The Formation of Affective Judgments: The Cognitive-Affective Model Versus the Independence Hypothesis

Similar documents
Consumer Behavior, Ninth Edition. Schiffman & Kanuk

Language Speech. Speech is the preferred modality for language.

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

REFERENCES. Hemispheric Processing Asymmetries: Implications for Memory

2. Which of the following is not an element of McGuire s chain of cognitive responses model? a. Attention b. Comprehension c. Yielding d.

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

Cognitive Authority. Soo Young Rieh. School of Information. University of Michigan.

Deception Detection Accuracy Using Verbal or Nonverbal Cues

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

A framework for predicting item difficulty in reading tests

Source memory and the picture superiority effect

The Learning Process. Learning is a Process. Behavioral Learning Theories. Chapter 3 Learning and Memory. How many of these do you remind?

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

FUNCTIONAL CONSISTENCY IN THE FACE OF TOPOGRAPHICAL CHANGE IN ARTICULATED THOUGHTS Kennon Kashima

Auditory Processing. Teach Inclusive Chapter 4, March Catherine Silcock, Speech Pathologist

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

Separate What and Where Decision Mechanisms In Processing a Dichotic Tonal Sequence

A Basic Study on possibility to improve stage acoustics by active method

MY RIGHT I: DECEPTION DETECTION AND HEMISPHERIC DIFFERENCES IN SELF-AWARENESS

Study Unit 3 -Part 2. Consumer Learning SIM University. All rights reserved. Introduction. In this presentation, you will learn:

Advances in Consumer Research Volume 12, 1985 Pages THE ELABORATION LIKELIHOOD MODEL: LIMITATIONS AND EXTENSIONS IN MARKETING

VALUES AND ENTREPRENEURIAL ATTITUDE AS PREDICTORS OF NASCENT ENTREPRENEUR INTENTIONS

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

Music and Emotional Expression in Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Thousand Oaks High School. April 20, 2017.

Module 3 - Scientific Method

Quantum Mind Booster User Guide

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping

Highlighting Effect: The Function of Rebuttals in Written Argument

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Technical Specifications

Dichotic Word Recognition of Young Adults in Noise. A Senior Honors Thesis

3/23/2017 ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT NEEDS OF THE INDIVIDUAL WITH A TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY: A SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGIST S PERSPECTIVE

Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification. and. Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect. Nila K Leigh

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

1.1 FEATURES OF THOUGHT

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Implicit Attitude. Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia. Mahzarin R. Banaji. Harvard University

Impression order effects as a function of the personal relevance of the object of description*

Best on the Left or on the Right in a Likert Scale

04/12/2014. Research Methods in Psychology. Chapter 6: Independent Groups Designs. What is your ideas? Testing

Qualitative Data Analysis. Richard Boateng, PhD. Arguments with Qualitative Data. Office: UGBS RT18 (rooftop)

OF NON-FOCAL EXPOSURE

No Evidence for Persuasion Effects of Emotional Advertisement on Attitudes towards Social Groups The Case of Ageism

Survey Research. We can learn a lot simply by asking people what we want to know... THE PREVALENCE OF SURVEYS IN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH

PELLISSIPPI STATE COMMUNITY COLLEGE MASTER SYLLABUS SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY PSYC 2120

The Ordinal Nature of Emotions. Georgios N. Yannakakis, Roddy Cowie and Carlos Busso

UPRIGHT AND INVERTED FACES: THE RIGHT HEMISPHERE KNOWS THE DIFFERENCE!

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening

THE INFLUENCE OF UNCONSCIOUS NEEDS ON COLLEGE PROGRAM CHOICE

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information on Consumer Elaboration and Brand Attitude

CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT

Neurobiology and Information Processing Theory: the science behind education

Illusory Correlation and Group Impression Formation in Young and Older Adults

UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS LIBRARY AT URBANA-CHAMPAIGN BOOKSTACKS

The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance

Dichotic Word Recognition in Young Adults with Simulated Hearing Loss. A Senior Honors Thesis

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Cognitive tuning, encoding, and the attribution of causality

Dichotic Word Recognition for Young Adults with Normal Hearing. A Senior Honors Thesis

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

ID# Exam 3 PS 217, Spring 2009 (You must use your official student ID)

There are often questions and, sometimes, confusion when looking at services to a child who is deaf or hard of hearing. Because very young children

Satiation in name and face recognition

THE EFFECT OF A REMINDER STIMULUS ON THE DECISION STRATEGY ADOPTED IN THE TWO-ALTERNATIVE FORCED-CHOICE PROCEDURE.

