Task-switching and memory retrieval processing: Electrophysiological evidence.

Similar documents
Material-speci c neural correlates of memory retrieval

In what way does the parietal ERP old new effect index recollection?

An event-related potential study of the revelation effect

ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: Direct versus indirect memory tasks

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Neuropsychologia xxx (2009) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Introduction

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Age effects on brain activity associated with episodic memory retrieval Mark, Ruth; Rugg, M.D.

The relationship between electrophysiological correlates of recollection and amount of information retrieved

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

ERP Correlates of Identity Negative Priming

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

ERP correlates of Remember/Know decisions: Association with the late posterior negativity

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Neurophysiological correlates of memory illusion in both encoding and retrieval phases

Recognition memory for emotionally negative and neutral words: an ERP study

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: an event-related brain potential study

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Identifying the ERP correlate of a recognition memory search attempt

The face-sensitive N170 encodes social category information Jonathan B. Freeman a, Nalini Ambady a and Phillip J. Holcomb a

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval

Synchronous cortical gamma-band activity in task-relevant cognition

Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions

The face-sensitive N170 encodes social category information Jonathan B. Freeman, Nalini Ambady and Phillip J. Holcomb

Neural events that underlie remembering something that never happened

Event-related brain potentials distinguish processing stages involved in face perception and recognition

The influence of criterion shifts on electrophysiological correlates of recognition memory. N. Azimian-Faridani* & E.L. Wilding. School of Psychology

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Memory for drawings in locations: Spatial source memory and event-related potentials

The worth of pictures: Using high density event-related potentials to understand the memorial power of pictures and the dynamics of recognition memory

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations

Discovering Processing Stages by combining EEG with Hidden Markov Models

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

Stages of Processing in Associative Recognition: Evidence from Behavior, EEG, and Classification

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Implicit effects of emotional contexts: An ERP study

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

The time-course of intermodal binding between seeing and hearing affective information

Semantic and perceptual effects on recognition memory: Evidence from ERP

The influence of directed attention at encoding on source memory retrieval in the young and old: An ERP study

Effects of dynamic rotation on event-related brain potentials

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction

Early brain potentials link repetition blindness, priming and novelty detection

This is an Open Access document downloaded from ORCA, Cardiff University's institutional repository:

Modeling Neural Correlates of Selective Attention

Evaluating Models of Object-Decision Priming: Evidence From Event-Related Potential Repetition Effects

Title change detection system in the visu

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm

Context-dependent repetition effects on recognition memory

Tilburg University. Published in: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. Publication date: Link to publication

Montages are logical and orderly arrangements of channels

ERP evidence for successful voluntary avoidance of conscious recollection

Working with EEG/ERP data. Sara Bögels Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics

Event-related brain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Source memory retrieval is affected by aging and prefrontal lesions: Behavioral and ERP evidence

Nonaffected ADHD (A) Siblings (S)

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects

EEG-Rhythm Dynamics during a 2-back Working Memory Task and Performance

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS

A Positive Generation Effect on Memory for Auditory Context

Attenuation of deep semantic processing during mind wandering: an event-related potential study Judy Xu, David Friedman and Janet Metcalfe

The Spatial-verbal Difference in the N-back Task: An ERP Study

Retrieval Success and the Prefrontal Cortex Evidence from fmri

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

The neural basis of the butcher-on-the-bus phenomenon: when a face seems familiar but is not remembered

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Investigating the Temporal Course of Attentional Processing A Test of the Response-Retrieval Account of Negative Priming

Attentional Blink Paradigm

Address for correspondence: School of Psychological Sciences, Zochonis Building, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, M139PL, Manchester, UK

No Behavioral or ERP Evidence for a Developmental Lag in Visual Working Memory Capacity or Filtering in Adolescents and Adults with ADHD

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention.

Neural correlates of conceptual implicit memory and their contamination of putative neural correlates of explicit memory

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

The Time Course of Negative Priming

ERP correlates of item recognition memory: Effects of age and performance

Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography

Electrophysiological indices of memory for temporal order in early childhood: implications for the development of recollection

Immediate and delayed stimulus repetitions evoke different ERPs in a serial-probe recognition task.

