A semantic verbal fluency test for English- and Spanish-speaking older Mexican-Americans

Similar documents
Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised*

Metabolic syndrome is a constellation of cardiovascular

Diversity and Dementia

Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised: Norms for Elderly African Americans

Everyday functioning in relation to cognitive functioning and neuroimaging in community-dwelling Hispanic and Non-Hispanic older adults

Neuropsychological Test Development and Normative Data on Hispanics

Acculturation and the Prevalence of Depression in Older Mexican Americans: Baseline Results of the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition

Normative data for a brief neuropsychological battery administered to English- and Spanish-speaking community-dwelling elders

Stress and the SES-Health Gradient: Getting Under the Skin. While the SES gradient in health is well established in many contexts, the physical

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Detecting Dementia With the Mini-Mental State Examination in Highly Educated Individuals

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Background on the issue Previous study with adolescents and adults: Current NIH R03 study examining ADI-R for Spanish speaking Latinos

Why study dementia in diverse populations? Results from the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging

Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston

Test-retest reliable coefficients and 5-year change scores for the MMSE and 3MS

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples

This PDF is available for free download from a site hosted by Medknow Publications

Mini Mental State Examination and the Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination: Effect of education and norms for a multicultural population

A normative study of the CERAD neuropsychological assessment battery in the Korean elderly

Test Assessment Description Ref. Global Deterioration Rating Scale Dementia severity Rating scale of dementia stages (2) (4) delayed recognition

The relation of education and gender on the attention items of the Mini-Mental State Examination in Spanish speaking Hispanic elders

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1

Conceptual framework! Definitions of race and ethnicity Census Questions, Genetics! Social Class, migration, language proficiency!

Equating UDS Neuropsychological Tests: 3.0>2.0, 3.0=2.0, 3.0<2.0? Dan Mungas, Ph.D. University of California, Davis

Overview. Case #1 4/20/2012. Neuropsychological assessment of older adults: what, when and why?

CHAPTER 2 CRITERION VALIDITY OF AN ATTENTION- DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER (ADHD) SCREENING LIST FOR SCREENING ADHD IN OLDER ADULTS AGED YEARS

Cognitive Reserve and the Relationship Between Depressive Symptoms and Awareness of Deficits in Dementia

INCREASING REPRESENTATION IN A MIXED-MODE QUALITY OF LIFE SURVEY OF MEDICARE ESRD BENEFICIARIES

Research Article Chinese-Language Montreal Cognitive Assessment for Cantonese or Mandarin Speakers: Age, Education, and Gender Effects

Patterns of Union Formation Among Urban Minority Youth in the United States

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

Selection and Combination of Markers for Prediction

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

APPENDIX: Supplementary Materials for Advance Directives And Nursing. Home Stays Associated With Less Aggressive End-Of-Life Care For

Supplementary Online Content

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

Outline. Minority Issues in Aging Research. The Role of Research in the Clinical Setting. Why Participate in Research

Correlation Between Intelligence Test Scores and Executive Function Measures

PRELIMINARY NORMS FOR YEAR OLDS ON THE MEMORY TEST FOR OLDER ADULTS (MTOA:S) ABSTRACT

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

Table of Contents. 2 P age. Susan G. Komen

517 Individuals 23 Families

Title: Estimates and Projections of Prevalence of Type Two Diabetes Mellitus in the US 2001, 2011, 2021: The Role of Demographic Factors in T2DM.

Correction for retest effects across repeated measures of cognitive functioning: a longitudinal cohort study of postoperative delirium

Projections of future numbers of dementia cases in Australia with and without prevention

Diabetes in the Latino/Hispanic Population The case for education and outreach

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007)

Profile of DeKalb County

WHI Memory Study (WHIMS) Investigator Data Release Data Preparation Guide April 2014

Arizona Department of Health Services. Hispanic Adult Tobacco Survey 2005 Report

Health Disparities Research. Kyu Rhee, MD, MPP, FAAP, FACP Chief Public Health Officer Health Resources and Services Administration

Methodology for the VoicesDMV Survey

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Implementing Diagnostic Criteria and Estimating Frequency of Mild Cognitive Impairment in an Urban Community

Cultural Neuropsychology Round Table: Training Issues and Methods for Gaining Competence in Working with Diverse Individuals

Table of Contents. 2 P a g e. Susan G. Komen

2.75: 84% 2.5: 80% 2.25: 78% 2: 74% 1.75: 70% 1.5: 66% 1.25: 64% 1.0: 60% 0.5: 50% 0.25: 25% 0: 0%

Geriatric performance on the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination (Cognistat) What is normal?

