Journal of Abnormal Psychology

Similar documents
Michael Armey David M. Fresco. Jon Rottenberg. James J. Gross Ian H. Gotlib. Kent State University. Stanford University. University of South Florida

Brooding and reflection: Rumination predicts suicidal ideation at 1-year follow-up in a community sample

Reconsidering the reappraisal scale of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Ryota SAKAKIBARA, Yu ISHII

David M. Fresco, Ph.D.

Avoidant Coping Moderates the Association between Anxiety and Physical Functioning in Patients with Chronic Heart Failure

Brooding and Pondering: Isolating the Active Ingredients of Depressive Rumination with Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Depressive Rumination and Co-Morbidity: Evidence for Brooding as a Transdiagnostic Process

The Role of Mindfulness and Psychological Flexibility in Somatization, Depression, Anxiety, and General Psychological Distress of a Nonclinical

Emotion Regulation and Vulnerability to Depression: Spontaneous Versus Instructed Use of Emotion Suppression and Reappraisal

Author's personal copy

Sex Differences in Depression in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis

(SCARED) DOI: /psy

Running head: EMOTION REGULATION MODERATES PERFECTIONISM 1. Depression in College Students. Jessica Drews. Faculty Advisor: Scott Pickett

Dear Author. Here are the proofs of your article.

Emotion Regulation Choice among Undergraduate Students with Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms

NOTICE: This is the author s version of a work that was accepted for publication in Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry.

The role of emotional schema in prediction of pathological worry in Iranian students

Emotion Regulation Choice: Theory and Findings. Gal Sheppes. Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel

Behaviour Research and Therapy

THE EFFECT OF ANGER RUMINATION IN THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER SYMPTOMS AND PRECURSORS

The Relationship between Mindfulness and Uncontrollability of Ruminative Thinking

Treatment of Anxiety as a Cooccurring Disorder

Rumination and Prospective

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Brooding rumination and attentional biases in currently non-depressed individuals: an eye-tracking study

The Repetitive Thinking Questionnaire : Psychometric properties in Iraninan Students, and Relationship with depression, Anxiety and Social Anxiety

A Behavioral Attention Task for Investigating Rumination in Borderline Personality Disorder: Final Report

Strategies of emotion regulation in students future human relations professionals

Personality and Individual Differences

Emotion Regulation and Internet Gaming Disorder

The Effect of Rumination and Reappraisal on Social Anxiety Symptoms During Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Social Anxiety Disorder

Metacognitive therapy for generalized anxiety disorder: An open trial

Emotion Regulation in Depression: Relation to Cognitive Inhibition. Jutta Joormann. University of Miami. Ian H. Gotlib. Stanford University

Understanding reappraisal as a multi-component process: The psychological health

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping

Negative Life Events, Self-Perceived Competence, and Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults

The Effects of Experiential Avoidance and Rumination on Depression among College Students

The Role of Experiential Avoidance in the Relationship Between Maladaptive Perfectionism and Worry 1

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related?

The Role of Rumination in Depressive Disorders and Mixed Anxiety/Depressive Symptoms

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health

Beliefs about Emotions, Depression, Anxiety and Fatigue: A Mediational Analysis (Extended Report)

Relations Among Self-Concealment, Mindfulness, and Internalizing Problems

Co-Rumination Predicts the Onset of Depressive Disorders During Adolescence

Sociotropy and Bulimic Symptoms in Clinical and Nonclinical Samples

Choosing How to Feel: Emotion Regulation Choice in Bipolar Disorder

Ironic Effects of Emotion Suppression When Recounting Distressing Memories

Psychological flexibility and self-concealment as predictors of disordered eating symptoms.

BRIEF REPORT. Depressive implicit associations and adults reports of childhood abuse

II3B GD2 Depression and Suicidality in Human Research

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA

Early Maladaptive Schemas And Personality. Disorder Symptoms An Examination In A Nonclinical

A Correlational Test of the Relationship Between Posttraumatic Growth, Religion, and Cognitive Processing

Repetitive Thought and Emotional Distress: Rumination and Worry as Prospective Predictors of Depressive and Anxious Symptomatology

Mindfulness Action-Based Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for Concurrent Binge Eating Disorder and Substance Use Disorders

The effects of mindfulness versus thought suppression instruction on the appraisal of emotional and neutral pictures

Emotion Regulation in Depression: Investigating Mechanisms Underlying Reappraisal

A Pilot Study of Interpersonal Psychotherapy for Depressed Women with Breast Cancer

ACT for PARENTS with a Child with Autism. Presented by Stephanie A. Sokolosky, EdD, BCBA-D, LBA, LSSP, LPA

Are Metacognitive Factors Common in Generalized Anxiety Disorder and Dysthymia?

