ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Motor Area Activation During Dreamed Hand Clenching: A Pilot Study on EEG Alpha Band

Similar documents
H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic Activation During Lucid REM Sleep: A Case Study

Lucid Dreaming: Physiological Correlates of Consciousness during REM Sleep

Report. Dreamed Movement Elicits Activation in the Sensorimotor Cortex

Applications of lucid dreams: An online study

Novel single trial movement classification based on temporal dynamics of EEG

An exploratory sleep laboratory study

A Brain Computer Interface System For Auto Piloting Wheelchair

Physiological Mechanisms of Lucid Dreaming. Stephen LaBerge Sleep Research Center Stanford University

The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events

Validity Established of DreamLight Cues for Eliciting Lucid Dreaming

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan

Benjamin Blankertz Guido Dornhege Matthias Krauledat Klaus-Robert Müller Gabriel Curio

Simultaneous Real-Time Detection of Motor Imagery and Error-Related Potentials for Improved BCI Accuracy

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

Simplest method: Questionnaires. Retrospective: past week, month, year, lifetime Daily: Sleep diary What kinds of questions would you ask?

Lucid dream induction using three different cognitive methods

LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART

CONTROL OF MOVEMENT BY THE BRAIN A. PRIMARY MOTOR CORTEX:

MOVEMENT RULES. Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program)

A. PRIMARY MOTOR CORTEX: - responsible for - like somatosensory cortex, primary motor cortex show (motor homunculus) - amount of cortex devoted to

Frontal Electroencephalogram Alpha Asymmetry During Sleep: Stability and Its Relation to Affective Style

Healing Through Lucid Dreams. Stephen LaBerge Stanford University & Saybrook Institute

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications

The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming

Discrimination of EEG-Based Motor Imagery Tasks by Means of a Simple Phase Information Method

Sleep Disorders. Sleep. Circadian Rhythms

Polysomnography Artifacts and Updates on AASM Scoring Rules. Robin Lloyd, MD, FAASM, FAAP 2017 Utah Sleep Society Conference

Non-REM Lucid Dreaming. Joe Dane Pain Management Center University of Virginia Medical School

How did you sleep last night? Were you in a deep sleep or light sleep? How many times did you wake up? What were you doing right before you went to

Lucid dreaming during NREM sleep: Two case reports

EEG Event-Related Desynchronization of patients with stroke during motor imagery of hand movement

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Chapter 6. Consciousness

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm

Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects

Many studies have investigated the effect of waking

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs?

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. The effect of Training on Interrater Reliability in Dream Content Analysis

Sleep Stage Estimation Based on Approximate Heartrate Calculated from Other Persons

linkedin.com/in/lizziehillsleeptechservices 1

Neurophysiology & EEG

Marieb, E.N. (2004), Human Anatomy & Physiology, Pearson Education, Inc, San Francisco, CA, USA, p459

ASSESSMENT OF EEG EVENT-RELATED DESYNCHRONIZATION IN STROKE SURVIVORS PERFORMING SHOULDER-ELBOW MOVEMENTS

Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night?

Running head: NEURAL CORRELATES OF LUCID DREAMS: INVESTIGATING THE ROLE OF METACOGNITION AND VOLITION IN DREAMS. Dreams: N.P.

Modules 7. Consciousness and Attention. sleep/hypnosis 1

EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE

Running head: Exploring the Cognitive Effects of Dreaming

The Stability and Variability of Dreaming

The Nervous System. Neuron 01/12/2011. The Synapse: The Processor

Sleep - 10/5/17 Kelsey

EEG-Rhythm Dynamics during a 2-back Working Memory Task and Performance

Lucid Dreaming: Neural Correlates and Practical Applications

ANALYSIS OF EEG FOR MOTOR IMAGERY BASED CLASSIFICATION OF HAND ACTIVITIES

CURRENT TRENDS IN GRAZ BRAIN-COMPUTER INTERFACE (BCI) RESEARCH

FEP Medical Policy Manual

Physiology of Normal Sleep: From Young to Old

Spontaneous EEG and Sleep Donders MEG/EEG Toolkit Martin Dresler

LEARNING DURING SLEEP: AN INDIRECT TEST OF THE ERASURE-THEORY OF DREAMING

RUNNING HEAD: Lucid dream induction review. Induction of lucid dreams: A systematic review of evidence. Tadas Stumbrys. Heidelberg University, Germany

