The conceptual cueing database: Rated items for the study of the interaction between. language and attention

Similar documents
Spatial metaphors in thinking about other people

Head Up, Foot Down. Object Words Orient Attention to the Objects Typical Location. Research Report

Tracking the compatibility effect of hand grip and stimulus size

Spatial Representation of Time in Mandarin- English Bilinguals Jennifer Wu

Metaphor Influence on Perception of Ambiguous Stimuli

Selective spatial enhancement: Attentional spotlight size impacts spatial but not. temporal perception

Do potential past and future events activate the Lateral Mental Timeline?

Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

Priming the mental time-line: effects of modality and processing mode

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Congruency between word position and meaning is caused by task-induced spatial attention

Root versus roof: automatic activation of location information during word processing

Deconstructing my thesis topic

David Sidhu, MSc PhD Candidate University of Calgary Supervised by Dr. Penny Pexman. (Updated August 2017) Research Interests

When the sunny side is down: Re-mapping the relationship between direction and valence

The Mechanism of Valence-Space Metaphors: ERP Evidence for Affective Word Processing

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Structure mapping in spatial reasoning

Subjective randomness and natural scene statistics

Which way is which? Examining symbolic control of attention with compound arrow cues

Sperling conducted experiments on An experiment was conducted by Sperling in the field of visual sensory memory.

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

Things are sounding up: Affective influences on auditory tone perception

A framework for predicting item difficulty in reading tests

Motion Language Shapes People s Interpretation of Unrelated Ambiguous Figures

MEASUREMENT, SCALING AND SAMPLING. Variables

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

Task-Dependent Motor Representations Evoked by Spatial Words: Implications for Embodied Accounts of Word Meaning

Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance

Visuospatial priming of the mental number line

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return

VERTICAL AND HORIZONTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF AFFECT 1. This is a pre-publication version of Damjanovic, L., & Santiago, J. (2016).

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

DATA is derived either through. Self-Report Observation Measurement

Attentional control and reflexive orienting to gaze and arrow cues

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

Note:- Receptors are the person who receives any images from outer environment.

An Activation-Based Model of Routine Sequence Errors

Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence. Edwin Blake

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making

Visual Selection and Attention

11-3. Learning Objectives

Pushing the Right Buttons: Design Characteristics of Touch Screen Buttons

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The path of visual attention

2ND EDITION. Introduction to Communication Studies Student Workbook - Chapter 4

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Perceptual learning in a non-human primate model of artificial vision

Acta Psychologica 141 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica

Supporting Information


CANTAB Test descriptions by function

Recognizing Scenes by Simulating Implied Social Interaction Networks

A negative compatibility effect in priming of emotional faces

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Psych 260

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

A Positive Generation Effect on Memory for Auditory Context

User Interface. Colors, Icons, Text, and Presentation SWEN-444

Supplementary Materials: Materials and Methods Figures S1-S2 Tables S1-S17 References

CLASS #21: SENSE MAKING: INTERPRETING THE TEXT. Definition: acceptable and approximating translation

OVERVIEW TUTORIAL BEHAVIORAL METHODS CLAIM: EMLAR VII EYE TRACKING: READING. Lecture (50 min) Short break (10 min) Computer Assignments (30 min)

Introduction to the Special Issue on Multimodality of Early Sensory Processing: Early Visual Maps Flexibly Encode Multimodal Space

Laboratory for Experimental Psychology KULeuven - Campus Kortrijk. Eva Van den Bussche. The mechanisms of subliminal semantic priming

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Left Handed Split Brain. Learning Objectives Topics

Intelligent Object Group Selection

Look, no hands: A perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention

Online Measurement of Conceptual Distance via the Implicit Association Test

The Color of Similarity

Psychology UNIT 1: PSYCHOLOGY AS A SCIENCE. Core

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

Reliability and Validity

Peripheral Cues and Gaze Direction Jointly Focus Attention and Inhibition of Return. Matthew Hudson 1*, Paul A. Skarratt 2. University of Hull, UK

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses?

ADMS Sampling Technique and Survey Studies

Concepts and Categories

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

AP Psychology -- Chapter 02 Review Research Methods in Psychology

How do the SLA Learners Understand the Deictic Verb, Come? A case study from the perspective of cognitive semantics.

