Exploring a brightness-drag illusion. Author. Published. Journal Title DOI. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Griffith Research Online

Similar documents
Chappell, Mark, Hine, Trevor, Acworth, Charmaine, Hardwick, David

The glare effect does not give rise to a longer-lasting afterimage

Effect of motion smoothness on the flash-lag illusion

Perceptual asynchronies between color and motion at the onset of motion and along the motion trajectory

Title: The flash-lag, Fröhlich and related motion illusions are artifacts of discrete sampling in the visual system

Attention enhances feature integration

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Latency differences and the flash-lag effect

Wobbling appearance of a face induced by doubled parts

Temporal recruitment along the trajectory of moving objects and the perception of position

SHORT AND SWEET When walls are no longer barriers: Perception of wall height in parkour

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

Orientation-selective adaptation to crowded illusory lines

Judging body weight from faces: The height ^ weight illusion

Going, Going, Gone: Localizing Abrupt Offsets of Moving Objects

Last but not least. Perception, 2003, volume 32, pages 249 ^ 252

Visual stream segregation has been proposed as a method to measure visual

Look before you leap: Jumping ability affects distance perception

Aesthetic preferences in the size of images of real-world objects

Active and passive movements give rise to different judgments of coldness

PERCEPTION OF AUDITORY-VISUAL SIMULTANEITY CHANGES BY ILLUMINANCE AT THE EYES

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

The Position of Moving Objects.

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

Perceived onset time and position of a moving stimulus

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search

Representational Momentum Beyond Internalized Physics

The fluttering-heart illusion: a new hypothesis

Viewpoint-dependent recognition of familiar faces

Independent coding of object motion and position revealed by distinct contingent afterevects

Sensorimotor Integration Compensates for Visual Localization Errors During Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect

Diamond Patterns: Cumulative Cornsweet Effects and Motion-Induced Brightening

Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex

A Motion-Dependent Distortion of Retinotopy in Area V4

Spatially localized time shifts of the perceptual stream

The persistence of position

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

The path of visual attention

When facial attractiveness is only skin deep

THE INFLUENCE OF VISUAL TRANSIENTS ON THE PERCEPTION OF SPACE AND TIME

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

TEMPORAL CHANGE IN RESPONSE BIAS OBSERVED IN EXPERT ANTICIPATION OF VOLLEYBALL SPIKES

A Multimodal Paradigm for Investigating the Perisaccadic Temporal Inversion Effect in Vision

Sensory Adaptation within a Bayesian Framework for Perception

XVI. SENSORY AIDS RESEARCH

Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory

Unconscious Color Priming Occurs at Stimulus- Not Percept- Dependent Levels of Processing Bruno G. Breitmeyer, 1 Tony Ro, 2 and Neel S.

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli

Active control does not eliminate motion-induced illusory displacement.

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

Motion onset really does capture attention

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Examining the locus of the attentional attraction effect

Neural delays, visual motion and the flash-lag effect

Temporal binding favors the early phase of color changes, but not of motion changes, yielding the colormotion asynchrony illusion

Illusory reversal of temporal order: the bias to report a dimmer stimulus as the first

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

Five shades of grey: Generalization in distractor-based retrieval of S-R episodes

The effect of contrast intensity and polarity in the achromatic watercolor effect

Motion streaks improve motion detection

Neural latencies do not explain the auditory and audio-visual flash-lag effect

Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items

The dependence of motion repulsion and rivalry on the distance between moving elements

On the Perception of Brightness and Contrast of Variegated Backgrounds

Object files can be purely episodic

Speed can go up as well as down at low contrast: Implications for models of motion perception

Effects of consciousness and consistency in manual control of visual stimulus on reduction of the flash-lag effect for luminance change

Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony Vroomen, Jean; Keetels, Mirjam; de Gelder, Bea; Bertelson, P.

Manual control of the visual stimulus reduces the Xash-lag evect

Second-order motion shifts perceived position

Some methodological aspects for measuring asynchrony detection in audio-visual stimuli

Visual Object Recognition Computational Models and Neurophysiological Mechanisms Neurobiology 130/230. Harvard College/GSAS 78454

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

Response latencies to chromatic. and achromatic visual stimuli

Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Seeing Sound: Changing Visual Perception Through Cross-Modal Interaction. Tracey D. Berger. New York University. Department of Psychology

Perception and Action Are Inseparable

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Illusory Causal Crescents: Misperceived spatial relations due to perceived causality

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online

Solving the correspondence problem within the Ternus display: The differential-activation theory

Segregation from direction differences in dynamic random-dot stimuli

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Systematic perceptual distortion of 3D slant by disconjugate eye movements

Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention

Three methods for measuring perception. Magnitude estimation. Steven s power law. P = k S n

Vision Research 51 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

June 16, The retina has two types of light-sensitive receptor cells (or photoreceptors) called rods and cones.

Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces

Cutaneous saltation within and across arms: A new measure of the saltation illusion in somatosensation

Purely temporal gure±ground segregation

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions.

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific?

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Transcription:

Exploring a brightness-drag illusion Author Habota, Tina, Chappell, Mark Published 2011 Journal Title Perception DOI https://doi.org/10.1068/p6767 Copyright Statement 2011 Pion Ltd., London. The attached file is reproduced here in accordance with the copyright policy of the publisher. Please refer to the journal's website for access to the definitive, published version. Downloaded from http://hdl.handle.net/10072/41169 Griffith Research Online https://research-repository.griffith.edu.au

Perception, 2011, volume 40, pages 101 ^ 103 doi:10.1068/p6767 SHORT AND SWEET Exploring a brightness-drag illusion Tina Habota, Mark Chappellô Department of Psychology (MG) and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, Griffith Health Institute, Griffith University, Nathan, 4111 QLD, Australia; e-mail: m.chappell@griffith.edu.au Received 2 July 2010, in revised form 5 November 2010 Abstract. A sudden luminance increment on a moving stimulus was perceived significantly along the trajectory, in the direction of motion, from its displayed position. A nonlinear relationship with stimulus speed, for a Fro«hlich-like illusion, but not for the luminance-increment illusion, challenges certain models of spatial mislocalisation and argues for different processes underlying the two illusions. Moving stimuli often transit between regions of different illumination but it may be important that the visual system's computation of position is not dramatically disrupted by this, for example at a late-afternoon game at Wimbledon. There has been considerable work on the perception of the position and/or timing of an (ecologically implausible) colour change from red to green on moving stimuli (eg Kreegipuu and Allik 2004; Moutoussis and Zeki 1997). However, as these colours are processed in an opponent fashion, it is likely that a complicated dynamic accompanies an abrupt change from one to the other, in line with Nishida and Johnston's (2002) finding that very different illusions result when there are repeated changes between red and green and back again, as opposed to a single change. A change in luminance not only corresponds to likely changes in the environmentöit is also likely to engage fewer and simpler processes, thus leading to more easily interpretable results. However, we are aware of only one study of the perceived position of a simple change in luminance on a moving stimulus (Kerzel 2003). In that study decrements only have been examined, and the position of the decrement was found to be misperceived in the direction of motion. Our participants compared the perceived position of a sudden increase in luminance (3 cd m 2 to 115 cd m 2, background 1 cd m 2 ) on a moving stimulus (at 1.35, 4.35, and 12 deg s 1 ) with that of a landmark stimulus which appeared just before the increment change, (1) and remained on for the remainder of the trial. For comparison with this increment trajectory, our participants also compared the point of appearance of a moving stimulus with the landmark (both appearing simultaneously) öan onset trajectory. Research with the Fro«hlich illusion (reviewed by Whitney 2002) suggested this point would be systematically misperceived in the direction of motion. As indicated by the 95% confidence intervals in figure 1, for individual participants the increment illusion was significant at all speeds for at least eight out of the twelve participants. Also revealed is a general tendency for participants' data to depart from linearity, or to exhibit a more pronounced compressive nonlinearity (where the graph with speed `curves over', but is still monotonically increasing), with the onset than with the increment trajectory. The 95% confidence intervals on the averages demonstrate that across participants all speeds produced a significant illusion with both trajectory types. As the literature did not provide much guidance as to the form of relationship we should expect (see below), we planned to separately test our increment and onset trajectories for linearity, as a function of speed. Using a metric (135, 453, and 1200), ô Author to whom all correspondence should be addressed. (1) Owing to an oversight, one frame (16.67 ms) before the increment change.

102 T Habota, M Chappell Illusion magnitude=deg Illusion magnitude=deg 0.7 0.5 0.3 0.1 0.1 0.7 0.5 0.3 0.1 0.1 Onset P1 P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 P7 P8 P9 P10 P11 P12 Illusion magnitude=deg Illusion magnitude=deg Illusion magnitude=deg 1.8 1.4 1.0 1.8 1.4 1.0 Increment P1 P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 Speed=deg s 1 Speed=deg s 1 1.5 deg Fixation 1.0 deg Landmark Moving stimulus 0.8 0.4 0.0 P7 P8 P9 P10 P11 P12 Average increment onset 0 4 8 12 Speed=deg s 1 Figure 1. Stimuli, individual (method of fixed stimuli and logistic regression on 144 trials/condition/ participant) and average (bottom right panel) results for 12 participants (P1 ^ P12). Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Note the different scale for the onset (left) and increment (right) trajectories, for individual-participant data. which accounted for the unequal spacing between the speeds, for the onset trajectory type an ANOVA revealed a significant linear effect (F 111 ˆ 19:72, p ˆ 0:001, Z 2, p ˆ 0:64) as well as a significant quadratic effect (F 111 ˆ 21:62, p ˆ 0:001, Z 2, p ˆ 0:66). For the increment trajectory type, there was a significant linear effect (F 111 ˆ 8:61, p ˆ 0:014, Z 2 p ˆ 0:44), but no quadratic effect ( p ˆ 0:79, Z 2, p ˆ 0:007). The position of an increment in luminance was systematically misperceived in the direction of motion, complementing Kerzel's (2003) similar finding with a luminance decrement. As an explanation of this brightness-drag illusion we will focus on what we call `differential latency for attribute'. Following Moutoussis and Zeki (1997) and Kerzel (2003), we phrase it thus: by the time the change in luminance has been processed, the processing of the moving object's position supports its perception further along