A Modified Method for Scoring Slow Wave Sleep of Older Subjects

Practical 3 Nervous System Physiology 2 nd year English Module. Dept. of Physiology, Carol Davila University of Medicine and Pharmacy

Unit 3 Psychology ~ 2013

FEP Medical Policy Manual

SOMNAMBULISM: CLINICAL ASPECTS AND PATHOPHYSIOLOGICAL HYPOTHESES. Zadra, A., Desautels, A., Petit, D., Montplaisir, J. (2013) The Lancet Neurology

EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Factors Affecting the Continuity Between Waking and Dreaming: Emotional Intensity and Emotional Tone of the Waking-Life Event

Brain Computer Interface. Mina Mikhail

Western Hospital System. PSG in History. SENSORS in the field of SLEEP. PSG in History continued. Remember

Week 4 Psychology. Theory of mind is an individual s understanding that they and others think, feel, perceive, and have private experiences.

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 9: Sleep and Biological Rhythms

Middle-Ear Muscle A~ctivity (MEMA) and Its Association with Motor Activity in the Extremities and Head in Sleep

DELSYS. Purpose. Hardware Concepts. Software Concepts. Technical Note 101: EMG Sensor Placement

Electrooculogram (EOG): eye movements. Air flow measurements: breathing Heart rate.

SLEEP STAGING AND AROUSAL. Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program)

Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects

The Concurrence of Fine Muscle Activity and Rapid Eye Movements During Sleep

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Hand of Hope. For hand rehabilitation. Member of Vincent Medical Holdings Limited

NORAH Sleep Study External Comment Mathias Basner, MD, PhD, MSc

Correlation Dimension versus Fractal Exponent During Sleep Onset

Sleep Stages Solution v0.1

Hemispheric asymmetry and regional differences in electroencephalographic alpha activity at the wake-sleep transition

Sleep Stage Scoring. emedicine.medscape.com. emedicine Specialties > Neurology > Sleep-Related Diseases

Sleep stages. Awake Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3 Stage 4 Rapid eye movement sleep (REM) Slow wave sleep (NREM)

Efficacy of Lucid Dream Induction For Lucid and Non-Lucid Dreamers

CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL

Neuromotor Rehabilitation by Neurofeedback

Neuroimaging and Assessment Methods

Understanding One s Dreams

Practice test 1 spring 2011 copy

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms

Exclusion criteria and outlier detection

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AG Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology

Sleep Stage Re-Estimation Method According to Sleep Cycle Change

Transcription:

ORIGINAL ARTICLES Motor Area Activation During Dreamed Hand Clenching: A Pilot Study on EEG Alpha Band Daniel Erlacher, M.A., Michael Schredl, Ph.D., Stephen LaBerge, Ph.D. In a single participant physiological responses to dreamed motor performance in REM lucid dreams (LD) were studied. Electroencephalographic (EEG) alpha power over motor areas (C3, Cz, C4) has been recorded while the participant performed specific motor tasks (hand clenching vs. counting) in a LD. The lucid dreamer marked those dream events by pre-arranged eye movement patterns evident in the recorded electrooculogram (EOG). Results showed that EEG alpha power over bilateral motor areas decreased while the lucid dreamer executed left or right hand clenching in contrast to dream counting, which supports the hypothesis that motor performance during lucid dreaming involves the same cortical areas as during waking performance. (Sleep and Hypnosis 2003;5(4):182-187) Key words: Lucid dreaming, psychophysiology, EEG INTRODUCTION The investigation of the relationship between dream content and REM sleep physiology provides an interesting area for the study of the mind-body interaction in general (1). In the context of human movement science insights from imagined movements on motor performance can be extended by research about movements in dreams and their physiological correlates (2). Several studies suggest that the same neural system that is used for voluntary motor performance is also used to perform imagined movements in wakefulness (3,4) as well as in rapid eye movement (REM) dreams From University of Heidelberg, Germany (Dr. Erlacher), Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany (Dr. Schredl), Stanford University, USA (Dr. LaBerge) Address reprint requests to: Dr. Daniel Erlacher, Institute for sport and sportscience, University of Heidelberg, Im Neuenheimer Feld 700, 69120 Heidelberg, Germany Phone: ++49-6221-544348, Fax: ++49-6221-544387 e-mail: daniel.erlacher@urz.uni-heidelberg.de Accepted March 27, 2003 (5,6). In contrast to physical performance, in REM dreams the efferent motor commands are actively suppressed by neural structures in the brain stem, keeping dreamers from actually acting out their dreams (7). Wolpert (8) showed that dreams consisting of dreamed body movements are-despite the general muscle atonia-associated with an elevated EMG activity in arms and legs. The studies of Grossman et al. (9) and Gardner et al. (10) replicated this finding and were even able to differentiate between dreamed arm and dreamed leg activity by analyzing the limb EMGs. On a higher level of the motor system Hong et al. (11) used positron emission tomography (PET) to observe that eye movements during REM sleep involve the same cortical areas that control waking saccadic eye movement and attention. In a single participant, Etevenon and Guillou (12) found an alpha band decrease over the left central cortical areas in correspondence with dreamed performance of the right hand. The 182

