Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children

Similar documents
Revision of the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire Lesa K. Ellis, M. S, & Mary K. Rothbart, Ph.D. University of Oregon

A twin study of the relationship between inhibitory control and behavior problems

Journal of Research in Personality

Approach/Positive Anticipation, Frustration/Anger, and Overt Aggression in Childhood

Frontal EEG Asymmetry and Regulation during Childhood

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 146 ( 2014 ) Third Annual International Conference «Early Childhood Care and Education»

Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality: A Longitudinal Multiple-Rater Twin Study

Personality Stability in Late Adulthood: A Behavioral Genetic Analysis

Discussion. were best fit by models of determination that did not include genetic effects.

Parenting as Phenotype: A Behavioral Genetic Approach to Understanding Parenting

Learning Abilities and Disabilities

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins

Psychometric Properties of Farsi Version State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2 (FSTAXI-2)

Testing the Multiple Intelligences Theory in Oman

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences

Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder:

Nonshared Environmental Influences and Personality Differences in Adult Twins

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Girls and Boys Gender-Typed and Gender-Neutral Values

Australian children of alcoholic female twins

What s it all about?

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 146 ( 2014 ) 89 93

A Study Looking at How Youth s Self- Perception is Affected by Their Temperament

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 233 ( 2016 )

Genotype-Environment Correlation and Novel Approaches to Twin and Sibling Research Jenae M. Neiderhiser, Ph.D.

A Behavioral Genetic Study of Trait Emotional Intelligence

Phenotype environment correlations in longitudinal twin models

The aim of the study was to assess the heritability

Can Personality Explain Genetic Influences on Life Events?

In Australian twins participating in three different

Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration

Commentary: Differentiated Measures of Temperament and Multiple Pathways to Childhood Disorders

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 237 ( 2017 )

Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) PSYSOC 2013

Identity Detection Models Among Iranian Adolescents

Control and Turnout. Christopher Dawes. University of California, San Diego. March 18, 2010

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 228 ( 2016 )

Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Religiousness: Findings for Retrospective and Current Religiousness Ratings

Addictive Behaviors 65 (2017) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Addictive Behaviors

Overview of Behavioral Genetics Research for Family Researchers

ScienceDirect. Emotional intelligence and school performance-correlational study. Nicolae Mitrofan a, Mihai-Florentin Cioricaru * PSIWORLD 2013

Variability and Stability in Cognitive Abilities Are Largely Genetic Later in Life

Genetic Factors in Temperamental Individuality

Nature and Nurture. Lecture 33

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Separation Anxiety Inventory: Validity and Reliability Study

The Heritability of Job Satisfaction Reconsidered: Only Unique Environmental Influences Beyond Personality

Vitanya Vanno *, Wannee Kaemkate, Suwimon Wongwanich

Examining the Origins of Gender Differences in Marital Quality: A Behavior Genetic Analysis

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION

Investigations of Temperament at Three to Seven Years: The Children s Behavior Questionnaire

SEMs for Genetic Analysis

Environmental Predictors of Personality Differences: A Twin and Sibling Study

A Behavior Genetic Study of Parenting Quality, Infant Attachment Security, and Their Covariation in a Nationally Representative Sample

What can we learn from twins about study and subject choice?

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children

THE DEVELOPMENT OF ADOLESCENT DEPRESSION

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 205 ( 2015 ) th World conference on Psychology Counseling and Guidance, May 2015

CHAPTER 9. Biological Foundations of Personality. The Biological Tradition. Temperament

Big five personality traits and coping styles predict subjective wellbeing: A study with a Turkish Sample

The Relationship between Personality Traits and Reading Proficiency

MARC Project 4: Australian Children of Alcoholic Female Twins

Personal Goals of Older Female Twins

Chapter 6 Genetics. Total Assessment Guide (T.A.G.) 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 61 22, 23, 25, 26, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 32, 34

