Normative Data for the Rey Osterrieth and the Taylor Complex Figure Tests in Quebec-French People

Similar documents
OLDER ADULTS LEARNING, MEMORY, AND COPY PERFORMANCE ON THE REY-OSTERRIETH AND MODIFIED TAYLOR COMPLEX FIGURES

Using the Rey Osterrieth and Modified Taylor Complex Figures with Older Adults: A Preliminary Examination of Accuracy Score Comparability

Trail Making Test part A part B. Mild Cognitive Impairment MCI MCI. Kennedy 1981 Bornstein Rey Osterrieth 3. Berry 1991.

PRELIMINARY NORMS FOR YEAR OLDS ON THE MEMORY TEST FOR OLDER ADULTS (MTOA:S) ABSTRACT

A semantic verbal fluency test for English- and Spanish-speaking older Mexican-Americans

Normative Data for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment in Middle-Aged and Elderly Quebec-French People

Trail making test A 2,3. Memory Logical memory Story A delayed recall 4,5. Rey auditory verbal learning test (RAVLT) 2,6

Base Rates of Impaired Neuropsychological Test Performance Among Healthy Older Adults

Adaptation and Standardization of the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test to the Moroccan Population

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised*

Hubley Depression Scale for Older Adults (HDS-OA): Reliability, Validity, and a Comparison to the Geriatric Depression Scale

Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Detecting Dementia With the Mini-Mental State Examination in Highly Educated Individuals

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is an intermediate

Recognizing Dementia can be Tricky

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Geriatric performance on the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination (Cognistat) What is normal?

Key words : I. Broca, P 150. Broca. Vol. 19 No

Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) Overview for Best Practice in Stroke and Complex Neurological Conditions March 2013

Cognitive Reserve and the Relationship Between Depressive Symptoms and Awareness of Deficits in Dementia

Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales

Moca test instructions

After the Diagnosis: Rehabilitation & Support Options for Mild Dementia

CHAPTER 2 CRITERION VALIDITY OF AN ATTENTION- DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER (ADHD) SCREENING LIST FOR SCREENING ADHD IN OLDER ADULTS AGED YEARS

Cognitive dysfunction in rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

LOTCA Assessment review. Georgina Wrack. University of the Sunshine Coast

Supplemental Information. Acquiring the Knowledge. of London s Layout. Drives Structural Brain Changes. Katherine Woollett and Eleanor A.

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments

NEUROPSYCHOMETRIC TESTS

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY PSYC32

Research Article The Takeda Three Colors Combination Test: A Screening Test for Detection of Very Mild Alzheimer s Disease

Assessment of Memory

On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013)

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

21/05/2018. Today s webinar will answer. Presented by: Valorie O Keefe Consultant Psychologist

COLOURED PROGRESSIVE MATRICES: ERROR TYPE IN DEMENTIA AND MEMORY DYSFUNCTION. D. Salmaso, G. Villaggio, S. Copelli, P. Caffarra

Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised: Norms for Elderly African Americans

Are people with Intellectual disabilities getting more or less intelligent II: US data. Simon Whitaker

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Meniere s Disease Case. Suzanne Beason-Hazen, Ph.D.

- Supporting Information. In order to better delineate the activity related to target processing, we analyzed

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents

Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture Fragment Completion Paradigm

The Short NART: Cross-validation, relationship to IQ and some practical considerations

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P

Identification of Cognitive Impairment in HIV patients. Belinda Vicioso MD FACP, AGSF Jose Garcia Professor of Medicine UTSW

Standardization and Validation of Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in the Moroccan Population

Sociodemographic Effects on the Test-Retest Reliability of the Big Five Inventory. Timo Gnambs. Osnabrück University. Author Note

COMPARISON OF THE BECK DEPRESSION INVENTORY-II AND GERIATRIC DEPRESSION SCALE AS SCREENS FOR DEPRESSION IN CARDIAC PATIENTS

Depression is frequent in older adults, affecting

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory

Test-retest reliable coefficients and 5-year change scores for the MMSE and 3MS

Alzheimer's Disease - Activities of Daily Living Inventory AD-ADL

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition

Intra-Individual Reaction Time Variability in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer s Disease: Gender, Processing Load and Speed Factors

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

APPENDIX A TASK DEVELOPMENT AND NORMATIVE DATA

HPS301 Exam Notes- Contents

THE ROLE OF ACTIVITIES OF DAILY LIVING IN THE MCI SYNDROME

Title: Reliability and validity of the adolescent stress questionnaire in a sample of European adolescents - the HELENA study

Plenary Session 2 Psychometric Assessment. Ralph H B Benedict, PhD, ABPP-CN Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry SUNY Buffalo

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly

Neuropsychological Test Development and Normative Data on Hispanics

Currently Used Measures of Cognitive Impairment An HBRN Scholar Special Presentation & Discussion

Neuropsychological Evaluation of

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Quality of Life Assessment of Growth Hormone Deficiency in Adults (QoL-AGHDA)

Test Assessment Description Ref. Global Deterioration Rating Scale Dementia severity Rating scale of dementia stages (2) (4) delayed recognition

Korean-VCI Harmonization Standardization- Neuropsychology Protocol (K-VCIHS-NP)

Carmen Inoa Vazquez, Ph.D., ABPP Clinical Professor NYU School of Medicine Lead Litigation Conference Philadelphia May 19, 2009 Presentation

Recognition of Alzheimer s Disease: the 7 Minute Screen

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Detecting neurocognitive impairment in HIV-infected youth: Are we focusing on the wrong factors?

Disclosure statement

Technical Specifications

DOWNLOAD PDF THE EFFECT OF AEROBIC EXERCISE ON INFORMATION PROCESSING IN OLDER ADULTS

The Flynn effect and memory function Sallie Baxendale ab a

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

International Forum on HIV and Rehabilitation Research

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome

Running head: INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 1. Why to treat subjects as fixed effects. James S. Adelman. University of Warwick.

Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Lambros Messinis PhD. Neuropsychology Section, Department of Neurology, University of Patras Medical School

On applying the matching law to between-subject data

Equating UDS Neuropsychological Tests: 3.0>2.0, 3.0=2.0, 3.0<2.0? Dan Mungas, Ph.D. University of California, Davis

SOCIABLE - NEXT GENERATION COGNITIVE TRAINING USING MULTI-TOUCH SURFACE COMPUTERS

UNDERSTANDING CAPACITY & DECISION-MAKING VIDEO TRANSCRIPT

The NART as an index of prior intellectual functioning: a retrospective validity study covering a 66-year interval

Test Validity. What is validity? Types of validity IOP 301-T. Content validity. Content-description Criterion-description Construct-identification

Transcription:

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 30 (2015) 78 87 Normative Data for the Rey Osterrieth and the Taylor Complex Figure Tests in Quebec-French People Marie-Pier Tremblay 1,2, Olivier Potvin 2, Brandy L. Callahan 1,2, Sylvie Belleville 3,4, Jean- François Gagnon 5,6, Nicole Caza 3,4, Guylaine Ferland 3,6,7, Carol Hudon 1,2, Joël Macoir 2,8, * 1 École de psychologie, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada 2 Centre de recherche de l Institut universitaire en santé mentale de Québec, Québec, QC, Canada 3 Centre de recherche de l Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada 4 Département de psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada 5 Département de psychologie, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada 6 Centre de recherche, Hôpital du Sacré-Cœur de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada 7 Département de nutrition, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada 8 Département de réadaptation, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada *Corresponding author at: Faculté de médecine, Département de réadaptation, Université Laval, Pavillon Ferdinand-Vandry, QC, Canada G1V 0A6. Tel.: +1-418-656-2131; Fax: +1-418-656-5476. E-mail address: joel.macoir@fmed.ulaval.ca (J. Macoir). Accepted 12 November 2014 Abstract The Rey-Osterrieth (ROCF) and Taylor (TCF) complex figure tests are widely used to assess visuospatial and constructional abilities as well as visual/non-verbal memory. Normative data adjusted to the cultural and linguistic reality of older Quebec-French individuals is still nonexistent for these tests. In this article, we report the results of two studies that aimed to establish normative data for Quebec-French people (aged at least 50 years) for the copy, immediate recall, and delayed recall trials of the ROCF (Study 1) and the TCF (Study 2). For both studies, the impact of age, education, and sex on test performance was examined. Moreover, the impact of copy time on test performance, the impact of copy score on immediate and delayed recall score, and the impact of immediate recall score on delayed recall performance were examined. Based on regression models, equations to calculate Z scores for copy and recall scores are provided for both tests. Keywords: Assessment; Learning and Memory; Norms/normative studies Introduction The Rey Osterrieth (ROCF; Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941) and Taylor (TCF; Taylor, 1969) complex figure tests are commonly used in both clinical and research settings to assess a variety of cognitive processes, such as memory, motor function, perceptual abilities, and components of executive function (Meyers & Meyers, 1995). For both tests, the standard procedure requires participants to copy an 18-item figure and then, without prior warning, to reproduce it from memory after a 3-min delay (Osterrieth, 1944). Some investigators or clinicians may also include a delayed recall and/or a recognition trial (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006). Based on the fact that the ROCF and the TCF contain equal numbers of elements that are presumably of comparable complexity, both figures have long been assumed to be equivalent (Lezak, 1983). Accordingly, for both these tests, scores are generally interpreted using norms developed for the ROCF (Strauss et al., 2006). However, previous studies have shown that participants obtain significantly higher scores on both immediate and delayed recall trials of the TCF when compared with the ROCF (e.g., Strauss & Spreen, 1990; Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Hubley, 1992; Yamashita, 2006). These results were also observed in test retest situations, even when the sequence of administration is counterbalanced (Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Hubley, 1992). Moreover, although one study in young adults found comparable scores between a modified version of the TCF and the ROCF (Yamashita, 2006), another # The Author 2014. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com. doi:10.1093/arclin/acu069 Advance Access publication on 3 December 2014

