AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

Similar documents
Distracted Driving. Stephanie Bonne, MD

Cell-Phone Induced Driver Distraction David L. Strayer and Frank A. Drews

PREVENTING DISTRACTED DRIVING. Maintaining Focus Behind the Wheel of a School Bus

Jessica Cook, Emily Gothberg, Rose Richardson, and Reagan Styles Supervisor: Dr. Cecile Nowatka, Ph.D.

Cell Phones and Driving 1. Why Do Cell Phone Conversations Interfere With Driving?

Distracted Driving among Teens. What We Know about It and How to Prevent It May 31 st, 2017

Distracted Driving Effects on CMV Operators

AUDITORY AND VISUAL REACTION TIME IN YOUNG ADULTS WITH CONCOMITANT USE OF CELL PHONES

Southern Legislative Conference: Why Focus on Distraction in Transportation? Chairman Deborah A. P. Hersman

THE EFFECT OF VOICE INTERACTIONS ON DRIVERS GUIDANCE OF ATTENTION

7/2/ Cell Phone Induced Failures of Visual Awareness. During Simulated Driving. David L. Strayer, Frank A. Drews, and William A.

The Misjudgment of Risk due to Inattention

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

In-Vehicle Communication and Driving: An Attempt to Overcome their Interference

FACTORS CONTRIBUTING TO SELF-REPORTED CELL PHONE USAGE BY YOUNGER DRIVERS IN THE PACIFIC NORTHWEST

Verbal Collision Avoidance Messages of Varying Perceived Urgency Reduce Crashes in High Risk Scenarios

Driver Distraction: Towards A Working Definition

A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD

Estimation of Driver Inattention to Forward Objects Using Facial Direction with Application to Forward Collision Avoidance Systems

Crash Risk of Alcohol Impaired Driving

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

DISTRACTION AN ACADEMIC PERSPECTIVE. Steve Reed Loughborough Design School

Multitasking: Why Your Brain Can t Do It and What You Should Do About It.

Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

Using the method of adjustment to enhance collision warning perception

Heimildaskrá. Baddeley, A. D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R. H., & Spinnler, H. (1991). The

SCA Functional Index Case Report Form

Undiagnosed ADHD Among Unionized Drivers in Ghana: Public Health and Policy Implications

The Effects of Age and Distraction on Reaction Time in a Driving Simulator

BOOK YOUR Date BefORe It s too Late!

Key Points For Physicians To Review With Adolescents and Adults Who Have ADHD

2015 LOUISIANA NIGHTTIME ADULT SEAT BELT OBSERVATION SURVEY RESULTS

Iran. T. Allahyari, J. Environ. et Health. al., USEFUL Sci. Eng., FIELD 2007, OF Vol. VIEW 4, No. AND 2, RISK pp OF... processing system, i.e

Research Review: Multiple Resource Theory. out in multi-task environments. Specifically, multiple display layout and control design

I just didn t see it It s all about hazard perception. Dr. Robert B. Isler Associate Professor School of Psychology University of Waikato Hamilton

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

To the Point of Distraction

Effects of Driver and Secondary Task Characteristics on Lane Change Test Performance

Driver Behaviour Issues relevant to Temporary Traffic Management solutions

Visual & Auditory Skills Lab

Commercial Vehicle Drivers Hours of Service Module 1 Overview

Expert Focus Group on Driver Distraction:

Protecting Workers with Smart E-Vests

Distraction, Cognition, Behaviour and Driving Analysis of a large data set

Potential Benefits of a Concurrent Verbal Task when Feeling Fatigued Due to Monotonous Driving Conditions

II: ALCOHOL - RELATED CRASHES

Selective Attention (dichotic listening)

95% of all injuries are behavioral 05% of all injuries are mechanical

Chapter 3: Information Processing

The Peripheral Detection Task and Related Methods: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

The Whole Child Approach of the Social Security Administration:

CRACIUN RESEARCH. Alaska Injury Prevention Center CHA. September 7, 2014

36 th International Traffic Records Forum New Orleans, Louisiana. Ron Beck Cristian Oros Missouri State Highway Patrol

