Critical Review: Speech Perception and Production in Children with Cochlear Implants in Oral and Total Communication Approaches

Similar documents
Kailey Leroux M.Cl.Sc SLP Candidate Western University: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 22 (1998) Indiana University

Measuring Auditory Performance Of Pediatric Cochlear Implant Users: What Can Be Learned for Children Who Use Hearing Instruments?

A PROPOSED MODEL OF SPEECH PERCEPTION SCORES IN CHILDREN WITH IMPAIRED HEARING

Development of Audiovisual Comprehension Skills in Prelingually Deaf Children With Cochlear Implants

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 26 ( ) Indiana University

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Cochlear Implants Int. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 March 15.

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 25 ( ) Indiana University

intervention compared with oral language intervention only for children with permanent hearing loss.

Factors Affecting the Development of Speech, Language, and Literacy in Children With Early Cochlear Implantation

Predicting the ability to lip-read in children who have a hearing loss

Comparing Speech Perception Abilities of Children with Cochlear Implants and Digital Hearing Aids

Assessing Hearing and Speech Recognition

===================================================================

Critical Review: The Effects of Age of Cochlear Implantation on Receptive Vocabulary Development

Results. Dr.Manal El-Banna: Phoniatrics Prof.Dr.Osama Sobhi: Audiology. Alexandria University, Faculty of Medicine, ENT Department

2/25/2013. Context Effect on Suprasegmental Cues. Supresegmental Cues. Pitch Contour Identification (PCI) Context Effect with Cochlear Implants

Multimodal Assessment and Speech Perception Outcomes in Children with Cochlear Implants or Hearing Aids

Kristina M. Blaiser, PhD, CCC-SLP Assistant Professor, Utah State University Director, Sound Beginnings

Kaitlin MacKay M.Cl.Sc. (AUD.) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Analysis of post implantation speech recognition abilities of children with hearing impairment using cochlear implants

YES. Davidson et al. (2013) Hassanzadeh et al. (2012) Rinaldi & Caselli (2014) Boons et al. (2012) Dettman et al. (2016) Dunn et al.

Long-Term Performance for Children with Cochlear Implants

Language Skills of Children with Early Cochlear Implantation

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY Academic in Confidence data removed

Expanded Core Curriculum for Students Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing Assessment Matrix

Cochlear Implants: The Role of the Early Intervention Specialist. Carissa Moeggenberg, MA, CCC-A February 25, 2008

COSD UNDERGRADUATE COURSES

C ritical Review: Can individuals with hearing impairment associated with Usher Syndrome benefit from a cochlear implant?

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 26 ( ) Indiana University

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

Cochlear Implants. What is a Cochlear Implant (CI)? Audiological Rehabilitation SPA 4321

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 August 28.

Building Skills to Optimize Achievement for Students with Hearing Loss

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 23 (1999) Indiana University

Critical Review: Late Talkers : What Can We Expect?

Speech Perception and Oral Language Development of Deaf Children in Mainstream schools

9/13/2017. When to consider CI or BAHA evaluation? Krissa Downey, AuD, CCC A

The relationship between electric charge requirements and speech recognition of adult cochlear implant recipients

Introduction. Performance Outcomes for Borderline Cochlear Implant Candidates. Introduction. Introduction. Introduction.

The clinical use of cochlear implants has rekindled the historic controversy

The ultimate goals of auditory habilitation programs for children

Lindsay De Souza M.Cl.Sc AUD Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

DOES EARLY SIGN LANGUAGE INPUT MAKE A DIFFERENCE ON DEAF CHILDREN WITH AUDITORY BRAINSTEM IMPLANTS?

I. Language and Communication Needs

Use of Auditory Techniques Checklists As Formative Tools: from Practicum to Student Teaching

Auditory Perception and Verbal Intelligibility in Children with Cochlear Implant, Hearing Aids and Normal Hearing

Critical Review: What are the objective and subjective outcomes of fitting a conventional hearing aid to children with unilateral hearing impairment?