M.-P. Tremblay et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 30 (2015); 78 87 79 study with older adults showed that scores at copy were higher on the modified TCF, but recall/memory performance was comparable between tests (Hubley, 2010). According to Strauss and Spreen(1990), using normative data established for the ROCF may result in an overestimation of memory levels in patients that are given the TCF. As such, norms should be developed for each figure separately. One study sought to establish norms specifically for the TCF in 407 participants aged between 20 and 79 years old (Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Schmidt, 1992). However, an intentional learning paradigm was employed with a subset of these participants (n ¼ 63). Thus, participants knew that they would be asked to recall the figure from memory after a delay. Unfortunately, no normative data are currently available for the original implicit learning TCF task, which is the typical mode of administration for this test. Sociodemographic variables such as age, education, and sex have been demonstrated to influence performance on neuropsychological tests. In previous studies, a relationship has been demonstrated between age and performance on the ROCF. While a few studies reported no change in copy performance with normal aging (e.g., Hartman & Potter, 1998; Mitrushina, Satz, & Chervinsky, 1990; Vingerhoets, Lannoo, & Wolters, 1998), several authors demonstrated an age-related decline in performance on every condition of the test (e.g., Boone, Lesser, Hill-Gutierrez, Berman, & D elia, 1993; Caffarra, Vezzadini, Dieci, Zonato, & Venneri, 2002; Machulda et al., 2007; Meyers & Meyers, 1995; Rosselli & Ardila, 1991). Similarly, increasing age has been associated with a decline in the ability to learn and remember the TCF (Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Hubley, 1992; Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Schmidt, 1992;). Regarding the effect of education, some studies have found lower education levels to be associated with poorer scores on the ROCF (Caffarra et al., 2002; Fastenau, Denburg, & Hufford, 1999; Pontón et al., 1996; Rosselli & Ardila, 1991), while others have found performance to be unaffected by education (Boone et al., 1993; Meyers & Meyers, 1995). Finally, some studies revealed small or no differences between males and females on ROCF performance (Berry, Allen, & Schmitt, 1991; Boone et al., 1993; Chiulli, Haaland, LaRue, & Garry, 1995; Meyers & Meyers, 1995; Peña-Casanova et al., 2009), while other researchers reported that sex had an influence on performance, with males performing better on every conditions of the test (Gallagher & Burke, 2007) or on the delayed recall trial only (Caffarra et al., 2002). Neither sex nor education is associated with performance on the TCF (Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Hubley, 1992; Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Schmidt, 1992). In addition to considering sociodemographic variables when interpreting non-verbal neuropsychological tasks, several authors have also highlighted the influence of culture on ROCF performance, which may be a result of specific training provided through formal education (e.g., Rosselli & Ardila, 1991; Rosselli & Ardila, 2003). Indeed, non-verbal tests may be culturally biased, and great caution is needed when interpreting the results of an individual belonging to a different culture than that of the normative sample (Rosselli & Ardila, 2003). The importance of developing norms adapted to a given culture has also been highlighted by a study showing that local norms are more sensitive and accurate than non-cultural specific norms for identifying cognitive difficulties in older adults (Arsenault-Lapierre et al., 2011). In this respect, normative studies have been carried out for the ROCF in community-dwelling older Italians (Caffarra et al., 2002), English (e.g., Boone et al., 1993; Fastenau et al., 1999; Machulda et al., 2007; Meyers & Meyers, 1995) and Spanish-speaking Americans (Pontón et al., 1996), as well as Spanish individuals (Peña-Casanova et al., 2009). Given these considerations, normative data are needed for Quebec-French individuals. Indeed, these individuals differ from other North-Americans subpopulations, as well as from other French-Canadians and French-speaking people worldwide on several aspects (e.g., spoken language and dialectal variation, education system). Considering the impact of culture and sociodemographic variables on ROCF and TCF task performance, the present work aimed to establish normative data for both tests in a Quebec-French population. The impact of age, education, and sex was examined for the copy score, immediate recall and delayed recall of the ROCF (Study 1) and the TCF (Study 2). In addition, the impact of copy time on test performance was also examined since it implies longer encoding time. Moreover, performance at copy may influence performance at memory trials. Therefore, the impact of copy score on immediate and delayed recall score and the impact of immediate recall score on delayed recall performance were examined. The two studies included different samples of older adults. Study 1 This study provides normative data for the ROCF in Quebec-French individuals. Methods Participants. Researchers across the province of Quebec (Canada) were invited to share anonymized data from healthy volunteers who had completed the ROCF as part of other research studies approved by local Research Ethics Boards. Secondary data from those studies were used in this normative study. Participants originated from four recruiting sites (i.e., three laboratories in Montreal and one in Quebec City). All participants included in the study were Caucasian, at least 50 years of age, living independently in the community, and reported French as their mother tongue and usual language. All subjects scored within normal ranges on the mini-mental state

80 M.-P. Tremblay et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 30 (2015); 78 87 examination (MMSE.26; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) or the Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA.26; Nasreddine et al., 2005) and on the geriatric depression scale (GDS,11; Yesavage, Brink, Rose, & Adey, 1983), indicating no cognitive impairment or depressive symptomatology. A self-reported medical and psychiatric history was also obtained from each participant. All participants self-reported good mental and physical health (e.g., no history of neurological disease, past or current psychiatric illness, traumatic brain injury, medical condition that could interfere with cognitive performance). The final normative sample consisted of 220 community-dwelling participants (64 males and 156 females), between 50 and 91 years of age (mean age ¼ 67.56 years; SD ¼ 7.51) who had between 3 and 24 years of formal education (mean education level ¼ 14.43 years; SD ¼ 3.69) (see Table 1 for normative sample description). Highly educated men and women of all age ranges are overrepresented in our sample compared with actual Quebec demographics. Also, women were overrepresented in our sample. Materials and Procedure. The original French version of the ROCF (Meyers & Meyers, 1995; Rey, 1941) was individually administered to all subjects as part of a larger neuropsychological battery. The only deviation from the standard administration is that participants were given no time limit to copy the figure. They were asked to copy the figure as accurately as possible and while doing so, participants used a series of colored pencils. This procedure allowed the experimenter to preserve a record of the order in which elements of the figure were reproduced. Either 0 or 3 min following completion of the copy, subjects were asked to draw the figure from memory, as accurately as possible and with no time limit. Participants were not informed that they would subsequently be asked to reproduce the figure from memory. Finally, 20 min following completion of the immediate recall, subjects were asked to draw the figure from memory again. The delay interval was filled with verbal neuropsychological tasks. According to Osterrieth s scoring system (adapted by Taylor, 1969), the maximum possible score is 36 for the copy and recall trials. Doctoral students and trained psychometrists administered and scored the test, assigning 0 2 points for each of the 18 elements of the figure based on distortion and placement. Statistical Analyses. First, to identify the variables influencing performance on the ROCF, a standard multiple regression analysis was performed for each dependent variable (copy score, immediate recall score, delayed recall score, and copy time) with Age, Sex, and Education as predictors. Second, as Copy time may impact performance on recall trials, it was also assessed as a predictor in the regression model for immediate and delayed recall scores. Similarly, Copy score was entered as a predictor in the regression model for immediate recall score. Third, Copy score and Immediate recall score were entered as predictors in the regression model for delayed recall score. Finally, interactions between predictors were tested. When significant, interactions were added as predictors. Because the length of the immediate recall delay (0 or 3 min) had no significant effect on delayed recall scores when entered as a predictor (p ¼.117), both procedures were grouped into a single analysis. All variables were entered in the analyses as continuous variables except Sex, which was coded as 0 for women and 1 for men. Visual and statistical analyses were conducted to verify the underlying assumptions of the regression model. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software (version 16.0) with the a level set at 5%. Table 1. Normative samples description Age range N (F : M) Education ROCF 50 59 31 (15 : 16) 14.65 (2.43) 10 19 60 64 44 (32 : 12) 14.98 (3.75) 5 23 65 69 43 (34 : 9) 14.33 (3.68) 6 22 70 74 69 (52 : 17) 14.33 (3.68) 3 21 75 79 19 (14 : 5) 14.79 (3.28) 11 22 80 91 14 (9 : 5) 12.50 (5.03) 5 24 TCF 70 74 128 (68 : 60) 13.30 (4.27) 5 23 75 79 166 (78 : 88) 13.61 (4.64) 2 23 80 91 138 (86 : 52) 11.86 (3.87) 3 20 M (SD) Range Notes: These categories of age and education were not used to produce normative data and are for indicative purpose only. One should note that people aged,60 years and 80 years or older are underrepresented in the ROCF sample while people,70 years are underrepresented in the TCF sample. Therefore, results for these age clustered should be interpreted carefully. ROCF ¼ Rey Osterrieth complex figure; TCF ¼ Taylor complex figure; M ¼ Mean; SD ¼ standard deviation.