The Impact of Ruminative Processing on the Experience of Self Referent Intrusive. Memories in Dysphoria. Alishia D. Williams & Michelle L.

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for affectively valenced and neutral stimuli in depression: Evidence from a novel matching task

The Interaction of Co-rumination, Coping Strategies, and Stress in Predicting Depression and. Anxiety. Nicole Marie Kramer

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Behav Res Ther. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 May 1.

The age of feeling in-between : Factors that influence emerging adult outcomes during and after residential substance use disorder treatment

Looming Maladaptive Style as a Specific Moderator of Risk Factors for Anxiety

Perfectionistic concerns suppress associations between perfectionistic strivings and positive life outcomes

Body Image 9 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Body Image. jou rnal h omepa g e:

Deployment Stressors, Coping, and. Psychological Well-Being Among Peacekeepers. Luigi Pastò, Ph.D., Don McCreary, Ph.D., Megan Thompson, Ph.D.

Cognitive Bias Modification: Induced Interpretive Biases Affect Memory

Emotional Schema Therapy for Generalized Anxiety Disorder: A Single-Subject Design

Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

The role of the family in child and adolescent posttraumatic stress following attendance at an. emergency department

The Role of Rumination and Reduced Concreteness in the Maintenance of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Depression Following Trauma

CHAPTER VI SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, IMPLICATIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS. Premenstrual syndrome is a set of physical psycho emotional and behavioral

LONGITUDINAL PREDICTIONS OF THE BROODING AND REFLECTION SUBSCALES OF THE JAPANESE RUMINATIVE RESPONSES SCALE FOR DEPRESSION 1

The everyday dynamics of rumination and worry: precipitant events and affective consequences

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder

Effects of Mindful Acceptance and Reappraisal Training on Maladaptive Beliefs About Rumination

BRIEF REPORT. and emotional reactivity to stressful events (Nolen-Hoeksema,

Using the WHO 5 Well-Being Index to Identify College Students at Risk for Mental Health Problems

Rumination, Negative Life Events, and Depressive Symptoms in a Sample of College Students

Stress Sensitization and Adolescent Depressive Severity as a Function of Childhood Adversity: A Link to Anxiety Disorders

SCREENING FOR SOCIAL ANXIETY DISORDER WITH THE SELF-REPORT VERSION OF THE LIEBOWITZ SOCIAL ANXIETY SCALE

Highlights of the Research Consortium 2002 Non-Clinical Sample Study

Affective Forecasting in Depression:The Effects of Rumination versus Reappraisal

Empowering and Enhancing Exposure Through Compassion, Acceptance and Mindfulness

Cognition and Emotion: The Cognitive Regulation of Emotions : A Review

Diagnosis of Mental Disorders. Historical Background. Rise of the Nomenclatures. History and Clinical Assessment

Kim L. Gratz Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior University of Mississippi Medical Center (UMMC)

Brooding Deficits in Memory: Focusing Attention Improves Subsequent Recall

Using the Penn State Worry Questionnaire to identify individuals with generalized anxiety disorder: a receiver operating characteristic analysis $

Binge Drinking in a Sample of College-Age Women at Risk for Developing Eating Disorders

Dispositional and Stressor-Related Emotion Regulation in the Context of a Chronic, Life-Limiting Stressor

Test Reactivity: Does the Measurement of Identity Serve as an Impetus for Identity Exploration?

Clinical Sport Psychology Services Based in a Doctoral Training Clinic

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION IN TREATING ANXIETY DISORDER. Mindfulness Meditation in Treating Anxiety Disorder. Evergreen Valley College

Relative Benefits of Narrowband and Broadband Tools for Behavioral Health Settings

Transcription:

Journal of Abnormal Psychology When Are Adaptive Strategies Most Predictive of Psychopathology? Amelia Aldao, and Susan Nolen-Hoeksema Online First Publication, May 9, 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0023598 CITATION Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011, May 9). When Are Adaptive Strategies Most Predictive of Psychopathology?. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0023598

Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol., No., 000 000 0021-843X/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0023598 When Are Adaptive Strategies Most Predictive of Psychopathology? Amelia Aldao and Susan Nolen-Hoeksema Yale University In recent work, we showed that putatively adaptive emotion regulation strategies, such as reappraisal and acceptance, have a weaker association with psychopathology than putatively maladaptive strategies, such as rumination, suppression, and avoidance (e.g., Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010; Aldao, Nolen- Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010). In this investigation, we examined the interaction between adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in the prediction of psychopathology symptoms (depression, anxiety, and alcohol problems) concurrently and prospectively. We assessed trait emotion regulation and psychopathology symptoms in a sample of community residents at Time 1 (N 1,317) and then reassessed psychopathology at Time 2 (N 1,132). Cross-sectionally, we found that the relationship between adaptive strategies and psychopathology symptoms was moderated by levels of maladaptive strategies: adaptive strategies had a negative association with psychopathology symptoms only at high levels of maladaptive strategies. In contrast, adaptive strategies showed no prospective relationship to psychopathology symptoms either alone or in interaction with maladaptive strategies. We discuss the implications of this investigation for future work on the contextual factors surrounding the deployment of emotion regulation strategies. Keywords: emotion regulation, adaptive regulation strategies, maladaptive regulation strategies, flexibility, longitudinal In the last decade, researchers have been increasingly interested in examining the process by which individuals regulate emotional states to respond to environmental demands (e.g., Gross, 1998; Kring & Sloan, 2010; Rottenberg & Gross, 2003). A substantial amount of theoretical and empirical work has focused on the processes by which emotion regulation strategies modify emotional experiences and their associated expressions, behavioral components, and physiological states, thereby increasing or decreasing the risk of psychopathology (e.g., Campbell-Sills, Barlow, Brown, & Hofmann, 2006; Gross, 1998; Hofmann, Heering, Sawyer, & Asnaani, 2009). The work on emotion regulation strategies has led to a functional differentiation of strategies based on their ability to facilitate adaptive versus maladaptive responding. Specifically, strategies such as cognitive reappraisal, acceptance, and problem solving generally have been associated with adaptive outcomes, including reductions in the experience of negative affect (e.g., Goldin, McRae, Ramel, & Gross, 2007), increased pain tolerance (e.g., Hayes et al., 1999), effective interpersonal functioning (e.g., Richards & Gross, 2000), and diminished maladaptive cardiac reactivity (e.g., Campbell-Sills et al., 2006). In addition, these putatively adaptive strategies are important components of a variety of treatment modalities, ranging from traditional cognitive behavioral Amelia Aldao and Susan Nolen-Hoeksema, Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut. We thank Elena Wright and Yael Levin for their thorough comments on earlier versions of this article. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Amelia Aldao, Department of Psychology, Yale University, 2 Hillhouse Avenue, New Haven, CT 06520. E-mail: Amelia.aldao@yale.edu therapy to newer, third-wave approaches (e.g., Beck, 1976; Hayes, 2008; Hofmann & Asmundson, 2008; Roemer, Orsillo, & Salters- Pedneault, 2008). On the other hand, strategies like rumination, suppression, avoidance, and worry generally have been associated with maladaptive outcomes, including rebounds in negative affect following engagement with emotion-eliciting stimuli (e.g., Campbell- Sills et al., 2006), memory difficulties (e.g., Richards, Butler, & Gross, 2003), increases in sympathetic activation (e.g., Gross & Levenson, 1993; Wegner, Broome, & Blumberg, 1997), and diminished autonomic flexibility (e.g., Hofmann et al., 2005). Furthermore, the self-reported use of these strategies has been associated with the development and maintenance of a wide range of disorders, including depression (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008), generalized anxiety disorder (Mennin, Holaway, Fresco, Moore, & Heimberg, 2007), alcohol problems (Nolen- Hoeksema & Harrell, 2002), and eating disorders (Nolen- Hoeksema, Stice, Wade, & Bohon, 2007). Recently, we showed that putatively adaptive and maladaptive strategies differ in their strength of association with psychopathology. Specifically, we observed that maladaptive strategies are consistently more strongly associated with psychopathology than adaptive strategies (e.g., Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010; Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010). It is interesting to note that we have seen this pattern of findings when examining both internalizing (e.g., depression, anxiety) and externalizing (e.g., alcohol, eating) pathology in community samples and samples including clinical and nonclinical groups, suggesting that this asymmetry in the strength of the relationship between adaptive and maladaptive strategies and psychopathology generalizes across the range of psychopathology (e.g., Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; Kring & Sloan, 2010). Understanding why adaptive strate- 1