LJEAVES II J EYSENCIZ 1\J G Nli\RTIN

The present study investigated the location of trait

Genetic Architecture of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Trail Making Test: Evidence for Distinct Genetic Influences on Executive Function

Genetic contribution to the relationship between personality and depressive symptoms among older women

Emotion and Cognition: An Intricately Bound Developmental Process

A Behavior Genetic Study of the Connection Between Social Values and Personality

Socioeconomic Status, Parental Support, Motivation and Self-Confidence in Youth Competitive Sport

The equal environments assumption, which holds

A Developmental Perspective on the Role of Genes on Substance Use Disorder. Elisa M. Trucco, Ph.D., Florida International University

Relationship between Tabriz elementary students' mothers' personality disorders and antisocial behavioral disorder of children in

We expand our previous deterministic power

Designing a psychologists core competencies validation method using Behaviorally Anchored Rated Scales

Heredity of Cognitive Functions and Personality in Twins

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Computer Science 75 (2015 )

Genetic Influences on Midlife Functioning

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

Interaction of Genes and the Environment

The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for three measures of disordered eating

Validation of the Russian version of the Quality of Life-Rheumatoid Arthritis Scale (QOL-RA Scale)

Genetic and environmental influences on eating behaviors in 2.5- and 9-year-old children: a longitudinal twin study

PSIWORLD Mustata Andreea- Elena * Bucuresti University, Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences,90 Panduri Avenue, Bucharest, Romania

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives)

The features of students self-organization structure, who study MBA in construction program

Electronic Supplementary Material. for. Pride, Personality, and the Evolutionary Foundations of Human Social Status

NETHERLANDS TWIN REGISTER. Dorret Boomsma

The Biology of Relationships: What Behavioral Genetics Tells Us about Interactions among Family Members

Social Identity and gender. Counseling needs in adults

The genetic basis of the relation between speed-of-information-processing and IQ

Measurement Models for Sexual Orientation in a Community Twin Sample

The Validity And Reliability Of The Turkish Version Of The Perception Of False Self Scale

Genetic and environmental risk factors for the weight and shape concerns characteristic of bulimia nervosa

Transcription:

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Scienc es 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 V Congress of Russian Psychological Society Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children Yulia Novgorodova a *, a, Elena Sabirova c, Philipp Barsky b, Marina Lobaskova a, Sergey Malykh b a Udmurt State University, Izhevsk, University str., 1, 426006, Russia b Psychological Institute of Russian Academy of Education, Moscow, Mokhovaya 9-4, 125009, Russia c Kyrgyz-Russian Slavic University, Bishkek,Kievskaya str., 44, 720000, Kyrgyzstan Abstract The current study estimated the contributions of genetic and environmental influences into the variability of temperament specifics in primary school children. We assessed the temperamental traits with the use of Russian-language parent version of M.Rothbart's questionnaire. The sample included 170 children (85 twin pairs) aged 7 to 10: 49 monozygotic twin pairs and 36 same-sex dizygotic twin pairs. Contributions of genetic and environmental influences were estimated by means of structural equation modeling. Factor analysis revealed three common factors: "Attention - control", "Extraversion - Positive Emotions", "Fear - Negative Emotions". The most significant contributions of the genotype were found for the following scales: "Fear", "Inhibitory Control", "Shyness" and "Depressive Mood". Common environment contributions were most significant for the scales "Activation Control", "Aggression", "Frustration" and "High Intensity Pleasure". Two common higher level factors corresponding to control and positive emotions were associated with the influences of "common environment", whereas negative emotions factor is influenced by genes. 2013 The Authors. Published by Elsevier by Elsevier Ltd. Open Ltd. access under CC BY-NC-ND license. Selection and/or peer-review under under responsibility of Russian of Russian Psychological Psychological Society Society. Keywords: genotype, environment, temperament, primary school children, self-regulation, positive emotionality, negative emotionality. 1. Introduction Research on etiology of individual differences in temperament is one of the most fast-developing fields in modern developmental psychology and behavioural genetics. Most approaches define temperament as individual * Corresponding author. Tel.: +8-909-953-49-98; fax: +8-909-953-49-98 E-mail address: novgorodova_yulia@inbox.ru 1877-0428 2013 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. Open access under CC BY-NC-ND license. Selection and/or peer-review under responsibility of Russian Psychological Society doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.08.593

