INTRODUCTORY REMARKS. WILLlAM BUSKIST Auburn University

Similar documents
THE REFERENTIAL NATURE OF RULES AND INSTRUCTIONS: A RESPONSE TO INSTRUCTIONS, RULES, AND ABSTRACTION: A MISCONSTRUED RELATION BY EMILIO RIBES-IÑESTA

Behavioral variability and rule generation: general restricted, and superstitious contingency statements

INTERACTIONS OF CONTINGENCIES, INSTRUCTIONAL ACCURACY, AND INSTRUCTIONAL HISTORY IN CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION

Conditioning The Behavior of the Listener. Conditioning the Behavior of the Listener

UNIVERSITY OF MANCHESTER. den, 1988) postulates that nonverbal behavior. can change under the direct control of reinforcement

The Relationship Between Motivating Operations & Behavioral Variability Penn State Autism Conference 8/3/2016

The Persistence-Strengthening Effects of DRA: An Illustration of Bidirectional Translational Research

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

The Creative Porpoise Revisited

J-01 State intervention goals in observable and measureable terms.

RECENT REINFORCEMENT-SCHEDULE RESEARCH AND APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS KENNON A. LATTAL AND NANCY A. NEEF

ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND

1 UNIVERSITY OF KANSAS 2 WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

Mental operations on number symbols by-children*

A Developmental-Behavioral Analysis of Lying

The Role of Joint Control in Teaching Complex Behavior to Children with Autism

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

Stimulus Control And Generalization Of Point- Loss Punishment With Humans

School of Nursing, University of British Columbia Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

The Quantitative/Qualitative Debate A False Dichotomy?

Mitch J. Fryling and Linda J. Hayes

Variability as an Operant?

Running Head: VISUAL SCHEDULES FOR STUDENTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013)

Five Misconceptions of Behavior Analysis 1) It ignores consciousness, feelings, and states of mind. 2) It neglects innate endowment and argues all

A PROCEDURE TO TEACH SELF-CONTROL TO CHILDREN WITH ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER LISA M. BINDER AND MARK R. DIXON PATRICK M.

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

Comments on Towards a new behaviorism by Peter Harzem

Behavior modification

. R EAS O ~ I NG BY NONHUMAN

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

Seminar: Skinner s Analysis of Verbal Behavior PRIVATE EVENTS

Temporalization In Causal Modeling. Jonathan Livengood & Karen Zwier TaCitS 06/07/2017

Measuring and Assessing Study Quality

HUMAN SIMULATIONS IN BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS ( ): FACILITATING RESEARCH IN COMPLEX HUMAN BEHAVIOR

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

Differential Item Functioning

The Concept of Automatic Reinforcement: Implications for Assessment and Intervention

A COMPARISON OF POINTS VERSUS SOUNDS AS REINFORCERS IN HUMAN OPERANT RESEARCH THESIS. Presented to the Graduate Council of the

Applied Behavior Analysis

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010.

Establishing Operations and the Mand

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms

Chapter 1: Behaviorism and the beginning of cognitive science

DESCRIPTIVE VERSUS FUNCTIONAL ACCOUNTS OF PSYCHOLOGICAL EVENTS: CYBERRAT AS A CASE IN POINT

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

VICARIOUS REINFORCEMENT PROCEDURES: AN ANALYSIS OF STIMULUS CONTROL AND POTENTIAL SIDE EFFECTS DANIELLE L. GUREGHIAN

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

A behaviour analysis of Theory of Mind: from interpretation to application

Emotional functioning in Systems Jim Edd Jones Lafayette, CO

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

Put simply. The ideas that influenced ACT. In a nutshell. Situating ACT in the cognitive behavioural tradition. ACT & CBT: many points of convergence

Further Properties of the Priority Rule

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

A Search for Derived Stimulus Relations in Rats

Dikran J. Martin Psychology 111

SIDE EFFECTS OF EXTINCTION: PREVALENCE OF BURSTING AND AGGRESSION DURING THE TREATMENT OF SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR DOROTHEA C.

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT, A DISTINCTION THAT IS NO LONGER NECESSARY; OR A BETTER WAY TO TALK ABOUT BAD THINGS 1

HOW CLOSE TO REAL CAN A NON-REAL CYBERRAT BEHAVE?