PSYCHOLOGY TSAP COMPETENCY MAPPING 1. Knowledge Base in Psychology

Selective attention and immediate memory in dichotic listening

Binaural Versus Monaural Listening in Young Adults in Competing Environments. A Senior Honors Thesis

Slide 3.1. Learning and memory

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

ABSTRACT. Professor Dr. Rebecca Hamilton and Professor Dr. Joydeep Srivastava, Department of Marketing

Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture Fragment Completion Paradigm

PSYCHOLOGY IAS MAINS: QUESTIONS TREND ANALYSIS

CONTEXTUAL CHANGE EFFECTS IN RETROSPECTIVE AND PROSPECTIVE DURATION JUDGMENT PARADIGMS USING MUSICAL PIECES. Breanne Kay Smith

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

Chapter 6: Learning The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

What is Psychology? chapter 1

8 th Grade Novel Study: Touching Spirit Bear

INTRODUCTION Use of images in multiple-choice tasks is common in linguistic comprehension

Controlled Experiments

What's in a face? FACIAL EXPRESSIONS. Do facial expressions reflect inner feelings? Or are they social devices for influencing others?

Depressed mood effects on processing of highand low content structure text in American and Australian college women

A Sound Idea: A Theory-Based Synthesis and Explanadum Supporting the Use of Music in Marketing Strategy

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

Dichotic listening in schizotypal personality disorder: Evidence for gender and laterality effects

APPLICATION OF SIMULATION TO STUDY DECISION MAKING IN HANDBALL

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

LESSON OBJECTIVES LEVEL MEASURE

Effects of "Mere Exposure" on Learning and Affect

Examining Effective Navigational Learning Strategies for the Visually Impaired

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

Transcription:

University of Portland Pilot Scholars Business Faculty Publications and Presentations Pamplin School of Business 12-1988 The Formation of Affective Judgments: The Cognitive-Affective Model Versus the Independence Hypothesis Punam Anand Morris B. Holbrook Debra Stephens University of Portland, stephens@up.edu Follow this and additional works at: http://pilotscholars.up.edu/bus_facpubs Citation: Pilot Scholars Version (Modified MLA Style) Anand, Punam; Holbrook, Morris B.; and Stephens, Debra, "The Formation of Affective Judgments: The Cognitive-Affective Model Versus the Independence Hypothesis" (1988). Business Faculty Publications and Presentations. 1. http://pilotscholars.up.edu/bus_facpubs/1 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Pamplin School of Business at Pilot Scholars. It has been accepted for inclusion in Business Faculty Publications and Presentations by an authorized administrator of Pilot Scholars. For more information, please contact library@up.edu.

