Emotion Regulation: Cognitive and Affective Consequences for Fund-Raising Advertisements

Similar documents
Escaping Affect: How Motivated Emotion Regulation Creates Insensitivity to Mass Suffering

Donate Different: External and Internal Influences on Emotion-Based Donation Decisions

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

Ackoff Doctoral Student Fellowship: 2013 Application

academic insights: designed

Running Head: DISSOCIABLE EMOTION REGULATION STRATEGIES. Divergent Cognitive Costs for Online Forms of Reappraisal and Distraction

What is a choice point?

International Journal of Educational Advancement (2011) 10, doi: /ijea

Academic year Lecture 16 Emotions LECTURE 16 EMOTIONS

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

Divergent Cognitive Costs for Online Forms of Reappraisal and Distraction

ESCAPING AFFECT: HOW MOTIVATED EMOTION REGULATION DRIVES THE COLLAPSE OF COMPASSION. C. Daryl Cameron

internal information search consumer behavior external information search consumer decision-making process nonmarketing-controlled information source

Emotion Regulation Strategy, Emotional Contagion and Their Effects on Individual Creativity: ICT Company Case in South Korea

Slide 3.1. Learning and memory

Emotion Regulation in Adulthood: Timing Is Everything. James J. Gross 1

Chapter 3 Learning and Memory

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ABILITY TO IDENTIFY AND MANAGE ONE S OWN AND OTHERS EMOTIONS. Report for Lucas Sample ID UH Date April 02, 2013

Look to see if they can focus on compassionate attention, compassionate thinking and compassionate behaviour. This is how the person brings their

The Learning Process. Learning is a Process. Behavioral Learning Theories. Chapter 3 Learning and Memory. How many of these do you remind?

CONCEPT OF PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR

Last month I reviewed the behavior-based principles of safety self-management.

Compassion Fade: Affect and Charity Are Greatest for a Single Child in Need

Stress & Health. } This section covers: The definition of stress Measuring stress

Services Marketing Chapter 10: Crafting the Service Environment

INTERCULTURAL COMMUNICATION

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

Study Unit 3 -Part 2. Consumer Learning SIM University. All rights reserved. Introduction. In this presentation, you will learn:

Silvia Grappi Department of Economics, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy

The Counselor as a Human Being: A Personal Perspective

Loving-kindness Meditation. Increases. Social Connectedness. Eileen R. Snyder. Article Review #4. PSYCH 214 Abnormal Psychology.

Empowering and Enhancing Exposure Through Compassion, Acceptance and Mindfulness

Assessing Readiness To Change

Emotion Regulation Choice among Undergraduate Students with Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms

Knowledge of the basic assumptions and principles of PCE counselling

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use

PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions

Emotional Intelligence

Emotional Quotient. Bernd Mustermann 1/2/2013

Marketing Religion DAIANA KAPLAN

The Effect of Training Context on Fixations Made During Visual Discriminations

THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION

Feel appeal: Using emojis to capture and quantify sympathetic sentiment October 2018

The Impact of Visualization and Expectation on Tourists Emotion and Satisfaction at the Destination

The Motivational Benefit of Emotion: How to get more engagement for safety

The Importance of the Mind for Understanding How Emotions Are

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels

The Science of Legacy Fundraising (Behavioural Economics) IOF Legacy Summit September 2015

Understanding Your Coding Feedback

24/10/13. Surprisingly little evidence that: sex offenders have enduring empathy deficits empathy interventions result in reduced reoffending.

Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel. University of Miami. Jonah Berger. University of Pennsylvania

No Evidence for Persuasion Effects of Emotional Advertisement on Attitudes towards Social Groups The Case of Ageism

Anti-Smoking Advertising and Youth Smoking

Consumer Behavior, Ninth Edition. Schiffman & Kanuk

Professor Tony Ward. Empathy, altruism and the treatment of sex offenders.

April A. Working with Individuals at risk for Suicide: Attitudes and Approach

Country of Origin Effects: Consumer Perceptions of Japan in South East Asia. Durairaj Maheswaran. Working Paper N-006

Health & Wellness Coach Certifying Examination CONTENT OUTLINE

The Co-Occurring Disorders Treatment Program

8/17/2012. Self-Concept Video. Cultural Differences in Defining the Self. Chapter 5. The Self: Understanding Ourselves in a Social Context

Proposal for Eat Wise, Win Prize Campaign. According to WHO, the major causes to death are heart disease, stroke, and lower respiratory infections.

