Event-related brain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries

Similar documents
Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

Effects of involuntary auditory attention on visual task performance and brain activity

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction

Electrophysiological evidence of enhanced distractibility in ADHD children

Title change detection system in the visu

Activation of the auditory pre-attentive change detection system by tone repetitions with fast stimulation rate

Temporal integration: intentional sound discrimination does not modulate stimulus-driven processes in auditory event synthesis

RAPID COMMUNICATION Scalp-Recorded Optical Signals Make Sound Processing in the Auditory Cortex Visible

Left-hemisphere dominance for processing of vowels: a whole-scalp neuromagnetic study

Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials

ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Synchronous cortical gamma-band activity in task-relevant cognition

Separate memory-related processing for auditory frequency and patterns

The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential

Auditory sensory memory in 2-year-old children: an event-related potential study

Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

The N1 hypothesis and irrelevant sound: evidence from token set size effects

Early Occurrence Of Auditory Change Detection In The Human Brain

An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm

The time-course of intermodal binding between seeing and hearing affective information

D. Debatisse, E. Fornari, E. Pralong, P. Maeder, H Foroglou, M.H Tetreault, J.G Villemure. NCH-UNN and Neuroradiology Dpt. CHUV Lausanne Switzerland

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY OF UNIMODAL AND AUDIOVISUAL SPEECH PERCEPTION


Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm

The EEG Analysis of Auditory Emotional Stimuli Perception in TBI Patients with Different SCG Score

When Loading Working Memory Reduces Distraction: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence from an Auditory Visual Distraction Paradigm

Primitive intelligence in the auditory cortex

Tilburg University. Published in: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. Publication date: Link to publication

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Atypical processing of prosodic changes in natural speech stimuli in school-age children with Asperger syndrome

Task-switching and memory retrieval processing: Electrophysiological evidence.

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Negative emotional context enhances auditory novelty processing

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

50 Hz Magnetic Field Exposure Influence on Human Performance and Psychophysiological Parameters: Two Double-Blind Experimental Studies

Development of infant mismatch responses to auditory pattern changes between 2 and 4 months old

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Functional Neuroanatomy and Traumatic Brain Injury The Frontal Lobes

What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3

fmri (functional MRI)

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

Neural correlates of perceptual separation-induced enhancement of prepulse inhibition of startle in humans

Ear and Hearing (In press, 2005)

Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

Stefan Debener a,b, *, Scott Makeig c, Arnaud Delorme c, Andreas K. Engel a,b. Research report

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fmri and ERP Results

AUDITORY ERPS IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL COORDINATION DISORDER

ANALYZING EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS

Age-Related Changes in Processing Auditory Stimuli During Visual Attention: Evidence for Deficits in Inhibitory Controls and Sensory Memory

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Head Injury: Classification Most Severe to Least Severe

EEG in the ICU: Part I

Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Design and ERP data: Basic concepts. Caitlin M. Hudac Graduate student, Developmental Brain Laboratory

Neural basis of auditory-induced shifts in visual time-order perception

Erratum to Age-related change in neural processing of time-dependent stimulus features [Cognit. Brain Res. 25 (2005) ]

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuroimage. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 March 1.

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

1. Processes nutrients and provides energy for the neuron to function; contains the cell's nucleus; also called the soma.

DETECTION OF CHANGE Event-Related Potential and jmri Findings

P300 differences between sinistrals and dextrals

Attention and prepulse inhibition: the effects of task-relevant, irrelevant, and no-task conditions

Processing Interaural Cues in Sound Segregation by Young and Middle-Aged Brains DOI: /jaaa

Adaptation of neuromagnetic N1 without shifts in dipolar orientation

Neuroscience Letters

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Human cortical functions in auditory change detection evaluated with multiple brain research methods

Material-speci c neural correlates of memory retrieval

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Part I. Traumatic Brain Injury: An Overview. Francesca A. LaVecchia, Ph.D.

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Neonatal EEG Maturation

Effects of dynamic rotation on event-related brain potentials

Magnetic stimulation at acupoints relieves mental fatigue: An Event Related Potential (P300) study

Effects of multiple congruent cues on concurrent sound segregation during passive and active listening: An event-related potential (ERP) study

Takwa Adly Gabr Assistant lecturer of Audiology

Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex

Event-related potential assessment of information processing after closed head injury

Supporting Information

Title Page. Altering the primacy bias How does a prior task affect mismatch negativity (MMN)?