Short-Term and Working Memory Impairments in Early-Implanted, Long-Term Cochlear Implant Users Are Independent of Audibility and Speech Production

Hearing Impaired K 12

Mounting evidence suggests that cochlear implants

Later-amplified NOW Later-amplified THEN Early compared to pre-unhs Delayed onset hearing loss Less hearing loss Mild hearing loss Average age of ID

AHM Cochlear Implant and Auditory Brainstem Implant

Perception and Production of Mandarin Tones in Prelingually Deaf Children with Cochlear Implants

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

The Effects of Speech Production and Vocabulary Training on Different Components of Spoken Language Performance

Fitting of the Hearing System Affects Partial Deafness Cochlear Implant Performance

EFFECT OF AGE AT IMPLANTATION ON AUDITORY-SKILL DEVELOPMENT IN INFANTS AND TODDLERS

Sound Outcomes: First Voice speech and language data. High-level overview of the findings from the data set

Identification of factors predicting spoken language development in young children with a cochlear implant

Clinical Policy Bulletin: Cochlear Implants and Auditory Brainstem Implants

ANNUAL REPORT

Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences. Discovery with delivery as WE BUILD OUR FUTURE

Paediatric Amplification

Cued Speech and Cochlear Implants: Powerful Partners. Jane Smith Communication Specialist Montgomery County Public Schools

Early Educational Placement and Later Language Outcomes for Children With Cochlear Implants

Paediatric cochlear implantation

STUDY OF MAIS (MEANINGFUL AUDITORY INTEGRATION SCALE) SCORE POST UNILAT- ERAL COCHLEAR IMPLANTATION IN PRELINGUAL DEAF PATIENTS

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: COCHLEAR IMPLANTS AND AUDITORY BRAINSTEM IMPLANTS

Cognitive and Demographic Predictors of Cochlear Implant Outcomes in Adults with Cochlear Implants

Implants. Slide 1. Slide 2. Slide 3. Presentation Tips. Becoming Familiar with Cochlear. Implants

Baker, A., M.Cl.Sc (AUD) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Localization Abilities after Cochlear Implantation in Cases of Single-Sided Deafness

Cochlear Implantation for Single-Sided Deafness in Children and Adolescents

Special Education: Contemporary Perspectives for School Professionals Fourth Edition. Marilyn Friend. Kerri Martin, Contributor

Emergence of Consonants in Young Children with Hearing Loss

The Deaf Brain. Bencie Woll Deafness Cognition and Language Research Centre

Outcome of Cochlear Implantation at Different Ages from 0 to 6 Years

Creating listening & spoken language futures

Pediatric Hearing Loss and Neurodevelopment. Susan Sidman, M.Ed.

Cochlear Implant. Policy Number: Last Review: 9/2018 Origination: 10/1988 Next Review: 9/2019

Medical Policy An independent licensee of the Blue Cross Blue Shield Association

Avg. age of diagnosis 3 mo. majority range.5-5 mo range 1-7 mo range 6-12 mo

Cochlear Implant. Description

THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING

Unexpected Findings:

Critical Review: The Impact of Structured Auditory Training on Musical Pitch and Melody Perception in Individuals with a Unilateral Cochlear Implant

Areas to Address with All Families

Elizabeth Adams Costa, PhD Kate Maina, MHS Nancy Mellon, MS Christine Mitchell, MS Meredith Ouellette, MS Sharlene Wilson Ottley, PhD

DEPARTMENT OF EAR, NOSE AND THROAT, HEAD AND NECK SURGERY, INSELSPITAL, UNIVERSITY OF BERN, SWITZERLAND

Speech perception of hearing aid users versus cochlear implantees

A New Era for the Identification and Treatment of Children with Auditory Disorders

PPVT-5/EVT-3 Special Group Study: Children With Hearing Loss who utilize Cochlear Implants and Spoken Language

Advocating for the Needs of the Oral Deaf Student

Study on Effect of Voice Analysis Applying on Tone Training Software for Hearing Impaired People

SPEECH PERCEPTION IN A 3-D WORLD

CULTURAL IDENTITY OF IMPLANTED YOUNG ADULTS IN COMPARISON TO DEAF AND HARD OF HEARING YOUNG ADULTS

The Phonemic Awareness Skills of Cochlear Implant Children and Children with Normal Hearing in Primary School

IMPORTANT REMINDER DESCRIPTION

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version***

Transcription:

Critical Review: Speech Perception and Production in Children with Cochlear Implants in Oral and Total Communication Approaches Leah Chalmers M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders This critical review examines the speech perception and production outcomes of children with cochlear implants in either oral or total communication settings. Results of several studies provide suggestive evidence for the advantage of oral communication for speech perception and production. However, one mixed groups longitudinal study provides compelling evidence of no significant difference in speech production outcomes between participants in either group, if children receive their implants in preschool. Clinical implications of results are explored below. Introduction The two communication methods commonly available for children with hearing impairment are oral and total communication (Bergeson, Pisoni & Davis, 2003). Oral Communication (OC) focuses on developing oral language and listening skills and includes methods like auditory verbal therapy and cued speech (Bergeson et al., 2003). In contrast, total communication (TC) methods include an emphasis on signed language, often with the use of sign and oral language simultaneously (Connor, Hieber, Arts & Zwolan, 2000). Families of children with profound hearing loss receiving cochlear implants are often faced with the decision of which communication method they would like their child to use. Therefore, professionals working with these families should be familiar with the research outcomes for both communication methods, in order to assist families in making informed decisions for their child. Objectives The primary objective of this review is to analyze and contrast the speech perception and production outcomes of children with cochlear implants who are in oral or total communication environments. The purpose is to understand whether any significant differences exist between outcome scores for children in either group. Methods Search Strategy The following terms were searched in the computerized databases PubMED, PsychINFO and CINAHL: (Child*) AND (Cochlear implants) AND (speech perception) OR (speech production) AND/OR (OC) AND/OR (TC). Some works meeting the selection criteria and referenced in other articles were included. Selection Criteria Papers were selected based on direct comparisons of pediatric cochlear implant users in oral and total communication environments and on their inclusion of speech perception and/or production measures. This resulted in the exclusion of several papers, or the exclusion of larger portions of a study with the exception of specific speech-related measures. Data Collection Papers included in this review are mixed group longitudinal studies (2), between groups studies (2), mixed groups studies (1), and a within groups study including a between groups analysis relevant to the current review (1). Results Miyamoto, Kirk, Svirsky & Sehgal (1999) This mixed groups study investigated the effects of age at cochlear implantation on development of speech and language skills. A group of 33 children participated and were divided by age at implantation (<3 years, 3-3;11 and 4-5;3) and communication mode (OC or TC). Although the study provides demographic information on the mean length of implant use, mean age at testing and mean pure tone averages in each group, authors do not reveal the number of children in each communication mode group. Children had all received implants prior to 6 years of age. Data were analyzed from the test interval closest to the child s chronological age of 4;6 months, in order to control for chronological age, while leaving age of implantation as an independent variable. Tests were administered prior to implantation and at 6 months intervals thereafter. A number of objective and appropriate measures were employed to measure speech perception and production including 1. Grammatical Analysis of Elicited Language: Pre-sentence Level (GAEL-P), measuring closed-set speech perception, 2. The Mr. Potato Head Task, measuring open-set speech perception and 3. Beginner s Intelligibility Test (BIT), measuring speech intelligibility. Auditory-only live voice conditions were used for all speech perception tests.