The Formation of Affective Judgments: The Cognitive-Affective Modei Versus the independence Hypothesis PUNAM ANAND MORRIS B. HOLBROOK DEBRA STEPHENS* A dichotic listening task within the context of hemispheric specialization provides evidence for enhanced affective responses toward con-ectly recognized stimuli and toward words transmitted to the right ear and music trevismitted to the left ear. These findings appear to support the cognitive-affective model over the independence hypothesis. Affective judgments are a key part of many everyday consumption experiences. Typically, consumer researchers have regarded such judgments according to a cognitive-affective model in which affective responses are the last step in a series of cognitive processes. Earlier steps in the model include the registration of stimuli and the retrieval of meaningful information via evaluative assessment (Fishbein and Ajzen 1975) or cognitive elaboration (Calder and Sternthal 1980). Further, communication strategies in accord with this cognitive-affective model often aim to teach consumers information about a brand and to evaluate the effectiveness of that advertising by viewer recall or message recognition. Two prominent psychologists with very different views challenge the pervasive assumption on which such practices rest. Zajonc and his colleagues (1980, 1982, 1984) contend that, according to the independence hypothesis, affect and cognition involve separate and partially independent systems; affective responses, therefore, do not depend on prior cognitions. In contrast, Lazarus (1982, 1984) vigorously defends the cognitive-affective model by arguing that affect depends on cognition that may occur at the unconscious level (cf. Tsal 1985). * Punam Anand is Assistant Professor and Morris B. Holbrook is Professor, both at the Graduate School of Business, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027. Debra Stephens is Assistant Professor, School of Business, University of Maryland, College Park. MD 20742. The second author gratefully acknowledges the support of the Columbia Business School's Faculty Research Fund. A longer, more detailed version of this article may be obtained by writing to thefirstauthor. 386 Given this debate, specification of the conditions that mediate the relationship between cognitive appraisal and affective response is of fundamental importance. Toward that end, in the present study we seek evidence for cognitive mediation in the formation of affect. First, we examine the conditions that produce the independence phenomenon via what we believe is an artifact involving the type of stimuli used in previous studies. Second, we suggest some factors associated with hemispheric specialization that should interact with these conditions. Third, we test the view that the extent of cognitive processing determines the level of affect by selecting laterality-related variables that enhance or undermine cognitive activity (i.e., words versus music presented in different ears). We expect this test to support the cognitiveaffective model over the independence hypothesis. CONDITIONS FAVORING THE COMPETING HYPOTHESES Supporters ofthe independence phenomenon marshal! evidence from two primary sources: (1) studies indicating that in the absence of recognition, preferences are enhanced for previously shown stimuli (Wilson 1979) and (2) studies suggesting that regardless of the extent of processing, stimuli presented to the left hemisphere are liked better than those presented to the right hemisphere (Seamon, Brody, and Kauff 1983). We refer to these two main effects as the objective familiarity effect and the lateralized valence effect, respectively. JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH Vol. 15 December 1988

THE FORMATION OF AFFECTIVE JUDGMENTS 387 Objective Familiarity Effect In a study by Wilson (1979), subjects performed a dichotic listening task during which they tracked a verbal passage presented in one ear while a series of musical tones was presented in the other ear. The findings indicated a more positive affect toward previously heard tones than toward new ones, despite only chance recognition of the objectively familiar tones. Wilson interpreted these findings as a support for the independence hypothesis. That affective evaluations are not so much independent of cognition as they are mediated by the level of uncertainty in recognition has been suggested in Other studies. Obermiller (1985) partially supports this theofy. By varying processing demands (minimal, structural, and elaborated) and the number of exposures to pairs of simple melodies, Obermiller showed that confidence and affective evaluation were enhanced when subjects correctly identified previously heard stimuli as familiar. These findings do not provide unequivocal support for the accuracy explanation, however, because processing demands may have enhanced evaluations (Obermiller 1985). In the present study, processing demands are held constant and the number of exposures (one or two) is consistent with previous studies examining the independence phenomenon. If our results indicate a pure main effect of objective familiarity that is, if previously shown stimuli are liked better than unfamiliar stimuli, regardless of recognition accuracy then we will obtain support for the independence hypothesis. By contrast, we will obtain support for the cognitiveaffective model if there is a significant interaction between objective and subjective familiarity such that an increase in recognition accuracy is accompanied by more positive affective responses. Lateralized Valence Effect Zajonc et al. (1982) claim neurological support for the independence phenomenon on the grounds that cognitive and affective responses engage different hemispheres ofthe brain. In a study that supports this theory, Seamon et al. (1983) presented visual shapes to the left (primarily right-hemisphere) or right (primarily left-hemisphere) visual field. On a subsequent recognition test, subjects performed better when the stimuli were first processed primarily in the right hemisphere. By contrast, they tended to prefer the shapes first processed in the left hemisphere. The fact that a differential effect on recognition and affective judgments occurs for stimuli presented to the left versus the right visual field supports the notion that cognitive and affective judgments are made on a different basis. Seamon et al. (1983) explain these outcomes in terms ofthe lateralized valence effect in which information processed in the left hemisphere ofthe brain generally is evaluated more positively than information processed in the right hemisphere (Tucker et al. 1981). However, Seamon et ai. presented visual shapes in all their experimental conditions (see also Sackeim, Gur, and Saucy 1978; Schwartz, Ahem, and Brown 1980). Because the right hemisphere dominates the cognitive processing of these nonverbal stimuli, the decrements in affective responses observed in the right hemisphere actually might result from these elevated processing demands. In other words, affective functions might compete for processing capacity with the cognitive operations in each hemisphere (Tucker et al. 1981). This theorizing would support the view that cognitive and affective systems are independent and compete for resources. Thus, if we observe decrements in affective responses toward nonverbal (verbal) stimuli presented to the right (left) hemisphere, our findings will support the independence hypothesis. This resource competition view, which supports the independence hypothesis, directly opposes the cognitive-affective model, which suggests that an increase in cognitive processing should result in greater positive affect that should move cognition and affect together. Abundant evidence indicates that the left hemisphere is more specialized than the right hemisphere for processing verbal information (Beaton 1985; Kimura 1967). To a more variable extent, the reverse is true for nonverbal information (Holbrook and Moore 1981). Therefore, the cognitive-affective model simply predicts more favorable affect toward verbal (nonverbal) stimuli in the left (right) hemisphere. Summary of Competing Hypotheses If we refer to the presentation of verbal material to the right ear (left hemisphere) and nonverbal material to the left ear (right hemisphere) as the "efficient conditions" (an assumption to be justified later), we can summarize the competing independence and cognitive-affective hypotheses concerning the objective familiarity and lateralized valence effects as follows: Objective familiarity effect Lateralized valence effect Independence hypothesis Simple main effect in which affect increases with prior exposures Less favorable affect in the efficient conditions Cognitiveaffective model Interaction effect in which affect increases with correct identification of stimuli as familiar or unfamiliar More favorable affect in the efficient conditions