Title. CitationAustralasian Journal on Ageing, 31(3): Issue Date Doc URL. Rights. Type. File Information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients

CLACLS. Prevalence of Chronic Health Conditions among Latinos in the United States between 1999 and 2011

Sleep and Neurocognitive aging in Population based studies

Developmental Assessment of Young Children Second Edition (DAYC-2) Summary Report


equation involving two test variables.

Technical Specifications

Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment

Dr. Hector Balcazar UT School of Public Health

CRITERIA FOR USE. A GRAPHICAL EXPLANATION OF BI-VARIATE (2 VARIABLE) REGRESSION ANALYSISSys

Accessibility and Disability Service. A Guide to Services for Students with

NO LOWER COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING IN OLDER ADULTS WITH ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER

Standardization and Validation of Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in the Moroccan Population

Presented By: Yip, C.K., OT, PhD. School of Medical and Health Sciences, Tung Wah College

Richard A. Van Dorn, Ph.D. 1 Jeffrey W. Swanson, Ph.D. 2 Marvin S. Swartz, M.D. 2 IN PRESS -- PSYCHIATRIC SERVICES

In this second module in the clinical trials series, we will focus on design considerations for Phase III clinical trials. Phase III clinical trials

WHI Memory Study (WHIMS) Investigator Data Release Data Preparation Guide December 2012

Health Disparities Research

The Cross-Cultural Neuropsychological Test Battery (CCNB): Effects of Age, Education, Ethnicity, and Cognitive Status on Performance

LATINO OLDER ADULTS AND ALCOHOL USE: A DESCRIPTIVE ANALYSIS. Andrea Soria California State University, Long Beach May 2015

Neuropsychological Correlates of Performance Based Functional Status in Elder Adult Protective Services Referrals for Capacity Assessments

SBIRT IOWA. Iowa Army National Guard THE IOWA CONSORTIUM FOR SUBSTANCE ABUSE RESEARCH AND EVALUATION. Iowa Army National Guard

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

A About Facebook 2. B Data linking and controls 2. C Sampling rates 5. D Activity categories 6. E Models included in Figure 4 9

Alzheimer s disease affects patients and their caregivers. experience employment complications,

University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, 1998 Ph.D. in Clinical Psychology (APA-Approved)

Measuring Perceived Social Support in Mexican American Youth: Psychometric Properties of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Comparison of the Short Test of Mental Status and the Mini-Mental State Examination in Mild Cognitive Impairment

Book review. Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS). By C.K. Conners, D. Erhardt, M.A. Sparrow. New York: Multihealth Systems, Inc.

BaptistHealth_FEB2014 1

Gait abnormalities as early signs of MCI

Gerardo Machnicki 1, Ricardo F. Allegri 1,2 *, Carol Dillon 1, Cecilia M. Serrano 1,2 and Fernando E Taragano 2 SUMMARY INTRODUCTION

PREVALENCE OF DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS IN NEW MEXICO HISPANIC AND NON-HISPANIC WHITE ELDERLY

SELF-REPORTED HEART DISEASE AMONG ARAB AND CHALDEAN AMERICAN WOMEN RESIDING IN SOUTHEAST MICHIGAN

COGNITIVE FACTORS IN EPILEPSY BY MARGARET DAVIES-EYSENCK

Chapter Fairness. Test Theory and Fairness

Addendum: Multiple Regression Analysis (DRAFT 8/2/07)

INTRODUCTION KURT F. GEISINGER. Copyright American Psychological Association

Validation of the Russian version of the Quality of Life-Rheumatoid Arthritis Scale (QOL-RA Scale)

Transcription:

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 199 208 A semantic verbal fluency test for English- and Spanish-speaking older Mexican-Americans Hector M. González a,, Dan Mungas b, Mary N. Haan a a University of Michigan, School of Public Health, Department of Epidemiology, 611 Church St., Room 315, Ann Arbor, MI 48104-2029, USA b University of California, Davis, School of Medicine, Department of Neurology, Sacramento, CA, USA Abstract Accepted 11 June 2004 Currently, there are few semantic verbal fluency tests available with appropriate and adequate normative data for older Latinos of Mexican ancestry. The purposes of this study were to examine the effects of language and socioeconomic variables on a semantic verbal fluency test, derive normative data and develop statistical corrections for the effects of significant variables. A large, community-dwelling sample of Latinos of primarily Mexican ancestry over age 60 was asked to name animals with four legs within 1 min in their preferred language of English or Spanish. The total sum of animals named was influenced by age, education, gender, and, to a much lesser extent, language of test administration. Regression coefficients derived from demographic variables were used to adjust raw scores in a general correction formula, and specific percentile cutoff values were identified for these adjusted scores. Normative data tables stratified on demographic variables are also presented. 2004 National Academy of Neuropsychology. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Hispanic-Americans; Latinos; Mexican-Americans; Neuropsychology; Semantic verbal fluency; Language; Aging 1. Introduction Latinos are now the largest ethnic minority group in the U.S. with about two-thirds being of Mexican ancestry (Census, 2001; Census, 2004; Llorente, Pontón, Taussig, & Satz, 1999). Mexican-Americans in the U.S. are a culturally and socioeconomically diverse group. Within Mexican-Americans, there is considerable variability in not only their adoption of U.S. culture, Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 734 615 9605; fax: +1 734 998 6837. E-mail address: hmgonz@umich.edu (H.M. González). 0887-6177/$ see front matter 2004 National Academy of Neuropsychology. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2004.06.001

200 H.M. González et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 199 208 but also in their cultural experiences and practices from their Mexican or U.S. region of origin. Associated with this cultural diversity are the economic and educational opportunities that are less available to some Mexican-Americans (Wade, 1997). Further, for many older Mexican American adults, educational opportunities were fewer than for younger adults. Finally, as is often the case today, the resources available to schools serving low income, ethnic minority communities were often less than those that are available to other communities. In combination, these present challenges to clinicians and investigators involved in the neuropsychological assessment of older adults and in this case older Mexican-Americans. Neuropsychological tests are needed that are minimally affected by cultural, linguistic, and educational biases that could potentially disadvantage population subgroups, and thereby could negatively affect cognitive impairment detection (Ardila, Rosselli, & Puente, 1994; Pontón & Ardila, 1999). Currently, there are few neuropsychological test batteries that are have appropriate and adequate normative data for older Latinos of Mexican ancestry (Mungas, Reed, Marshall, & González, 2000; Pontón, Satz, Herrera, & Ortiz, 1996). Although there are verbal fluency tests available for Spanish-speakers in other countries (Butman, Allegri, Harris, & Drake, 2000; Rosselli et al., 2000) and Latinos of Caribbean ancestry (Jacobs, Sano, Albert, & Schofield, 1997), there are few semantic verbal fluency tests with appropriate normative data for older Mexican-Americans (La Rue, Romero, Ortiz, Liang, & Lindeman, 1999; Pontón et al., 1996). In the assessment of individuals from culturally different backgrounds and lower educational levels, it is reasonable to select tests that first and foremost address the referral or research questions and are likely to provide a measure of functional ability rather than noise associated with test bias. Tests that rely on commonly experienced events or objects (e.g., shopping lists for recall or animal names) would be preferable to those that are culturally bound and highly literacy dependent (e.g., items from the WAIS Information subtest). Semantic or category verbal fluency tests requiring an individual to name objects that are experienced in daily life would be a sensible choice. These tests are designed to assess verbal abilities and executive function. In addition, the task demands experiential or semantic knowledge (e.g., naming animals), regardless of cultural context, that is not absolutely reliant on formal educational experiences of a particular culture. For example, among Hindi-speaking Indians of the rural district of Ballabgarh which has low literacy rates, semantic verbal fluency was shown to be less influenced by educational level than phonemic verbal fluency (Ratcliff et al., 1998). If tests of semantic fluency are less affected by formal education in groups with low levels of education or who speak a given language, then it becomes important to quantify these influences. There are two objectives in this report: (1) to determine the association of formal education and language, on verbal fluency and (2) to provide normative data for a semantic verbal fluency test based from a community-dwelling sample of 1276 of Mexican-Americans ages 60 and over. 2. Methods 2.1. Participants Participants in this study were recruited as part of the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging (SALSA), which is a prospective, cohort study of aging and health among Latinos