2017 LOUISIANA NIGHTTIME ADULT SEAT BELT OBSERVATION SURVEY RESULTS

Validity of Haptic Cues and Its Effect on Priming Visual Spatial Attention

The Effectiveness of Drinking-and-Driving Policies in the American States: A Cross-Sectional Time Series Analysis for

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

Effects of practice, age, and task demands, on interference from a phone task while driving

Mitigating cognitive distraction and its effects with interface design and collision avoidance systems

Cell-phone use diminishes self-awareness of impaired driving

Distracted Driving: Review of Current Needs, Efforts and Recommended Strategies OVERVIEW

THE EFFECT OF COGNITIVE TASKS ON PREDICTING EVENTS IN TRAFFIC

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

ADHD and road accident risk: Results from meta-analysis

Evaluating the Safety of Verbal Interface Use while Driving

Multimodal Driver Displays: Potential and Limitations. Ioannis Politis

Public Awareness and Attitudes Toward Traffic Safety Issues in Texas: 2011 Survey Results

Alcohol-impaired driving in the lab: Efforts to reduce the incidence of drinking and driving

THE EFFECTS OF MOMENTARY VISUAL DISRUPTION ON HAZARD ANTICIPATION IN DRIVING. Massachusetts, USA

EFFECTS OF DISTRACTION TYPE, DRIVER AGE, AND ROADWAY ENVIRONMENT ON REACTION TIMES AN ANALYSIS USING SHRP-2 NDS DATA

BOOK YOUR Date BefORe It s too Late!

ASSESSMENT OF DRIVING-RELATED SKILL (ADReS)

ROAD SAFETY MONITOR. ALCOHOL-IMPAIRED DRIVING IN THE UNITED STATES Results from the 2018 TIRF USA Road Safety Monitor

Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI?

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

A critical analysis of an applied psychology journal about the effects of driving fatigue: Driver fatigue and highway driving: A simulator study.

The Accuracy of Drivers Judgments of the Effects of Headlight Glare: Are We Really Blinded by the Light?

Multi-tasking. Vera Demberg. Language Processing and Aging. Nov 13th, 2014

Subject: The Case for a Provincial 0.00% BAC Limit for All Drivers Under the Age of 21

EPILEPSY AND DRIVING- A POSITION PAPER OF EPILEPSY SOUTH AFRICA DEVELOPED IN MARCH 2016

Review of Pre-crash Behaviour in Fatal Road Collisions Report 1: Alcohol

Development and Analysis of a Drug and Alcohol Driving Awareness Program

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions.

DRINKING AND DRIVING. Alcohol consumption, even in relatively small quantities, increases the risk of road crashes.

Cognitive and Psychomotor Correlates of Self- Reported Driving Skills and Behavior

Koji Sakai. Kyoto Koka Women s University, Ukyo-ku Kyoto, Japan

Did You See the Unicycling Clown? Inattentional Blindness while Walking and Talking on a Cell Phone

TASC Manual and Scoresheet Printing Suggestions

Hazard Perception and Distraction in Novice Drivers: Effects of 12 Months Driving Experience

Perceptions of Risk Factors for Road Traffic Accidents

Relationship between reaction time, mental processing speed and motor speed in individuals with mild to moderate brain injury

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

The Severity of Pedestrian Injuries in Alcohol-Related Collisions

SAFE COMMUNITIES SURVEY RESULTS 2000

The British Columbia Coroners Service is committed to conducting a thorough, independent examination of the factors contributing to death in order to

AWARENESS INTERACTION TRAINING

Multitasking in the Automobile

Alcohol Addiction. Peer Pressure. Handling Social Pressures. Peer Pressure 2/15/2012. Alcohol's Effect on One s Health and Future.

