Affective reactivity of speech and emotional experience in patients with schizophrenia

Similar documents
SCHRES1520. Schizophrenia Research 000 (2001) 000±000. Affective reactivity of language and right-ear advantage in schizophrenia

Emotional Valence and Reference Disturbance in Schizophrenia

Supplementary Online Content

Neurological Signs and the Heterogeneity of Schizophrenia. Celso Arango, M.D., Brian Kirkpatrick, M.D., and Robert W. Buchanan, M.D.

Rating Mental Impairment with AMA Guides 6 th edition:

Language comprehension and working memory language comprehension and working memory deficits in patients with schizophrenia

Positive and Negative Symptom Response to Clozapine in Schizophrenic Patients With and Without the Deficit Syndrome

Conceptual Sequencing and Disordered Speech in Schizophrenia

Psychopathology in patients with schizophrenia attending a psychiatry outpatient clinic at a tertiary care hospital

RATING MENTAL WHOLE PERSON IMPAIRMENT UNDER THE NEW SABS: New Methods, New Challenges. CSME/CAPDA Conference, April 1, 2017

By Jason H. King DECONSTRUCTING THE DSM-5 ASSESSMENT AND DIAGNOSIS OF SCHIZOPHRENIA SPECTRUM DISORDERS THE NEW LANDSCAPE

Schizophrenia: New Concepts for Therapeutic Discovery

Bizarre delusions and DSM-IV schizophrenia

Schizoaffective Disorder

EVALUATION OF WORRY IN PATIENTS WITH SCHIZOPHRENIA AND PERSECUTORY DELUSION COMPARED WITH GENERAL POPULATION

Psychometric Properties and Concurrent Validity of the Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale

Course and Outcome for Schizophrenia Versus Other Psychotic Patients: A Longitudinal Study

Expressed Emotion, Attributions, and Schizophrenia Symptom Dimensions

Running head: FORMAL THOUGHT DISORDER IN EARLY-STAGE PSYCHOSIS. Affective Systems Induce Formal Thought Disorder in Early-Stage Psychosis.

Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status as a Screening Test in Schizophrenia, I: Sensitivity, Reliability, and Validity

OUTCOMES OF DICHOTOMIZING A CONTINUOUS VARIABLE IN THE PSYCHIATRIC EARLY READMISSION PREDICTION MODEL. Ng CG

Schizophrenia and Other Psychotic Disorders

SCHIZOPHRENIA AN OVERVIEW

Chapter Three BRIDGE TO THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES

Psychotic disorders Dr. Sarah DeLeon, MD PGYIV, Psychiatry ConceptsInPsychiatry.com

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

THE STABILITY OF SYMPTOMS AND SYNDROMES IN CHRONIC SCHIZOPHRENIC PATIENTS MILIND BORDE 1, ELIZABETH J.B. DAVIS 1 AND L.N. SHARMA 2

The Brief Negative Symptom Scale: Psychometric Properties

Accurate Diagnosis of Primary Psychotic Disorders

Psychosis. Paula Gibbs, MD Department of Psychiatry University of Utah

EARLY ONSET SCHIZOPHRENIA

Use of the Booklet Category Test to assess abstract concept formation in schizophrenic disorders

CURRICULUM VITAE: DOUGLAS W. HEINRICHS, MD. License: Licensed to practice medicine in Maryland 1978 to present (D22279)

Schizophrenia FAHAD ALOSAIMI

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire

Dichotic listening in schizotypal personality disorder: Evidence for gender and laterality effects

DSM5: How to Understand It and How to Help

Psychotic Disorders. Schizophrenia. Age Distribution of Onset 2/24/2009. Schizophrenia. Hallmark trait is psychosis

Contemporary Psychiatric-Mental Health Nursing. Features of Schizophrenia. Features of Schizophrenia - continued

ENTITLEMENT ELIGIBILITY GUIDELINE SCHIZOPHRENIA

Program Outline. DSM-5 Schizophrenia Spectrum and Psychotic Disorders: Knowing it Better and Improving Clinical Practice.