METHODOLOGY FOR DISSERTATION

Behaviorists and Behavior Therapy. Historical Background and Key Figures

Group Behavior By Michael Stahl

Recent work in applied behavior analysis has begun to examine the variables that influence

Teaching Language to Young Children. Functional Analysis and Intervention

Emergence of Equivalence Relations: Comparing Sorting and Match-to-Sample Procedures. Lindsay J. Grimm. The New England Center for Children

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

Rule-governed behavior

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics February 2008, Volume 10, Number 2:

USING A SELF-CONTROL TRAINING PROCEDURE TO INCREASE APPROPRIATE BEHAVIOR MARK R. DIXON AND LINDA J. HAYES

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY

Pausing time allocation and constraint on output

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

The Self and Responding to the Own s Behavior. Implications of Coherence and Hierarchical Framing *

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

Editorial. From the Editors: Perspectives on Turnaround Time

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

Human Schedule Performance with Hypothetical Monetary Reinforcement

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M.

II. The Behavioral Approach to Understanding Cognition

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Principal Features. "First, the term learning does not apply to (168)

Protocol analysis and Verbal Reports on Thinking

A History of the Behavioral Therapies 2002

Behaviorism: An essential survival tool for practitioners in autism

12/19/2016. Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview. What is the Autism Spectrum? Autism Spectrum Disorder

Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview

PECS and Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Andy Bondy, Ph.D. Lori Frost, MS, CCC-SLP

Improving Education and Treatment in Autism with Organizational Behavior Management 2009 National Autism Conference, August 3, 2009

CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION AND STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE: EFFECTS OF SUPPRESSING DERIVED SYMMETRICAL RESPONSES ON THE EMERGENCE OF TRANSITIVITY

What is Science 2009 What is science?

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Transcription:

5 INTRODUCTORY REMARKS WILLlAM BUSKIST Auburn University During the late 1950s and early 1960s a small group of behaviorists began investigating a peculiar dependent variable, at least for operant researchers at that time-human behavior (e.g., Azrin, 1958; Laties & Weiss, 1960; Lindsley, 1956). A few of these researchers were interested in behavior that might be considered uniquely human, such as certain verbal and social behavior, but the majority preferred to work with human performance on basic reinforcement schedules, a strategy sired from interest in interspecies generality. Although there have been several researchers who dared forays into other areas, such as cooperation (e.g., Hake & Vukelich, 1973; Schmitt & Marwell, 1968), sharing (e.g., Hake, Vukelich, & Olvera, 1975); verbal behavior (e.g., Rosenfeld & Baer, 1970); multiresponse settings (e.g., Bernstein & Ebbesen, 1978), and aging (Baron, Menich, & Perone, 1983) to name a few, the tradition of researching human performance under schedules of reinforcement has continued to the present (e.g., Lowe, Beasty, & Bentall, 1983; Poppen, 1981; Ruddle, Bradshaw, Szabadi, & Foster, 1982). Interspecies Generality and Basic Schedule Work While the investigation of human performance under different schedules of reinforcement might be described by persons outside the field as tedious and probably irrelevant to human behavior in ordinary life, this type of work has proven to be of significant import. The dozens of systematic replications involving human performance on reinforcement schedules have consistently shown that human behavior, in the majority of cases and regardless of the schedule in effect, is different, in varying degrees, than the behavior of rats and pigeons on similar schedules (for a review, see Lowe, 1979; Weiner, 1983). These performance differences have been used by some to point to the weaknesses of operant research involving nonhuman animal subjects: (1) It is not generalizable to the human population; (2) human behavior is not amenable to experimental analysis, and (3) operant psychology simply does not do what its proponents said it could do (see e.g., Brewer, 1974; Spielberger & DeNike, 1966). At first blush, these conclusions appear justified: There is an undeniable difference in the performance of human and nonhuman subjects under similar experimental conditions; operant research with non humans produces consistent and clear-cut results both between subjects and between replications while similar research with humans has produced large intersubject differences both within and across experimental conditions. Recent research, however, has shown that human behavior is affected by the same classes of things that affect non human animal behavior, namely, antecedent and consequent stimuli, but that specific types and arrangements of stimuli may be different. While nonhuman animal behavior is considered to be primarily shaped and controlled by experimental contingencies, human behavior appears to be a blend of both experience with experimental con-