CONSUMER BEHAVIOR UNIT II

A Commentary on The Senses in Anthropological and Marketing Research: Investigating a Consumer-Brand Ritual Holistically

Lecture 9. Control and Personality. Professor Ian Robertson

The Unique Psychological World of Lawyers

Are Impulsive buying and brand switching satisfactory and emotional?

Stress, Health, & Coping. Radwan Banimustafa MD

Decision Making CHAPTER 11 DONELSON FORSYTH. Groups have great strength but their limitations can only be Ignored at great risk

Chapter 6. Learning Objectives. Analyzing Consumer Markets. buying behavior? consumer responses to the marketing program? decisions?

Empirical testing of evolutionary hypotheses has used to test many theories both directly and indirectly. Why do empirical testing?

Denise J. Larsen, Ph.D., R. Psych. Kristine Iaboni, M.A. University of Alberta. Professor and Associate Dean University of Alberta

Self-Observation of Pain Triggers Exercise Instructions

Simple Pure Whole TM Wellness

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice.

autonomic ne rvous system The autonomic ne rvous system The autonomic ne rvous system Pa rasympathetic Sympathetic

CALL FOR PAPERS: EMBEDDING THE CONCEPT OF SUSPICION IN RESEARCH ON BUSINESS AND APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY. Guest Co-Editors

Howard Sheth Model. The model claims that a person s purchase decision is often influenced by more than one individuals.

K I N G. mentally ill E N. 38 myevt.com exceptional veterinary team March/April 2012

Celebrity Endorsement: The Effects of Social Comparisons on Women s Self-Esteem and Purchase Intensions

Artificial Emotions to Assist Social Coordination in HRI

Why Do People Give? The Role of Identity in Giving

Moods and Their Relevance to Systems Usage Models within Organizations: An Extended Framework

Emotion Coaching. A tool to help you to work successfully with young people with SEN&D

THE CUSTOMER SERVICE ATTRIBUTE INDEX

Chapter 7 Consumer Learning. Consumer Behavior, Ninth Edition. Schiffman & Kanuk. Chapter Outline

Perception and Processing of Safe Driving Messages: A Multi-Method Approach

UNDERSTANDING GIVING: ACROSS GENERATIONS

STRESSED? What is Stress? What is Stress? Healthy Stress and the Biology of Stress

Durham Research Online

TTI Success Insights Emotional Quotient Version

W. Brooke Elliott 2012 Audit Symposium

2011 FEP Donor Retention Supplement November 17, 2011

Why People Hate Health Economics Two Psychological Explanations

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. smoking causes cancer, heart related diseases, impotent, and serious damage to unborn

Master's Thesis. Present to

Applied Social Psychology Msc.

Learning Outcomes. To understand what we mean by the terms validation and reality orientation.

Emotional Quotient. Megan Nice. Owner Sample Co Your Address Here Your Phone Number Here Your Address Here

4/9/2012. Happiness & Positive Emotion. Making choices choose what makes you happy

Emotion Regulation During Relationship Conflict. Shiri Ben-Naim Gilad Hirschberger Tsachi Ein-Dor Mario Mikulincer

Transcription:

1 Emotion Regulation: Cognitive and Affective Consequences for Fund-Raising Advertisements Nonprofit organizations are seen as warm, whereas for-profit organizations are perceived as highly competent. This research proposed that inducing people to think about warmth-related stereotypes of nonprofit organizations activates an affective mind-set. However, inducing people to think about competence-related stereotypes of for-profit organizations activates a cognitive mind-set. These mind-sets have an impact on choices of emotion regulation strategy. When an affective mind-set is activated in the nonprofit domain, people are more likely to regulate their emotions toward high emotion-eliciting situations (e.g., multiple sad-faced victims) than low emotion-soliciting situations (e.g., a single sad-faced victim) by means of reappraisal. In contrast, when a cognitive mind-set is activated in the for-profit domain, people are more likely to inhibit their ongoing emotions by means of suppression, and they are largely insensitive to variations in the number of victims. Since tendencies to suppress emotional responses consume a continual outlay of attentional resources from working memory, people engaged in feeling suppression in the for-profit domain are less to recall information about a fund-raising advertisement than those who regulate their emotions by forms of reappraisal, and consequent decreases in the effectiveness of the CSR strategy in corporate image building. Keywords: emotion regulation, corporate social responsibility, stereotype, warm, competent