D. Erlacher et al. decrease of alpha power can be seen as a correlate of an activated cortical area with an increase in the excitability level of the neurons (13). The problem in non-lucid REM dream studies about the correlations between the dreamer's subjective reports and physiological activity is the inability to match the dream experience obtained after awaking with the EMG of EEG recording exactly. Lucid dream research can be used to cope with this problem. In a lucid dream (LD) the dreamer is aware while dreaming that she/he is dreaming (14,15). This state of consciousness was validated by Hearne (16) and LaBerge (17); they instructed participants with the ability to have lucid dreams in the sleep laboratory to conduct pre-arranged eye movements during their dreams. This eye movement signal can be measured polysomnographically (EOG), indicating that the participants had indeed been lucid during uninterrupted REM sleep as other characteristic features of REM sleep (EEG, EMG) remained unchanged. Thus, lucid dreamers can remember pre-sleep instructions to carry out experiments during their dreams and mark the exact time of particular dream events with eye movement signals, allowing precise correlations between the dreamer's subjective reports and recorded physiological responses. Using this method, LaBerge (18) was able to demonstrate that time intervals estimated in lucid dreams are very close to real time, that dreamed breathing corresponds to actual respiration rhythm and that dreamed sexual activity is associated with physiological responses very similar to those that accompany sexual activity in the waking state. With respect to voluntary movements during LD, LaBerge et al. (19) observed that a sequence of left and right fist clenches carried out in the LD resulted in a corresponding sequence of left and right forearm twitches as measured by EMG. In order to extend the findings of LaBerge et al. (19), we focused on central processes of the neural motor system during dreamed motor activity. We hypothesized that voluntary motor activity performed in lucid dreams will decrease the power of the EEG alpha band (8-12 Hz) over the motor areas (C3, Cz, C4). For selfpaced voluntary finger movements in wakefulness Pfurtscheller and Berghold (20) found a decrease of the alpha band over bilateral motor areas after movement onset, whereas for motor imagery a predominantly contralateral decrease was found by Pfurtscheller and Neuper (4). In the study by Etevenon and Guillou (12), an alpha band decrease over the left central cortical areas corresponded with dreamed performance of the right hand. In this study left and right hand movements have been investigated in order to obtain further clarification on the issue of contralateral respectively bilateral activity over the motor areas for one-sided limb movements. METHODS Participants A volunteer male (27 years), trained in the method of lucid dreaming and able to carry out experiments in a LD (15), spent three nonconsecutive nights in the sleep laboratory. Experimental protocol Before the night in the sleep laboratory the participant received precise experimental instructions. The participant was asked to signal when he realized he was dreaming by means of a pair of left-right eye movement signals (LRLR) (19). Then he had to sit down in the dream and to carry out the clenching task. The experimental sequence can be summarized as follows: (1) LRLR eye signal to mark beginning of clenching the left hand; (2) open and clench the left hand four times; (3) LRLR eye signal to mark end of clenching the left hand and beginning of clenching the right hand; (4) open and clench the right hand four times; (5) LRLR eye signal to mark end of 183