6 BUSKIST tingencies and any experimenter-provided and/or subject-produced verbal description (rules or instructions) of those contingencies. Experiments by Catania, Matthews, and Shimoff (Catania, Matthews, & Shimoff, 1982; Matthews, Shimoff, Catania, & Sagvolden, 1977; Shimoff, Catania, & Matthews, 1981) as well as by Harzem, Lowe, and Bagshaw (1978) have shown that human performance under highly controlled conditions is affected by both the level and quantity of instructions provided by the experimenter and by a subject's own verbal behavior regarding experimental tasks. It is thus not the case that human behavior is not subject to environmental control. Rather, the controlling variables are different, both in themselves and in interaction with other variables. An Updated Census of the EAHB Literature In order to sketch the development of the experimental analysis of human behavior (EAHB) as well as to provide the backdrop necessary to appreciate the papers presented in this symposium, I wish to address four questions briefly. 1. The first question is elementary: What is the experimental analysis of human behavior? EAHB is simply the application of behavior-analytic theory, methodology, and philosophy to the study of human behavior. Human behavior is the dependent variable. As Jim Johnston (1983) has put it, "any and all behavioral phenomena or relations qualify for experimental attention, regardless of their origins, nature, mechanisms or traditional research context" (p. 2). The accent is on human behavior and not that of other animals for at least three reasons. First, nonhuman animal work has focused increasingly on conditioning to the exclusion of other significant variables. Second, and as Hake (1982) has pointed out, there may be cases such as the analysis of verbal behavior and certain social behavior in which humans are the only appropriate subjects to use. The acid test of our science is, of course, whether or not our theories and procedures work with the real McCoy. Third, operant psychology, despite passing itself off as the "science of human behavior" has concentrated primarily on the investigation of the behavior of nonhumans. Partly as a reaction to these conditions and partly because of serious interest in human behavior itself, both laboratory scientists and others have begun to encourage the development of EAHB and to refine its methodology. All this is not to say that non human animal work is not important or useful. We do not indeed need less nonhuman animal research; we do, however, need proportionately more human work. Contributions to knowledge about the variables that control behavior in general will come from the joint efforts of both types of research, but knowledge about the special circumstances under which much human behavior takes place will only come about through intensified laboratory research programs involving human subjects. 2. The second question is a quantitive one: How much human operant research has been conducted? I counted the number of articles involving human subjects published in JEAB from 1958 through November 1985 and found that they (199 studies) comprise roughly 10% of the total research-oriented articles that JEAB has published (cf. Buskist & Miller, 1982). The figure over the last 5 years is a bit more cheerful: 17.5%. Granted, these figures may not give us an accurate picture of the actual amount of EAHB research that has been conducted as many authors may have

EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF HUMAN BEHAVIOR SYMPOSIUM 7 had their research rejected by JEAB and either did not resubmit their report or submitted it elsewhere. However, in a survey of several other behavioral journals conducted by myself and Hal Miller in 1982, we found that JEAB was the primary publication outlet for EAHB research. A perusal of the same journals today would no doubt yield a similar finding. 3. What kinds of human subjects have been employed typically in EAHB research? Surveying EAHB research published in JEAB from 1958 through November 1985, I found the following: normal adults (university and college undergraduates), 56%; normal children, 23%; retarded adults, 10%; and retarded children, 11 %. These figures have changed moderately during the past 5 years: normal adults down to 51%; normal children, up to 37%; and both retarded adults and children down to 5 and 7%, respectively. 4. What types of human behavior have been studied most frequently? To answer this question, I inventoried the various types of EAHB reports published in JEAB and came up with several general categories. In descending order of popularity they are: schedule performance and reinforcement effects, stimulus control, aversive control of behavior, social behavior and verbal behavior. Thus with the exception of the social and verbal behavior studies, which comprise only 11 % of the total EAHB articles published in JEAB, EAHB research has followed, quite justifiably, traditional lines of nonhuman animal research. This trend is changing though, as 40% of the EAHB articles published in JEAB since January 1981 have involved social and verbal behavior. In summary, the percentage of EAHB research published in JEAB over the last five years is nearly double that published during the period 1958-1980. Although the most popular subject for research is the normal adult, the use of normal children appears to be increasing rapidly, presumably, because researchers interested in language development and complex stimulus control such as that involved in the formation of equivalence classes have found children particularly suitable subjects (see Lowenkron, 1983). Similarly, whereas basic schedule work (i.e., examination of performance solely as a function of schedule parameters) with humans is still popular, the application of reinforcement schedules as a tool to study social and verbal behavior is also increasing. Each of our presenters has contributed to the progress currently being made in the experimental analysis of human behavior. Mark Galizio discusses the importance of rigorous experimental control in a science of human behavior, with specific emphasis in the area of rule-governed control, in our first paper. In the second paper, authored by Barbara Etzel, the history and current role of children as experimental subjects in behavioral research is presented and compared to the role that nonhuman-animals play in such research. The relation between the experimental analysis of human behavior and applied behavior analysis is considered in the third paper, written by Sam Deitz. The final paper authored by Aaron Brownstein and Rick Shull presents the case for the study of complex human functioning. Jack Michael has provided his comments as discussant.