Transcription:

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 3, Number 1 Submitted: August 10, 2007 Revisions: August 20, 2007 Accepted: August 27, 2007 Publication Date: September 10, 2007 The Effect of Cell Phone Use on Fine Motor Functioning Bourgeois, M., Dilks, L., Bartling, C., Libersat, A., White, R., Bourassa, J., Martin, C., and Dilks, S. McNeese State University, ldilks@mcneese.edu ABSTRACT It may be an imprudent endeavor to talk on a cell phone while performing a task that requires the coordination of fine motor functioning such as driving a motor vehicle. The purpose of this study is to evaluate the effects of cell phone use on fine motor functioning as might be required in a driving situation. Fifty-three undergraduate psychology students volunteered to participate and were randomly assigned to Group A (experimental) and Group B (control). In Group A, each participant performed a grooved pegboard fine motor task while answering arithmetic questions on a cell phone. Group B performed the motor task without the cell phone. The results indicated a significantly higher overall mean time for Group A than for Group B. Group A also had a significantly higher mean time for dominant and non-dominant hand assessment. There was no significant difference between men and women or between right-handed and left-handed individuals for time achieved on the assessment. INTRODUCTION Cellular phones were introduced in 1983 and have infiltrated every aspect of modern day life. A growing concern is the increased use of cell phones by individuals while operating a motor vehicle. Cell phones can distract motorists in a number of ways. Simply finding, turning on and off, answering, and ending a phone call redirects the driver s hands from steering and his or her eyes from watching the road. Phone conversations can also affect the driver s attention and performance. 49

Glassbrenner (2005), with the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA), conducted an observational survey of drivers in the United States and found that during daylight hours 10 % of vehicles are operated by someone using a cell phone. Of the individuals observed, 10 % were 16-24 years of age. Another study done by the NHTSA (2005), found that young drivers, ages 15-20, account for 12.6 % of fatal crashes and 16 % of drivers involved in a crash requiring a police report. There is much debate over the distractibility of cell phones while operating a motor vehicle especially with young drivers. With the increase in driver cell phone use and the high crash rates for young drivers, empirical evidence has and will play an important role in state and national legislation. Cell Phones and Likelihood of a Crash Based on empirical studies, many researchers have concluded that using a cellular phone while driving significantly increases the risk of a crash. According to Redelmeier and Tibshirani (1997), the estimated risk of a crash while using cellular phones was an average of four times higher than driving while not using a cellular phone, similar to driving while intoxicated. The researchers also concluded that hands-free phones seemed to offer no safety advantage over hand-held phones. Redelmeier and Tibshirani (2006) reiterated earlier findings and suggested that earlier studies have even underestimated the risk associated with driving while using a cell phone. Seo and Torabi (2004) reported that 21 % of the college students surveyed experienced an accident or near accident with at least one of the drivers using a cell phone. Cellular Use While Driving Affects Cognitive Processes Why does cell phone use increase the likelihood of a crash? The empirical research points to impaired cognitive processes that in turn divert attention from the primary task of operating the motor vehicle and reacting to hazardous stimuli. According to Trimmel and Poelzl (2006), even low background noise (traffic noise, irrelevant speech, and barking dogs) impaired performance in spatial attention by hindering parts of the cerebral cortex. Strayer and Johnston (2001) suggested that cell phone use, hand-held or hands-free, negatively affects driving performance by redirecting attention to an another cognitive activity. Horrey and Wikens (2006) had similar conclusions: they found that cell phone use negatively affects a driver s reaction to external hazards. This study also concluded that passenger cell phone use has a similar effect on the driver s reactions. Hunton and Rose (2005) addressed the issue of passenger conversation versus cellular conversations and found cell phone conversations are more hazardous in a driving situation than passenger conversations. They suggested an increase in utilized working memory in a cell phone conversation as opposed to when engaged in a conversation with a passenger. The lack of nonverbal cues with cellular conversations involves more cognitive resources. Cell Phone Usage Effect on Reaction Time and Visual Motor Attention The University of North Carolina Highway Safety Research Center reviewed a number of on-road and driver-simulated studies. The effect of cell phone use in both types of research showed a slower reaction time in response to a number of stimuli 50