H.M. González et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 199 208 201 over age 60 (details of the study design can be found in Haan et al., 2003). Participants were eligible for SALSA if: (1) they self-identified as being of Hispanic or Latino ancestry (parents or grandparents), (2) age 60 or over, and (3) English or Spanish was their primary language. All testing was conducted by bilingual/bicultural technicians who received supervised training from a neuropsychologist (H.M.G). Face-to-face interviews were supervised and observed by the same neuropsychologist in the participants home during testing. For this analysis, participants meeting National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Diseases and Stroke/Alzheimer s Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria for Alzheimer s disease or California Alzheimer s Disease and Treatment Center criteria for ischemic vascular dementia were excluded from the sample. Details of the multi-stage, dementia ascertainment procedures can be found in Haan et al. (2003). The Spanish English Animal Naming Test (SEANT), like other verbal fluency tasks, required the participant to name animals with four legs within 60 s. The SEANT was embedded within the Modified Mini Mental State exam (3MS; Teng & Chui, 1987). The standard 3MS administration has a 30 s animal fluency task as one of its regular items, but limits the number of responses to 10 items for that 30 s interval. For the SEANT, the time was extended to 60 s, and the number of items admissible was unrestricted. This was done to lessen ceiling effects and to provide an additional test of cognitive functioning that would yield normative neuropsychological data. Initial experience with the animal naming item of the 3MS indicated that 41.2% of SALSA participants gave the ceiling number of 10 responses on this item. The SEANT was administered to 1276 SALSA participants in the first follow-up year (FY1) after the baseline year of SALSA (July 1999 to June 2000). Participants self-selected their preferred language (i.e., English or Spanish) for SEANT administration. This was used in data analyses. Education was measured in years of formal school. 2.2. Statistical adjustment for demographic variables and norms Linear regression was used to estimate the effects of the demographic variables on the dependent measure. The dependent measure was the sum of four-legged animals that were given by the respondent in 60 s. The predictor variables were (age and education in years and language group) (English versus Spanish) and gender (M/F). The unstandardized regression coefficients for the demographic variables derived from these analyses were used to adjust raw scale scores for effects of demographic variables. The general formula for this adjustment was: Adjusted Score = Raw Score b Language b Gender (b Age (Age-70)) (b Education (Education-12)) In this equation, b Language is the coefficient corresponding to the language group of the participant, b Gender is the coefficient corresponding to the gender of the participant, and b Age and b Education are the unstandardized coefficients for age and education. The age of 70 and 12 years of education are arbitrary values selected as standard reference points. The resulting adjusted score is uncorrelated with the demographic variables, but maintains a scale of measurement

202 H.M. González et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 199 208 similar to that of the raw scores. This is the same approach previously used to adjust the Folstein Mini Mental State exam (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975; Mungas, Marshall, Weldon, Haan, & Reed, 1996) and SEVLT (González, Mungas, Reed, Marshall, & Haan, 2001a,b) for effects of demographic variables. 3. Results In this sample of 1276 older Latinos, the mean age was 70.3 (S.D. = 6.8); mean education was 7.8 (S.D. = 5.4). Women accounted for 57.4% of the sample, 53.6% were Spanish-speaking and 52.5% were U.S.-born. The Spanish-speaking group was older, t(1, 1274) = 3.85, P <.001, and had fewer years of education, t(1, 1274) = 23.23, P <.001, but the groups did not differ in the percentage of females (χ 2 = 1.58, P =.209). Eighty-five percent of the sample was from Mexico and 15% from another Latin American country. In this sample, 53.3% were administered the SEANT in Spanish. 3.1. Statistical adjustment of scores and norms A normal quantile (Q Q) plot of SEANT responses did not indicate a significantly nonnormal distribution. Polynomial regression was used to evaluate non-linear effects of education and age on SEANT performance. Curve estimations of the functions of years of education indicated that there were significant linear, quadratic and cubic effects; however, the alternatives to the linear function offered very small increases in variance explanation. The linear effect alone accounted for 9.9% of SEANT variance, whereas the combined linear, quadratic, and cubic accounted for 10.2%, which indicates that the non-linear terms explained only 0.3% of additional variance. Age did not show a significantly non-linear relationship with the SEANT. The cross-product interaction term for education and language was not statistically significant (P =.234) and was dropped from the model. This indicates that effects of education were similar in English- and Spanish-speakers. Results of the linear regression model are presented in Table 1. Significant main effects were found between the SEANT and age, education, and gender, and language. Education (4.1%) accounted for the largest proportion of variance, followed by age (3.9%), gender (1.2%), and language (0.4%). The derived unstandardized regression coefficients are presented in Table 2. The dichotomous independent variable coefficients for language or gender were either added or subtracted Table 1 Results of the Regression Analyses for the Spanish English Animal Naming Test B S.E. Beta t P Age (years).09.01.17 6.28.00 Education (years).16.02.24 7.44.00 Gender.82.20.11 4.21.00 Language.47.23.06 2.07.04