2 ALDAO AND NOLEN-HOEKSEMA gies are relatively weak predictors of psychopathology is important given the centrality of such strategies to treatment and prevention efforts (e.g., Beck, 1976; Hayes, 2008; Hofmann & Asmundson, 2008; Roemer et al., 2008). In particular, understanding when adaptive strategies are more or less useful would be critical to teaching clients how to use these strategies effectively. Using adaptive strategies may be more difficult for people who are also prone to using maladaptive strategies, because maladaptive strategies may interfere with the use of adaptive strategies. Indirect evidence for this interference hypothesis comes from work showing that maladaptive strategies may narrow attentional focus, therefore preventing individuals from noticing the context around them (e.g., Borkovec, Alcaine, & Behar, 2004; Richards et al., 2003; Richards & Gross, 2000; Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000). For example, Joormann and colleagues (see Joormann & D Avanzato, 2010) have found that rumination interferes with the ability to shift attention away from negative stimuli. Therefore, to the extent that maladaptive strategies narrow attentional focus, they might interfere with individuals ability to access and properly deploy adaptive regulation strategies. On the other hand, a compensatory hypothesis suggests that adaptive strategies may be most important in preventing psychopathology in people who also tend to use maladaptive strategies. This hypothesis is consistent with models suggesting that people have a repertoire of emotion regulation strategies that they use flexibly depending on circumstances (e.g., Bonanno, Papa, O Neill, Westphal, & Coifman, 2004; Cheng, 2003; Kashdan & Rottenberg, in press; Westphal, Seivert, & Bonanno, 2010). Among people whose repertoire includes maladaptive strategies, adaptive strategies may be particularly predictive of psychopathology outcomes. In this investigation, we sought to further investigate the asymmetry in the strength of the relationship between adaptive and maladaptive strategies and psychopathology. Specifically, we tested whether trait maladaptive strategies moderated the relationship between trait adaptive strategies and symptoms of psychopathology. Based on previous work (e.g., Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010; Aldao et al., 2010), we predicted that maladaptive strategies would be a stronger predictor of psychopathology than adaptive strategies. In addition, we predicted that adaptive and maladaptive strategies would interact with each other. Two competing possibilities were examined. First, an interference hypothesis would suggest that adaptive strategies would have a weaker negative relationship with psychopathology when levels of maladaptive strategies are high than when they are low. On the other hand, a compensatory hypothesis would suggest that adaptive strategies would have a stronger negative relationship with psychopathology when levels of maladaptive strategies are high than when they are low. In addition to examining this relationship concurrently, we evaluated the interplay between adaptive and maladaptive strategies and changes in psychopathology symptoms over time. Previous work has shown that the use of maladaptive strategies prospectively predicts the development and maintenance of psychopathology after controlling for baseline levels of psychopathology (for a review, see Aldao et al., 2010). However, little is know about how adaptive and maladaptive strategies interact to predict changes in psychopathology over time. Therefore, in the present investigation we also examined how the interaction between adaptive and maladaptive strategies at baseline (i.e., Time 1) predicted psychopathology symptoms one year later (i.e., Time 2), while controlling for baseline levels of psychopathology. Method Participants and Procedures Participants were recruited through random-digit dialing of residential telephone numbers in San Francisco, San Jose, and Oakland, California; 1,789 individuals were initially called. Of those, 1,317 participated in the first interview, and 1,132 of the original participants responded to a second interview 1 year later. Individuals who did not participate in the second interview scored higher in psychopathology than those who did, t(1316) 3.05, p.01. 1 At Time 1, the mean age of the sample was 47 years old (SD 15.22; range 24 82), and 52.7% of the sample identified as female. Seventy percent of the sample was Caucasian, 9.2% Hispanic, 8% African American, 6.3% Asian or Pacific Islander, and 6.5% identified as other or did not respond (for other demographic information, see Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000). At both Time 1 and Time 2, all participants were interviewed in person. All measures were read aloud to participants and response options were presented on cards. Interviewers were extensively trained in standardized responses to participants requests for clarification of items and probes to clarify participants answers to questions when necessary. Measures Depression was assessed with the 13-item Beck Depression Inventory Short Form (BDI-SF; Beck & Beck, 1972). At Time 1, internal consistency was good (.83), and the test retest correlation was.60 ( p.01). In addition, the module for major depression from the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (SCID; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997) was administered. Interrater reliability on this module was 100% (see Nolen- Hoeksema, 2000). We created a continuous measure of depression by summing the number of symptoms recorded for each respondent. Internal reliability at Time 1 was.68, and the test retest reliability was.48 ( p.01). Anxiety was assessed with the 21-item Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck & Steer, 1990). Internal reliability at Time 1 was good (.88), and the test retest correlation was.63 ( p.01). We did not administer the SCID modules on anxiety for this study because it was designed as a study of depression, but interviewers completed a one-item global rating of the anxiety levels of the respondents immediately after the interview on a scale ranging from 0 (none) to 4(disabling), using information reported by the respondents on the BAI, as well as information provided by the respondents verbal and nonverbal behavior throughout the interview. Test retest reliability was.32 ( p.01). 1 In the results section, we present analyses conducted with the full sample. Analyses conducted with the smaller set of individuals who participated in both assessments paralleled the results obtained with the full sample.