(Reinfurt, Huang, Feaganes, & Hunter, 2001). Another study by Richard, et al. (2002) indicated that response times were significantly slower when coupled with a simultaneous auditory task than with an image-flicker task. Both studies alluded to visual inattention as a possible cause for the slower reaction time. Garcia-Larrea, Perchet, Perrin, and Amenedo (2001) suggested that a decrease of attention to sensory inputs and the manipulation of the phone negatively effected response to a visual target. The studies discussed compare cell phone use and a number of effects. There are numerous studies on the overall effect of cell phone use and driving. Many attribute the effect to disrupted cognitive processes such as attention. A number of researchers have reported an increase in reaction time and hindered perception of visual stimuli (e.g., Reinfurt, Huang, Feaganes, & Hunter, 2001). However, the effect of cell phone use on motor function, specifically fine motor, has received much less attention. Cell Phone Use and Fine Motor Functioning Fine motor functions generally refer to small actions, such as movements of the hands, wrists, fingers, and feet. Many tasks, such as driving, require visual motor coordination, synchronization of vision, and movements of the body. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effect of cell phone use on fine motor functioning. This study hypothesized cellular phone use coupled with answering a series of simple arithmetic problems would negatively affect performance on fine motor assessment, which required visual motor coordination. This effect should result in a significantly higher mean time (in seconds) for group A (with cell phone) than group B (without cell phone). Because women have smaller hands and fingers than men we expected women to have an advantage when performing a task requiring fine manual dexterity (Peters, Servos, & Day, 1990). We also expected left-handed individuals to have an advantage with the use of their non-dominant hand, resulting in a lower mean time (Judge & Stirling, 2003). Participants METHOD Fifty-three undergraduate psychology students from a Southern regional state university, ages 18 and above, participated in this study for course extra credit. Group A (experimental) consisted of 24 participants: a random assignment of 16 women and eight men. Group B (control) consisted of 29 participants: a random assignment of 17 women and 12 men. Forty-nine participants were right-handed and four were left-handed. Design We used a randomized two-group (Group A: cell phone and pegboard vs. Group B: peg-board) independent-groups design. The independent variable involved answering or not answering simple arithmetic questions on a cell phone. The dependent variable was the time achieved on the Grooved Pegboard Test, Model 32025. We treated gender and dominant hand as participant variables. 51

Instrument We used the Grooved Pegboard Test, Model 32025 from the Lafayette Instrument Company to assess fine motor function. The Grooved Pegboard is a manipulative dexterity test consisting of 25 holes with randomly positioned slots. Pegs with a key along one side must be rotated to match the hole before they can be inserted. This test requires complex visual-motor coordination (Lafayette Instrument Company, 1997). The cell phone used in this experiment was a Samsung X496 flip phone. Procedure We randomly assigned each participant to Group A (experimental) or Group B (control), and assessed each participant individually. We greeted and informed each participant that the experiment dealt with cell phone use and fine motor function. We gave each participant a demographic survey and a pencil (see Appendix A) and requested that each participant answer all the questions and turn the survey and pencil in upon completion. Researcher #1 then informed each participant in Group A that the next portion of the assessment included participation in a timed fine motor assessment task. Researcher #1 placed the Grooved Pegboard Test on the table directly in front of the seated participant. Researcher #1 instructed each participant to place as many pegs into the Grooved Pegboard Test as he or she could within three minutes using his or her dominant hand, while answering simple arithmetic questions on the cell phone (see Appendix C). (Researcher #2, in another room, placed the call to the cell phone.) Researcher #1 handed the phone to the participant, asked the participant to hold the cell phone in his or her nondominant hand, and instructed the participant to say, when ready, ready, set, go. After the participant had said go the assessment began. Researcher #2 called out the arithmetic problems to the participant and recorded his or her response. Researcher #1 used a stop watch to time the assessment and then recorded the time (see Appendix B). The researcher also insured that the participant was following the instructions of the experiment. The assessment ended when the participant had placed all 25 pegs into the pegboard or upon completion of the three-minute time limit. Researcher #1 instructed the participant to shift the cell phone to his or her dominant hand and to then perform the Grooved Pegboard Test with his or her non-dominant hand while answering simple arithmetic problems (see Appendix D). In Group B each participant participated in the same timed fine motor assessment task first with his or her dominant hand and then with his or her non-dominant hand. Researcher #1 gave the same explanation and instructions to Group B, but omitted the cell phone instructions. The time was kept and recorded for each participant. After the conclusion of motor assessment task, we debriefed each participant and answered all questions. We thanked each participant for his or her participation and 52