Results were appropriately analyzed using ANOVAs. Findings revealed that children in OC settings performed significantly better on closed-set speech perception tests than children in TC settings, and earlier implanted groups achieved significantly higher closedset speech perception scores. The OC group achieved significantly higher open-set word recognition and speech intelligibility than the TC group. Although authors controlled for chronological age, they did not control for duration of implant use. Their sample size was not large (n=33), and they did not specify the number of children in OC and TC groups. Due to the above limitations, the study s evidence was determined to be suggestive of the advantage of OC over TC approaches for better speech perception and production outcomes. Jiménez, Pino & Herruzo (2009) This between groups study examined speech perception outcomes of children in OC and TC environments. Participants included 18 prelingually deaf children between 4;3 and 8 years of age (M=6.25 years). All children had received unilateral cochlear implants between the ages of 15 months and 5 years (M=3.2 years). Children were divided by communication mode used (OC and TC) prior to and post-implantation. The number of children in each group was not included. The lack of significant group differences in the characteristics of gender, age, age of implantation, age of diagnosis, and duration of implant use was confirmed using appropriate statistical analyses. Highly recognized standardized tests and other evaluations were completed by the lead author of the paper and two clinical psychologists viewed recordings of the assessments, resulting in an inter-observer reliability of 100%. An appropriate ANOVA analysis was used to compare the OC and TC groups. OC children performed significantly better than TC children on subtests of the Illinois Test of Psycho-linguistic Abilities (ITPA) pertaining to speech perception (Auditory Reception and Auditory Association) as well as had significantly greater intelligibility scores. However, the TC group performed better on subtests of manual expression (gesture use) and verbal expression (verbal fluency). The authors explain that the TC children were able to use more words using a picture stimulus, suggesting generalization of signing skills into spoken language. Strengths of this study include the matching of particular children based on common characteristics and confirming similarities statistically. However, the study s sample size was small and the authors do not reveal how many children were in the OC and TC groups. Due to the above limitations, the study provides suggestive evidence of the advantage of OC approaches for speech perception outcomes. Bergeson, Pisoni & Davis (2003) This mixed longitudinal study examined the development of audiovisual perception skills in children with cochlear implants. Selection criteria included profound hearing loss before 36 months and implantation before the age of 9. Participants were divided into groups by communication mode (either OC or TC) and date of implantation (early <53 months and late > 53 months of age). There were 38 participants in the OC group and 42 in the TC group, which were divided further into early and late implanted groups. The Pediatric Sentence Intelligibility (PSI) test was employed to assess closed-set speech perception in auditory-only, visual-only, and audiovisual conditions. In addition, the Phonetically Balanced Kindergarten (PBK) test was used as an open-set auditory alone speech perception measure and the Beginner s Intelligibility Test (BIT) was used as a speech intelligibility measure. Children were tested every 6 months to a year for 3 years. An appropriate Maximum Likelihood estimation method was employed to avoid eliminating participants due to missing data, and a SAS Mixed procedure was used to analyze effects within the study. Results indicate that overall across groups, closed-set speech perception was consistently better in the audiovisual conditions compared to auditory-only and visual-only conditions. Results of open-set speech perception and speech intelligibility were combined across groups and used to determine if they were correlated with closed-set speech perception outcomes. Results indicated that children in OC educational settings had consistently higher closed-set speech perception scores than children in TC settings and showed larger improvements over time. OC children performed better than TC children on visual alone and auditory alone presentation conditions prior to implantation and 3 years post-implantation. These outcome scores in auditory-only, visual only, and audiovisual conditions were highly correlated with speech intelligibility scores, suggesting that children who had better speech perception in each of the conditions also had greater speech intelligibility scores. TC children s performance reached that of the OC children 2 years post implantation. A strength of this study is its large sample size (n=80). However, due to its limited control for duration of implant use and not revealing the age range of