388 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH Preview OVERVIEW: PREVIEW AND JUSTIFICATION We attempt in the present study to assess these two competing interpretations by creating an experimental situation that pits the cognitive-affective model against the independence hypothesis. Specifically, we will use a dichotic listening task to assess the role of hemispheric specialization in the formation of affective responses to words and music. We will vary the nature of the stimulus and hemispheric access by transmitting a passage of verbal text to one ear and a passage of instrumental music to the other (Kimura 1967). Subjects will be instructed to attend to both passages. Their overall experience, their affective reactions, and their recognition judgments for the words and music will serve as the dependent variables. Some general considerations to justify our approach are discussed next. Justification The conventional approach for testing hemispheric specialization, using normal subjects, is based on the assumption that task requirements will be met better if the information is transmitted to the more proficient hemisphere directly rather than indirectly through the corpus callosum (Kinsbourne 1970; Olson and Ray 1983). This assumption is supported by evidence indicating that some information is lost in the transfer across the corpus callosum (Porter and Hughes 1983). We will use a dichotic listening task to vary hemispheric access. The relationship between ear transmission and hemispheric access is well established in the literature. Kimura (1967) first showed the validity of dichotic listening as a tool for testing hemispheric dominance for verbal and nonverbal stimuli. Using a dichotic listening task, her experiments on recognition indicated a significant right ear advantage for verbal functions and left ear advantage for music tones for split-brain patients as well as normal subjects. These ear advantages in normal subjects were attributed to a combination of two factors. First, the crossed auditory pathways inhibit the uncrossed pathways in a way that stimuli presented to the right (left) ear arrive intact at the left (right) hemisphere. Second, verbal (nonverbal) information either is degraded in passing from the right (left) to the left (right) hemisphere or is less efficiently processed by the right (left) hemisphere. Use of dichotic listening tasks to assess hemispheric asymmetry for verbal and nonverbal functions has been replicated in more than 50 subsequent studies (Witelson 1977; Young 1982). Following publication of Kimura's study, the dichotic listening technique has been widely adopted as a valid and reliable means of accessing cerebral hemispheres in normal populations. Substantial evidence from clinical studies and experiments (Celesia 1976) supports the view that the crossed pathways dominate the uncrossed pathways when transmitting auditory information. This pathway domination occurs because impulses travelling along the crossed pathways suppress or inhibit impulses travelling to the cortex by way of uncrossed fibres. Supression may take place at different levels, subcortical as well as cortical, within the auditory system. The effect of suppression, wherever it occurs, is that stimuli presented to the left ear are destined primarily for the right hemisphere whereas stimuli presented simultaneously to the right ear are destined primarily for the left hemisphere. METHOD Textual and Musical Stimuli The target stimuli consisted of 24 passages 12 textual and 12 musical recorded on two different tapes (A and B) with six randomly paired textual and musical passages presented simultaneously in channels one and two of each tape. The verbal text contained test reports on the Pioneer CLD-900 laserdisc/cd player. The musical material came from 12 piano sonatas by Domenico Scarlatti. From these sources, six carefully synchronized one-minute passages of words and of music were recorded in randomized order on channels one and two of Tapes A and B. Thus, Tapes A and B each contained six different 60-second passages of verbal and nonverbal material, ordered randomly and recorded simultaneously on channels one and two, respectively. Though all ofthe passages were discriminably different, they purposely were taken from reviews of the same piece of audio equipment and performances of sonatas by the same composer and pianist to maintain a certain amount of difficulty in the recognition task (described next). Use of passages from different sources might have made it too easy to spot previously unexposed stimuli, resulting in a bias toward the recognition test. Procedure Thirty graduate students from two large Eastern universities each performed the dichotic listening task alone. Each subject was told to wear the headphones, to relax, and to listen to the words and the music. We used fbur different, randomly assigned exposure patterns that counterbalanced ear transmission and words/music selections. A quarter of the

