relationships across development. The pattern of relationship-relevant thoughts, feelings, and actions and especially their representation in working models is what links early interpersonal experiences with caregivers to later interpersonal experiences with peers and adult romantic partners. Although the features of good versus poor emotion regulation look somewhat different at each developmental stage, their underlying functions and meaning remain fairly stable over time. Our program of research has revealed that important adult romantic outcomes are systematically related to relationship experiences that occur very early in life, well before individuals can form conscious memories. This connection, however, is affected by what happens in different types of relationships at intervening stages of social development, including relationships with romantic partners in adulthood. Recommended Reading Oriña, M.M., Collins, W.A., Simpson, J.A., Salvatore, J.E., Haydon, K.C., & Kim, J.S. (2011). (See References). A representative project study that discusses how exposure to early supportive parenting and relationship conflict with best friends in adolescence predicts becoming the weak-link partner in later romantic relationships. Salvatore, J.E., Kuo, S.I., Steele, R.D., Simpson, J.A., & Collins, W.A. (2011). (See References). A representative project study that reveals how early attachment security versus insecurity is related to the way in which adults recover emotionally from major romantic relationship conflicts. Simpson, J.A., Collins, W.A., Tran, S., & Haydon, K.C. (2007). (See References). A representative project study that describes the interpersonal pathways through which early attachment security versus insecurity is linked to the experience and expression of negative emotions in later romantic relationships. Sroufe, L.A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E.A., & Collins, W.A. (2005). (See References). An excellent, comprehensive overview of the entire Minnesota Longitudinal Study of Risk and Adaptation project. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding This research was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) to Byron Egeland, L. Alan Sroufe, and W. Andrew Collins (R01-MH40864) and to Jeffry A. Simpson (R01- MH49599); a National Institute for Child Health and Human Development grant to W. Andrew Collins, Byron Egeland, and L. Alan Sroufe (R01-HD054850); and an NIMH pre-doctoral training grant to Jessica E. Salvatore (T32-MH015755-32). References Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Attridge, M., Berscheid, E., & Simpson, J.A. (1995). Predicting relationship stability from both partners versus one. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 254 268. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.2.254 Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York, NY: Basic Books. Carlson, E.A., Sroufe, L.A., & Egeland, B. (2004). The construction of experience: A longitudinal study of representation and behavior. Child Development, 75, 66 83. Erikson, E. (1963). Childhood and society. New York, NY: Norton. Freud, S. (1940). An outline of psychoanalysis. New York, NY: Hogarth Press. Gottman, J.M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship between marital processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Gottman, J.M., & Levenson, R.W. (1999). Rebound from marital conflict and divorce prediction. Family Process, 38, 287 292.

Early Interpersonal Experience and Adult Romantic Relationship Functioning 359 Oriña, M.M., Collins, W.A., Simpson, J.A., Salvatore, J.E., Haydon, K.C., & Kim, J.S. (2011). Developmental and dyadic perspectives on commitment in adult romantic relationships. Psychological Science, 22, 908 915. doi:10.1177/09567976114105735 Roisman, G.I., Collins, W.A., Sroufe, L.A., & Egeland, B. (2005). Predictors of young adults representations of and behavior in their current romantic relationship: Prospective tests of the prototype hypothesis. Attachment and Human Development, 7, 105 121. doi:10.1080/14616730500134928 Rönkä, A., Oravala, S., & Pulkkinen, L. (2002). I met this wife of mine and things got onto a better track: Turning points in risk development. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 47 63. doi:10.1006/ jado.2001.0448 Salvatore, J.E., Kuo, S.I., Steele, R.D., Simpson, J.A., & Collins, W.A. (2011). Recovering from conflict in romantic relationships: A developmental perspective. Psychological Science, 22, 376 383. doi:10.1177/0956797610397055 Schore, A.N. (2005). Back to basics: Attachment, affect regulation, and the developing right brain: Linking developmental neuroscience to pediatrics. Pediatrics in Review, 26, 204 217. Simpson, J.A., Collins, W.A., Tran, S., & Haydon, K.C. (2007). Attachment and the experience and expression of emotions in adult romantic relationships: A developmental perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 355 367. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.355 Sroufe, L.A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E.A., & Collins, W.A. (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Sroufe, L.A., Egeland, B., & Kreutzer, T. (1990). The fate of early experience following developmental change: Longitudinal approaches to individual adaptation in childhood. Child Development, 61, 1363 1373. doi:10.2307/1130748 Sroufe, L.A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the construction of relationships. In W.W. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.), Relationships in development (pp. 51 71). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Thompson, R. (2008). Early attachment and later development: Familiar questions, new answers. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (2nd ed., pp. 348 365). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Tran, S., & Simpson, J.A. (2009). Prorelationship maintenance behaviors: The joint roles of attachment and commitment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 685 698. doi:10.1037/ a0016418