Exploring a brightness-drag illusion 103 its trajectory. When the luminance change is available to perception it is attached, or bound, to the currently perceived position of the moving object. Provided the differential latency between processing of position and luminance is constant as a function of speed, our linear dependence of the increment illusion on speed is accounted for. While the comparison stimulus with our onset trajectory was not constantly visible for the duration of the trial, as we believe it should be for a pure Fro«hlich paradigm, we would expect our paradigm to yield very similar results to that one. Some studies of the Fro«hlich illusion have found an approximately linear relationship (eg experiments 1, 3, and 5 of Kerzel and Mu«sseler 2002), whereas others have found a compressively nonlinear relationship with speed (eg Mu«sseler and Aschersleben 1998; for review see also Kerzel and Gegenfurtner 2004; Kreegipuu and Allik 2003). Of course, it would be desirable to replicate our finding with more levels of speed. Should the nonlinear relationship prove to be a reliable finding with the onset trajectory, then differential latency for luminance change would need to be supplemented, or replaced, with other nonlinear (with speed) processes, as would the simplest forms of temporal integration (Lappe and Krekelberg 1998) and spatial projection (Nijhawan 1994; Eagleman and Sejnowski 2007) accounts. That different processes are involved in the two trajectories is supported by the fact that correlations computed for each of the three speeds, between illusion magnitudes with the different illusions, were all nonsignificant ( ps 4 0:25), (2) and indeed it would be surprising if the appearance of a new moving stimulus did not entail more processes (Yantis 1996). Players do misperceive the position of luminance changes on tennis balls, but whether this affects their perception of the position of the ball remains an Open question. Acknowledgments. We would like to thank the Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Unit for support, and David Hardwick for help with graphics. This work was the basis of an Honours Thesis project for the first author. References Eagleman D M, Sejnowski T J, 2007 ``Motion signals bias localization judgments: A unified explanation for the flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-jump, and Fro«hlich illusions'' Journal of Vision 7(4):3, 1 ^ 12 Kerzel D, 2003 ``Asynchronous perception of motion and luminance change'' Psychological Research 67 233 ^ 239 Kerzel D, Gegenfurtner K R, 2004 ``Spatial distortions and processing latencies in the onset repulsion and Fro«hlich effects'' Vision Research 44 577 ^ 590 Kerzel D, Mu«sseler J, 2002 ``Effects of stimulus material on the Fro«hlich illusion'' Vision Research 42 181 ^ 189 Kreegipuu K, Allik J, 2003 ``Perceived onset time and position of a moving stimulus'' Vision Research 43 1625 ^ 1635 Kreegipuu K, Allik J, 2004 ``Confusion of space and time in the flash-lag effect'' Perception 33 293 ^ 306 Lappe M, Krekelberg B, 1998 ``The position of moving objects'' Perception 27 1437 ^ 1449 Mu«sseler J, Aschersleben G, 1998 ``Localizing the first position of a moving stimulus: The Fro«hlich effect and an attention-shifting explanation'' Perception & Psychophysics 60 683 ^ 695 Moutoussis K, Zeki S, 1997 ``A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision'' Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 264 393^399 Nijhawan R, 1994 ``Motion extrapolation in catching'' Nature 370 256 ^ 257 Nishida S, Johnston A, 2002 ``Marker correspondence, not processing latency, determines temporal binding of visual attributes'' Current Biology 12 359 ^ 368 Whitney D, 2002 ``The influence of visual motion on perceived position'' Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 211 ^ 216 Yantis S, 1996 `Àttentional capture in vision'', in Converging Operations in the Study of Visual Selective Attention Eds A F Kramer, M G H Coles, G D Logan (Washington, DC: APA Press) pp 45 ^ 76 (2) Suggested by an anonymous reviewer. ß 2011 a Pion publication

ISSN 0301-0066 (print) ISSN 1468-4233 (electronic) www.perceptionweb.com Conditions of use. This article may be downloaded from the Perception website for personal research by members of subscribing organisations. Authors are entitled to distribute their own article (in printed form or by e-mail) to up to 50 people. This PDF may not be placed on any website (or other online distribution system) without permission of the publisher.