THE FORMATION OF AFFECTIVE JUDGMENTS 389 subjects heard Tape A with the words in the right ear (primarily left-hemisphere processing) and the music in the left ear (primarily right-hemisphere processing). Another quarter heard Tape A with the directionality reversed. A similar procedure was followed for the other two groups who heard Tape B. After listening to the appropriate tape in one or the other hemispheric directionality, the subjects heard the recognition trials (transmitted to both ears). Each trial lasted 15 seconds and consisted of verbal text or instrumental music. Among the 12 randomly arranged text trials, six of the selections had appeared during the exposure phase, whereas the other six came from the previously unheard text passage contained on the other tape. A similar procedure was followed for the music trials. For half of the subjects, the music trials preceded the text trials. The subjects were instructed to listen to each trial and to indicate how much they liked each selection and whether they had previously heard it. The order of these affect and recognition ratings also was counterbalanced. Because the recognition and affect ratings were collected separately for 24 passages, time constraints and subjects' fatigue precluded using multi-item scales for each separate passage. Therefore subjects' recognition was measured on a category scale, with "yes" indicating that they had heard it before (subjective Familiarity) and "no" indicating that they had not (subjective Unfamiliarity). The affect measure consisted of a single seven-point scale with "disliked it" and "liked it" as the bipolar extremes. Though this verbal affect scale does not necessarily incorporate all nonverbal aspects of affective response, it does conform to the practice of previous research on the independence phenomenon (e.g., Wilson 1979; Zajonc 1980). Indeed, Zajonc explicitly claims that like-dislike ratings are "reliable affective discriminations" (1980, p. 151). General Approaches to Data Analysis The first step of our analysis was to examine whether recognition accuracy mediates the objective familiarity effect by comparing affective responses toward stimuli that were and were not correctly recognized. The second step focused on testing the hypotheses associated with the lateralized valence effect by examining affective responses to the efficient versus inefficient conditions. RESULTS AND INTERPRETATION The Objective Familiarity Effect Analysis of variance for the effects of objective and subjective familiarity on affective ratings showed a significant main effect of objective familiarity. Evaluations of stimuli heard during the exposure phase (Old = 4.03) were more favorable than those for previously unexposed stimuli (New = 2.88; F{\,27) = 41.88; p < 0.001). By contrast, the main effect of subjective familiarity was not significant (F < 1). By itself, the significant main effect of objective familiarity is consistent with the independence hypothesis. However, in accord with the cognitive-affective model, the objective familiarity effect was moderated by a significant objective X subjective familiarity interaction (F(l,27) = 62.78; p < 0.0()l). Results indicated a greater liking for old stimuli correctly recognized (Old/Familiar = 4.57) than for those not recognized (Old/Unfamiliar = 3.48; F(l,27) = 20.09; p < 0.001). Similarly, new stimuli correctly identified as not recognized (New/Unfamiliar = 3.60) were liked more than new stimuli erroneously recognized (New/Familiar = 2.15; F(l,27) = 15.79;p < 0.001). in other words, for both old and new stimuli, affect depended positively on the accuracy ofthe cognitive process involved in correct identification. This finding suggests a mediating role for cognition in the familiarity effect and therefore supports the cognitiveaffective model. The Lateralized Valence Effect Additional evidence for cognitive mediation involved the combined effect of stimulus type and ear transmission on the lateralized valence phenomenon. Affective scores were combined across music and verbal stimuli as two 12-item affective indices. Reliabilities for these indices as measured by coefficient alpha were 0.73 and 0.84, respectively. An ANOVA indicated a marginally significant main effect of hemisphere (/="( 1,28) = 2.59; p < 0.10; one-tailed) in which right- versus left-ear transmission (left- versus right-hemisphere processing) yielded a more favorable affective response (4.58 versus 4.00), thus supporting the general lateralized valence effect. However, as expected, further analysis showed support forthe cognitive-affective model. In accord with the cognitive-affective model and contrary to the independence hypothesis, stimuli should be evaluated more positively in the efficient than in the inefficient condition. Consistent with these predictions, the eartransmission main effect was qualified by a significant ear-by-stimulus interaction in which more favorable affective responses occurred in the efficient (versus inefficient) condition with words in the right (versus left) ear and music in the left (versus right) ear (f(l,28) = 3.00; p < 0.05). Simple effects tests indicated that text evaluations were significantly more favorable in the left versus the right hemisphere (4.84 versus 3.25; F( 1,28) = 11.98; p < 0.001). The comparable simple effect was marginally significant for the