8 BUSKIST References AZRIN, N. H. (1958). Some effects of noise on human behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 183-200. BARON, A, MENICH, S. R., & PERONE, M. (1983). Reaction times of older and younger men and temporal contingencies of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 275-287. BERNSTEIN, D. J., & EBBESEN, E. B. (1978). Reinforcement and substitution in humans: A multi-response analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 30, 243-253. BREWER, W. F. (1974). There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. In W. B. Weimer & D. S. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. BUSKIST, W. F., & MILLER, H. L. (1982). The analysis of human operant behavior: A brief census of the literature, 1958-1981. The Behavior Analyst, 5, 137-141. CATANIA, A C., MATTHEWS, B. A., & SHIMOFF, E. (1982). Instructed versus shaped behavior: Interactions with nonverbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 233-248. HAKE, D. F. (1982). The basic-applied continuum and the possible evolution of human operant social and verbal research. The Behavior Analyst, 5, 21-28. HAKE, D. F., & VUKELlCH, R. (1973). Analysis of the control exerted by a complex control procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19, 3-16. HAKE, D. F., VUKELlCH, R., & OLVERA, D. (1975). The measurement of sharing and cooperation as equity effects and some relationships between them. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 63-79. HARZEM, P., LOWE, C. F., & BAGSHAW, M. (1978). Verbal control in human operant behavior. The Psychological Record, 28, 405-423. JOHNSTON, J. M. (1983). What is the experimental analysis of human behavior? In W. F. Buskist (Chair), Conceptual issues in the experimental analysis of behavior. Symposium presented at the meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Milwaukee, WI. LATIES, V. G., & WEISS, B. (1960). Human observing after signal detection. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3, 27-33. LlNDSLEY, O. R. (1956). Operant conditioning methods applied to research in chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatric Research Reports, 5, 118-139. LOWE, C. F. (1979). Determinants of human operant behavior. In M. D. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.), Reinforcement and the organization of behavior (Vol. 1). New York: Wiley. LOWE, C. F., BEASTY, A., & BENTALL, R. P. (1983). The role of verbal behavior in human learning: Infant performances on fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 39, 157-164. LOWENKRON, B. (1983). Children are human too. The Behavior Analyst, 6, 191-192. MATTHEWS, B. A, SHIMOFF, E., CATANIA, A C., & SAGVOLDEN, T. (1977). Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity to ratio and interval contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 453-467. POPPEN, R. (1981). Human fixed-interval performance with concurrently programmed schedules: A parametric analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 251-266. ROSENFELD, H. M., & BAER, D. M. (1970). Unbiased and unnoticed verbal conditioning: The double agent robot procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14, 99-107. RUDDLE, H. V., BRADSHAW, C. M., SZABADI, E., & FOSTER, T. M. (1982). Performance of humans in concurrent avoidance/positive-reinforcement schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 51-61. SCHMITT, D. R., & MARWELL, G. (1969). Stimulus control in the experimental study of cooperation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 571-574. SHIMOFF, E., CATANIA, A C., & MATTHEWS, B. A (1981). Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity of low-rate performance to schedule contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 207-220.

EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF HUMAN BEHAVIOR SYMPOSIUM 9 SHIMOFF, E., & MATTHEWS, B. A. (1977). Unequal reinforcer magnitudes and relative preference for cooperation in the dyad. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of 8ehavior, 24, 1-16. SPIELBERGER, C. D., & DeNIKE, L. D. (1966). Descriptive behaviorism versus cognitive theory in verbal conditioning experiments. Psychological Review, 73, 306-326. WEINER, H. (1983). Some thoughts on discrepant human-animal performances under schedules of reinforcement. The Psychological Record, 33, 521-532.