M.-P. Tremblay et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 30 (2015); 78 87 81 Results Using univariate analyses, it was found that participants who were administered the immediate recall 3-min delay procedure were older (t(218) 2.347, p ¼.020, d ¼ 0.35), more highly educated (t(218) 2.665, p ¼.008, d ¼ 0.40), and included a higher proportion of women compared with participants with the 0-min delay (x 2 ¼ 32.27, df:1,p,.001). However, despite these differences in characteristics, participants did not differ in terms of immediate recall scores (t(218) 1.324, p ¼.187, d ¼ 0.20) or delayed recall scores (t(180) ¼ 0.936, p ¼.350, d ¼ 0.15). As such, both procedures were grouped into a single regression analysis. In this section, results for the copy, immediate recall (0- and 3-min), and delayed recall (20-min) conditions will be presented separately. Descriptive statistics for each condition of the test as well as for copy time are presented in Table 2. One should note that data were missing for 38 participants on the delayed recall trial and for 8 participants regarding copy time. Regression analyses were first run with sociodemographic variables as predictors of copy time and copy, immediate recall and delayed recall scores. Analyses indicated that Age (b ¼ 0.175, p ¼.010) and Education (b ¼ 20.169, p ¼.014) explained a significant amount of the variance of the copy time (R 2 ¼.077, F(3, 208) ¼ 5.775, p ¼.001). More precisely, older and less educated people took more time to copy the figure. Also, analyses showed that Education (b ¼ 0.185, p ¼.007) explained a significant amount of the variance of the copy score (R 2 ¼.044, F(3, 216) ¼ 3.312, p ¼.021); less educated people performed worse at copy trial. Copy time was significantly correlated with copy score (r ¼ 2.145, p ¼.035), with higher copy time associated with lower copy score. Next, Age (b ¼ 20.179, p ¼.006) and Education (b ¼ 0.257, p,.001) explained a significant amount of the variance of the immediate recall score (R 2 ¼.113, F(3, 216) ¼ 9.209, p,.001). Finally, Age (b ¼ 20.187, p ¼.010) and Education (b ¼ 0.249, p ¼.001) both explained a significant amount of the variance of the delayed recall score (R 2 ¼.105, F(3, 178) ¼ 6.959, p,.001). For both memory trials, higher age and lower education level were associated with poorer performance. Regression analyses were subsequently re-run with sociodemographic variables and Copy time as predictors of copy score. Results indicated that Education (b ¼ 0.165, p ¼.019) explained a significant amount of variance of the copy score (R 2 ¼.057, F(4, 207) ¼ 3.148, p ¼.015); less educated people performed worse at copy trial. Regression analyses were also run with sociodemographic variables, Copy time and Copy score as predictors of immediate recall scores. Three multivariate outliers were identified according to Mahalanobis distances (p,.001) and were removed from the analyses. Results showed that Age (b ¼ 20.116, p ¼.042), Education (b ¼ 0.150, p ¼.010), Copy score (b ¼ 0.215, p ¼.001), and Copy time (b ¼ 20.314, p,.001) explained a significant amount of variance of the immediate recall score (R 2 ¼.391, F(6, 202) ¼ 21.654, p,.001). Specifically, older, less educated people who took longer to copy the figure and obtained poorer copy score were those who obtained poorer score at immediate recall trial. Immediate recall score was also influenced by the Sex Copy score interaction (b ¼ 0.232, p,.001). This interaction indicated that immediate recall score was more strongly influenced by copy score in men than in women. Finally, regression analyses were run with sociodemographic variables, Copy time, Copy score, and Immediate recall score as predictors of delayed recall score. Analyses indicated that Copy time (b ¼ 0.087, p ¼.011), Copy score (b ¼ 0.127, p,.001), and Immediate recall score (b ¼ 0.893, p,.001) explained a significant amount of the variance of the delayed recall score (R 2 ¼.852, F(7, 166) ¼ 136.345, p,.001). Participants who obtained higher copy and immediate recall scores and who took more time to copy the figure obtained higher score at delayed recall. Delayed recall score was also influenced by the Immediate recall score Copy time interaction (b ¼ 0.083, p ¼.007). This interaction indicated that longer copy time enhanced performance on the delayed recall in participants with high, but not low, immediate recall score. The bivariate correlation between immediate and delayed recall scores was very strong (r ¼.908, p,.001) and the semi-partial correlations from the regression model indicated that this relationship remained strong (r ¼.731) in presence of all other predictors, which had modest associations (p ¼.116). Table 2. Basic descriptive statistics for each variable Variable N M SD Range ROCF Copy score 220 31.30 3.78 17.5 36 Immediate recall 0-s 64 14.58 6.50 1.5 28 Immediate recall 3-min 156 15.75 5.76 4 31 Delayed recall 182 15.25 5.83 2 30 Copy time 212 239.46 92.62 69 480 TCF Copy score 432 30.37 2.78 19 36 Immediate recall 3-min 432 18.20 5.15 4 34 Delayed recall 430 17.93 4.90 5 33 Copy time 427 242.89 96.48 80 480 Notes: ROCF ¼ Rey Osterrieth complex figure; TCF¼Taylor complex figure. Copy time is measured in seconds.