436 Yulia Novgorodova et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 differences in behavioural style (activation, emotionality and self-regulation), which become noticeable during the early childhood (A.Thomas, S.Chess, A.Buss, R.Plomin, J.Kagan, M.Rothbart, B.M.Teplov, V.D.Nebylitsyn, V.M.Rusalov, V.S.Merlin, etc.). M. Rothbart and D. Derryberry [1] define temperament as constitutionally determined features of reactivity and self-regulation. The authors of this approach suppose that temperament develops over time: different emotions and their components appear in different ages [2]. Most modern theories suppose that temperament acquisition experiences the influence of genetic and environmental factors and is associated with the features of brain systems responsible for control of behaviour and functional states. The results on contribution of different components into variability of temperamental traits are similar in most twin studies: 40-50 % of variability are explained by genetic variance, the other 50 % belong to the remaining non-shared environment component. The role of genetic factors is studies on the basis of data form various diagnostic methods, including participants' self-report [3], experts' report (people who know the participants well; [4]), twins' report on each other [5], and also assessment of the participants' behaviour by judges [6]. Heritability of different temperamental traits is approximately the same [3]. For some temperamental traits MZ correlations are twice as high as DZ twins correlations; possibly, non-additive factors influence the variability of these traits (for example, extraversion; [7]). Analysis of sex differences in heritability shows possible gender differences in genetic effects on some temperamental traits, for example, neiroticism [3,7]. The studies of variability of temperamental traits in school age are rather rare. However, such works are quite relevant in terms of essential physiological changes, which significantly affect the changes of the structure of temperament. Apart from that, the adolescents can adequately estimate their own emotional states and behavioural traits, consequently, their self-reports can be used in research. For adolescents who can not give an adequate self-report, parent forms of quesrionnaire are used. An important limitation in past studies of temperament of adolescents and primary school children was the fact that most of their samples included the participants whose age varied within a wide range. Furthermore, data analysis was conducted without separation into smaller age groups. From this point of view, it would be interesting to analyze the data of genetically informative research of temperament in primary school children. We should notice that most genetic research on temperament has been conducted on Australian, American and European samples. Since the heritability index depends on the investigated population, it is advisable to conduct research of temperament on a Russian sample. The current study estimated the contributions of genetic and environmental influences into the variability of temperamental traits in children aged 7 to 10. 2. Study design and methods We assessed the temperament specifics with the use of Russian-language parent version of Rothbart's questionnaire "Early Adolescents Temperament" (EATQ-R; [8]). The questionnaire includes 10 scales: 1. Activation Control; 2. Affiliation; 3. Attention; 4. Fear; 5. Frustration; 6. Inhibitory Control; 7. Shyness; 8. High Intensity Pleasure; 9. Aggression; 10. Depressive Mood. Parent version of the questionnaire used in our research can be applied to primary school children as well. The sample included 170 children (85 twin pairs) aged 7 to 10: 49 monozygotic (MZ) twin pairs and 36 samesex dizygotic twin pairs. Twin subsamples were balanced in geder. Children age on average was 8.3 years (st.dev. 0.9). Questionnaires were completed by mothers. Contributions of genetic and environmental factors were estimated by means of structural equation modeling (model-fitting) using the [9]. Model-fitting is a statistical technique for testing complex hypotheses about variance structure. The method is based on mathematical algorithms, which allow for the

