Closing a gap to normal hearing

Similar documents
Oticon Opn S Clinical Evidence

What can pupillometry tell us about listening effort and fatigue? Ronan McGarrigle

OpenSound Navigator for Oticon Opn Custom Instruments

FRAMEWORK FOR UNDERSTANDING EFFORTFUL LISTENING

BREAK CONVENTION AND OPEN UP TO THE WORLD

Ear and Hearing Relations between self-reported daily-life fatigue, hearing status and pupil dilation during a speech perception in noise task

Oticon Agil success 2011 An International Survey

The open sound paradigm continues to expand

Are hearing aids the better rehabilitative choice when compared to PSAPs? On speech-intelligibility and soundquality,

The open sound paradigm continues to expand

Measuring listening-related effort and fatigue in young adults and children. Ronan McGarrigle

Research Article Processing Load Induced by Informational Masking Is Related to Linguistic Abilities

Open up to the world. A new paradigm in hearing care

Data-driven hearing care with time-stamped data-logging

Norms for the Reading Span Test: English Version

Pupillometry as a window to listening effort

LISTENING EFFORT IN ADVERSE LISTENING

Assessing cognitive spare capacity

The effect of wearing conventional and level-dependent hearing protectors on speech production in noise and quiet

The Opn Evolution. The remarkable journey of the development of Oticon Opn, and the groundbreaking results that prove life-changing benefits

Cognitive Spare Capacity as a Window on Hearing Aid Benefit

Improving Speech Understanding in Multiple-Speaker Noise. By Douglas L. Beck AuD, and Nicolas Le Goff PhD

Hearing Research 323 (2015) 81e90. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Hearing Research. journal homepage:

Lindsay De Souza M.Cl.Sc AUD Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

WIDEXPRESS THE WIDEX FITTING RATIONALE FOR EVOKE MARCH 2018 ISSUE NO. 38

Effects of Hearing Impairment and Hearing Aid Amplification on Listening Effort: A Systematic Review

Predictors of aided speech recognition, with and without frequency compression, in older adults.

Listening effort and fatigue What are we measuring? Dr Piers Dawes

The role of periodicity in the perception of masked speech with simulated and real cochlear implants

Hearing-loss-dependent personalization

Validation Studies. How well does this work??? Speech perception (e.g., Erber & Witt 1977) Early Development... History of the DSL Method

Evaluation of a Danish speech corpus for assessment of spatial unmasking

About Varibel. Varibel strives to improve the quality of life for everyone who comes in touch with our products.

Cognition, Spatial Sound & Noise Reduction: 2016

Aging and Hearing Loss: Why does it Matter?

Your Guide to Hearing

How to use mycontrol App 2.0. Rebecca Herbig, AuD

96% of users say they hear better with Oticon Opn. Experience the life-changing benefits of a game-changing hearing aid

Spatial unmasking in aided hearing-impaired listeners and the need for training

How to use mycontrol App 2.0. Rebecca Herbig, AuD

Single channel noise reduction in hearing aids

Take another look: Pupillometry and cognitive processing demands

Using Speech Recall in Hearing Aid Fitting and Outcome Evaluation Under Ecological Test Conditions

Roger TM at work. Focus on work rather than on hearing

Why directional microphone technology for young children?

ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION

Solutions for better hearing. audifon innovative, creative, inspired

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

SpeechZone 2. Author Tina Howard, Au.D., CCC-A, FAAA Senior Validation Specialist Unitron Favorite sound: wind chimes

Speech in Noise: Using Measurements in Fitting. Presenter: Lori Bunkholt Exclusive Networks Education and Training Starkey Hearing Technologies

Speech intelligibility in simulated acoustic conditions for normal hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

EFFECT OF DIRECTIONAL STRATEGY ON AUDIBILITY OF SOUNDS IN THE ENVIRONMENT. Charlotte Jespersen, MA Brent Kirkwood, PhD Jennifer Groth, MA

April 23, Roger Dynamic SoundField & Roger Focus. Can You Decipher This? The challenges of understanding

Giving Your Child s Developing Brain What It Needs

Prescribe hearing aids to:

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY Academic in Confidence data removed

Your personal hearing analysis

Make the world louder!