participants, it presents suggestive evidence of stronger speech perception skills for children in OC settings. Lachs, Pisoni & Kirk (2001) This within groups study examined the influence of audiovisual information on speech perception in children with cochlear implants. Participants included 27 children (4.2-8 years) with prelingual deafness who had used their implants for 2 years. Mean age of implantation was 4.52 years (range 2.2-5.8 years), and mean age for onset of deafness was 0.51 years. Mean unaided pure tone averages were 112 db HL. During the study, all participants were enrolled in aural rehabilitation programs that provided training and therapy. The Common Phrases Test was administered live voice to children under auditory-only, visual-only and audiovisual conditions. The following open-set tests were also administered in auditory-only conditions: The Lexical Neighborhood Test (LNT), Multisyllabic Lexical Neighborhood Test (MLNT), Phonetically Balanced Kindergarten (PB-K) word lists. Although this study was a within group study, authors included a between group analysis of children in OC and TC environments. Speech intelligibility of 11 children in OC settings and 12 children in TC settings was measured with the The Beginner s Intelligibility Test (BIT). Appropriate t-tests for independent samples found that speech intelligibility scores for the OC group were significantly higher than the scores for the TC group. Scores in auditory-only and audiovisual conditions on the Common Phrases Test were used to compute the relative improvement in speech perception due to additional visual information. A significant correlation was found between speech intelligibility scores and audiovisual gain. Correlations suggest that children who receive more gain from audiovisual information have more intelligible speech, suggesting a link between speech perception and production skills in implanted children. A marginally significant correlation was found between audiovisual enhancement and speech intelligibility for children in the OC group but no significant correlation was found for the TC group. Authors concluded that speech intelligibility scores could be predicted from the audiovisual gain scores for children in OC environments, but not for children in TC environments. The strengths of this study include that it controlled for duration of implant use and that all children had similar access to aural rehabilitation services during the study. Limitations of this study include smaller sample sizes for the OC (n=11) and TC groups (n=12). Due to the above limitations, this study provides suggestive evidence for stronger speech production outcomes for children in OC approaches. Hodges, Dolan Ash, Balkany, Schloffman & Butts (1999) This between groups study examined factors contributing to speech perception outcomes of children using cochlear implants. Participants were 40 children (2-17 years) who received their implants at a mean age of 6 years old (range 2-14 years old). CI use ranged from 3 months to 5 years. There were 21 children using TC and 19 using OC who did not differ significantly on preoperative pure tone averages. Speech perception tests administered included the Early Speech Perception Test (ESP), Northwestern University Children s Perception of Speech Test (NU-CHIPS), Minimal Pairs, and Phonetically Balanced Kindergarten (PBK) word lists. Speech perception testing was done in auditory-only conditions. Implants were verified to be functioning before testing. Results, using appropriate t-tests, demonstrated that those in OC environments achieved significantly higher scores on both closed and open-set speech perception tests than those in TC environments. In addition, authors used regression analysis to analyze other factors contributing to speech perception outcomes. Results indicated that children who were in oral communication environments, attending private schools, in higher SES groups and with access to private therapy scored higher on speech perception measures. Factors which least influenced speech perception outcomes were age at surgery, device type, and bilingualism in the home. The strongest correlation to open-set speech perception scores was communication mode. However, authors note that children in oral communication environments were more likely to come from higher SES groups, attended private schools and received private therapy services. It should be noted that children in the study using TC approaches all attended public school programs, while only 2 children in the OC group attended public schools. In examining the results of TC and OC children in SES group 4 (both groups with n=8), the OC group scored significantly higher on speech perception tests, suggesting that although SES is a contributing factor, it alone does not account for the differences in speech perception outcomes. This study had a reasonable sample size (n=40), but the range of age at implantation (2-14 years) and duration of use (3 months to 5 years) was large. These factors were not controlled for in the data analysis, and although the regression analysis states that age of implantation and length of use did not significantly impact results, other studies have shown that these are significant factors in communication outcomes (Connor et al., 2000), putting the validity of results in question.