ADAPTIVE STRATEGIES AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 3 Alcohol-related problems were assessed with the relevant module of the SCID (First et al., 1997). We summed the number of problems recorded to create a continuous score from 0 to 8 (see Nolen-Hoeksema & Harrell, 2002). The internal consistency was good (.77). The test retest reliability was.57 ( p.01). Recent work shows that emotion regulation strategies are associated with all three types of psychopathology we assessed (e.g., Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010; Kring & Sloan, 2010). Furthermore, measures of the three symptom types were intercorrelated (all ps.01). Thus, we standardized scores on each of the measures (BDI, SCID depressive symptoms, BAI, anxiety rating, SCID alcohol use problems) and averaged them for an overall index of psychopathology. We note, however, that a similar pattern of results is found whether we use this aggregated index of psychopathology or separate indices of internalizing symptoms (depression and anxiety) or alcohol problems. 2 For parsimony, we report only the analyses of the overall psychopathology index. The test retest reliability for this index was.67 ( p.01). The Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS; Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003) is a 22-item measure that assesses the tendency to engage in ruminative behavior in response to stress. Treynor and colleagues (2003) have removed those items with a high content overlap with depressive symptoms. The resulting brooding subscale contains 5 items and reflects the moody rumination at the core of Nolen-Hoeksema s (1991) rumination theory (e.g., What am I doing to deserve this ). In this sample, internal reliability of the brooding subscale was good (.77). The COPE Inventory Short Version (Carver, 1997) is a shorter version of the original COPE inventory (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989) that was adopted for this study because respondent burden was already high. We utilized the following scales: acceptance, positive reframing, behavioral disengagement, and denial. In the initial investigation (Carver, 1997), the internal consistency for these scales ranged from.57 to.64 and in the current investigation the range was from.53 to.70. In addition, because at the time this study was conducted (mid-1990s), there was no widely used measure of emotional, thought, and expressive suppression, we created a 4-item subscale assessing all aspects of suppression (i.e., expressive, thought, and emotional). These items had good internal consistency (.76). To model the relationship between adaptive and maladaptive strategies, we created two composite scores reflecting the use of each type of strategy. The composite score of adaptive strategies consisted of the average between COPE acceptance and COPE positive reframing and the composite score of maladaptive strategies consisted of the average of COPE behavioral disengagement, COPE denial, suppression, and RRS brooding (they were all on the same scale). We submitted these subscales to a factor analysis and showed that the maladaptive scales loaded onto one factor (34% of the variance), and the adaptive scales loaded onto a separate factor (23% of the variance). The internal consistency was adequate for adaptive strategies (.68) and good for maladaptive strategies (.81). The scores were mildly correlated with each other (r.13, p.01), likely because individuals who experience elevated levels of distress tend to use many types of coping or emotion regulation in their attempts to regulate their negative affect (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Results Cross-Sectional Analyses Zero-order correlations. We examined zero-order correlations at Time 1 for the full sample. Consistent with previous work, the psychopathology composite score was positively correlated with the maladaptive strategies composite score (r.50, p.01) and was not correlated with the adaptive strategies composite score (r.03, ns). Regression analyses. We predicted the psychopathology composite score using a stepwise regression. In the first step, we entered adaptive and maladaptive strategies scores (which were centered to reduce multicollinearity; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007), R 2.25, F(2, 1314) 217.06, p.01. Maladaptive strategies contributed significantly to the prediction of psychopathology, t(1314) 20.81, p.01,.50. Adaptive strategies were not a significant predictor, t(1314) 1.58, ns,.04. In a second step, we entered the interaction term between adaptive and maladaptive strategies, R 2.004, F(1, 1313) 7.34, p.01. Maladaptive strategies remained a significant predictor, t(1313) 20.88, p.01,.50, and adaptive strategies became a significant predictor, t(1313) 2.05, p.05,.056. More important, the interaction term between adaptive and maladaptive strategies was significant, t(1313) 2.71, p.01,.07. We followed this interaction with simple slopes analyses (Aiken & West, 1991). Both the interference hypothesis and the compensatory hypothesis entail predictions (in opposite directions) about the relationship between adaptive strategies to psychopathology in the presence of high versus low levels of maladaptive strategies. Thus, we evaluated the role of adaptive strategies as a predictor of psychopathology at high and low levels of maladaptive strategies (i.e., 1 SD above/below the mean). At low levels of maladaptive strategies, adaptive strategies were unrelated to psychopathology, t(1313).41, ns,.01. However, at high levels of maladaptive strategies, greater levels of adaptive strategies were related to lower levels of psychopathology, t(1313) 3.08, p.01,.11. Longitudinal Analyses Correlations. We examined the relationship between strategies at Time 1 and psychopathology symptoms at Time 2. In parallel with the cross-sectional results, maladaptive strategies at Time 1 were correlated with the composite score of psychopathology at Time 2 (r.41, p.01), even after controlling for symptoms at Time 1 (r.12, p.01). In addition, mimicking the cross-sectional results, adaptive strategies at Time 1 were not correlated with the psychopathology at Time 2 (r.03). Regression analyses. We predicted psychopathology symptoms at Time 2 with adaptive and maladaptive strategies at Time 1. In Step 1, we entered psychopathology symptoms at Time 1 and, as expected, we found that it was a significant predictor, t(1124) 29.87, p.01,.67, R 2.44. In Step 2, we entered adaptive and maladaptive strategies at Time 1 centered, R 2.01, F(2, 1122) 8.13, p.01. Psychopathology symptoms at Time 1 2 Available from the first author.