You and Your Student with a Hearing Impairment

Håkan Hua. Employees with aided hearing impairment the effects of noise on working ability, cognitive skills and perceived disturbance

Measuring Working Memory in Audiologic Evaluations. Disclaimers. Listeners with Hearing Loss 10/4/2017

Open The Door To. Of Better Hearing. A Short Guide To Better Hearing. People First.

Hearing device technology that s. making waves

08/06/2016. Welcome to Opn! Open up with Oticon Opn. What is the most difficult situation for hearing aid users?

Using QuickSIN Speech Material to Measure Acceptable Noise Level for Adults with Hearing Loss

The Nottingham eprints service makes this work by researchers of the University of Nottingham available open access under the following conditions.

The Effect of Analysis Methods and Input Signal Characteristics on Hearing Aid Measurements

It takes two. Why it s a good idea to wear a hearing aid in both ears.

Possibilities for the evaluation of the benefit of binaural algorithms using a binaural audio link

How the LIVELab helped us evaluate the benefits of SpeechPro with Spatial Awareness

Satisfaction Survey. Oticon Intiga gets it right with first-time users

CLASSROOM AMPLIFICATION: WHO CARES? AND WHY SHOULD WE? James Blair and Jeffery Larsen Utah State University ASHA, San Diego, 2011

Development of Realistic Stimuli for the Evaluation of Listening Effort using Auditory Evoked Potentials

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

WIDEXPRESS. no.30. Background

IMPROVING THE PATIENT EXPERIENCE IN NOISE: FAST-ACTING SINGLE-MICROPHONE NOISE REDUCTION

Music. listening with hearing aids

Oticon Product portfolio 2010

Roger TM. for Education Bridging the understanding gap

Preparing For Your Hearing Consultation. PREPARING for your visit

Putting the focus on conversations

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Psychoacoustics of hearing impairment

Use it or Lose It: The Importance Of Hearing Protection

Best Practice Protocols

Why? Speech in Noise + Hearing Aids = Problems in Noise. Recall: Two things we must do for hearing loss: Directional Mics & Digital Noise Reduction

Cochlear Implants and SSD: Initial Findings With Adults: Implications for Children

Role of F0 differences in source segregation

Phonak Field Study News

Fitting Decisions and their Impact on Hearing Aid User Benefit. Mallory Maine, AuD Audiologist, GN ReSound

Effects of Multi-Channel Compression on Speech Intelligibility at the Patients with Loudness-Recruitment

Telephone Follow-Ups New hearing Aids

Erin C. Schafer 1 Maggie Hill 1. University of North Texas Departments of Speech and Hearing Sciences 1 & Department Educational Psychology 2

Your New Life. starts now

HOW TO USE THE SHURE MXA910 CEILING ARRAY MICROPHONE FOR VOICE LIFT

DRAFT. 7 Steps to Better Communication. When a loved one has hearing loss. How does hearing loss affect communication?

Reclaim your office. Oticon case study with Sennheiser MB 660

Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Services: 2016/17 in review

Spatial processing in adults with hearing loss

Clinical fitting guide

Influence of acoustic complexity on spatial release from masking and lateralization

Transcription:

w h i t e pa p e r 218 Closing a gap to normal hearing New BrainHearing evidence on speech understanding and listening effort SUMMARY For people with hearing loss, understanding speech in noise is known to be exhausting. While processing speech in quiet may be effortless even for people with hearing loss, understanding speech in noise becomes increasingly exhausting as the noise increases. People with normal hearing, however, may likewise experience difficulties understanding speech in noise, with increased effort as the noise level rises. A recent study investigated the listening effort and speech understanding in normal-hearing listeners using the objective methodology of pupillometry. The study examined the point of giving up for the normal-hearing listeners. Giving up can occur when a person tries to understand speech but it requires so much effort that the result is not rewarding enough (such as unsuccessful speech understanding). This can happen when the acoustical scenario is too difficult, for example. In addition to providing useful knowledge about listening effort in a normal-hearing population, this study may be used in the future as a reference to studies that investigate listening effort in people with hearing loss. Josefine Juul Jensen, M.A. Clinical Research Audiologist Centre for Applied Audiology Research, Oticon A/S