Space-valence priming with subliminal and supraliminal words

Running head: EFFECTS OF EMOTION ON TIME AND NUMBER 1. Fewer Things, Lasting Longer: The Effects of Emotion on Quantity Judgments

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003)

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

The Effect of Earcons on Reaction Times and Error-Rates in a Dual-Task vs. a Single-Task Experiment

Name: Per:_ Advanced Placement Psychology Semester 1 Final Exam Study Guide

Visual Design. Simplicity, Gestalt Principles, Organization/Structure

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Transcription:

1 The conceptual cueing database: Rated items for the study of the interaction between language and attention Stephanie C. Goodhew 1,2 and Evan Kidd 1,2 1 Research School of Psychology, The Australian National University 2 Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language Word count: (main text): 1,578 Corresponding Author: Stephanie C. Goodhew Address: Research School of Psychology (Building 39) The Australian National University, Canberra, 0200 Email: stephanie.goodhew@anu.edu.au

2 Abstract Humans appear to rely on spatial mappings to describe and represent concepts. In particular, conceptual cueing refers to the effect whereby after reading or hearing a particular word, the location of observers visual attention in space can be systematically shifted in a particular direction. For example, words such as sun and happy orient attention upwards, whereas words such as basement and bitter orient attention downwards. This area of research has garnered much interest, particularly within the embodied cognition framework, for its potential to enhance our understanding of the interaction between abstract cognitive processes such as language and basic visual processes such as attention and stimulus processing. To date, however, this area has relied on subjective classification criteria to determine whether words ought to be classified as having a meaning that implies up or down. The present study, therefore, provides a set of 498 items that have each been systematically rated by over 90 participants, providing refined, continuous measures of the extent to which people associate given words with particular spatial dimensions. The resulting database provides an objective means to aid item-selection for future research in this area. Keywords: language; metaphor; visual attention; cueing; conceptual cueing; conceptual metaphor; embodied cognition; embodied semantics.

3 There is considerable interaction between abstract cognitive processes on the one hand, and one s body and external environment on the other. For example, memory is dramatically enhanced by mentally representing content in familiar spatial locations (Maguire, Valentine, Wilding, & Kapur, 2003), insight problem solving is facilitated by prescribed physical movements (Thomas & Lleras, 2007, 2009), and visual awareness of an object is affected by proximity of the observer s hands (Goodhew, Gozli, Ferber, & Pratt, 2013). Furthermore, humans appear to draw on concrete spatial layouts in order to describe and represent concepts (e.g., Boroditsky, Fuhrman, & McCormick, 2011). For example, English speakers describe someone who is sad as down, describe improvement as things looking up, and we look forward to the future or back to the past. A growing body of studies documents the entwined relationship between concepts and space, in particular, how activating word meaning can systematically shift visual attention in space (e.g., Ansorge, Khalid, & Konig, 2013; Chasteen, Burdzy, & Pratt, 2010; Dudschig, De la Vega, & Kaup, 2015; Dudschig, Souman, Lachmair, de la Vega, & Kaup, 2013; Estes, Verges, & Barsalou, 2008; Fischer, Castel, Dodd, & Pratt, 2003; Gozli, Chow, Chasteen, & Pratt, 2013; Louwerse & Jeuniaux, 2010; Meier & Robinson, 2004; Santiago, Lupianez, Perez, & Funes, 2007; Setic & Domijan, 2007; Weger & Pratt, 2008; Zwaan & Yaxley, 2003). For example, after reading a word associated with up (such as sun or joy ), participants are faster to respond to subsequent visual targets above the centre of the screen and slower to respond to targets below the centre, whereas the reverse is true after reading a word associated with down (such as basement or bleak ). This occurs for both concrete and abstract words. This ability of the words to orient attention in space is known as conceptual cueing. Conceptual cueing not only affects target response efficiency, it can also affect saccade trajectories. That is, when participants task is to move their eyes from the centre of the screen to a horizontally-displaced target, the path of their eye movement will veer

4 upwards after exposure to a word associated with up, and downwards after reading a word associated with down (Dudschig, et al., 2013; Dunn, Kamide, & Scheepers, 2014; Gozli, Chow, et al., 2013). In short, the meaning of the words has a potent impact on visualattentional processes. Despite the striking patterns of results produced in this line of research, it has been plagued by a significant methodological limitation: the classification of words as associated with either up or down has relied almost exclusively on researchers intuitions. While there appears to be agreement on some items, others have been differently classified by different researchers (e.g., mosquito has been classed as both up and down by different researchers). With no recourse to an objective measure, there is no way of resolving such discrepancies. Moreover, it is scientifically unsatisfying to lack an objective criterion for classification for experimental stimuli. Where researchers have reported having items rated, these are typically done with a small number of people on a small number of items, and merely to support the categorical classifications of the researchers. The purpose of the present study, therefore, was to provide researchers with an extensive of database of items rated by a large number of participants in order to use in this line of research. This approach allowed us to provide a continuous measure of the extent to which each of the 498 items people associated with up versus down, such that in future researchers can select items with particular magnitudes of associations. It is anticipated this database will provide a more precise tool for further studies to disentangle the competing theoretical explanations for conceptual cueing (e.g., perceptual simulation, conceptual metaphor theory, language-based explanations). This is important because resolving such theoretical debates will provide important insight into the nature of the interaction between language and attention that underlies conceptual cueing effects. Method