H.M. González et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 199 208 203 Table 2 Derived Unstandardized Regression Coefficients used to create Adjusted Scores for the Spanish English Animal Naming Test Unstandardized beta coefficients Age (years).09 Education (years).16 Male a.41 Female a.41 English b.24 Spanish b.24 a Coefficient values are added or subtracted from the equation depending on the individual s gender. b Coefficient values are added or subtracted from the equation depending on the language of the individual. from the scores depending on the characteristics of the individual. The coefficients for age and education were multiplied by the difference from the reference values of 70 and 12 in the general correction formula, respectively. In this way, the coefficients were entered into the general correction formula previously described and used to calculate the demographically adjusted scores for each of the seven dependent measures for the entire sample. Along with adjusted sample means and standard deviations, the specific percentile cut-offs are presented in Table 3. Normative data for the SEANT derived from descriptive statistics (numbers per cell, means, and standard deviations) for English- (Table 4) and Spanish-speakers (Table 5) were stratified by 10-year age group, education tertile and gender. Table 3 Specific cutoffs for age, education, gender, and language Adjusted Scores corresponding to different percentile values for Spanish English Animal Naming Test N 1276 M 11.04 S.D. 3.45 Percentiles 5 5.5 10 6.8 15 7.8 20 8.5 30 9.4 40 10.3 50 11.1 60 11.9 70 12.8 80 13.8 85 14.4 90 15.3 95 16.5

204 H.M. González et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 199 208 Table 4 Normative data for the Spanish English Animal Naming Test for 592 English-speaking Community-dwelling Latinos over age 60 from the Sacramento area Latino study on aging Age groups Education groups Gender N M S.D. 60 69 12+ Male 111 12.41 4.17 Female 96 11.68 3.38 Total 207 12.07 3.83 5 11 Male 41 11.39 3.60 Female 62 10.89 3.00 Total 103 11.09 3.25 0 4 Male 8 12.50 4.04 Female 4 12.50 1.00 Total 12 12.50 3.26 Total Male 160 12.16 4.03 Female 162 11.40 3.22 Total 322 11.77 3.66 70 79 12+ Male 43 12.63 4.50 Female 69 11.14 3.36 Total 112 11.71 3.89 5 11 Male 31 10.65 3.53 Female 54 9.74 3.49 Total 85 10.07 3.51 0 4 Male 10 8.00 3.27 Female 22 9.00 2.37 Total 32 8.69 2.67 Total Male 84 11.35 4.28 Female 145 10.30 3.37 Total 229 10.68 3.75 80+ 12+ Male 8 7.88 3.44 Female 6 7.50 4.32 Total 14 7.71 3.69 5 11 Male 7 8.43 2.30 Female 12 9.83 2.33 Total 19 9.32 2.36 0 4 Male 4 5.50 5.20 Female 4 9.00 2.83 Total 8 7.25 4.30 Total Male 19 7.58 3.49 Female 22 9.05 3.08 Total 41 8.37 3.31 Total 12+ Male 162 12.25 4.32 Female 171 11.32 3.47 Total 333 11.77 3.93 5 11 Male 79 10.84 3.54 Female 128 10.30 3.19 Total 207 10.51 3.33 0 4 Male 22 9.18 4.62 Female 30 9.47 2.54 Total 52 9.35 3.53 Total Male 263 11.57 4.23 Female 329 10.75 3.34 Total 592 11.11 3.78