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Occasional changes in sound location enhance middle latency evoked responses

Further characterization of subject s own name (SON) negativity, an ERP component reflecting early preattentive detection of SON

Cognition and Event -Related Pot en t ials

Visual activity in the human frontal eye eld

Transcription:

Neurophysiology 10, 2125±2129 (1999) EVENT-RELATED brain potentials (ERPs) to auditory stimuli were recorded from 11 closed head injured (CHI) and 10 age-matched healthy adults. Auditory stimuli consisted of sequences of repetitive standard tones (600 Hz), occasionally replaced by deviant tones (660 Hz) or by natural novel sounds. Subjects were instructed to ignore auditory stimuli while concentrating on a demanding visuo-motor tracking task. CHI patients showed, in comparison to control subjects, signi cantly enhanced late P3a component in the ERPs to novel sounds. This suggests that novel stimuli cause greater distraction in CHI patients than in controls, demonstrating that ERPs provide a powerful tool to determine the physiological basis of attentional de cits in CHI patients. 10:2125±2129 # 1999 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Key words: Attention; Audition; Closed head injury; Distractibility; Event-related brain potential (ERP); Mismatch negativity; Novel sounds; P3a Event-related brain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries Marja-Liisa Kaipio, 1,CA Kimmo Alho, 1,2 IstvaÂn Winkler, 1,3 Carles Escera, 2 Outi Surma-aho 1 and Risto NaÈaÈtaÈnen 1 1 Cognitive Brain Research Unit, Department of Psychology, PO Box 13, FIN-00014 University of Helsinki, Finland; 2 Neurodynamics Laboratory, Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, P. Vall d'hebron 171, E-08035 Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain; 3 Institute for Psychology, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, PO Box 398, H-1394 Budapest, Hungary CA,1 Corresponding Author and Address Introduction Closed head injuries (CHIs) occur as a consequence of sudden movements and stretching of the brain tissue within the skull by external acceleration± deceleration forces commonly resulting in a predictable focal injury in the frontal poles and in a diffuse axonal injury (DAI) [1]. DAI essentially reduces connections between the frontal and other brain areas. However, the pathophysiological consequences of CHI, for instance DAI in its whole extent, are not detected by brain imaging techniques such as CT, MRI and SPECT [2]. The typical symptoms accompanying CHI include disorders in planning, initiation of motor or cognitive actions, and altered social±emotional and behavioral functioning, as well as forgetfulness, fatigue, slowness of information processing, distractibility and other attentional de cits [3]. Adaptive and psycho-social functioning may be altered even in the absence of measurable intellectual decline with routine neurocognitive examination [4,5]. The de cits in attention and speed of information processing in CHI appear to be caused by frontal lesions and DAI [6]. These de cits are essential from 0959-4965 # Lippincott Williams & Wilkins the point of recovery and psychosocial outcome [6,7]. However, the nature of the attentional de cits in CHI still is unclear because of dif culties in their objective measurement [8,9]. Recordings of human brain activity have revealed that the prefrontal cortex is crucial in controlling voluntary and involuntary attention [10,11]. For example, event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to task-irrelevant novel stimuli show decreased P3a responses (a positive ERP peaking at about 250± 300 ms from stimulus onset with its maximum over the fronto-central scalp areas) in patients with dorsolateral prefrontal lesions, suggesting abnormal involuntary attention [11]. The P3a to novel sounds appears to have generator sources in several cortical areas, including prefrontal and auditory cortices [11±15]. The mismatch negativity (MMN), peaking over the frontal scalp areas 100±200 ms from stimulus onset is elicited by any discriminable deviant sounds occurring among repetitive standard stimuli and is probably associated with initiation of involuntary attention to auditory stimulus changes [10]. The MMN has its major generator sources bilaterally in the auditory cortices [15±18]. Since the MMN is Vol 10 No 10 13 July 1999 2125