5 Participants. Participants were 180 undergraduate students from the Australian National University (average age = 21.4 years). Item selection. A total of 498 items were compiled. Of these, 364 were collated from items used in previous studies (Ansorge, et al., 2013; Chasteen, et al., 2010; Dudschig, et al., 2013; Estes, et al., 2008; Goodhew, McGaw, & Kidd, 2014; Gozli, Chasteen, & Pratt, 2013; Gozli, Chow, et al., 2013; Meier & Robinson, 2004; Setic & Domijan, 2007), and were items classified as having associations with up, down, or having no clear vertical spatial association (neutral). A further 134 items were included that we developed and selected as likely to have associations with up, down, or no clear vertical spatial associations. Procedure. The 498 items were split into two 250-item sets (note that items car and light appeared in both sets) and participants were given one of these sets to rate. Ratings were completed with pen and paper. For each item, participants were asked: For each word, please circle whether you associate it more with the spatial dimension of up or down. There are no correct or incorrect answers; we are interested in your responses. Please take care to complete the task meaningfully. Each item was placed on a separate row. For example: Lucky Up Down In order to avoid issues of response bias, the ratings were forced choice. That is, there was no neutral or don t know option available. We reasoned that for items that are not systematically associated with either up or down, responses should average out at 50% of ratings with up and 50% with down. In contrast, words that do have systematic associations with vertical space should show much higher proportions with either up or down.

6 Results & Discussion Scoring was done by assigning a value of -1 to responses were down was circled, and +1 to where up was circled. Missing data were exceedingly rare: participants completed on average 99.7% of the items they were asked to rate. Where missing data was present, it was simply omitted from the calculation of average ratings. As a preliminary check, we needed to ensure that the ratings collated measured something systematic, rather than random variation. To gauge this, we examined the frequency of rating responses for each word. Random variation would amount to similar frequencies for all items, which should converge on approximately zero (indicative of 50% of responses with each dimension). In contrast, however, the ratings showed clear systematic tendencies, as illustrated in Figure 1. Individual participant ratings for each item and summary averages can be found at: http://dx.doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/n3thi

7 Figure 1. An illustration of the frequency (number of words) associated with each average rating value. -1 = invariably associated with down by all participants, +1 = invariably associated with up by all participants, 0 = no more likely to be associated with up or down, that is, 50% of responses associated with each. Given that the ratings were clearly tapping a systematic association between the meaning of the words and the spatial dimension, our next question was to extent to which our participants ratings converged with the way in which researchers had previously classified items. If items were associated with up, they were assigned a value of +1, if they were associated with neutral, they were assigned a value of 0, and if they were associated with

8 down, they were assigned a value of -1. For items where different researchers had previously classified them differently, we went with the classification that was either most common, or where there was not a consensus we made classification decisions. We then calculated the Pearson s correlation coefficient between the researcher s classifications of these 364 previously-used items and participants ratings, which revealed considerable agreement: r =.87, p <.001. This suggests that researchers intuitions do appear to be gauging something meaningful that corresponds with the way in participants classify items. What might this commonality be? One possibility, which has been gaining increasing traction, is that language-use patterns play a pivotal role in creating and transmitting these systematic associations (Goodhew, et al., 2014; Hutchinson & Louwerse, 2013; Louwerse, 2008; Louwerse & Jeuniaux, 2010). In other words, happy is associated with up because the words happy and up systematically co-occur together in language with greater frequency than happy and down. Previously we have shown that the magnitude of collocation (co-occurrence of a concept word with the word up and above versus down and below) could predict the extent to which 24 conceptual cues systematically shifted visual attention up or down in space (Goodhew, et al., 2014). To examine the relationship between language collocation and the present ratings, we calculated the correlation between the collocation difference scores from Goodhew et al. (2014)and the ratings of those 24 items (track, street, ground, road, drain, spider, sky, head, ceiling, lid, castle, candy, delay, Satan, sad, vain, hostile, bitter, God, dream, heaven, happy, earnest, sensible). This Pearson s correlation coefficient demonstrates a significant correlation between these two variables, r =.47, p =.022. This further supports the notion that systematic tendencies in language play a pivotal role in the transmission of these concept-space associations. In other words, patterns of language use are a strong predictor of the impact of concepts on our visual attention.