Yulia Novgorodova et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 437 to be decomposed into that due to genetic (A), shared environment (C), and nonshared environment (E) sources of variation according to standard biometrical methods. The full model (i.e., one specifying that there are influences from A, C, and E) was first fit to the data for each scale and factor and then modified, with a series of reduced models tested. The AE model tested no effect of shared environment, the CE model tested no genetic effect, and the E model tested no effect of either shared environment or genetics. A variant of maximumlikelihood approach (full-information maximum likelihood) was used for model-fitting with the raw data. The fit of nested models was evaluated using a -2*log- 2 equivalent) and the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) [10]. A more negative AIC value indicates a better fit. 3. Results and discussion 3.1. Psychometric analyses of EATQ questionnaire (parental form) The analysis of reliability for the scales of EATQ questionnaire (Cronbach's Alpha estimation for each scale) has shown that most of the scales are corresponding to the reliability standards for the questionnaire measures after some minor corrections. Fifty four items were left out of 62 original questions. The Cronbach's Alpha coefficients (see Table 1) for the 7 scales were higher than 0.6 (4 of them are closer to 0.7), and the remaining 3 scales (Fear, Inhibitory Control and Depressive Mood) are close to 0.6. Hence the psychometric properties of the questionnaire are acceptable for the use in research context. 3.2. Correlation analysis Correlation analyses of the EATQ scales yielded high correlations between such scales as Attention and Activation Control (.69), Attention and Inhibitory Control (.51), Aggression and Frustration (.61), all significant at the.01 level. 3.3. Factor analysis Factor analysis of EATQ scales (Principal Components, Varimax rotation) resulted in three higher order factors: Attention-Control-Consciousness, Extraversion-Positive Emotionality, and Fear-Negative Emotionality- Aggression (see Table 2), explaining 60.6% of summary variance. 3.4. Quantitative genetic analyses of the EATQ scales The results of structural equation modeling are presented in Table 3. The pattern of genetic and environmental is highly varied for different scales of EATQ (see Table 3). The E model did not fit well in any of the cases. The environmental CE (shared and non-shared environment) model gave the best fit for the 4 scales (Activation Control, Frustration, High Intensity Pleasure, and Aggressiveness). Moreover, the shared environment component explained more than 50% in all 4 instances. The Fear scale got a high genetic loading (over 0.70), and the ACE model appeared to be the best for the 4 scales (Attention, Shyness, Affiliation, and Depressive Mood), with the non-shared environment being the most significant component for all of them (see Table 3). Table 1. Reliability analyses for the EATQ scales

438 Yulia Novgorodova et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 EATQ Scale N items Alpha 1. Activation Control 7.73 2. Affiliation 6.62 3. Attention 6.65 4. Fear 5.59 5. Frustration 5.66 6. Inhibitory Control 5.56 7. Shyness 5.72 8. High Intensity Pleasure 7.69 9. Aggressiveness 5.68 10. Depressive Mood 3.59 Table 2. The results of factor analysis of EATQ scales Scale Components 1 2 3 1. Attention.836 2. Activation Control.810 3. Inhibitory Control.681 -.237 4. Aggressiveness -.604.212.543 5. Frustration -.435.587 6. Shyness -.799 7. Affiliation.639.250 8. High Intensity Pleasure.624 9. Depressive Mood -.491.431 10. Fear.824 % of explained variance 29.4 18.7 12.5 3.5. Discussion We conducted a genetically informative study of temperament on the basis of parental rating data. The le level. The within-pair correlations for parental reports are consistently high, and this finding corresponds to the reported results (e.g., [11]). Consequently, the contrast effects are possible here, so the results should be interpreted with caution [12].