PAGE 2 Introduction For many years, speech intelligibility has been used as benchmark in audiological research for measuring speech understanding (Keidser, 26). While speech intelligibility as a measurement certainly gives a great deal of information, it may not provide enough aspects of the cognitive process that goes behind making sense of speech in everyday communication. When communicating in noisy environments, more cognitive resources are engaged in order to focus on and recognise speech, ignore the noise, interpret the meaning, and remember the speech (Rönnberg et al., 213). In these situations, people may experience higher listening effort because of the increased need for cognitive resources. Effort and motivation According to Mattys et al. (212), effort may depend on the interaction of two factors: those imposed by the demands of the task, and listener-related factors. Task-related demands can be the type of noise, such as steady-state or multi-talker babble, or the acoustic environment in a given situation, for example whether it is easy or difficult. An example of an easy acoustic environment is when the speech is louder than the noise, and a difficult acoustic environment is when the noise is louder than the speech. Factors related to the listener can be an aspect such as the person s hearing status, for example whether they have normal hearing or hearing loss. Keeping these factors in mind, it is necessary to include the factor of motivation. Motivational intensity theory (Brehm & Self, 1989) describes motivation in relation to pursuing a goal. The theory explains that people conserve their resources by only investing resources in tasks where the goal can be pursued successfully. When task conditions become too difficult, people will at some point just give up. That is, when a person realises that they are not getting enough success out of solving a task compared to how many resources they are investing, they will discontinue allocating mental resources to solving the task (Pichora-Fuller et al., 216). We can interpret this situation as the tipping point or the give up point. Pupillometry Pupillometry is a useful and objective measurement for measuring the effort as well as when the point of giving up occurs (e.g. Beatty, 1982; Zekveld et al., 21; 211; Wendt et al., 217; Ohlenforst et al., 217). In this method, the pupil dilation is continuously recorded (Kramer et al., 213, as cited in Pichora-Fuller et al, 216). Pupil dilation has previously been shown to quantify effort (please see Opn Clinical Evidence white paper, Le Goff et al., 216). Pupillometry can measure effort because the pupil dilation is partly connected with areas of the brain that govern the fight or flight response (McCorry, 27). Essentially, when a person needs to put in effort to solve a task, the sympathetic nervous system, which is known as the system for fight or flight responses, triggers physiological changes in the body such as pupil dilation. Thus, when presented with speech in noise, the person either invests resources to fight the noise, or give up trying to process the speech in noise. A previous study investigated the effects of the acoustic environment on the pupil dilation in a group with hearing impairment (Ohlenforst et al., 217). The study found that the pupil dilation changes as a function of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) in an inverse U shape curve. In other words, when going from very difficult SNRs to easy SNRs, the peak pupil dilation increases until a certain point, and then decreases again. The study showed that people with hearing loss give up trying to process speech at sound environments of around -1 db SNR (Ohlenforst et al., 217). This means that many opportunities for socialising are sacrificed, because situations like conversing in a restaurant has an SNR of -5 db or even poorer. However, the same study also found that the OpenSound Navigator (OSN) can facilitate moving the point of giving up to situations with lower SNRs, meaning that OSN enables people wearing hearing aids to actively participate in more social situations (see white paper Pushing the Noise Limit, Le Goff & Beck, 217). Results from another pupillometry study have shown that the OpenSound Navigator can reduce the listening effort for people with hearing loss even in environments where the level of noise is relatively low (see Opn Clinical Evidence white paper, Le Goff et al., 216; Wendt et al., 217). Thus, a collection of studies using pupillometry is available to provide evidence that OSN in Opn hearing aids reduces the listening effort significantly, making communication less exhausting for those with hearing loss, and moves the give up point for understanding speech in noise, enabling them to participate actively in even more social situations.