9 In conclusion, conceptual cueing sits at the exciting intersection of research on visual attention and language. A greater theoretical understanding of the mechanisms that underlie these systematic spatial associations and their effect on attention will provide significant insight. The purpose of the present study was to develop a sharper, more refined tool for this purpose. Here we provide the first systematic database of conceptual cues: 498 items ranked according to the average rating of their association with up and down spatial dimensions. These ratings are intended to be useful for disentangling competing theoretical accounts for conceptual cueing, such as perceptual simulation versus language association theory.

10 Acknowledgements This research was supported by an Australian Research Council (ARC) Discovery Early Career Research Award (DE140101734) awarded to S.C.G and Evan Kidd is supported by the Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence grant (CE140100041). The authors thank Erin Walsh and Reuben Rideaux for assistance with data entry. Correspondence regarding this study should be addressed to Stephanie Goodhew (stephanie.goodhew@anu.edu.au), Research School of Psychology, The Australian National University.

11 References Ansorge, U., Khalid, S., & Konig, P. (2013). Space-valence priming with subliminal and supraliminal words. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 81. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00081 Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O., & McCormick, K. (2011). Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition, 118(1), 126-132. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.09.010 Chasteen, A. L., Burdzy, D. C., & Pratt, J. (2010). Thinking of God moves attention. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 627-630. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.029 Dudschig, C., De la Vega, I., & Kaup, B. (2015). What's up? Emotion-specific activation of vertical space during language processing. Acta Psychologica, 156, 143-155. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.09.015 Dudschig, C., Souman, J., Lachmair, M., de la Vega, I., & Kaup, B. (2013). Reading "sun" and looking up: The influence of language on saccadic eye movements in the vertical dimension. Plos One, 8(2). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056872 Dunn, B. M., Kamide, Y., & Scheepers, C. (2014). Hearing moon and looking up: Wordrelated spatial associations facilitate saccades to congruent locations. CogSci Proceedings. Estes, Z., Verges, M., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Head up, foot down: Object words orient attention to the objects' typical location. Psychological Science, 19(2), 93-97. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02051.x Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 555-556. Goodhew, S. C., Gozli, D. G., Ferber, S., & Pratt, J. (2013). Reduced temporal fusion in nearhand space. Psychological Science, 24(6), 891-900. doi: 10.1177/0956797612463402

12 Goodhew, S. C., McGaw, B., & Kidd, E. (2014). Why is the sunny side always up? Explaining the spatial mapping of concepts by language use. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5), 1287-1293. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0593-6 Gozli, D. G., Chasteen, A. L., & Pratt, J. (2013). The cost and benefit of implicit conceptual cues for visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi: 10.1037/a0030362 Gozli, D. G., Chow, A., Chasteen, A. L., & Pratt, J. (2013). Valence and vertical space: Saccade trajectory deviations reveal metaphorical spatial activation. Visual Cognition, 21(5), 628-646. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2013.815680 Hutchinson, S., & Louwerse, M. M. (2013). Language statistics explain the spatial-numerical association of response codes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0492-2 Louwerse, M. M. (2008). Embodied relations are encoded in language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(4), 838-844. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.4.838 Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition, 114(1), 96-104. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.002 Maguire, E. A., Valentine, E. R., Wilding, J. M., & Kapur, N. (2003). Routes to remembering: the brains behind superior memory. Nature Neuroscience, 6(1), 90-95. doi: 10.1038/nn988 Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15(4), 243-247. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00659.x Santiago, J., Lupianez, J., Perez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 512-516. doi: 10.3758/BF03194099

13 Setic, M., & Domijan, D. (2007). The influence of vertical spatial orientation on property verification. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(2), 297-312. doi: 10.1080/01690960600732430 Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2007). Moving eyes and moving thought: On the spatial compatibility between eye movements and cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 663-668. Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2009). Swinging into thought: Directed movement guides insight into problem solving. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 719-723. Weger, U. W., & Pratt, J. (2008). Time flies like an arrow: Space-time compatibility effects suggest the use of a mental timeline. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(2), 426-430. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.2.426 Zwaan, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2003). Spatial iconicity affects semantic relatedness judgements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 954-958. doi: 10.3758/BF03196557