Due to the above limitations, the evidence in this study is equivocal. Connor, Hieber, Arts & Zwolan (2000) This mixed group longitudinal study followed 147 children with cochlear implants to determine the effects of communication mode in educational settings on speech production and receptive and expressive language. Participants were grouped according to age at implantation and communication mode (OC or TC). Communication mode groups consisted of 81 and 66 children, respectively, which were classified based on teaching approach in the school setting and parent report. All children attended public schools, however, the degree of inclusion in mainstream classes in both communication groups was variable. Participant selection criteria included: deafened prelinguistically, age at implantation of 1-10 years, and nonverbal cognitive abilities that were within normal limits as assessed by a clinical psychologist. Participants used the Nucleus 22 Processor with MPEAK of SPEAK encoding strategies. Speech production skills were measured using either the Arizona Articulation Proficiency Scale: Revised or the Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation and were modified in order to better describe speech errors produced by deaf children. Pictures were presented without a verbal model and productions were transcribed fully, including additional words produced. Transcribed data was entered into PROPH + software, which calculated a percent consonants correct (PCC) score. Appropriate inter-rater reliability measures were taken and found to be acceptable. Two levels of hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) were appropriately used to analyze the trajectory of outcome growth curves by chronological age over time for each variable. Authors controlled for age at implantation, preoperative aided speech detection thresholds, type of cochlear implant used, and whether active electrode arrays were implanted. Results indicated that overall, children in the OC group achieved higher expected PCC scores and their scores grew at a more rapid rate in comparison to the TC group. There was no significant difference in effect size and rate of growth over time between PCC outcome scores for children in OC and TC groups if they received their implants in preschool. When comparing OC and TC groups within the early elementary and late elementary age of implantation groups, OC children had significantly higher expected PCC scores with greater rates of growth than children in the TC group. However, the effect of communication mode was either mitigated or enhanced by factors including age of implantation, the sophistication of implant technology and the presence of active electrode arrays. Strengths of this study include its large sample size and variety of variables controlled. This study was also longitudinal and provided a realistic picture of speech development over time for children with cochlear implants. This study provides compelling evidence for no difference in speech production outcomes between the two groups, provided children are implanted in preschool. Discussion Several of the above studies suggest that children with cochlear implants in oral communication environments have better speech perception and production skills (Miyamoto et al., 1999; Jiménez et al., 2009; Bergeson et al., 2003 & Lachs et al., 2001). Bergeson et al. (2003) suggests that children in OC environments have more experience combining both auditory and visual cues during speech, while children in TC environments typically divide their attention visually between the hands and mouth of the speaker, accounting for the difference in speech perception outcomes between the two groups. Lachs et al. (2001) suggests that better perception of these auditory and visual cues results in greater speech intelligibility. Although evidence suggests that communication mode has a significant effect on outcome scores, several studies identified crucial factors affecting speech perception and production outcomes including socioeconomic status, enrollment in private schools, access to private therapy (Hodges et al., 1999);, and age at implantation (Connor et al., 2000; Miyamoto et al., 1999). Despite studies suggesting the advantage of OC approaches for speech production outcomes (Miyamoto et al., 1999; Lacks et al., 2001), Connor et al. (2000) provides compelling counter evidence indicating no significant difference in speech intelligibility between communication mode groups if children are implanted in preschool. It is important to note that speech perception and production skills are not a measure of the overall effectiveness of a child s communication skills. Both receptive and expressive language skills are required for effective communication. Although Connor et al. s (2000) study found no significant difference between OC and TC receptive vocabulary scores in age at implant groups, the TC group achieved overall significantly higher expected receptive vocabulary scores over time. TC children also achieved significantly higher expected expressive vocabulary scores over time overall and if they received their implant in preschool or early elementary (ages 5-6.9 years) (Connor et al., 2000). Ultimately, regardless of communication mode, children implanted in preschool achieved significantly greater expressive vocabulary scores in comparison with children who were implanted after the age of 5 (Connor et al., 2000). Therefore, it seems that for overall speech and language development, age at implantation, rather

than communication approach, is a key factor influencing the development of skills required for effective communication. Further longitudinal research needs to be done with this population to investigate communication outcomes of these two approaches, ensuring the use of large sample sizes, the monitoring of progress over time, and the control for a variety of variables. Clinical Implications Based on the above evidence, it is essential for clinicians to advocate for early diagnosis and implantation, and to present both oral and total communication methods as valid communication approaches for children and families. In addition, to ensure effective speech perception and production development, clinicians should support children in developing audiovisual speech perception skills by learning to perceive audiovisual cues for the articulatory movements of speech. References Bergeson, T. R., Pisoni, D. B., & Davis, R. A. (2003). A longitudinal study of audiovisual speech perception by children with hearing loss who have cochlear implants. The Volta Review, 103(4), 347-370. Connor, C. M., Hieber, S., Arts, H. A., & Zwolan, T. A. (2000). Speech, vocabulary, and the education of children using cochlear implants: Oral or total communication? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research : JSLHR, 43(5), 1185-1204. Hodges, A. V., Dolan Ash, M., Balkany, T. J., Schloffman, J. J., & Butts, S. L. (1999). Speech perception results in children with cochlear implants: Contributing factors. Otolaryngology-- Head and Neck Surgery : Official Journal of American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, 121(1), 31-34. Jimenez, M. S., Pino, M. J., & Herruzo, J. (2009). A comparative study of speech development between deaf children with cochlear implants who have been educated with spoken or spoken+sign language. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 73(1), 109-114. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2008.10.007; 10.1016/j.ijporl.2008.10.007 Lachs, L., Pisoni, D. B., & Kirk, K. I. (2001). Use of audiovisual information in speech perception by prelingually deaf children with cochlear implants: A first report. Ear and Hearing, 22(3), 236-251. Miyamoto, R. T., Kirk, K. I., Svirsky, M. A., & Sehgal, S. T. (1999). Communication skills in pediatric cochlear implant recipients. Acta Oto- Laryngologica, 119(2), 219-224.