PAGE 3 This evidence would be much easier to relate to if there was a benchmark to compare with. In research that involves hearing loss, normal hearing is the toughest benchmark possible, and the ideal to compare hearing aid benefits with. A recent study investigated the listening effort and speech understanding in a normal-hearing population. With this new study, we can investigate the gap between normal-hearing listeners and listeners with hearing loss in terms of how much effort these two groups use for speech understanding in noisy environments. Pupillometry study on normal-hearing listeners Method The study involved 29 participants with normal hearing for their age group in the age range from 5 to 77 years (mean 65.7 years). The participants performed the Danish Hearing in Noise Test (HINT, Nielsen & Dau, 211), in which everyday sentences are presented in noise. The babble noise consisted of four competing talkers (two males, two females), where each talker was presented from a loudspeaker (see figure 1). The participants were told to listen to and repeat each sentence, while an eye-tracking camera continuously recorded their pupil response. The speech and noise stimuli were presented in a spatial set up, which is visualised in figure 1. For both types of noise stimuli, the sentences were presented at eight different SNRs. The eight SNRs were randomised, and ranged in ascending order from -2, -16, -12, -8, -4,, 4, to 8 db +15-15 SPL. Thus, data on speech understanding and listening effort was obtained simultaneously from a total of 16 conditions; in the results, only data from the babble noise will be presented. There were two outcome measures in the experiment: speech understanding, measured by word recognition in percent, and pupil response, measured by peak pupil dilation. Three practice trials were performed prior to the experiment, consisting of 3x2 sentences. Analysis and Results The analysis of the pupil data is based on a study by Wendt and colleagues (217) (see also previous white papers, e.g. Opn Clinical Evidence, Le Goff et al., 216, and Tinnitus and Pupillometry, Juul Jensen, 217). For each participant and condition, peak pupil dilation (PPD) was calculated (see Figure 2, which visualises normalised pupil dilation over time). Data processing was carried out in the following way: data was measured for 25 trials in each condition. For each condition, the first three trials were removed in order to remove training effects from the beginning of the condition. For the remaining trials, a baseline correction was performed by subtracting a baseline value that was estimated by the mean pupil size within one second previous to the onset of the sentence. By correcting to baseline, pupil artifacts related to aspects like nervousness and excitement were controlled for. Furthermore, pupil data consisting of greater than 2% of blinks, eye movements, or missing data were excluded from further analyses. A linear interpolation and a smoothing filter were passed over the remaining trials, thus removing eye blinks and high frequency artifacts. The mean and standard deviation of the pupil dilation was calculated from the noise onset to the noise offset. The total time of pupil dilation was thus from 1 to 7 seconds, in which the sentence onset was at 3 seconds. +9-9 Figure 1 Spatial setup, with speech presented from the front ( ), and noise from loudspeakers to the sides and the back (+/- 9 and +/- 15 ). Participants were seated in the middle, with an eye-tracking camera in front of them that continuously recorded their pupil dilation. The distance from the participant to the loudspeaker(s) was 1.2 m., and from the participant to the camera was 6 cm. Normalised pupil dilation.15.1.5 sentence presentation Peak pupil dilation (PPD).5 noise baseline presentation.1 2 1 1 2 3 4 Time (s) Figure 2 Normalised pupil dilation (mm.) over time (s.), indicating the pupil baseline, the peak pupil dilation, and the time onset for noise and sentence presentation 1 ISO 729-217