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

When remembering causes forgetting: Electrophysiological correlates of retrievalinduced forgetting

Woollams et al. 1. In Press, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. ERPs associated with masked priming of test cues reveal multiple potential

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Transcription:

COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Task-switching and memory retrieval processing: Electrophysiological evidence. E. L. Wilding CA and A. C. Nobre 1 School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, CF10 3YG; 1 Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, Oxford, OX1 3UD, UK CA Corresponding Author Received 31 August 2001; accepted 7 September 2001 Participants attempted to retrieve phonological or imagerybased information under conditions where either the two kinds of retrieval were required in separate blocks, or where frequent switches between retrieval tasks were required within blocks. Electrophysiological indices of processes engaged in pursuit of accurate memory judgments, elicited by contrasting ERPs evoked by correctly identi ed new test items, differed according to retrieval task only when the tasks were completed in separate blocks. The principal conclusion suggested by these ndings is that the requirement to alternate frequently between the two retrieval tasks attenuated the engagement of task-speci c processes that form part of a retrieval attempt. NeuroReport 12:1±5 & 2001 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Key words: Episodic memory; Event-related potentials; Retrieval mode; Retrieval orientation; Task-switching INTRODUCTION A cognitive set is a preparatory state that determines the processing to which task stimuli will be subjected [1]. In studies of episodic retrieval, retrieval mode describes the set that is entered into when there is a need to retrieve information from episodic memory [2]. Entering retrieval mode ensures that subsequent stimulus events are treated as cues for episodic retrieval [3]. A related term, retrieval orientation [4,5], is a fractionation of mode. It determines the task-speci c retrieval operations that are set in train by a retrieval cue according to the type of information that is to be retrieved. Thus orientation, but not mode, may vary according to the speci c demands of an episodic retrieval task [5]. DC potential recordings can be employed to monitor slow (low frequency) changes in neural activity that can be sustained for several seconds, and which may re ect the adoption and maintenance of a cognitive set. Duzel and colleagues [6] employed this measure to contrast the setrelated neural activity that is engaged during episodic and semantic retrieval tasks. Over right-frontal scalp the slow potentials evoked during the episodic task were more positive-going than those evoked during the semantic task, and the authors interpreted this difference as an electrophysiological signature of retrieval mode. The time course of this putative signature of mode [6] indicates that it takes as much as 5±6 s before participants are engaged fully in retrieval mode. It follows from this observation that if mode does in fact ensure that stimuli are treated as retrieval cues, then the processing afforded test items in pursuit of retrieval should vary according to whether participants have suf cient time to engage fully in retrieval mode. The same logic should apply to retrieval orientation. That is, the task-speci c retrieval operations that are set in train by a retrieval cue should be engaged to a lesser degree when participants are given insuf cient time to adopt and maintain an orientation. This prediction was tested in the study reported here. In the rst of two experiments, participants completed different retrieval tasks in separate blocks, thereby providing them with the opportunity to maintain the same retrieval orientation for the duration of each block. In the second experiment, participants completed the same number of blocks, but within each block they were cued on a trial-bytrial basis as to which retrieval task to complete. Frequent switches between retrieval tasks were required and the cues indicating which task to complete were presented 2.1 s before each test item. Thus, in contrast to Experiment 1, in Experiment 2 participants should encounter a higher proportion of test stimuli when they have had insuf cient time to adopt fully the relevant retrieval orientation. The principal contrast of interest was between the ERPs elicited by new (unstudied) test items in the two experiments, separated according to retrieval task. The contrasts were restricted to ERPs elicited by unstudied items because this electrophysiological measure should provide correlates of retrieval orientation (task-speci c retrieval operations) that are not confounded with correlates of retrieval success [5]. If retrieval orientation does in fact determine the speci c form of retrieval processing that is afforded test items, then any electrophysiological indices of retrieval processing that are evident in Experiment 1 should be attenuated in Experiment 2 due to the time demands that are imposed by the task-switching requirement. 0959-4965 & Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Vol 12 No 16 16 November 2001 1 Article number = 12451