requested they not share any information about this study with other individuals that may participate. RESULTS We used an independent t-test to compare the two groups on all measures and a conservative alpha level of.01 for all statistical tests since we performed multiple t-tests. Group A and Group B Group A (experimental) (M=159.33, SD=5.98) had a significantly higher mean Grooved Pegboard Test time than Group B (control) (M=133.52, SD=4.32), t(51)= 3.57, p=.001. Group A (M=77.08, SD=3.19) had a significantly higher mean time for the dominant hand than Group B (M=61.43, SD=3.11), t(51)=3.48, p=.001. Additionally Group A (M=82.25, SD=3.08) had a significantly higher mean time for the non-dominant hand than Group B (M=70.10, SD=2.46), t(51)=3.117, p=.003. Men and Women We did not find a significant difference between the total mean times for men (M=152.75, SD=28.118) and women (M=140.64, SD=28.966), with a t(51)=-1.492, p=.142. For the dominant hand there was no significant difference between men (M=73.15, SD=14.8125) and women (M=65.71, SD=19.252), with t(51)=-1.481, p=.145. Also there was no significant difference for the non-dominant hand assessment between men (M=79.60, SD=15.796) and women (M=73.18, SD=14.636), with t(51)=-1.502, p=.139. Left-Handed and Right-Handed Individuals The dominant hand assessment for left-handed (M=51.87, SD=35.333) and righthanded (M=69.88, SD=15.661) individuals approached statistical significance, with a t(51)= 1.985, p=.053. For the non-dominant hand assessment left-handed (M=63.00, SD=11.605) and right-handed (M=76.63, SD=15.150) individuals approached statistical significance with t(51)=1.752, p=.086. There was not a significant difference for the total mean time achieved for left-handed (M=129.28, SD=21.172) and right-handed (M=146.51, SD=29.316) individuals, t(51)=1.149, p=.256. DISCUSSION The results of the analysis support the hypothesis that the use of a cell phone may distract individuals, causing poorer performance on a fine motor task. The researchers hypothesized that women may have an advantage on fine motor assessments due to smaller finger and hand size. We did not find a significant difference between men and women. The researchers observed that many of the female participants had long artificial fingernails and this may have slightly impaired their performance. We did not find a significant advantage for left-handed individuals (N=4); however, they achieved a lower mean time in all three assessments. We suggest future research be conducted with a larger sample of left-handed individuals. 53

The researchers concluded that cell phone use negatively affects performance on a fine motor task. The researchers performed this simple task in a controlled environment without any notable internal or external distractions. There are reasons to believe that the effects of cell phone use and a task that requires the coordination of fine motor functioning, such as driving a motor vehicle, would be amplified in a real world situation. There are many internal and external distractions in play when operating a motor vehicle. Richard, et al. (2002) stated that driving a vehicle involves not only the execution of motor processes, but also perceptual and cognitive processing, which also requires attention. It may be an imprudent endeavor to talk on a cell phone while performing a task that requires the coordination of fine motor functioning such as driving a motor vehicle. REFERENCES Garcia-Larrea, L., Perchet, C., Perrin, F., & Amenedo, E. (2001). Interference of cellular phone conversations with visuomotor tasks: An ERP study. Journal of Psychophysiology, 15(1), 14-21. Glassbrenner, D. (2005). Driver cell phone use in 2005, overall results. Retrieved November 18, 2006, from page 1 at http://www.nrd.nhtsa.dot. gov/pdf/nrd- 30/NCSA/RNotes/2005/809967.pdf Horrey, W., & Wickens, C. (2006). Examining the impact of cell phone conversations on driving using meta-analytic techniques. Human Factors, 48(1), 196-205. Hunton, J., & Rose, J. (2005). Cellular telephones and driving performance: The affects of attentional demands on motor vehicle crash risk. Risk Analysis, 25(4), 855-866. Judge, J., & Stirling, J. (2003). Fine motor skill performance in left- and right-handers: Evidence of an advantage for left-handers. Laterality, 8(4), 297. Lafayette Instrument Company. (1997). Model 32025 Grooved Pegboard Test. Retrieved September 29, 2006, from http://www.lafayetteinstrument.com NHTSA: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. (2005). Young drivers. Retrieved November 18, 2006, from page 1 at http://wwwnrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/pdf/nrd-30/ncsa/tsf2003/809774.pdf Peters, M., Servos, P., & Day, R. (1990). Marked sex differences on a fine motor skill task disappear when finger size is used as covariate. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75(1), 87-90. Redelmeier, D. A., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1997). Association between cellular telephone calls and motor vehicle collisions. The New England Journal of Medicine, 336, 453-458. Reinfurt, D., Huang, H., Feaganes, J., & Hunter, W. (2001). Cell phone use while driving in North Carolina. The University of North Carolina Highway Safety Research Center. Retrieved November 18, 2006, from page 4 at http://ntl.bts.gov/ lib/12000/12900/12959/cellphone.pdf Richard, C. M., Wright, R. D., Ee, C., Prime, S. L., Shimizu, Y., & Vavrik, J. (2002). Effect of a concurrent auditory task on visual search performance in a drivingrelated image-flicker task. Human Factors, 44(1), 108-119. 54