PAGE 4 When testing the pupil response and speech intelligibility in a variety of SNRs, the typical result would be something like the graph shown in figure 3 (random SNRs are chosen for the example). That is, the PPD as a function of SNR presents itself in an inverted-u shape, with the maximum PPD around the middle and the gradual decrease of PPD on each side of the maximum. The maximum PPD indicates the acoustical environment in which listeners spend the most effort. The bottom part of the figure shows a typical psychometric function of speech intelligibility in different SNRs, ranging from % to 1%. The inverted-u and -S shapes shown in figure 3 are characteristic shapes from these two measurements. In the results section, specific data points from the present study that are of particular interest are shown. PPD (normalized to baseline) 1,5 1,5-8 -4 SNR (db SPL) NH HI OSN On HI OSN Off NH,25,2 1 9 HI OSN On HI OSN Off PPD (mm.) Word recogni on (%),15,1,5, 1 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1-2 -15-1 -5 5 1 15-2 -15-1 -5 5 1 15 db SNR Figure 3 Examples of PPD (top) and speech intelligibility (bottom) as a function of SNR SNR (db SPL) Selected results from the condition with babble noise with normal-hearing listeners and hearing impaired listeners from the previous study are presented in figure 4. Roughly put, the higher the PPD, the higher the effort, and the higher the point on the word recognition scale, the better speech understanding. Speech Understanding (%) 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1-8 -4 SNR (db SPL) Figure 4 Condition with babble noise. The top figure shows the average peak pupil dilation for -8 and -4 db SNR. The bottom figure shows average scores for word recognition in percent, indicating speech understanding in -8 and -4 db SNR. The results from the normal-hearing listeners show that the word recognition is at 4% at -8 db SNR, and increases to approximately 7% at -4 db SNR. The pupil dilation data showed a maximum PPD at -4 db SNR, indicating the maximum allocation of effort of all the conditions. Going from -4 to -8 db SNR (a harder acoustical condition), the PPD decreases.

PAGE 5 Interpretation This study investigated the speech understanding and listening effort in persons with normal hearing. There were two aims with the study: to examine when these listeners reach the give up-point of allocating effort, meaning when the listeners start to give up trying to make sense of the speech, and to compare this with a similar study that used listeners with hearing loss. It was found that the normal-hearing listeners expended the most amount of effort around 7% word recognition score (-4 db SNR, where the PPD was at the maximum). After this point, data suggested that the listeners started to give up putting in the effort (around -8 db SNR), because it was seen that the PPD began to decrease together with a decrease in word recognition below 5%. Figure 5 visualises what the difference of OSN can mean for hearing impaired listeners: with OSN, the amount of effort allocated to a task is similar to that for normal-hearing listeners. Without OSN, the hearing impaired listeners will give up much quicker than the normal-hearing listeners (for more details, see Le Goff & Beck, 217). PPD (normalized to baseline) 1,5 1,5 Figure 5 Visualization of the difference for hearing impaired listeners when OpenSound Navigator is activated in their hearing aids. High PPDs indicate high processing effort a positive sign of continuous task engagement! Give up without OSN NH HI OSN On HI OSN Off -8-4 SNR (db SPL) }High processing effort = task engagement! Listening effort in normal-hearing listeners compared to Opn users Results from this study alone are interesting, but they become even more exciting when compared to the results from a similar study with the same experimental setup of people with hearing loss (see white paper Le Goff & Beck, 217). Comparing the two studies, which used similar methodology but different populations (normal-hearing listeners and listeners with hearing loss, respectively, who were age-matched), the collective evidence shows that the point of giving up for persons with hearing loss with OpenSound Navigator activated is the same point of giving up as for persons with normal hearing. This is visualised in figure 6. Hearing loss with amplification Hearing loss with OpenSound Navigator Normal hearing db SNR 15 1 5-5 Quiet environments Home Family dinner Restaurant Noisy environments Figure 6 Comparison of the giving up point from studies using listeners with hearing loss using Opn and normal- hearing listeners. Environments that correspond to different signal-to-noise ratios are visualised.