E. L. WILDING AND A. C. NOBRE MATERIALS AND METHODS There were 24 participants in each experiment and no participant completed both. Each experiment consisted of two study-test cycles. Study phases comprised 90 words, presented one at a time on a computer monitor. Test phases comprised 180 words, 90 of which were encountered in the preceding study phase. The remainder were new to the experiment. All words had a frequency of occurrence of, 7/million [7]. For each study word participants completed one of two encoding tasks: generating a phonological representation of the word and stating a word that rhymed with it, or generating a mental image of the concept denoted by the word and stating a word denoting a concept with a similar mental image. In both experiments participants completed the two study tasks in separate blocks. Retrieval tasks were preceded by a single study block (90 words) in Experiment 1 and a pair of study blocks (one for each study task, 45 words in each) in Experiment 2. Study blocks were selfpaced, controlled via key-press. In each test phase participants made one of three speeded key-press responses on each trial. They were asked to make one response to new (unstudied) words, and one of two responses: either a remember (R) or a know (K) response, to old (previously studied) words [8,9]. R judgments were made to words for which participants could remember the associate (phonological or imagery based) generated when that word had been encountered at study. K judgments were made when participants knew no more than that the word had been encountered at study. In Experiment 1 test trials commenced with a xation point (an asterisk: 1000 ms duration) with a 100 ms interval between xation offset and onset of the target word (300 ms duration). The screen was blanked for a variable period following the offset of the target word. The length of this period on each trial comprised the reaction time (measured from target onset), plus 1600 ms. Finally, a blink instruction (duration 3000 ms) was followed by a 1000 ms blank interval before the next trial. Participants were asked to restrict eye-blinks to the period when the blink instruction was visible. In Experiment 2 the initial xation was one of two cues ( or XXXX: duration 2000 ms) that directed participants as to which type of encoded information to retrieve. An equal number of new words followed each cue type. When an old word followed a cue it was always one encoded in the task denoted by the cue. Order of cue presentation was pseudo-random: an equal number of phonological and imagery-based retrieval cues were presented, and on average 45% of trials were switch trials. That is, the cue was different from the cue on the preceding trial. All other task parameters were as for Experiment 1. In both experiments, study and test list combinations were balanced such that, on different lists, all words occurred as either old or new items, and all words were encountered in the phonological and the imagery-based retrieval task. Eight study±test list combinations were used in each experiment. EEG was recorded continuously from 54 tin electrodes embedded in an elasticised cap. Locations were based on the extended international 10-20 system [10], and included midline (FPz, Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, Pz, POz, Oz) as well as left/right hemisphere sites (FP1/FP2, AF7/AF8, AF3/AF4, F7/F8, F5/F6, F3/F4, FT7/FT8, FC5/FC6, FC3/FC4, FC1/ FC2, T7/T8, C5/C6, C3/C4, TP7/TP8, CP5/CP6, CP3/ CP4, CP1/CP2, T5/T6, P5/P6, P3/P4, PO7/PO8, PO3/ PO4, O1/O2). The continuous EEG was acquired with a left-mastoid reference (250 Hz acquisition, 0.03±30 Hz ltering during acquisition), re-referenced off-line to linked mastoids, and epoched (2048 ms duration, 100 ms prestimulus baseline). Electrodes mounted above and below and to the left and right of the eyes were employed to monitor EOG artifacts. Only participants who contributed. 16 artifact-free trials to the critical response categories were included [11]. RESULTS Behavioural data: The probabilities of correct old judgments (hits) and correct new judgments (correct rejections) in Experiment 1 were as follows: rhyme±hit 0.78, rhyme± correct rejection 0.85, image±hit 0.93, image±correct rejection 0.94. Behavioural performance in Experiment 2 was markedly similar: rhyme±hit 0.77, rhyme±correct rejection 0.88, image±hit 0.92, image±correct rejection 0.93. In both experiments the probability of a hit was computed by summing the probabilities of correct R and K judgments. The ability to discriminate between old and new items in each experiment was assessed by contrasting the values obtained in each retrieval task by subtracting the probability of an incorrect judgment to a new item (a false alarm) from the probability of a hit [12]. For the imagery-based and phonological retrieval conditions in both experiments these discrimination values were reliably above chance (t. 30, p, 0.001: Experiment 1 rhyme 0.63, Experiment 2 rhyme 0.65, Experiment 1 imagery 0.87, Experiment 2 imagery 0.85). These measures were submitted to ANOVA, where experiment and retrieval task were between- and within-participant factors, respectively. Equivalent analyses were performed for the probabilities of a correct rejection, and the reaction times to correct rejections. In each case, only reliable effects of task were revealed, re ecting the fact that discrimination measures (F(1,46) ˆ 188.40, p, 0.001) as well as the probabilities of correct rejections (F(1,46) ˆ 33.39, p, 0.001) were superior in the imagery based retrieval tasks, and reaction times to correct rejections were slower in the phonological tasks (F(1,46) ˆ 29.54, p, 0.001). Mean reaction times for correct rejections were: Experiment 1 rhyme 1351 ms, Experiment 2 rhyme 1390 ms, Experiment 1 imagery 1189 ms, Experiment 2 imagery 1288 ms. ERP data: The analysis was restricted to ERPs evoked by correctly identi ed new items (correct rejections). Analyses were performed using a 3x3 grid of frontal (F3, Fz, F4), central (C3, Cz, C4) and parietal electrode locations (P3, Pz, P4), these choices being guided by the locations of reliable differences between ERPs evoked by classes of correct rejections in previous studies [13±15]. The data were submitted to an initial ANOVA which comprised the factors of Experiment (1 vs 2), epoch (500±800 vs 1100± 1600 ms), condition (imagery vs phonological), and two factors that clustered the sites according to their location along the lateral (left, central, right) and anterior/posterior (frontal, central, parietal) dimensions. The analysis revealed two three-way interactions involving the factor of 2 Vol 12 No 16 16 November 2001