82 M.-P. Tremblay et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 30 (2015); 78 87 Table 3. Information to calculate Z scores with sociodemographic variables for the Rey Osterrieth complex figure Equations to calculate Z scores Z ¼ (real score 2 expected score)/square root of the mean square residual Expected score Square root of the mean square residual Copy 0.187E 2 0.946S 2 0.029A + 30.858 3.664 Immediate recall 0.417E + 0.568S 2 0.143A + 18.905 5.679 Delayed recall 0.393E + 0.000S 2 0.145A + 19.388 5.557 Notes: E ¼ Education in years; A ¼ Age in years; S ¼ Sex (1 ¼ men and 0 ¼ women). Table 4. Information to calculate Z scores with all predictors for the Rey Osterrieth complex figure Based on the results from the regression models, equations to calculate Z scores for the copy, immediate and delayed recall scores are presented in Tables 3 and 4. Table 3 includes regression equations to calculate Z scores with sociodemographic variables alone, while Table 4 includes regression equations to calculate Z scores with all variables (i.e., sociodemographic, copy time, copy score, and immediate recall score). The application of the equations in clinical practice is explained in the General discussion. Discussion Equations to calculate Z scores Z ¼ (real score 2 expected score)/square root of the mean square residual Expected score Copy 0.166E 2 1.018S 2 0.019A 2 0.005CT + 31.673 3.648 Immediate recall 0.243E + 0.966S 2 0.095A 2 0.021CT + 0.353 (CS 2 31.2955) + 0.941 (SX(CS 2 31.2955)) + 23.147 4.705 Delayed recall 0.010E 2 0.280S 2 0.030A + 0.006 (CT 2 238.1975) + 0.199CS + 0.868 (IRS 2 15.4114) + 0.001 ((IRS 2 15.4114) (CT 2 238.1975)) + 11.205 2.289 Square root of the mean square residual Notes: A ¼ Age (years); E ¼ Education (years); CT ¼ Copy time (s); CS ¼ Copy score (max ¼ 36); IRS ¼ Immediate recall score (max ¼ 36); S ¼ Sex (1 ¼ men and 0 ¼ women). The aim of Study 1 was to assess the effect of sociodemographic variables on ROCF performance and establish normative data for the Quebec-French population. Age was found to be associated with copy time as well as immediate and delayed recall scores. The positive association between age and copy time can be explained by the fact that processing speed is known to be highly associated with age, with older adults being generally slower than younger adults (Verhaeghen & Salthouse, 1997). Also, the negative associations between age and immediate and delayed recall scores are consistent with the fact that memory for visuospatial stimuli decreases with age (Haaland, Linn, Hunt, & Goodwin, 1983; Koss, Haxby, DeCarli, Shapiro, & Rapoport, 1991; Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Hubley, 1992). Overall, the results obtained here are in line with previous work that showed an age-related decline in ROCF performance (Boone et al., 1993; Caffarra et al., 2002; Machulda et al., 2007; Meyers & Meyers, 1995; Rosselli & Ardila, 1991). Results also indicated that participants education level was associated with all test measures (i.e., copy time, copy score, and immediate and delayed recall scores). Higher education level was associated with faster copy time and better scores on the other measures, which is generally in line with results of previous normative studies (Caffarra et al., 2002; Fastenau et al., 1999; Pontón et al., 1996; Rosselli & Ardila, 1991). Finally, consistent with previous normative studies (Berry et al., 1991; Boone et al., 1993; Chiulli et al., 1995; Meyers & Meyers, 1995; Peña-Casanova et al., 2009), sex was not directly associated with performance on any condition of the test. However, one should note that sex had an impact on the relationship between copy time and immediate recall, which was not examined in previous studies. Copy time was found to be associated with immediate recall score. Indeed, longer copy time was associated with poorer performance at immediate recall. According to Bennett-Levy (1984), this may be due to the fact that subjects using less effective strategies tend to lose their way while copying the figure, resulting in higher copy time. In other words, longer copy time may reflect a loss of set, in that patients may lose track of initial objective. Accordingly, Newman and Krikorian (2001) suggested that the use of an organized strategy at copy, resulting in more efficient encoding, might yield less demanding recall. Consequently, longer copy time may be a result of less efficient organization strategies and therefore lead to lower recall scores. Copy time was

M.-P. Tremblay et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 30 (2015); 78 87 83 also found to be associated with delayed recall score. However, here longer copy time was associated with higher performance at delayed recall trial in participants with higher immediate recall score. This association is more intuitive and can be explained by the fact that longer encoding time may strengthen the memory trace for subsequent retrieval performance. In fact, according to Bennett-Levy (1984), longer copy time may also be the result of better care taken to copy the figure, or better attention paid to details. Consequently, different cognitive processes may underlie immediate and delayed recall; the former might rely more on working memory and organizational abilities and the latter on visual episodic memory consolidation. However, further research is needed to investigate this hypothesis. Finally, immediate recall score was more strongly related to copy score in men than in women. That is, a higher copy score is more likely to result in a higher immediate recall score in men than in women, a result for which literature does not allow us to provide any possible explanation. Also, immediate recall score was more strongly related to delayed recall when copy time was longer. In other words, as copy time increases, so does the strength of the relationship between immediate and delayed recall scores. Study 2 This study was aimed at providing normative data for the TCF in the Quebec-French population. Methods Participants. Participants were recruited as described in Study 1. The same inclusion and exclusion criteria were used, but participants from Study 2 were recruited independently from Study 1. Thus, no participants were administered both the ROCF and the TCF. In this study, participants originated from only one recruiting site (i.e., one laboratory in Montreal). The final sample consisted of 432 community-dwelling older adults (233 women and 199 men) between 71 and 86 years old (mean age ¼ 77.38 years; SD ¼ 3.92). Years of education varied between 3 and 23 years (mean education level ¼ 12.92 years; SD ¼ 4.23) (see Table 1 for a full description of the normative sample). Highly educated men and women of all age ranges are overrepresented in our sample compared with actual Quebec demographics (Institut de la statistique du Québec, 2006). Materials and Procedure. The TCF (Taylor, 1969) was administered individually to all subjects using the same procedure, instructions, and scoring method as in Study 1 (Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941). However, in the present study, the 0-min immediate recall trial was not used. Statistical Analyses. The analyses were identical to those of Study 1. The dependent variables were copy time, copy score, immediate recall score (3-min), and delayed recall score. Results Descriptive statistics for each condition of the test as well as for copy time are presented in Table 2. Data were missing for two participants on the delayed recall trial and for five participants regarding the copy time. Regression analyses were first run with sociodemographic variables as predictors of copy, immediate and delayed recall scores. The results of the regression analyses indicated that Age (b ¼ 0.134, p ¼.005) and Sex (b ¼ 20.107, p ¼.029) explained a significant amount of the variance of the copy time (R 2 ¼.046, F(3, 423) ¼ 6.764, p,.001), with women and older people taking more time to copy the figure. Results also indicated that Age (b ¼ 20.144, p ¼.002), Education (b ¼ 0.216, p,.001), and Sex (b ¼ 20.095, p ¼.048) explained a significant amount of the variance of the copy score (R 2 ¼.073, F(3, 428) ¼ 11.193, p,.001). More specifically, lower age and higher education were associated with higher copy score. Moreover, women were found to score higher than men. Copy time was not significantly correlated with copy score (r ¼.075, p ¼.120). Regarding recall scores, results revealed that only one predictor explained a significant amount of the variance of the immediate recall (R 2 ¼.047, F(3, 428) ¼ 6.972, p,.001) and delayed recall (R 2 ¼.057, F(3, 426) ¼ 8.629, p,.001) scores. It was found that only Age significantly predicted immediate (b ¼ 20.198, p,.001) and delayed (b ¼ 20.213, p,.001) recall scores. On both trials, the oldest participants obtained the lowest scores. Regression analyses were also performed with sociodemographic variables and Copy time as predictors of copy score. Results showed that Age (b ¼ 20.160, p ¼.001), Education (b ¼ 0.226, p,.001), and Copy time (b ¼ 0.117, p ¼.014) explained a significant amount of the variance of the copy score (R 2 ¼.0864, F(4, 422) ¼ 9.905, p,.001). More precisely, older and less educated people who took less time to copy the figure obtained lower scores. Regression analyses were then performed with sociodemographic variables, Copy time, and Copy score as predictors of immediate recall score. Results indicated that Age