elicited even by slightly deviant stimuli occurring in an unattended auditory input, it evidently indicates automatic stimulus discrimination occurring in the auditory cortex [10]. In addition, the MMN appears to have a further contribution from the frontal cortex, as indicated by scalp-current density analysis of ERPs [17] and neuromagnetic recordings [18]. This frontal cortex MMN activity might be associated with the initiation of involuntary switching of attention to acoustic changes [10,17]. The important role of prefrontal cortex in MMN elicitation is also indicated by MMN attenuation in patients with lesions in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [19,20]. Several studies have shown increased reaction times (RT) and decreased numbers of correct responses to auditory or visual target stimuli following MMN- and P3a-eliciting task-irrelevant deviant or novel sounds [21±23]. These ndings strongly support the role of MMN and P3a generators in the neural network of involuntary orienting of attention to changes occurring in the auditory environment outside the focus of attention. Therefore the purpose of the present study was to determine whether ERPs could be used to evaluate attentional problems of CHI patients, who are easily distracted and frequently complain about such problems. The present hypothesis was that MMN and P3a might indicate in CHI patients enhanced engagement of attention by changes in task-irrelevant sounds re ecting increased distractibility. Materials and Methods M.-L. Kaipio et al. ERPs were recorded in 11 male patients (Table 1) with chronic severe CHI (volunteers, age 23±47 years, mean 33 years) 1±3 years post-injury. Subjects with any history of psychiatric problems or alcohol or drug abuse were excluded. All patients were right-handed, had normal hearing, and normal or corrected-to-normal vision. In the patient group, the length of unconsciousness had varied between 10 min and 21 days, and the length of post-traumatic amnesia between 4 days and 3 months (Table 1). Structural changes detected by CT or MRI are described in Table 1. In two patients, no parenchymal changes were found on CT (Table 1). Since diffuse axonal injury, proportional to injury severity, seems to be a consistent feature of CHI, most patients can be suspected to have DAI. In every patient, typical neuropsychological de cits following severe CHI were reported, for example dif culties in attention, memory and behavioral changes. All patients were undergoing neuropsychological rehabilitation at the time of the study. All patients were psycho-socially independent, but only one of them (patient 7) was working on the previous level. The control group included 10 healthy men (age 22±41 years, mean 31 years). Subjects were presented with auditory stimulus sequences consisting of 600 Hz standard tones (probability of occurrence, p ˆ 0.85), 660 Hz deviant Table 1. Age, duration of unconsciousness (LOC), duration of post-traumatic amnesia (PTA), Glasgow Coma Score (GCS) at admission to the emergency and neuroradiological ndings (CTs, one MRI in patient 7) in the patient group Age LOC GCS PTA Radiological ndings 1. 33 5 days no data 3 weeks bifrontal contusion 2. 34 10 min 14 4 days bifrontal contusion 3. 47 6 h 7 3 weeks ventromedial bifrontal contusion (right,left) 4. 30 3 h 5 5 weeks DAI in ventricular caudate 5. 23 13 days 7 3 weeks some retained uid in sphenoidal sinus 6. 25 21 days 7 3 months no parenchymal changes in CT 7. 41 12 h 14 4 days ventromedial bifrontal contusion, extradural hematoma 8. 31 10 days 9 2 months frontal hygroma, slight focal change in corpus callosum 9. 33 2 h 10 1 week left temporal extradural hematoma 10. 35 3 days 9 1 month right temporal fracture and extradural hematoma 11. 27 5 days 9 1 month left frontal and temporal contusion, DAI in ventricular caudate DAI ˆ diffuse axonal injury. 2126 Vol 10 No 10 13 July 1999