390 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH music trials (F(l,28) = 2.25; p < 0.10), with music selections better liked when they were received primarily in the right versus the left hemisphere (4.75 versus 4.31). In all, these results support the cognitive-affective hypothesis that the lateralized valence effect is mediated by hemispheric specialization for verbal versus nonverbal stimuli. DISCUSSION Marketing practitioners have typically designed communication strategies based on the cognitiveaffective model. However, such assumptions have been challenged by recent evidence in support of a rival independence hypothesis in which the formation of affect might occur without extant knowledge about the brand. Clearly, the relative validity of these two hypotheses is important from conceptual and practical points of view. In the present study, we examined this issue in a carefully controlled experimental setting. Our findings replicate the conventional main effects of objective familiarity and lateralized valence; affective responses toward previously heard stimuli were more favorable and judgments were marginally more positive when the stimuli for processing were presented in the left rather than in the right hemisphere. However, in support of the cognitive-affective model, the findings also indicate enhanced affect for stimuli that were judged correctly on familiarity and for verbal (nonverbal) passages transmitted to the left (right) hemisphere. As in virtually any research ofthis type, our results might differ if we used some other type of subjects (e.g., professional musicians versus business students), different cultural contexts (e.g., Japan versus the United States), or different stimuli (e.g.. jazz versus classical music). We suggest these possibilities as potentially interesting topics for future research. Meanwhile, our findings suggest the presence of some cognitive mediation in the formation of affective responses toward correctly/incorrectly recognized objects. Enhanced affect may be due to reduction in uncertainty (Obermiller 1985) that is, as learning about a stimulus increases, aversive tension due to unfamiliarity decreases, (cf Sawyer 1981). However, the increase in positive evaluations also may stem from subjects' rewarding themselves for task performance (i.e., correct recognition of old and new stimuli). Because our design did not allow us to distinguish between the relationships of recognition judgments and reduced uncertainty versus recognition judgments and increased rewards, the possibility requires further investigation. Our results also indicate that the nature ofthe stimulus interacts with the lateralized valence effect in such a way that the left hemisphere's association with positive affect is stronger for verbal but not for nonverbal stimuli. Thus, the affective and cognitive systems do not appear to compete for resources in each hemisphere. Rather, an increase in hemispheric specialization in the efficient conditions exerted a positive effect on liking. Our findings therefore suggest that a more favorable affect resulted from the appropriate or efficient match between the verbal (nonverbal) stimulus and left (right) hemispheric specialization. Overall, the present research extends theorizing on the cognitive-affective model versus the independence hypothesis as competing views of consumer affect. In accord with the cognitive-affective model, our findings suggest that the objective familiarity effect increases with accuracy of recognition, while the lateralized valence effect shows a sensitivity to hemispheric specialization for handling verbal versus nonverbal material. These results tend to advance the cognitive-affective model over the independence hypothesis as an explanation for the formation of affective judgments. [Received March 1987. Revised April 1988.] REFERENCES Beaton,.Man (1985), Left Side, Right Side: A Review ofthe Laterality Research, New Haven. CT: Yale University Press. Calder. Bobby J. and Brian Sternthal (1980), "Television Commercial Wearout: An Information Processing View." Journal of Marketing Research, 17 (May) 173-186. Celesia. Gastone G. (1976), "Organization of Auditory Cortical Area in Man," Brain, 99 (Part III, September), 403-414. Fishbein. Martin and Icek Ajzen (1974), "Attitudes About Objects as Predictors ofsingle and Multiple Behavioral Criteria," Psychological Review, 81(1). 59-74. Holbrook. Morris B. and William L. Moore (1981). "Feature Interactions in Consumer Judgments of Verbal Versus Pictorial Presentations," Journal of Consumer Research, B (June), 103-113. Kimura. Doreen (1967), "Functional Asymmetry ofthe Brain in Dichotic Listening." Cortex, 3 (2), 163-168. Kinsbourne, Marcel (1970), "The Cerebral Basis of Lateral Asymmetries in Attention," Acta Psvchologica, 33. 193-201. Lazarus. Richard S. (1982), "Thoughts on the Relations Between Emotion and Cognition," American Psychologist, 31 (9). 1019-1024. (1984), "On the Primacy of Cognition," American Psychologist, 39(2), 124-129. Obermiller, Carl (1985), "Varieties of Mere Exposure: The Effects of Processing Style and Repetition on Affective Responses." Journal of Consumer Research, 12 (June), 17-30. Olson, Jerry C. and William J. Ray (1983). "Using Brain- Wave Measures to Assess Advertising Effects." Work-