H.M. González et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 199 208 205 Table 5 Normative data for the Spanish English Animal Naming Test for 684 Spanish-speaking Community-dwelling Latinos over age 60 from the Sacramento area Latino study on aging Age groups Education groups Gender N M S.D. 60 69 12+ Male 21 10.48 3.84 Female 29 11.79 3.40 Total 50 11.24 3.61 5 11 Male 47 9.96 3.88 Female 66 9.52 3.02 Total 113 9.70 3.40 0 4 Male 61 10.36 3.28 Female 95 9.68 3.34 Total 156 9.95 3.32 Total Male 129 10.23 3.58 Female 190 9.95 3.32 Total 319 10.06 3.42 70 79 12+ Male 13 12.38 3.64 Female 19 10.53 3.26 Total 32 11.28 3.48 5 11 Male 40 10.75 3.33 Female 50 8.04 3.09 Total 90 9.24 3.46 0 4 Male 65 9.26 3.55 Female 92 8.76 2.77 Total 157 8.97 3.11 Total Male 118 10.11 3.61 Female 161 8.75 3.00 Total 279 9.32 3.34 80+ 12+ Male 4 12.50 3.00 Female 4 10.25 3.40 Total 8 11.38 3.20 5 11 Male 7 8.71 4.31 Female 17 6.59 4.20 Total 24 7.21 4.25 0 4 Male 22 8.36 4.42 Female 32 6.91 3.00 Total 54 7.50 3.68 Total Male 33 8.94 4.36 Female 53 7.06 3.51 Total 86 7.78 3.94 Total 12+ Male 38 11.34 3.74 Female 52 11.21 3.35 Total 90 11.27 3.50 5 11 Male 94 10.20 3.69 Female 133 8.59 3.35 Total 227 9.26 3.58 0 4 Male 148 9.58 3.63 Female 219 8.89 3.19 Total 367 9.17 3.38 Total Male 280 10.03 3.70 Female 404 9.09 3.36 Total 684 9.47 3.53

206 H.M. González et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 199 208 4. Discussion This study provides normative data for a semantic verbal fluency test based upon a very large community-dwelling sample of 1276 older (60 years plus) English- and Spanish-speaking Latinos of predominately Mexican ancestry. The demographics of this large sample closely reflect the Latino population in Sacramento County in terms of age and gender characteristics from the Census 2000 Dress Rehearsal (Census, 1999). Comparisons with data from the 1990 Census indicates that the SALSA sample reflects Latinos over age 60 in California in terms of education, age, and gender (Census, 1995). The representativeness of the SALSA sample would suggest that the SEANT would be appropriate for use with most older Mexican-Americans (Census, 2000). SEANT performance was significantly influenced by the demographic factors studied (i.e., age, education, gender and language). However, in combination, these factors accounted for less than 10% (9.67%) of the variance. Education had the largest affect on SEANT performance, but it is likely that the statistical significance of these factors was largely related to the large sample size in this study. Nevertheless, the findings of this study indicate how the SEANT performance was affected by demographic variables and provide methods for accounting for these effects in interpreting test scores that should be considered when using this exam. It is possible that SEANT performance could be affected by other factors not examined in this study; however, the factors presented above are likely to be of utility to clinicians and researchers. As such, SEANT users that are interested in statistical adjusting test SEANT test performance are encouraged to use the adjustments or normative data provided in the tables above. As in the previous reports, two approaches for deriving the adjusted scores based on significant demographics variables are provided (González, Mungas, & Haan, 2002; González et al., 2001a,b). With the appropriate coefficients for demographics in Table 2, the general formula above can be applied to derive an adjusted score for the acceptable words produced. The derived adjusted score can then be compared to the normative data in Table 3 for deviation of the adjusted score from the means and to determine its percentile rank. Additionally, the SEANT score can be interpreted with a more conventional approach using the normative data for English and Spanish-speakers in Tables 4 and 5, respectively. Although the former approach will yield a more finely calibrated adjusted score, the two approaches will yield similar results. An advantage of the derived adjusted score is that it avoids problems with small sample sizes that are seen some of the cells of the normative tables (Tables 4 and 5), which could potentially result in unstable estimates resulting from the more infrequent demographic variable combinations. The potential disadvantage of the derived score approach involves underlying assumptions for linear regression. The results of this study indicate the linear associations between variables had a better fit than the non-linear associations, which would indicate that this regression assumption was met. The normative data provided for the SEANT in this manuscript should be of utility clinicians and researchers working with older Latino individuals of Mexican ancestry. There is considerable ethnic and cultural variability within populations of Latinos and further research with the SEANT will be needed to demonstrate its applicability to other groups. Tests, like the SEANT, hold promise for assessing the cognitive function of culturally and educationally diverse populations. Providing normative data for assessing the cognitive functioning of