84 M.-P. Tremblay et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 30 (2015); 78 87 Table 5. Information to calculate Z scores with sociodemographic variables for the Taylor complex figure Equations to calculate Z scores Z ¼ (real score 2 expected score)/square root of the mean square residual Expected score Square root of the mean square residual Copy 0.142E 2 0.102A 2 0.528S + 36.700 2.685 Immediate recall 0.083E 2 0.261A 2 0.184S + 37.378 5.046 Delayed recall 0.105E 2 0.267A 2 0.501S + 37.438 4.778 Notes: E ¼ Education in years; A ¼ Age in years; S ¼ Sex (1 ¼ men and 0 ¼ women). Table 6. Information to calculate Z scores with all predictors for the Taylor complex figure (b ¼ 20.129, p ¼.004) and Copy score (b ¼ 0.377, p,.001) explained a significant amount of the variance of the immediate recall score (R 2 ¼.204, F(6, 420) ¼ 17.977, p,.001). Specifically, higher age and lower copy score were associated with lower immediate recall score. Immediate recall was also significantly influenced by the Copy time Sex interaction (b ¼ 20.213, p,.001), indicating that Copy time was negatively associated with immediate recall in men but positively associated with this variable in women. Finally, regression analyses were performed with sociodemographic variables, Copy time, Copy score, and Immediate recall score as predictors of the delayed recall score. Results indicated that only Immediate recall score (b ¼ 0.887, p,.001) significantly predicted delayed recall score (R 2 ¼.838, F(6, 418) ¼ 360.331, p,.001). More specifically, higher immediate recall score was associated with higher delayed recall score. The bivariate correlation between these variables was very strong (r ¼.913, p,.001) and immediate recall score remained strongly associated with delayed recall score even when adjusting for all other predictors, as shown by the semi-partial correlation from the regression model (r ¼.894). Based on the results from the regression models, equations to calculate Z scores for the copy, immediate and delayed recall scores of the TCF are presented in Tables 5 and 6. Table 5 includes regression equations to calculate Z scores with sociodemographic variables alone, while Table 6 includes regression equations to calculate Z scores with all variables (i.e., sociodemographic, copy time, copy score, and immediate recall score). The clinical use of these equations will be explained in the General discussion. Discussion Equations to calculate Z scores Z ¼ (real score 2 expected score)/square root of the mean square residual Expected score Copy 0.148E 2 0.113A 2 0.459S + 0.003 (CT 2 242.891475) + 36.624 2.669 Immediate recall 20.023E 2 0.170A + 0.032S + 0.699CS + 0.002 (CT 2 242.891475) 2 0.017 4.627 ((CT 2 242.891475) XS) + 10.273 Delayed recall 0.022E 2 0.034A 2 0.335S 2 0.001CT + 0.074CS + 0.845IRS + 3.163 1.988 Square root of the mean square residual Notes: A ¼ Age (years); E ¼ Education (years); CT ¼ Copy time (seconds); CS ¼ Copy score (max ¼ 36); IRS ¼ Immediate recall score (max ¼ 36); S ¼ Sex (1 ¼ men and 0 ¼ women). The aim of Study 2 was to assess the effect of demographic variables on TCF performance and establish normative data for the Quebec-French population. Results indicated an age-related decline in the ability to copy the TCF and recall it after 3- and 20-min delays, which corroborate results from previous studies using the intentional learning variant of the TCF (Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Hubley, 1992; Tombaugh, Faulker, & Schmidt, 1992). These findings are also in line with those of Study 1 as well as with previous studies using the ROCF (Boone et al., 1993; Caffarra et al., 2002; Machulda et al., 2007; Meyers & Meyers, 1995; Rosselli & Ardila, 1991). In fact, the negative effect of age on all conditions of the test is consistent with previous work that showed an age-related decrement in organizational abilities (Mitrushina et al., 1990), visuospatial skills, and visual memory (Haaland et al., 1983; Koss et al., 1991). Education was found to be positively associated with copy score, but not with immediate or delayed recall performance, which is in line with the results reported by Tombaugh, Faulkner, and Schmidt (1992). Moreover, the positive association between