THE FORMATION OF AFFECTIVE JUDGMENTS 391 ing Paper No. 83-108, Marketing Science Institute, Cambridge, MA 02138. Porter, Robert J. and Larry F. Hughes (1983), "Dichotic Listening to CVs: Method, Interpretation, and Application," in Cerebral Hemisphere Asymmetry: Method, Theory, and Application, ed. Joseph B. Hellige, Westport, CT: Praeger, 177-218. Sackeim, Harold A., Ruben C. Gur, and Marcel C. Saucy (1978), "Emotions are Expressed More Intensely on the Left Side ofthe Face," Science, 202,434-435. Sawyer, Alan (1981), "Repetition, Cognitive Responses and Persuasion," in Cognitive Responses in Persuasion, eds. Richard E. Petty, Tom M. Ostrom, and Timothy C Brock, Hillsdale, NJ: Eribaum, 237-261. Schwartz, Gary E., Geoffrey L. Ahern, and Serena-Lynn Brown (1979), "Lateralized Facial Muscle Response to Positive and Negative Emotional Stimuli," Psychophysiology, 16 (6), 561-571. Seamon, John G., Nathan Brody, and David M. Kauff (1983), "Affective Discrimination, of Stimuli That Are Not Recognized: Effects of Shadowing, Masking, and Cerebral Laterality," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9 (3), 544-555. Tsal, Yehoshua (1985), "On the Relationship Between Cognitive and Affective Processes: A Critique of Zajonc and Markus," Journal of Consumer Research, 12 (December), 358-362. Tucker, Don M., Stenslie E. Craig, Randy S. Roth, and Stevens Shearer (1981), "Right Frontal Lobe Activation and Right Hemisphere Performance," /4rc/i/ve5 of General Psychiatry, 38 (February), 169-174. Wilson, William R. (1979), "Feeling More Than We Can Know: Exposure Effects Without Learning," 7o«rna/o/ Personality and Social Psychology. 37 (June), 811-821. Witelson, Sandra F. (1977), "Early Hemisphere Specialization and Interhemisphere Plasticity: An Emprical and Theoretical Review," in Language Development and Neurological Theory, eds. Sidney Segalowitz and Frederic A. Gruber, New York: Academic Press, 213-287. Young, W. (1982), "Asymmetry of Cerebral Hemispheric Function During Development," in Brain and Behavioral Development, eds. John W. Dickerson and Harry McGurk, Glasgow, Scotland: Blackie, 168-202. Zajonc, Robert B. (1980), "Feeling and Thinking: Preferences Need No Inkrcnces," American Psychologist, 35 (), (1984), "On the Primacy of Affect," American Psychologist, 36 (2), 117-123. and Hazel Markus (1982), "Affective and Cognitive Factors in Preferences," Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (September), 123-131., Paula Pietromonaco, and John Bargh (1982), "Independence and Interaction of Affect and Cognition," in Affect and Cognition: The Seventeenth Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition, eds. M.S. Clark and Susan T. Fiske, Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum Associates, 211-227.