RETRIEVAL PROCESSES AND TASK-SWITCHING condition: experiment 3 epoch 3 condition (F(1,46) ˆ 7.14, p, 0.025), and epoch 3 condition 3 lateral (F(1.6,75.0) ˆ 9.17, p, 0.01). This and all subsequent analyses of variance incorporated, where necessary, the Geisser±Greenhouse correction for non-sphericity [16]. This global analysis was followed up by four subsidiary analyses where the data were separated according to experiment and time window. These analyses con rmed the impression given by Fig. 1: that for both time windows reliable effects of condition were evident in Experiment 1 only (500±800 ms F(1,23) ˆ 5.86, p, 0.025; 1100±1600 ms F(1,23) ˆ 5.42, p, 0.05). These results re ect the fact that, in comparison to those evoked in the phonological retrieval condition, the ERPs evoked by correct rejections in the semantic retrieval condition were reliably were negativegoing over the early (500±800 ms) epoch and reliably more positive-going over the later (1100±1600 ms) epoch. In addition, for the early epoch the interaction between condition and the lateral dimension approached signi cance (F(2,45) ˆ 3.11, p, 0.06) re ecting the fact that the differences between the two classes of ERPs evoked by correct rejections tend to be largest at midline (Fz, Cz, Pz) electrode locations. DISCUSSION The principal nding in this study is that electrophysiological indices of retrieval orientation were reliable in Experiment 1 but not in Experiment 2. In both experiments the critical contrast was between the ERPs that were evoked by correct rejections in the imagery-based and the phonological retrieval tasks. Reliable differences between ERPs elicited by correct rejections in tasks with different retrieval requirements are held to index processes which form part of what is de ned broadly as a retrieval attempt [4,5]. The ndings therefore suggest that task-speci c processes which are set in train in pursuit of accurate memory judgments were engaged to a greater extent in Experiment 1 than in Experiment 2. The primary difference between the two experiments was that in Experiment 1 participants completed separate retrieval blocks in which either semantic or phonological retrieval was emphasised. In Experiment 2 participants also completed two study-test blocks, but within each block they were cued on a trial-by-trial basis as to which of these two retrieval tasks to complete. The time period from the onset of the cue designating the retrieval task to be completed and the subsequent test item was 2.1 s, whereas the results of a previous DC potential study episodic retrieval suggest that it may take as much as 5 or 6 s for participants to engage fully in a retrieval orientation [6]. Thus the opportunity to process test stimuli while engaged fully in a given retrieval orientation was more limited overall in Experiment 2 than in Experiment 1. On the basis of these differences in task structure it seems reasonable to conclude that the attenuation of the indices of a retrieval attempt in Experiment 2 came about as a result of the requirement to switch frequently between retrieval tasks, thereby providing support for the view that it takes a period of several seconds for participants to engage fully in a retrieval orientation. There is, however, a rather more mundane interpretation for the ndings in Experiment 2. The absence of reliable indices of task-speci c retrieval processing might have come about because participants ignored the cues preceding each target item. If this is an accurate description of the behaviour of participants then one would predict that the processing afforded new test items should be equivalent, which is indeed what was observed. This account seems unlikely to be correct, however, since reaction times for correct rejections were reliably slower following cues that denoted phonological retrieval than following those that denoted imagery-based retrieval. If participants had paid Experiment 1 Experiment 2 F3 Fz F4 F3 Fz F4 C3 Cz C4 C3 Cz C4 P3 9 Pz P4 6 3 µv 0 3 P3 9 6 3 µv 0 3 Pz P4 0 600 1200 1800 msec Image-Based Phonological 0 600 1200 1800 msec Fig. 1. Grand average ERPs evoked by correct rejections in the imagery-based and phonological retrieval tasks for Experiments 1 and 2 at frontal (F3, Fz, F4), central (C3, Cz, C4) and parietal (P3, Pz, P4) electrode sites. Vol 12 No 16 16 November 2001 3