M.-P. Tremblay et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 30 (2015); 78 87 85 education and copy score is consistent with the results obtained in Study 1 as well as with previous studies using the ROCF (Caffarra et al., 2002; Fastenau et al., 1999; Pontón et al., 1996; Rosselli & Ardila, 1991). In the present sample, sex was not associated with immediate or delayed recall of the TCF. This finding is consistent with results from a previous study using the TCF with an intentional learning paradigm (Tombaugh et al., 1992). On the copy trial, however, women obtained higher scores than men, which might be associated with the fact that women generally took more time to copy the figure than men. However, further research is needed to examine the association between copy time and copy score on the TCF. Finally, it was shown that longer copy time was associated with better immediate recall score in women, but with worse immediate recall performance in men. It is possible that longer copy time may reflect increased attention to detail or scrupulousness in women, whereas it may be a mark of impairment in men. However, further studies are needed to investigate and clarify this hypothesis. General Discussion Considering the impact of culture and sociodemographic variables on ROCF and TCF performance, the present studies aimed to establish Quebec-French normative data for the copy as well as immediate and delayed recall scores of both tests. The impact of sociodemographic factors on task performance was investigated. Also, the impact of copy time on test performance was examined. Because it has been determined that the ROCF and TCF are not equivalent memory measures (Strauss & Spreen, 1990; Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Hubley, 1992; Yamashita, 2006), normative data were developed for each test separately. To our knowledge, this study is the first to establish normative data for the ROCF and the TCF in a French-Canadian sample. An original aspect of both studies was to measure the association between copy time and subsequent recall scores. Copy time was found to be an important variable in the prediction of performance on immediate and delayed recall for both figures. However, its effect on both was not consistent. For the ROCF, copy time was found to be positively associated with delayed recall score, but negatively associated with immediate recall score. For the TCF, the direction of the association varied as a function of sex. More precisely, it was found that longer copy time was associated with better immediate memory performance in women but worse memory performance in men. Consequently, copy time may represent a contaminated index of performance (Bennett-Levy, 1984) and may be influenced by numerous factors such as motor slowing, cognitive difficulties or simply thoroughness. As such, it is important in clinical practice to carefully interpret Z scores calculated using copy time as a predictor of task performance, and use these measures in conjunction with clinical observations and other test results. Another original aspect of the present study was the estimation of the association between copy score and subsequent recall (immediate and delayed), and the association between immediate and delayed recall performance. Results showed that higher copy score is predictive of better immediate recall performance which, in turn, leads to better delayed recall performance. In clinical practice, this can have implications for the accuracy of estimation of recall performances. For instance, taking copy score into account may improve clinicians accuracy in estimating memory performance in patients with poor constructional abilities. Without including copy score as a predictor of performance, memory for the ROCF and/or the TCF could be incorrectly appraised. To illustrate this, let us imagine a 65-year-old man with 19 years of education, who took 300 s to copy the ROCF, scored 20 on the copy trial, obtained 10 on the immediate recall trial, and 10 on the delayed recall score. First, based on the regression equations for the copy, immediate and delayed recall scores from Table 3, the patient s Z scores are 23.16, 21.43, and 21.34, respectively. These results appear to be indicative of impaired constructional abilities and mild memory difficulties. However, based on the regression equations from Table 4, which considers all the variables (i.e., sociodemographic variables, copy time, copy score, and immediate recall score), the patient s Z score are now 23.04, 1.78 and 0.66, respectively. These results are still indicative of impaired constructional abilities. However, Z scores for the immediate and delayed recall trials now fall within normal limits. This example shows how important it is to take into account copy performance while interpreting subsequent ROCF memory performance. The previous example applies only to ROCF performance. To interpret TCF performance, one should refer to Tables 5 and 6. While Table 5 includes sociodemographic variables alone, Table 6 includes all variables (i.e., sociodemographic, copy time, copy score, and immediate recall score). To ease calculation of Z scores, an Excel spreadsheet containing automatic formulas has been prepared and is available by writing to the corresponding author of the manuscript. (Please contact the corresponding author for the Excel spreadsheet containing automatic formulas.) Interestingly, results from this study have shown that immediate recall score was strongly related to delayed recall score, calling into question the usefulness of the delayed recall score. However, the present study was conducted in healthy older adults and the immediate and delayed recall scores may be more likely to differ in older adults with memory impairment, especially in those with weakened storage processes. Further studies are needed to verify this hypothesis. Finally, the study used regression equations to calculate Z scores for performance on the ROCF and TCF. These equations differ from typical normative methods (e.g., percentiles or standard Z scores calculated from a mean and a standard deviation). According to Fastenau (1998), these regression-based norms can lead to higher rates of false negatives when used in older

86 M.-P. Tremblay et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 30 (2015); 78 87 women and less educated people. However, this normative method has the advantage of better estimating the expected performance of a participant given his/her specific characteristics. The main limitation of the present studies was the use of an incidental sampling method, resulting in an underrepresentation of people aged 50 59 and 80 years or older. Also, for the ROCF, women in all age ranges were overrepresented. Although a random sampling method would have been ideal, the sampling method used here is a practical starting point in the establishment of ROCF and TCF norms for the Quebec-French population which were nonexistent until now. To our knowledge, this study is the first to provide normative data for the ROCF and the TCF in a Quebec-French population, which is necessary to allow accurate interpretation of test results to distinguish between normal and pathological cognitive states. Also, it is the first to provide separate normative data for the standard procedure of the TCF and the ROCF. However, it is important to note that these normative data were developed with different samples of older adults. As such, one should be careful when comparing performance on both tests in a patient. Also, it should only be used for individuals who have been tested using the standard, incidental learning protocol, and results should be interpreted carefully with individuals within the youngest (50 59) and oldest ( 80 years) age clusters, for lower educated people as well as for men. Moreover, as these norms were developed in older Quebec-French individuals, results should be interpreted carefully in individuals from other groups (e.g., English-speaking older adults in Quebec, French-speaking older adults from other countries). The fact that no time limit was given to copy the ROCF or the TCF could also be considered as a limitation of the study. For instance, some participants took up to 8 min to copy the figures and this brings into question the possibility that some participants could be considered impaired. All participants were screened using brief cognitive tests (i.e., MMSE and MoCA) but the visuocontructional items of these instruments are easier than the ROCF or the TCF. Thus, one cannot exclude the possibility that the samples included a few participants with some degree of visuospatial impairment. Finally, although all researchers used the same task instructions and versions, and subjects from each site met the same inclusion/exclusion criteria, some variability may exist between the recruiting sites. Moreover, although all examiners were individually trained to administer these tests, no common training was conducted and no inter-rater reliability estimates were tested. As such, despite the standardized administration and scoring of the tests, some variability may exist between the examiners. Funding This work was funded by the Réseau québécois de recherche sur le vieillissement, the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. In addition, BC was supported by a doctoral research award from the Alzheimer Society of Canada. OP was supported by a Fellowship award from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. MPT was supported by a scholarship from the Centre de recherche sur le cerveau, le comportement et la neuropsychiatrie. CH, JFG, and NC were supported by a salary award from the Fond de recherche du Québec Santé. JFG is also supported by a New Investigator award from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. Conflict of Interest None declared. References Arsenault-Lapierre, G., Whitehead, V., Belleville, S., Massoud, F., Bergman, H., & Chertkow, H. (2011). Mild cognitive impairment subcategories depend on the source of norms. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33, 596 603. Bennett-Levy, J. (1984). Determinants of performance on the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test: An analysis, and a new technique for single-case assessment. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, 109 119. Berry, D. T., Allen, R. S., & Schmitt, F. A. (1991). Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Psychometric characteristics in a geriatric sample. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 143 153. Boone, K. B., Lesser, I. M., Hill-Gutierrez, E., Berman, N. G., & D elia, L. F. (1993). Rey-Osterrieth complex figure performance in healthy, older adults: Relationship to age, education, sex, and IQ. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 22 28. Caffarra, P., Vezzadini, G., Dieci, F., Zonato, F., & Venneri, A. (2002). Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: Normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurological Science, 22, 443 447. Chiulli, S. J., Haaland, K. Y., LaRue, A., & Garry, P. J. (1995). Impact of age on drawing the Rey-Osterrieth figure. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 219 224. Fastenau, P. S. (1998). Validity of regression-based norms: An empirical test of the comprehensive norms with older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 906 916. Fastenau, P. S., Denburg, N. L., & Hufford, B. J. (1999). Adult normsfor the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure testand for supplemental recognition and matchingtrials from the extended complex figure test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13, 30 47. Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189 198.