ERPs and distractibility in closed head injuries tones ( p ˆ 0.075), and complex novel sounds ( p ˆ 0.075) delivered in a random order, except that each deviant tone and novel sound was preceded by at least one standard tone. In each stimulus sequence, 400 stimuli were delivered binaurally through the headphones at a constant rate of 1/ 900 ms. Thus, each sequence had a duration of 6 min. There were six auditory stimulus sequences. The stimulus delivery was controlled by Stim software (NeuroScan Ltd., USA). Standard and deviant tones had sinusoidal waveforms, an intensity of 75 db SPL at each ear, and duration of 200 ms including 10 ms rise and fall times. The novel sounds were drawn from a pool of 60 different digitized complex sounds (telephone ringing, electric drill, rain etc.) with a duration of 200 ms, including 10 ms rise and fall times, and a maximum intensity of 70± 80 db SPL at each ear. None of the novel sounds appeared twice within the same stimulus sequence. Subjects were instructed to ignore sounds and to perform a visuo-motor tracking task. The visual display consisted of ve small red circles moving on a yellow background with random speed and to random directions, one of them being a slightly differing target. Subjects were instructed to follow the target circle with a cursor circle controlled by a computer trackball manipulated by subjects with their right hand. The distance between the cursor and the target was continuously measured with a sampling rate of 500 Hz. The EEG (DC-100 Hz) was continuously sampled at a rate of 500 Hz with SynAmps and Scan software (Neuroscan Ltd., USA) from 21 scalp sites with Ag/ AgCl electrodes. Changes in electro-oculogram (EOG) due to eye movements or blinks were monitored with electrodes at the canthi of the right and left eye and at the forehead. All EEG and EOG electrodes were referred to an electrode attached to the tip of the nose. EEG epochs of 900 ms starting 100 ms before stimulus onset were averaged in each condition separately for the standard tones, deviant tones, and novel sounds. Epochs contaminated by ocular or muscle activity (EOG or EEG variation during an epoch exceeding 100 ìv) were rejected from averaging, as well as the epochs for the rst ve sounds of each sequence. Frequencies. 30 Hz were digitally ltered out from the averaged ERPs. The ERP amplitudes were measured in relation to the mean voltage during a 100 ms pre-stimulus baseline. The peak latencies and amplitudes of the centrally maximal N1 and P2 de ections elicited by standard tones were measured in each subject at the central midline electrode (Cz). The peak latency and amplitude of the frontally maximal MMN elicited by deviant tones were measured from the difference waves obtained for the frontal midline electrode () by subtracting, in each subject, the ERP to the standard tones from that to the deviant tones. The peak latencies and amplitudes of the negativity to novel sounds (consisting of N1 and MMN [15]) and of the successive centro-frontally maximal P3a were separately determined for each subject at Cz from the ERPs to novel sounds. Moreover, the amplitude of the later portion of the P3a, after the P3a peak, was measured at Cz as a mean amplitude of the novel-sound ERP over a period 350±450 ms from stimulus onset. The signi cance of the betweengroup differences in ERP amplitudes and latencies were studied with t-tests. Results The performance in the visual tracking task (the mean distance on the screen between the target object and the cursor) did not signi cantly differ between the groups (average distance 31.6 mm, s.d. 7.2 in the patients and 25.4 mm, s.d. 5.1 mm in the controls), t(19) ˆ 1.96, p, 0.07. There were no signi cant between-group differences in the N1 and P2 peak amplitudes and latencies for standard tones (Fig. 1; mean N1 peak latency and amplitude: patients 102 ms and 1.9 ìv; controls 105 ms and 1.9 ìv; mean P2 peak latency and amplitude: patients 169 ms and 0.9 ìv; controls 167 ms and 0.7 ìv). Moreover, there were no signi cant differences in the MMN latencies or amplitudes to deviant tones between CHI patients and control subjects (Fig. 1; mean MMN peak latency and amplitude: patients 126 ms and 4.0 ìv, controls 124 ms and 3.3 ìv). The ERPs to novel sounds (Fig. 2) showed no between-group differences in the peak latency and amplitude of the negativity composed by N1 and MMN (the mean peak amplitude and latency: patients 101.5 ms and 1.9 ìv; controls 105 ms and 1.6 ìv) or P3a (the mean P3a peak amplitude and latency: patients 224 ms and 5.5 ìv; controls 226 ms and 5.4 ìv). However, as shown in Fig. 2, the amplitude of the later portion of the P3a, measured as the mean amplitude at Cz over 350± 450 ms, was signi cantly larger in patients than in controls (mean 1.2 ìv vs 1.1 ìv; t(19) ˆ 2.64, p, 0.02). Discussion There were no signi cant differences between CHI patients and controls in the N1 or P2 amplitudes or latencies in this study. This suggests that the early cortical processing of auditory stimuli [10] was not affected in CHI patients. The MMN indicates Vol 10 No 10 13 July 1999 2127

M.-L. Kaipio et al. ERPs to deviant and standard tones during visual task standard deviant MMN MMN 400 ms 400 ms Controls Patients FIG. 1. Grand-average ERPs in the patient (right) and control (left) groups at frontal, central, and parietal scalp sites to the deviant (thick lines) and standard (thin lines) tones. Recordings are also shown for the channels used to monitor EOG (Fp1, Fp2,, ). Subjects concentrated on the visual tracking task. The mismatch negativity (MMN) to deviant tones, which was similarly elicited in both groups, is indicated by the shaded difference between the ERPs to deviant and standard tones. Stimulus onset is at 0 ms. patients contents ERPs to novel sounds during visual task performance P3a 500 ms FIG. 2. Grand-average ERPs in the patients (thick lines) and control subjects (thin lines) to the novel sounds. The later portion of P3a was larger in the patients than in the controls. For other details, see Fig. 1. sensory-memory related pre-attentive discrimination of auditory stimulus changes [10,24]. Since no signi cant difference was found in the MMN amplitudes or latencies between the groups pre-attentive auditory discrimination and sensory memory can be regarded as normal in the present patients. Interestingly, previous studies indicate that in focal dorsolateral prefrontal lesions the MMN amplitude is attenuated [19,20]. This difference in results may be explained such that mechanical forces causing frontal and temporal injuries and diffuse axonal injury in CHI [3] do not necessarily affect the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The main result of the present study was that the later portion of P3a was enhanced in patients as compared with controls. P3a is probably associated with involuntary switching of attention to stimulus changes, its earlier portion being generated at least partly in the auditory cortex [15,23] and its later portion mainly in the frontal cortex attention mechanisms [11,14]. The increased later portion of the P3a component to novel sounds among unattended auditory stimuli in the present CHI patients suggests enhanced processing of distracting sounds [7]. This could indicate stronger involuntary switching of attention in the patient group because of their measured dif culties in suppressing of irrelevant stimuli. 2128 Vol 10 No 10 13 July 1999