Yulia Novgorodova et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 439 Table 3. Within-pair correlations and model-fitting results for the EATQ scales and factors Scale (factor) rmz rdz A C E p AIC AIC 1 Attention.49.33.36 (0-.68).48 (0-.56).16 (.31-.72) 0.597.90 131.02-5.403 2 Activation.40.39 -.41 (.21-.57).59 (.43-.79) 0.019.89 142.82-1.981 Control 3 Inhibitory Control.43 -.04.39 (.13- -.61 (.41-.87) 0-140.00-2.59) 4 Aggressiveness.46.60 -.53 (.35-.66).47 (.33-.64) 0-138.84-2 5 Frustration.55.54 -.54 (.37-.67).46 (.32-.62) 0.062.80 138.41-1.938 6 Shyness.48 -.09.40 (0-.58) 0 (0-.38).60 (.41-.83) 10.321.16 129.06 4.321 7 Affiliation.64.56.18 (0-.72).45 (0-.70).37 (.23-.55) 1.471.69 114.10-4.529 8 High Intensity.41.69 -.57 (.21-.70).43 (.28-.59) 9.155-114.01-2 Pleasure 9 Depressive Mood.68.45.47 (0-.79).21 (0-.61).32 (.20-.49) 0.564.90 111.44-5.436 1 0 Fear.79.24.75 (.60-.83) -.25 (.16-.39) 2.2-110.15-2 1 Attention- Control- Consciousness.48.55 -.52 (.44-.66).48 (.33-.65) 0.428-130.65-2 2 Extraversion- Positive Emotionality.51.46 -.49 (.30-.63).51 (.36-.69) 1.115.78 114.28-1.926 3 Fear-Negative Emotionality- Aggression.71.54.35 (0-.78).36 (0.-70).29 (.18-.45) 0.065.99 116.45-5.935 rmz, rdz, -pair correlations; A additive genetic factors component; C shared environment 2 component; E non-shared environment and measurement error component; goodness of fit index (the increment of -2LnL) for the nested models; p likelihood index (probability); AIC Criterion index increment for the nested models. 4. Conclusions Overally, our study has revealed a differentiated pattern of genetic and environmental influences on the temperamental traits in 7-10 years old children. The most significant contributions of genotype were found for such traits as Fear, Inhibitory Control, Shyness, and Depressive Mood. Significant shared environment contributions were obtained for the Activation Control, Affressiveness, Frustration, and High Intensity Pleasure. In general, these results are corresponding with the previously published genetic studies of temperament in youth samples (e.g., [7]). The results are still considered preliminary, and the study should be replicated on a larger sample to check for the contrast effects and possible nonadditive genetic factors contribution. 2

440 Yulia Novgorodova et al. / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 References [1]. Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. In M. E. Lamb & A. L. Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 37-86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [2]. Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Developing attentional skills. In J. Richards (Ed.),Cognitive neuroscience of attention: A developmental perspective. (pp. 317-323). Mahwah, NJ:Erlbaum. [3]. Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Genes and environment in personality development. Sage series on individual differences and development, Vol. 2. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. [4]. Riemann, R., Angleitner, A. & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and environmental influences on personality: A study of twins reared together using the self- and peer-report NEO-FFI scales.journal of Personality, 65, 449-475. [5]. Heath, A. C, Neale, M. C, Kessler, R. C, Eaves, L. J., & Kendler, K. S. (1992). Evidence for genetic influences on personality from self-reports and informant ratings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 85-96. [6]. Borkenau, P., Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Spinath, F. M. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on observed personality: Evidence from the German Observational Study of Adult Twins. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 80, 655-668. [7]. Eaves, L. J., Eysenck, H. J. & Martin, N. G. (1989). Genes, culture and personality: An empirical approach. San Diego, CA: Academic Press [8]. Ellis, L. K., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Revision of the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire. Poster presented at the 2001 Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, Minnesota. [9]. Neale, M.C. & Cardon, L.R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Norwood, MA: Kluwer Academic. [10]. Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52,317 332. [11]. Reiss, D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin, R. (2000). The Relationship Code: Deciphering Genetic and Social Influences on Adolescent Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [12]. Spinath, F. M., & Angleitner, A. (1998). Contrast effects in Buss and Plomin's EAS questionnaire: a behavioral-genetic study on early developing personality traits assessed through parental ratings. Personality and Individual Differences, 25: 947 963.