M.-P. Tremblay et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 30 (2015); 78 87 87 Gallagher, C., & Burke, T. (2007). Age, gender and IQ effects on the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 35 45. Haaland, K. Y., Linn, R. T., Hunt, W. C., & Goodwin, J. S. (1983). A normative study of Russell s variant of the Wechsler memory scale in a healthy elderly population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 878 881. Hartman, M., & Potter, G. (1998). Sources of age differences on the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 513 524. Hubley, A. M. (2010). Using the Rey-Osterrieth and modified Taylorcomplex figures with olderadults: A preliminaryexamination of accuracyscore comparability. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25, 197 203. Institut de la statistique du Québec (2006). Population de 25 ans et plus, selon le plus haut degré de scolarité atteint, le sexe et le groupe d âge Québec, 2006. Retrieved November 13, 2013, from http://www.stat.gouv.qc.ca/donstat/societe/education/etat_scolr/tab1_niv_sco_2006.htm. Koss, E., Haxby, J. V., DeCarli, C., Shapiro, M. B., Friedland, R. P., & Rapoport, S. I. (1991). Patterns of performance preservation and loss in healthy aging. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 99 113. Lezak, M. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment. New-York: Oxford University Press. Machulda, M. M., Ivnik, R. J., Smith, G. E., Ferman, T. J., Boeve, B. F., Knopman, D., et al. (2007). Mayo s older Americans normative studies: Visual form discrimination and copy trial of the Rey Osterrieth complex figure. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 377 384. Meyers, J. E., & Meyers, K. R. (1995). The Meyers scoring system for the Rey complex figure and the recognition trial: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Mitrushina, M., Satz, P., & Chervinsky, A. B. (1990). Efficiency of recall on the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure in normal aging. Brain Dysfunction, 3, 148 150. Nasreddine, S. Z., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, A., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53, 695 699. Newman, P. D., & Krikorian, R. (2001). Encoding and complex figure recall. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 728 733. Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d une figure complexe: Contribution à l étude de la perception et de la mémoire. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 286 356. Peña-Casanova, J., Gramunt-Fombuena, N., Quiñones-Úbeda, S., Sánchez-Benavides, G., Aguilar, M., Badenes, D., et al. (2009). Spanish multicenter normative studies (NEURONORMA Project): Norms for the Rey Osterrieth complex figure (copy and memory), and free and cued selective reminding test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 371 393. Pontón, M. O., Satz, P., Herrera, L., Ortiz, F., Urrutia, C. P., Young, R., et al. (1996). Normative data stratified by age and education for the Neuropsychological Screening Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): Initial report. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2 (2), 96 104. Rey, A. (1941). L examen psychologique dans les cas d encéphalopathie traumatique. Archives de Psychologie, 28, 286 340. Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (1991). Effects of age, education, and gender on the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 370 376. Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (2003). The impact of cultureand education on non-verbal neuropsychological measurements: A critical review. Brainand Cognition, 52, 326 333. Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Strauss, E., & Spreen, O. (1990). A comparison of the Rey and Taylor figures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 417 420. Taylor, L. B. (1969). Localization of cerebral lesions by psychological testing. Clinical Neurosurgery, 16, 269 287. Tombaugh, T. N., Faulkner, P., & Hubley, A. M. (1992). Effects of age on the Rey-Osterrieth and Taylor complex figures: Test-retest data using an intentional learning paradigm. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 647 661. Tombaugh, T. N., Faulkner, P., & Schmidt, J. P. (1992). A new procedure for administering the Taylor complex figure: Normative data over a 60-year age span. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6, 63 79. Verhaeghen, P., & Salthouse, T. A. (1997). Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: Estimates of linear and non-linear age effects and structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 231 249. Vingerhoets, G., Lannoo, E., & Wolters, M. (1998). Comparing the Rey-Osterrieth and Taylor complex figures: Empirical data and meta-analysis. Psychologica belgica, 38, 109 119. Yamashita, H. (2006). Comparability of the Rey Osterrieth complex figure, the Taylor complex figure, and the modified Taylor complex figure in a normal sample of Japanese speakers. Psychological Reports, 99, 531 534. Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., & Adey, M. B. (1983). The geriatric depression rating scale: Comparison with other self-report and psychiatric rating scales. In T. Crook, S. Ferris, & R. Bartus (Eds.), Assessment in geriatric psychopharmacology (pp. 153 165). New Canaan, CT: Mark Powley Associates.