PAGE 6 Short discussion of closing a gap Based on the normal hearing data, we can establish a give-up point of roughly -8 db SNR. This is similar to the give-up point observed in the study with OpenSound Navigator, as also indicated in figure 6 above. One of the barriers that people with hearing loss constantly face is that they avoid going to social situations where listening becomes too difficult so difficult that they are no longer willing to invest effort in following conversations. Eventually, they give up and thus withdraw from these situations, which they could have otherwise enjoyed. As previously mentioned, normal hearing is the toughest benchmark available, and these two studies together show that OpenSound Navigator breaks the barrier in listening scenarios and empowers users to participate in the same social situations as their normal-hearing peers. In other words, OpenSound Navigator is closing a gap to normal hearing, both in terms of speech of understand, and in terms of listening effort. This is important because it is possible for people with hearing loss using Opn hearing aids to have active communication in difficult listening situations. Therefore, clinicians can encourage people using Opn hearing aids to explore listening and social situations they may have avoided or given up on in the past.

PAGE 7 References Aston-Jones, G., Cohen, J. D., 25. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience 28, 43 45 Beatty, J., 1982. Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin 91, 276 292. Juul Jensen, J. 217. Benefits of Oticon Opn for people with hearing loss and tinnitus. Oticon White Paper. Keidser, G. 216. Towards Ecologically Valid Protocols for the Assessment of Hearing and Hearing Devices. JAAA Kramer, S.E., Lorens, A., Coninx, F., et al. 213. Processing load during listening: The influence of task characteristic on the pupil response. Journal of Language Processing, 28, 426-442 Le Goff, N., Ng, E., Wendt, D. & Lunner, T. 216. Opn Clinical Evidence. Oticon White Paper Le Goff, N. & Beck, D. 217. Pushing the noise limit. Oticon White Paper. Mattys, S.L., David, MH., Bradlow, A.R. & Scott, S.K. (212). Speech recognition in adverse conditions: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes. Vol. 27, pp. 953-97 McCorry, L.K. (27). Physiology of the Autonomic Nervous System. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, Vol. 71(4) Nielsen, J. B., Dau, T., 211. The Danish hearing in noise test. International Journal of Audiology 5, 22 28. Ohlenforst, B., Wendt, D., Lunner, T., Zekveld, A.A., Naylor, G., Wang, Y., Kramer, S.E., (217a), Impact of SNR, masker type and noise reduction on processing effort as indicated by the pupil dilation, CHScom, Linkoping Pichora-Fuller, M.K., Kramer, S.E., Eckert, M.A., Edwards, B., Hornsby, B.W., Humes, L.E., Lemke, U., Lunner, T., Matthen, M., Mackersie, C.L., 216. Hearing impairment and cognitive energy: The framework for understanding effortful listening (FUEL). Ear and hearing 37, 5S-27S. Rönnberg, J., Lunner, T., Zekveld, A.A., Sörqvist, P., Danielsson, H., Lyxell, B., Hahlström, Ö., Signoret, C., Stenfelt, S., Pichora-Fuller, K.M., Rudner, M., Rudner, M., 213. The Ease of Language Understanding (ELU) model: theoretical, empirical, and clinical advances. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 1 17 Wendt, D., Hietkamp, R.K. & Lunner, T. (217). Impact of noise and noise reduction on processing effort: A pupillometry study. Ear & Hearing Wendt, D., Koelewijn, T., Książek, P., Kramer, S.E. & Lunner, T. Toward a more comprehensive understanding of the impact of masker type and signal-to-noise ratio on the pupillary response while performing a speech-in-noise test. Submitted for review. Zekveld, A., Kramer, S., Festen, J., 21. Pupil Response as an Indication of Effortful Listening: The Influence of Sentence Intelligibility. Ear and hearing. Zekveld, A. A., Kramer, S. E., Festen, J. M., 211. Cognitive load during speech perception in noise: The influence of age, hearing loss, and cognition on the pupil response. Ear and hearing 32, 498 51.

oticon.global 29497UK / 218.4.16