E. L. WILDING AND A. C. NOBRE no attention to the cues in Experiment 2 there is no reason for the reaction times to items that were not encountered at study to vary according to cue type. An important limitation of the design of this study is that it was not possible to separate trials in Experiment 2 according to the number of preceding trials that had also been completed in the same retrieval task. This is because doing so would have resulted in there being too few trials in the ERP categories of interest to maintain a satisfactory signal-to-noise ratio. Thus the present ndings do not speak to the issue of whether the extent to which taskspeci c retrieval processes are engaged is greatest on the rst trial that participants completed in one or other retrieval task (a switch trial) having completed the alternate task on the preceding trial. This question is especially pertinent because of previous ndings that switching between tasks results in increased reaction times, with the brunt of this switch cost being carried by the rst trial participants complete in a given task [17]. Exploring this issue while using ERPs as one of the dependent measures would necessitate using a paradigm in which there is more control over the number of switch trials and the number of trials between switches than in the study reported here. The alternating runs paradigm [17], in which participants alternate between completing two trials in task A and then two in task B, is one task that ful ls these criteria. Finally, it is important to note that while these observations suggest further lines of enquiry, they do not mitigate against the principal conclusion drawn earlier. Namely, that the reason for the attenuation of the indices of retrieval processing in Experiment 2 compared to Experiment 1 is that overall there was less opportunity in Experiment 2 for participants to engage in task-speci c retrieval processing by virtue of the requirement to switch frequently between retrieval tasks. In a recent review of the concept of retrieval orientation and related retrieval processes, Rugg and Wilding [5] observed that an important research question was what components of memory retrieval processing were in uenced by the adoption of a retrieval orientation. In particular, they identi ed two (not mutually exclusive) possibilities. The rst was that orientation in uences which aspects of an episode are retrieved from memory. The ndings in this study provide little support for this possibility, since ERP indices of task-speci c retrieval processes were evident in Experiment 1 only, while the behavioural measures of accuracy (old/new discrimination and the likelihood of a correct rejection) did not differ across the two experiments. This pattern of results is inconsistent with the view that retrieval orientation determines what is retrieved from memory. The claim that retrieval orientation in uences directly what is retrieved would have been supported by the nding that the attenuation of the indices of orientation in Experiment 2 compared to Experiment 1 was accompanied by lower memory accuracy in Experiment 2. The second possibility identi ed by Rugg and Wilding [5] was that retrieval orientation in uences the way in which the products of retrieval are processed. The ndings in this study provide some support for this account. The ERP indices of retrieval processing were attenuated in Experiment 2 compared to Experiment 1, while for the behavioural measures the only disparity across the two experiments was the fact that, for both the imagery-based and the phonological retrieval tasks, reaction times were longer in Experiment 2. One interpretation of these ndings, which also acknowledges the similarity in memory accuracy across the two experiments, is that engaging fully in a retrieval orientation facilitates the task-relevant processing afforded test stimuli, thereby permitting faster, but not necessarily more accurate, memory judgments. It remains to be seen whether the patterns of accuracy, reaction time, and electrophysiological data that are obtained in similar paradigms but with different retrieval tasks to those employed in this study are consistent with these proposals. Beyond these constraints, what further light can be shed on the functional signi cance of the ERP indices of retrieval processing that were evident primarily in Experiment 1? The ERPs evoked by correct rejections in the semantic retrieval task were relatively more negative-going than those evoked in the phonological retrieval task from 500 to 800 ms, and relatively more positive-going from 1100 to 1600 ms. The statistical analyses also suggested that the distribution of the two modulations is not equivalent, tending to have a midline maximum in the early epoch and a more diffuse distribution in the later epoch. In combination these ndings suggest that the early and late modulations index distinct processes. The precise functional signi cance of these modulations awaits more information about the conditions under which they are elicited than is available currently. Nonetheless, it is noteworthy that for the earlier of the two modulations, the time course and distribution of this effect is similar to the N400 ERP component, which is larger (more negativegoing) in tasks where participants focus on meaningrelated rather than surface features of stimuli [18,19]. This observation suggests that the modulation reported here re ects the fact that participants adopted distinct analysis strategies that involved greater emphasis on either semantic or non-semantic properties of test stimuli in the imagery-based and the phonological retrieval tasks, respectively (for a similar argument and conclusion, see [13]). From a more general perspective, the design of Experiment 2 here is comparable to other paradigms which have been designed so as to disrupt memory retrieval processing by imposing demands at the time of retrieval. Two other examples are dual-task approaches [20,21], and studies of the revelation effect, where completion of a different task immediately before making memory judgments can, under certain circumstances, in uence the likelihood of hits as well as false alarms [22]. The principal focus in studies of this type has been on how these manipulations in uence the accuracy of memory judgments. To our knowledge, there are no published studies in which these paradigms have been employed in conjunction with ERP measures of neural activity. As is the case for the taskswitching approach reported here, recording ERPs in these paradigms alongside accuracy and reaction time measures offers a means of providing more insight into the stages of retrieval processing that are in uenced by manipulations during retrieval tasks than can be achieved using behavioural measures alone. 4 Vol 12 No 16 16 November 2001