H.M. González et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 199 208 207 ethnically diverse populations is consistent with the American Psychological Association s General Principles and Ethics Standards and should guide the future direction of neuropsychological test development. References Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., & Puente, A. E. (1994). Neuropsychological evaluation of the Spanish speaker. New York, NY, US: Plenum Press. Butman, J., Allegri, R. F., Harris, P., & Drake, M. (2000). Spanish verbal fluency. Normative data in Argentina. Medicina (B Aires), 60(5 Pt 1), 561 564. Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189 198. González, H. M., Mungas, D., & Haan, M. N. (2002). A verbal learning and memory test for English- and Spanish-speaking older Mexican-American adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16(4), 439 451. González, H. M., Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Marshall, S., & Haan, M. N. (2001). A new Verbal Learning and Memory Test for English- and Spanish-speaking older people. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(5), 544 555. González, H. M., Mungas, D. M., Reed, B. R., Marshall, S., & Haan, M. (2001). A new Verbal Learning and Memory Test for English- and Spanish-speaking older people. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 544 555. Haan, M. N., Mungas, D. M., Gonzalez, H. M., Ortiz, T. A., Acharya, A., & Jagust, W. J. (2003). Prevalence of dementia in older Latinos: The influence of type 2 diabetes mellitus, stroke and genetic factors. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 51(2), 169 177. Jacobs, D. M., Sano, M., Albert, S., & Schofield, P. (1997). Cross-cultural neuropsychological assessment: A comparison of randomly selected, demographically matched cohorts of English- and Spanish-speaking older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 331 339. La Rue, A., Romero, L. J., Ortiz, I. E., Liang, H. C., & Lindeman, R. D. (1999). Neuropsychological performance of Hispanic and non-hispanic older adults: An epidemiologic survey. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13(4), 474 486. Llorente, A. M., Pontón, M. O., Taussig, I. M., & Satz, P. (1999). Patterns of American immigration and their influence on the acquisition of neuropsychological norms for Hispanics. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(7), 603 614. Mungas, D., Marshall, S. C., Weldon, M., Haan, M., & Reed, B. R. (1996). Age and education correction of Mini-Mental State Examination for English and Spanish-speaking elderly. Neurology, 46(3), 700 706. Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Marshall, S. C., & Gonzalez, H. M. (2000). Development of psychometrically matched English and Spanish language neuropsychological tests for older persons. Neuropsychology, 14(2), 209 223. Pontón, M. O., & Ardila, A. (1999). The future of neuropsychology with Hispanic populations in the United States. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(7), 565 580. Pontón, M. O., Satz, P., Herrera, L., & Ortiz, F. (1996). Normative data stratified by age and education for the Neuropsychological Screening Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): Initial report. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2(2), 96 104. Ratcliff, G., Ganguli, M., Chandra, V., Sharma, S., Belle, S., Seaberg, E., et al. (1998). Effects of literacy and education on measures of word fluency. Brain and Language, 61(1), 115 122. Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Araujo, K., Weekes, V. A., Caracciolo, V., Padilla, M., et al. (2000). Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish-English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology, 7(1), 17 24. Teng, E. L., & Chui, H. C. (1987). The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 48(8), 314 318. U.S. Census Bureau. (1995). Education in the United States: 1990 census of population and housing. Washington, DC.

208 H.M. González et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 199 208 U.S. Census Bureau. (2001). Overview of race and Hispanic origin 2000. Census 2000 brief. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. U.S. Census Bureau. (1999). Census 2000 dress rehearsal 100-percent summary file. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. U.S. Census Bureau. (2000). The Hispanic population of the United States. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. U.S. Census Bureau. (2004). Current population series (Vol. 15). Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. Wade, P. (1997) Race and ethnicity in Latin America. London, Chicago, IL: Pluto Press.