Motor Area Activation During Lucid Dreaming clenching the right hand. As reference sequence the participant was asked to count in his dream from zero to four and mark again the beginning and end of this sequence by LRLR eye signals; during this period the participant was asked not to move in the dream. After finishing the complete task the participant had to wake himself up by focusing a spot in the dream (14) and record the dream experience. Data acquisition and analysis Standard polysomnography (21), 28-channel EEG (expanded 10-20 system), horizontal and vertical EOG and chin EMG were recorded. The electrode sites over the primary motor area C3, CZ, and C4 were considered for further data analysis. Impedance was kept below 5 kω. Data were sampled at 250 Hz, the time constant was set to DC (DC amplifiers and software by NeuroScan Inc., USA). Linked mastoid electrodes served as reference. For the analysis only correct eye signalled lucid dreams were taken into account. In two lucid dreams the dreamer carried out two sequences of left hand clenching, two sequences of right hand clenching and two sequences of counting in the dream. Each sequence lasted approximately 2 sec. Those sequences were segmented into 500 ms epochs so that for each condition (left hand clenching, right hand clenching, dream counting) eight epochs were available. The epochs were digitally filtered on the alpha band (8-12 Hz). Finally, power values for 125 data points of each epoch were calculated and averaged (for methodological details see 22). RESULTS In Figure 1, a correctly signalled lucid dream is depicted. In the corresponding dream report the participant described his experience with the hand clenching task as follows: I gave the LRLR signal and started clenching the left hand for 3-5 times, then another LRLR signal and right hand clenching (as practiced). I gave a final LRLR to signal the end of the task. (The performance of the last eye movement) is probably not very good. Figure 1. Recording of a correctly signaled lucid dream: Three clear LRLR eye movement signals are shown in the horizontal EOG. The participant reported that he had opened and clenched his left hand between the first and the second LRLR eye signal and his right hand between the second and the third LRLR eye signal for four to five times. 184

D. Erlacher et al. The data analysis of both lucid dreams provided averaged power values (µv 2 ) for the eight epochs per condition for each electrode site. The results depicted in figure 2 clearly reflect-on a descriptive level-the initial hypothesis; the averaged power values from the eight epochs of left hand clenching and right hand clenching are considerably smaller than those for dream counting. Table 1 shows the mean values and standard deviations for electrode sites C3, Cz, and C4 under the condition of left hand clenching, right hand clenching and dream counting. Statistical analysis supports the hypothesis that the averaged power values (µv 2 ) at the electrode sites over bilateral motor area (C3, Cz, C4) are significant smaller during hand clenching than during dream counting only for left hand clenching (C3: t(14)=-3.7, p=.002; Cz: t(14)= -3.1, p=.005; C4: t(14)=-3.1, p=.008). During right hand clenching the difference to dream counting failed to reach significance (C3: t(14)=-1.2, p=.124; Cz: t(14)=-1.3, p=.106; C4: t(14)=-1.6, p=.062); obviously due to two outliers in epoch six and seven (see Figure 2). DISCUSSION The results support the hypothesis that dreamed motor performance activates primary motor areas measured as a decrease in the EEG alpha band over electrode sites C3, Cz, and C4. Although the differences during right hand clenching failed to reach significance, on a descriptive level the averaged power values from the eight epochs of dream counting are mainly larger than those for right hand clenching except for two outliers in epoch six and seven. In contrast to the findings from Etevenon and Guillou (12), this study found bilateral activity for one-sided hand movements. Thus, it can be speculated that dreamed movements are more related to physical movements than to movement imagery (4,20); so that dreamed movements are essentially the same as in wakefulness, except for the spinal paralysis. In interpreting the present findings several methodological issues have to be considered: First, our data is based on one person succeeding in two lucid dreams and, thus, the findings Figure 2. Averaged power values of the alpha band (µv 2 ) derived over the motor areas (C3, Cz and C4). Table 1. Mean averaged power values in µv 2 and standard deviation for each condition over electrode sites C3, Cz, and C4; representing the primary motor area Electrode site C3 Cz C4 condition a M SD M SD M SD left hand clenching 2.1 0.5 4.5 1.93.0 1.0 right hand clenching 3.2 2.5 6.4 4.3 4.3 2.7 dream counting 4.4 1.7 9.1 3.9 6.7 3.2 a n=8 epochs for each condition 185