Seo, D., & Torabi, M. (2004). The impact of in-vehicle cell-phone use on accidents or near-accidents among college students. Journal of American College Health, 53(3), 101-107. Strayer, D., & Johnston, W. (2001). Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychological Science, 12(6), 462-466. Trimmel, M., & Poelzl, G. (2006). Impact of background noise on reaction time and brain DC potential changes of VDT-based spatial attention. Ergonomics, 49(2), 202-208. Appendix A Demographics Age Years of Education Id # Please check the appropriate blank Sex: Male Female Dominant Hand (hand you write with): Right Left Ambidextrous Do you have full use of your left and right hand and fingers? Yes No --If you checked No then please specify which hand: Right Left Do you use a cell phone? Yes No --If you checked yes, how many hours per day Appendix B Score Sheet: Grooved Pegboard Id # Right Hand Grooved Pegboard time to complete: Over 2 minute s Left Hand Grooved Pegboard time to complete: Over 2 minute s 55

Appendix C Arithmetic Questions- Dominant Hand Dominant Hand 7/14 5+12 8x8 11+11 7-9 2x2 2x8 5x5 3+9 3+4 8x11 4-6 3-3 12/12 6x7 5-6 12+12 8/16 8+9 2-4 11/11 6/12 5+10 6-10 6-12 7-7 9x11 2+4 10+11 2x6 2x11 3+12 4x11 4-4 4x8 7+11 6x12 7-11 2x4 3+7 4x6 10-10 2/14 7+9 3x6 2/18 2-6 5x9 4-8 4+9 9+10 6x9 6-7 4x4 10/10 2-8 7+7 2+2 4-11 11-11 5-10 6-9 2+6 8+10 3+5 3-5 2+8 5x11 5-12 3-11 8-8- 8+12 3-7 5-8 5/10 3x4 8-11 3x8 4/8 4+7 6+11 4+12 9x9 9+12 6+8 4+5 5/20 9-12 4+10 5x7 11x11 6+6 3x12 2+11 9-10 5+8 7x12 3+10 7x8 2-10 5+6 3/15 56

Appendix D Arithmetic Questions- Non-Dominant Hand Non-Dominant Hand 9x12 5x12 4+4 8+8 3x5 7/7 2+5 10+10 8+11 6x11 7-12 5-7 4-7 5+5 3-8 2x5 3+8 2+3 2-5 11-12 2/16 6x8 7-10 12/1 4x12 5+11 9+11 2x9 3+6 6+10 2-11 11+12 4+11 7x9 7+8 7/14 5-5 4x9 8x12 3-6 3+5 5x6 2+12 10-11 3x7 9-11 6-6- 12/12 6+7 4-12 6x6 4+6 2x12 4-9 4/16 2-9 2x7 5/20 6-8 2-3 7x7 4-5 9-9 5-11 2x3 6+9 3-4 2-7 4+8 2+10 8-12 4/8 11/1 5-9 7+10 7x11 3x3 4x5 5+9 8-10 3+11 3-12 10+12 2+9 12-12 3x11 8x9 7-8 6-11 5x8 5+7 4-10 3x9 3-10 3/9 9/18 11x12 9+9 10/20 8-9 6+12 7+12 57