RETRIEVAL PROCESSES AND TASK-SWITCHING CONCLUSION The ndings from Experiment 1 provide further support for the concept of retrieval orientation by demonstrating that the engagement of task-speci c retrieval processes varies according to the information that participants are directed to retrieve from episodic memory. Furthermore, in combination, the ndings from the two experiments are consistent with the view that there is at best partial engagement of these retrieval processes when the time available to adopt a retrieval orientation is limited. REFERENCES 1. Gibson JJ. Psychol Bull 38, 781±817 (1941). 2. Tulving E. Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1983. 3. Wheeler MA, Stuss D and Tulving E. Psychol Bull 121, 331±354 (1997). 4. Wilding EL. Neuropsychologia 37, 441±454 (1999). 5. Rugg MD and Wilding EL. Trends Cogn Sci 4, 108±115 (2000). 6. Duzel E, Cabeza R, Picton TW et al. Natl Acad Sci USA 96, 1794±1799 (1999). 7. Kucera H and Francis WN. Computational Analysis of Present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press; 1967. 8. Tulving E. Can Psychol 26, 1±12 (1985). 9. Gardiner JM and Java RI. Recognising and Remembering. In: Collins A, et al., eds. Theories of Memory. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum; 1993, pp. 163±188. 10. Jasper HA. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 10, 371±375 (1958). 11. Wilding EL and Rugg MD. Brain 119, 889±905 (1996). 12. Snodgrass JG and Corwin J. J Exp Psychol Gen 117, 34±50 (1988). 13. Rugg MD, Allan K and Birch CS. J Cogn Neurosci 12, 664±678 (2000). 14. Ranganath C and Paller KA. Neuron 22, 605±613 (1999). 15. Ranganath C and Paller KA. Cogn Brain Res 9, 209±222 (2000). 16. Keselman HJ and Rogan JC. Psychophysiology 17, 499±503 (1980). 17. Rogers RD and Monsell S. J Exp Psychol Gen 124, 207±231 (1995). 18. Chwilla DJ, Brown CM and Hagoort P. Psychophysiology 32, 274±285 (1995). 19. Rugg MD, Furda J and Lorist M. Psychophysiology 25, 55±63 (1988). 20. Craik FIM, Naveh-Benjamin M and Anderson ND. Encoding and retrieval: similarities and differences. In: Conway M, Gathercole S and Cornoldi C, eds. Theories of Memory. Hove: Psychology Press; 1998, pp. 61±86. 21. Baddeley A, Lewis V, Eldridge M et al. J Exp Psychol Gen 113, 518±540 (1984). 22. Westerman DL and Green RL. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 24, 377±386 (1998). Acknowledgements: This research was conducted in the electrophysiology laboratory of A.C.N. at the Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford. E.L.W. was supported by a UK MRC research fellowship and A.C.N. is supported by the Wellcome Trust. Vol 12 No 16 16 November 2001 5