4 ALDAO AND NOLEN-HOEKSEMA Figure 1. Cross-sectional interaction between adaptive and maladaptive strategies. High and low levels correspond to 1 SD above or below the mean, respectively. Adaptive strategies have a negative association with psychopathology only at high levels of maladaptive strategies. remained a significant predictor, t(1122) 24.11, p.01,.62, and the maladaptive strategies score was also a significant predictor, t(1122) 3.97, p.01,.10, but adaptive strategies was not a significant predictor, t(1122).02, ns,.00. In step 3, we added the interaction between adaptive and maladaptive strategies at Time 1, but, unlike in the cross-sectional models, it was not a significant predictor, t(1121).57, ns,.01. Discussion We examined the relationship between trait adaptive (positive reframing and acceptance) and maladaptive (rumination, suppression, behavioral disengagement, and denial) emotion regulation strategies in the prediction of psychopathology symptoms concurrently and prospectively. At the cross-sectional level, we found that maladaptive strategies moderated the relationship between adaptive strategies and psychopathology symptoms: adaptive strategies had a negative association with psychopathology symptoms only when levels of maladaptive strategies were elevated. This provides support for the compensatory hypothesis that adaptive strategies have significant relationships to psychopathology symptoms in the presence of maladaptive strategies. On the other hand, in prospective analyses, only maladaptive strategies predicted psychopathology symptoms at Time 2. In previous work (Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010), we argued that adaptive strategies might show a weaker association with psychopathology because their usefulness might be context dependent (e.g., reappraisal might be mostly adaptive when the situation can actually be reframed, or acceptance might be most appropriate when there is little action an individual can take to change his or her circumstances). One type of context may be the affective and/or regulatory state of the individual. Sheppes, Cartan, and Meiran (2009) have shown that the effects of reappraisal depend on the point in time in the emotion process in which it is deployed. Similarly, in this investigation we found that adaptive strategies showed a negative association with concurrent levels of psychopathology only when levels of maladaptive strategies were elevated, therefore suggesting that their effectiveness might depend on the other regulation strategies present in an individual s repertoire. Future empirical work should examine the process by which individuals select one strategy over another one (e.g., Egloff, Schmukle, Burns, & Schwerdtfeger, 2006) and can flexibly switch between using different strategies that might be differentially adaptive for varying contexts (e.g., Westphal et al., 2010). In addition, it will be important to examine other contextual factors, such as trait (e.g., emotion intensity, personality) and state (e.g., emotion reactivity, attention processes, physiology) components of affective processes. Although this study provided an important examination of the moderating role of maladaptive strategies on adaptive strategies, it had limitations. The first was our use of self-report measures of psychopathology, which are subject to reporting bias (e.g., Robinson & Clore, 2002). We attempted to reduce the influence of such biases by administering these self-report measures in the context of an interview and combining them with indices of psychopathology that came from structured interviews and interviewer ratings. Still, replicating our findings with measures of psychopathology that are all based on structured clinical interviews will be important. Second, our sample was a representative community sample, raising the question of whether our results would be replicated in comparisons of individuals with and without diagnosed disorders. In our previous meta analytic review (Aldao et al., 2010), we showed that maladaptive strategies were even more strongly related to psychopathology when clinical and nonclinical samples were compared than when community samples with a range of psychopathology were used; in contrast, adaptive strategies were weakly and inconsistently related to psychopathology in both community samples and comparisons of clinical and nonclinical samples. Thus, our meta analysis suggests that we would find similar results in comparisons of clinical and nonclinical samples. Furthermore, emotion regulation theories suggest that the relationship between specific strategies and emotional distress or unhealthy behaviors (such as substance misuse) can be characterized as continuous (for reviews, see Kring & Sloan, 2010); thus, the use of a community sample with a representative range of psychopathology provide an appropriate test of these theories. Still, the generalizability of our results to comparisons of clinical versus nonclinical samples remains to be tested. Third, emotion regulation was measured through self-report questionnaires, although they were administered in the context of a structured interview. Future studies should utilize experimental designs to assess the multiple emotional domains (e.g., subjective, physiological, and behavioral; Bradley & Lang, 2000). Furthermore, we only examined 6 regulation strategies, so it will be important for future work to incorporate a wider range of strategies (e.g., problem solving, seeking social support). Fourth, an alternative hypothesis for the stronger association between maladaptive strategies and psychopathology is that the scales used to measure maladaptive strategies have content overlap with psychopathology scales. In this investigation, we sought to minimize this potential confound by utilizing the brooding scale of the RRS (Treynor et al., 2003), from which the items that most obviously overlapped with distress were removed. In addition, the COPE scales we used had minimal overlap with symptoms of negative affect. In this investigation, we examined the relationship between adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in the prediction of psychopathology symptoms concurrently and prospectively. When predicting psychopathology symptoms crosssectionally, we found support for a context-dependent role of