2 Emotion Regulation: Cognitive and Affective Consequences for Fund-Raising Advertisements Nonprofit and for-profit organizations engage in charity fund-raising campaigns in various forms and for different reasons. While consumers may perceive fund-raising campaigns organized by nonprofit organizations as warm and altruistic, they may attribute at least some intent of for-profits engaged in fund-raising campaigns to corporate self-interest, such as generating a positive corporate image by communicating itself as socially responsible (Dean, 2004; Sen & Bhattacharya, 2001). These stereotypes about nonprofit and for-profit organizations lead to an important question: Do people experience different emotions when viewing fund-raising advertisements of nonprofit versus for-profit organizations? If so, how would such emotions influence people to construe the fund-raising information? Furthermore, what are the likely downstream consequences on corporate image building? Emotion Regulation The literature on emotion regulation provides important insights into responses to fund-raising advertisements launched by nonprofit and for-profit organizations. Fund-raising advertisements display photographs of sad-faced victims to evoke the sympathy and compassion that engender giving. Interestingly, recent research on emotion regulation has demonstrated that people do not necessarily display more compassion or give more to charity when the number of sad-faced victims shown in the fund-raising advertisement increases (Cameron & Payne, 2011; Kogut & Ritov, 2005; Slovic, 2007). The reason is that people very often seek to control, avoid, and even redirect unwanted emotions to prevent being overwhelmed emotionally (Koole, 2009). Helping multiple sad-faced victims is anticipated to be more costly than helping a single sad-faced victim, thus spontaneously leading individuals to regulate their emotions in response to multiple sad-faced victims. However, what remains unknown is how the number of sad-faced victims displayed in fund-raising advertisements works, particularly in the nonprofit and for-profit domains. Affective and Cognitive Mind-sets This research examines how stereotypes about nonprofit and for-profit organizations influence the ways in which consumers construe imagery of sad-faced victims. In particular, we propose that stereotypes about nonprofit and for-profit organization give rise to two types of mind-set: affective and cognitive. We argue that because viewing fund-raising advertisements of nonprofit organization increases the accessibility of affective-related stereotypes, such as altruistic and warm associated with nonprofit organization (Aaker, Vohs, & Mogilner, 2010), consumers are more likely to rely on spontaneous affective reactions as a basis of judgment, giving rise to an affective mind-set. However, fund-raising advertisements of for-profit organizations closely tied to corporate self-interest may cue the retrieval of concepts such as profit-making and competent (Aaker et al., 2010). Consumers may easily refute the validity of the solicitations and calculate the expected

3 benefits of their donation, thus giving rise to a cognitive mind-set (Xu & Wyer, 2008). Affective and Cognitive Consequences We postulate that the existence of an affective (cognitive) mind-set, once activated in the course of viewing fund-raising information in a nonprofit (for-profit) domain, persists to influence use of emotion regulation strategies when viewing photographs of sad-faced victims. When an affective mind-set is activated in the nonprofit domain, people are highly sensitive with regard to emotion. To avoid becoming emotionally overwhelmed, individuals who are expected to view imagery of multiple sad-faced victims may reappraise the situation by quickly shifting attention away from the upsetting fund-raising advertisement, thus leading to a decrease in their emotional experience. When a cognitive mind-set is activated in the for-profit domain, people wish to function at their best cognitively; therefore, though anticipating that viewing imagery of multiple sad-faced victims will be emotion-eliciting, emotion regulation occurs only after an emotion has been generated. In other words, consumers regulate their emotions by means of suppression to preserve cognitive functioning. The major difference between the two emotion regulation strategies is that reappraisal is an antecedent-focused strategy, and people attempt to divert their attention away from an emotion-arousing stimulus before the emotion response tendencies have become fully activated. In contrast, suppression is considered a response-focused strategy (Gross 1998a, 1998b). However, regulating emotions by suppression at the same time requires continual self-monitoring, which depletes the cognitive resources available for careful processing of fund-raising information (Gross, 2002; Qiu, Lee, & Yeung, 2009; Richards & Gross, 2000). If this is the case, we expected that consumers who suppress their negative emotion when that emotion is already underway in the for-profit domain experience no difference in negative emotion for multiple victims compared to a single victim, but actually impair their ability to recall details of the for-profit fund-raising advertisement. Methodology We plan to recruit 100 undergraduate students for this study. Participants will be assigned to one of the four donation conditions, comprising the 2 (number of victims: one vs. eight) x 2 (type of organization: nonprofit vs. for-profit) between-subjects design. Participants will receive a folder containing a fund-raising advertisement and the measures. In the fund-raising advertisement, half the participants will be told that a nonprofit organization is the fund-raiser for a children s cancer fund-raising project; the other half will be told that an investment corporation is the fund-raiser. Next, participants will view an image of one child in the single victim condition and eight separate child images in the multiple children condition. Before viewing the image, participants will be warned that they will be viewing an image of a single (multiple) sad-faced victims and asked to rate their emotions toward the child (children). Expected Findings We expect that participants in the nonprofit condition will report greater compassion