Motor Area Activation During Lucid Dreaming should be seen as preliminary. Second, dividing the sequences into several epochs might lead to epochs in which no clear movement occurred - especially at the beginning and end of a sequence when the participant gave LRLR eye signals and start/stop with the task. This problem can be solved by measuring additional left and right forearm EMG, matching with the EEG recording. The small twitches which can be measured by EMG (10) can be used to select start/end points of epochs. Third, some authors (23) conceive lucid dreams as too different from non-lucid dream cognition and question the generalizibility of the results on the mind-body interaction. Whereas the results concerning non-lucid dreams and physiology (8-12) contradict this critique, because the studies reported similar measurable responses to non-lucid dreamed activity as measured in the present study during lucid dreaming. To summarize, hand movements performed in a lucid dream are associated with corresponding activations of specific motor brain areas. A one-sided hand movement, left hand clenching, results in bilateral decrease of the EEG alpha band, C3, Cz, and C4. This supports the idea that dreamed movements are parallel to movements in waking. These preliminary findings need to be replicated in additional samples. The phenomena of lucid dreaming provides an important research paradigm for studying mind-body interactions. REFERENCES 1. Schredl M. Body-mind interaction: Dream content and REM sleep physiology. North American Journal of Psychology 2000;2:59-70. 2. Erlacher D. Techniktraining im Klartraum. Theoretische und empirische Annäherung an ein neues Feld der Bewegungswissenschaft, University of Heidelberg: Unpulished MA-thesis, 2000. 3. Roland PE, Larsen B, Lassen NA, Skinhøj E. Supplementary motor area and other cortical areas in organization of voluntary movements in man. Journal of Neurophysiology 1980;43:118-136. 4. Pfurtscheller G, Neuper Ch. Motor imagery activates primary sensorimotor area in humans. Neuroscience Letters 1997;239:65-68. 5. LaBerge S. Lucid dreaming: Evidence and methodology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2000;23:962-963. 6. Schenck CH, Scott R, Ettinger MG, Mahowald MW. Chronic behavioral disorders of human REM sleep: a new category of parasomnia. Sleep 1986;9:293-308. 7. Hobson JA, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R. Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behavioral & Brain Sciences 2000;23:793-842. 8. Wolpert EA. Studies in psychophysiology of dreams: II. an electromyographic study of dreaming. Archives of General Psychiatry 1960;2:231-241. 9. Grossman WI, Gardner R, Roffwarg HP, Fekek AF, Beers L, Weiner H. Relation of dreamed to actual movement. Psychophysiology 1972;9:118-119. 10. Gardner R, Grossman WI, Roffwarg HP, Weiner H. The relationship of small limb movements during REM sleep to dreamed limb action. Psychosomatic Medicine 1975;37:147-159. 11. Hong CC-H, Gillin JC, Dow BM, Wu J, Buchsbaum MS. Localized and lateralized cerebral glucose metabolism associate with eye movements during REM sleep and wakefulness: a positron emission tomography (PET) study. Sleep 1995;18:570-580. 12. Etevenon P, Guillou S. EEG cartography of a night of sleep and dreams: a longitudinal study with provoked awakenings. Neuropsychobiology 1986;16:146-151. 13. Pfurtscheller G. Event-related synchronization (ERS): an electrophysiological correlate of cortical areas at rest. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1992;83:62-69. 14. Tholey P, Utecht K. Schöpferisch Träumen. Der Klartraum als Lebenshilfe, Eschborn: Klotz, 1992. 15. LaBerge S, Rheingold H. Exploring the world of lucid dreaming, New York: Ballantine, 1990. 16. Hearne KMT. Lucid dreams: an electrophysiological and psychological study, University of Liverpool: Doctoral dissertation, 1978. 17. LaBerge S. Lucid dreaming: an exploratory study of consciousness during sleep, Stanford University: Doctoral dissertation, 1980. 18. LaBerge S. The psychophysiology of lucid dreaming. In: Gackenbach, J, LaBerge S, eds. Conscious mind, sleeping brain: Perspectives on lucid dreaming, New York: Plenum, 1988;135-153. 186

D. Erlacher et al. 19. LaBerge S, Nagel L, Dement W, Zarcone V. Lucid dreaming verified by volitional communication during REM sleep. Perceptual & Motor Skills 1981;52:727-732. 20. Pfurtscheller G, Berghold A. Patterns of cortical activation during planning of voluntary movement. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1989;72:250-258. 22. Pfurtscheller G. Graphical display and statistical evaluation of event-related desynchronization (ERD). Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1977;43:757-760. 23. Solms M. Forebrain mechanisms of dreaming are activated form a variety of sources. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2000;23:1035-1040. 21. Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques, and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects, Los Angeles: BIS/BRI, UCLA, 1968. 187