ERPs and distractibility in closed head injuries Conclusion The results of the present study are in concordance with previous studies. Attentional de cits form the basic problem in the sequelae of CHI and may lead to devastating dysfunctions. ERPs appear to provide a powerful tool for indicating these kinds of attentional problems. Thus measuring attentional de cits with ERPs might provide an additional method for making diagnostics as well as following recovery and rehabilitation. Further studies are needed to construct optimal ERP paradigms to reveal the exact nature of the attentional de cit in this patient group. Speci c ERP paradigms might also be used for clinical purposes even when the related lesions are not detectable in CT or MRI scanning or suggested by neurobehavioral tests assessing cognitive functioning. References 1. Levin HS. Predicting the neurobehavioral sequelae of closed head injury. In: Wood RL, ed. Neurobehavioral Sequelae of Traumatic Brain Injury. London: Taylor and Francis, 1990: 89±109. 2. Ichise M, Chung D-G, Wang P et al. J Nucl Med 35, 217±226 (1994). 3. Richardson JTE. Clinical and Neuropsychological Aspects of Closed Head Injury. London: Taylor and Francis, 1990. 4. Damasio A. Descartes' error. Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York: Avon Books, 1994. 5. Dywan J and Segalowitz SJ. J Head Trauma Rehabil 1, 79±95 (1996). 6. van Zomeren AH and Brouwer WH. Closed head injury. In van Zomeren AH and Brouwer WH, eds. Clinical Neuropsychology of Attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1994: 63±94. 7. Campbell KB and Duncan R de Lugt. Event related potential measures of cognitive de cits following closed head injury. In: Boller F and Grafman J, eds. Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 10. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science B.V., 1995: 269±297. 8. Mateer CA and Mapou RL. J Head Trauma Rehabil 11, 1±16 (1996). 9. Schmitter-Edgecombe M. J Head Trauma Rehabil 11, 17±30 (1996). 10. NaÈaÈtaÈnen R. Attention and Brain Function. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1992. 11. Knight RT. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 59, 9±20 (1984). 12. Knight RT, Scabini D, Woods DL and Clayworth C. Brain Res 502, 109±116 (1989). 13. Halgren E, Baudena P, Clarke JM et al. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 94, 191±220 (1995). 14. Baudena P, Halgren E, Heit G and Clarke JM. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 94, 251±264 (1995). 15. Alho K, Winkler I, Escera C et al. Psychophysiology 35, 211±224 (1998). 16. Scherg M, Vajsar J and Picton T. J Cogn Neurosci 1, 336±355 (1989). 17. Giard MH, Perrin F, Pernier J and Bouchet P. Psychophysiology 27, 627±640 (1990). 18. LevaÈnen S, Ahonen A, Hari R et al. Cerebr Cortex 6, 288±296 (1996). 19. Alho K, Woods DL, Algazi A et al. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 91, 353±362 (1994). 20. Alain C, Woods DL and Knight RT. Brain Res 812, 23±37 (1998). 21. SchroÈger E. J Cogn Neurosci 8, 527±539 (1996). 22. Alho K, Escera C, Diaz R et al. 8, 3233±3237 (1997). 23. Escera C, Alho K, Winkler I and NaÈaÈtaÈnen R. J Cogn Neurosci 10, 590±604 (1998). 24. NaÈaÈtaÈnen R. Ear Hear 16, 6±18 (1995). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS: This research was supported by The Academy of Finland, The National Scienti c Research Fund of Hungary (OTKA TO22681), and the Spanish Ministry of Education and Culture (DGES UE96-0038). The authors wish to thank Ms Elena Yago and Dr Marie Cheour for their constructive comments during preparation of this report. Received 12 April 1999; accepted 5 May 1999 Vol 10 No 10 13 July 1999 2129