4 toward a single victim than toward multiple victims, whereas the number of victims should have no significant impact for participants in the for-profit condition. To provide further evidence for the emotion regulation mechanism, we will perform a memory check on participants ability to recall advertisement information. Prior research has demonstrated that suppression indeed impairs memory (Gross, 2002; Richards & Gross, 2000). When viewing the fund-raising advertisement of a for-profit organization, suppression participants should exhibit a poorer memory of the advertisement than those who engage in reappraisal when viewing the fund-raising advertisement of a nonprofit organization. Theoretical Contributions The first contribution of this research is to specify that the relationship between the number of victims and people s emotional reactions depends on the type of organization soliciting support. We postulate that viewing fund-raising advertisements of nonprofit organizations activates an affective mind-set, whereas those for for-profit organizations activate a cognitive mind-set. Consequently, once consumers foresee an emotion-eliciting situation (viewing multiple photographs of sad-faced victims), an affective mind-set triggers them to regulate their emotions by reappraisal so as to avoid being emotionally overwhelmed. However, when a cognitive mind-set is activated, consumers are more likely to rely on situational information rather than affective reactions in making judgments. This leads them to contend with the emotions by means of suppression. To our knowledge, this research is the first to demonstrate that activating the stereotypes of nonprofit and for-profit organizations will heighten the accessibility of an affective and a cognitive mind-set, which influences the subsequent emotion regulation strategies that people choose to use in a later stage. The second contribution is to advanced the literature on emotion regulation by examining what determines how people regulate their emotion when viewing images of sad-faced victims in fund-raising advertisements. More importantly, when will it lead to reappraisal and suppression? Previous research on emotion regulation has shown that individuals differ in their use of two common emotion regulation strategies: reappraisal and suppression (Gross & John, 2003). Although research has identified that reappraisal may successfully down-regulate negative emotion and suppression may not be helpful in decreasing negative emotional experience, the relevant studies do not paint a clear picture of when these efforts lead consumers to reappraise and suppress their negative emotion (Gross, 1998a, 1998b; Gross & John, 2003; Sheppes, Scheibe, Suri, & Gross, 2011).

5 References Aaker, J., Vohs, K. D., & Mogilner, C. (2010). Nonprofits are seen as warm and for-profits as competent: Firm stereotypes matter. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 224-236. Cameron D. C., & Payne, B. K. (2011). Escaping affect: How motivated emotion regulation creates insensitivity to mass suffering. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 1-15. Dean, D. H. (2004). Consumer perception of corporate donations: Effects of company reputation for social responsibility and type of donation. Journal of Advertising, 32, 91-102. Gross, J. J. (1998a). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271-299. Gross, J. J. (1998b). Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and psychology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224-237. Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39, 281-291. Gross, J. J. & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348-362. Hsee, C. K., & Rottenstreich, Y. (2004). Music, Pandas, and Muggers: On the affective psychology of value. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 133, 23-30. Kogut, T., & Ritov, I. (2005). The identified victim effect: An identified group, or just a single individual? Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 18, 157-167. Koole, S. L. (2009). The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 4-41. Qiu, C., Lee, Y. H., & Yeung, C. W. M. (2009). Suppressing feelings: A double-edged sword to consumer judgement and choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19, 407-439. Richards, J. M., & Gross. J.J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one s cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 410-424. Sen, S., & Bhattacharya, C.B. (2001). Does doing good always lead to doing better? Consumer reactions to corporate social responsibility. Journal of Marketing Research, 38, 225-243. Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J.J. (2011). Emotion regulation choice. Psychological Science, 22 (11), 1-6. Slovic, P. (2007). If I look at the mass I will never act: Psychic numbering and genocide. Judgment and Decision Making, 2, 79-95. Xu, A. J., & Wyer, R. S. (2008). The comparative mind-set: From animals comparisons to increased purchase intentions, Psychological Science, 19, 859-864.