ADAPTIVE STRATEGIES AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 5 adaptive strategies, so that their association with psychopathology symptoms was moderated by levels of maladaptive strategies. Specifically, adaptive strategies had a negative association with psychopathology symptoms only at high levels of maladaptive strategies, providing support for a compensatory hypothesis. In contrast, adaptive strategies did not show an association with psychopathology symptoms longitudinally, suggesting their predictive power might be limited. References Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regressions: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: A transdiagnostic examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 974 983. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.002 Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217 237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004 Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York, NY: International University Press. Beck, A. T., & Beck, R. W. (1972). Screening depressed patients in family practice: A rapid technique. Postgraduate Medicine, 52, 81 85. Retrieved from http://www.postgradmed.com/ Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1990). Manual for the revised Beck Anxiety Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., O Neill, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science, 15, 482 487. doi: 10.1111/j.0956 7976.2004.00705.x Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O., & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, & D. S. Mennin (Eds.) Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp. 77 108). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, O. J. (2000). Measuring emotion: Behavior, feeling, and physiology. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 242 276). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2006). Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1251 1263. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001 Carver, C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol s too long: Consider the brief COPE. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 92 100. Retrieved from http://www.springer.com/ medicine/journal/12529 Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267 283. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/ pubs/journals/psp/ Cheng, C. (2003). Cognitive and motivational processes underlying coping flexibility: A dual-process model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 425 438. doi: 10.1037/0022 3514.84.2.425 Egloff, B., Schmukle, S. C., Burns, L. R., & Schwerdtfeger, A. (2006). Spontaneous emotion regulation during evaluated speaking tasks: Associations with negative affect, anxiety expression, memory, and physiological responding. Emotion, 6, 356 366. doi: 10.1037/1528 3542.6.3.356 First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM IV Axis I Disorders Non-patient edition (version 2.0). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute Biometrics Research Department. Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 577 586. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031 Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271 299. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/gpr/ Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970 986. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/ pubs/journals/psp/ Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behavioural processes across psychological disorders. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. Hayes, S. C. (2008). Climbing our hills: A beginning conversation on the comparison of acceptance and commitment therapy and traditional cognitive behavioral therapy. Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice Reviews, 15, 286 295. Retrieved from http://www.wiley.com/bw/ journal.asp?ref 0969 5893 Hayes, S. C., Bissett, R. T., Korn, Z., Zettle, R. D., Rosenfarb, I. S., Copper, L. D., & Grundt, A. M. (1999). The impact of acceptance versus control rationales on pain tolerance. The Psychological Record, 49, 33 47. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/ Hofmann, S. G., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2008). Acceptance and mindfulness-based therapy: New wave or old hat. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1 16. Retrieved from http://www.wiley.com/bw/ journal.asp?ref 0969 5893 Hofmann, S. G., Heering, S., Sawyer, A. T., & Asnaani, A. (2009). How to handle anxiety: The effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and suppression strategies on anxious arousal. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 389 394. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.010 Hofmann, S. G., Moscovitch, D. A., Litz, B. T., Kim, H., Davis, L. L., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2005). The worried mind: Autonomic and prefrontal activation during worrying. Emotion, 5, 464 475. doi: 10.1037/1528 3542.5.4.464 Joormann, J., & D Avanzato, C. (2010). Emotion regulation in depression: Examining the role of cognitive processes. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 913 939. doi: 10.1080/02699931003784939 Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (in press). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review. doi: 10.1016/ j.cpr.2010.03.001 Kring, A. M., & Sloan, D. S. (2010). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Mennin, D. S., Holaway, R., Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2007). Delineating components of emotion and its dysregulation in anxiety and mood psychopathology. Behavior Therapy, 38, 284 302. doi: 110.1016/j.brat.2006.12.004 Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569 582. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 504 511. doi: 101037/10021-843X.109.3.504 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Harrell, Z. A. (2002). Rumination, depression, and alcohol use: Tests of gender differences. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 16, 391 403. Retrieved from http://www.ingentaconnect.com/ content/springer/jcogp Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Stice, E., Wade, E., & Bohon, C. (2007). Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 198 207. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.198 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400 424. Retrieved from http://pps.sagepub.com/ Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). Emotion regulation in

6 ALDAO AND NOLEN-HOEKSEMA romantic relationships: The cognitive consequences of concealing feelings. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20, 599 620. doi: I0.I037//0022 3514.79.3.41 Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one s cool. Personality Processes and Individual Differences, 79, 410 424. doi: I0.I037//0022 3514.79.3.41 Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Episodic and semantic knowledge in emotional self-report: Evidence for two judgment processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 198 215. doi: 10.1037//0022 3514.83.1.198 Roemer, L., Orsillo, S. M., & Salters-Pedneault, K. (2008). Efficacy of an acceptance-based behavior therapy for generalized anxiety disorder: Evaluation in a randomized control trial. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 76, 1083 1089. doi: 10.1037/a0012720 Rottenberg, J., & Gross, J. J. (2003). When emotion goes wrong: Realizing the promise of affective science. Clinical Psychology Science and Practice, 10, 227 232. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/bpg012 Sheppes, G., Catran, E., & Meiran, N. (2009). Reappraisal (but not distraction) is going to make you sweat: Physiological evidence for selfcontrol effort. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71, 91 96. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.06.006 Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 248 259. Retrieved from http://www.springerlink.com/ content/101589/ Wegner, D. M., Broome, A., & Blumberg, S. J. (1997). Ironic effects of trying to relax under stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 11 21. Retrieved from http://journals.elsevier.com/00057967/behaviourresearch-and-therapy/ Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 59 91. Retrieved from http://www.annualreviews.org/journal/psych Westphal, M., Seivert, N. H., & Bonanno, G. A. (2010). Expressive flexibility. Emotion, 10, 92 100. doi: 10.1037/a0018420 Received November 18, 2010 Revision received March 4, 2011 Accepted March 8, 2011