Diagnostic orphans for alcohol use disorders in a treatment-seeking psychiatric sample

8/22/2016. Contemporary Psychiatric-Mental Health Nursing Third Edition. Features of Schizophrenia. Features of Schizophrenia (cont'd)

Supplementary Online Content

Suicide Risk and Melancholic Features of Major Depressive Disorder: A Diagnostic Imperative

4. General overview Definition

Positive and Negative Symptoms of Psychosis The Care Transitions Network

Psychosis, Mood, and Personality: A Clinical Perspective

Treatment of Schizophrenia

Perspective-taking deficits in people with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a prospective investigation

November 2014 MRC2.CORP.X.00004

Narrating Emotional Events in Schizophrenia

True or False? Chapter 14 Psychological Disorders. What is Abnormal Behavior? 12/9/10. Characteristics of Abnormal Behavior

Schizophrenia Research

Condensed Clinical Practice Guideline Treatment Of Patients With Schizophrenia

Relationship between Positive and Negative Symptoms of Schizophrenia and Psychotic Depression with Risk of Suicide

Characteristics of trees drawn by patients with paranoid schizophrenia

Emotional responses to psychosocial stress in schizophrenia: the role of individual differences in affective traits and coping

Schizophrenia. Psychotic Disorders. Schizophrenia. Chapter 13

Running head: EMOTION REGULATION MODERATES PERFECTIONISM 1. Depression in College Students. Jessica Drews. Faculty Advisor: Scott Pickett

Dealing with Feelings: The Effectiveness of Cognitive Behavioural Group Treatment for Women in Secure Settings

Overview. Classification, Assessment, and Treatment of Childhood Disorders. Criteria for a Good Classification System

Diagnosis of Mental Disorders. Historical Background. Rise of the Nomenclatures. History and Clinical Assessment

Week #1 Classification & Diagnosis

CONVERGENT VALIDITY OF THE MMPI A AND MACI SCALES OF DEPRESSION 1

Is Lurasidone more safe and effective in the treatment ofschizoaffective disorder and schizophrenia than other commonanti-psychotic medications?

APPENDIX 11: CASE IDENTIFICATION STUDY CHARACTERISTICS AND RISK OF BIAS TABLES

Clinical Guidelines for the Pharmacologic Treatment of Schizophrenia

The Effect of A Symptom Self-Management on Psychotic Symptoms for Multiple episodes Schizophrenic Patients

NeuRA Schizophrenia diagnosis May 2017

LOUISIANA MEDICAID PROGRAM ISSUED: 04/13/10 REPLACED: 03/01/93 CHAPTER 13: MENTAL HEALTH CLINICS SECTION13.1: SERVICES PAGE(S) 9 SERVICES

Visualizing Psychology

Goal: To recognize and differentiate different forms of psychopathology that involve disordered thinking and reasoning and distorted perception

Supplementary Online Content

BRIEF PSYCHIATRIC RATING SCALE ANCHORED. Introduce all questions with During the past week have you..

BADDS Appendix A: The Bipolar Affective Disorder Dimensional Scale, version 3.0 (BADDS 3.0)

Challenges to Recovery Following Early Psychosis: Implications of Recovery Rate and Timing

Diagnostic and prognostic significance of Schneiderian first-rank symptoms: a 20-year longitudinal study of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder

An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample

The Influence of a Working Memory Load Manipulation on Language Production in Schizophrenia

Psychological Disorders (mental illnesses): A Little History What are psychological disorders? What are some major classes of psychological disorders?

INVESTIGATOR INITIATED RESEARCH PROTOCOL

Schizophrenia. Nikita Verma 2017 Page 1

Symptomatology of the Initial Prodromal Phase in Schizophrenia

Language Generation in Schizophrenia and Mania: The Relationships Among Verbosity, Syntactic Complexity, and Pausing

Suicidal Ideation and Suicide Attempts in Recent-Onset Schizophrenia

Affective Reactivity in Language: The Role of Psychophysiological Arousal

Chapter 3. Klinefelter's syndrome (karyotype 47,XXY) and schizophrenia-spectrum pathology. Sophie van Rijn, André Aleman, Hanna Swaab, René S Kahn

BRIEF PSYCHIATRIC RATING SCALE-ANCHORED (BPRS-A) BRIEF PSYCHIATRIC RATING SCALE-ANCHORED (BPRS-A):

Assessment: Interviews, Tests, Techniques. Clinical Psychology Lectures

Personality traits predict current and future functioning comparably for individuals with major depressive and personality disorders

INTRODUCTION TO MENTAL HEALTH. PH150 Fall 2013 Carol S. Aneshensel, Ph.D.

Office Practice Coding Assistance - Overview

Schizophrenia: A Lifelong Illness

Results. NeuRA Family relationships May 2017

In the last few years, evidence for the efficacy of psychotherapy

DESIGN TYPE AND LEVEL OF EVIDENCE: Randomized controlled trial, Level I

Mary Schepler

Preferred Practice Guidelines Bipolar Disorder in Children and Adolescents

Outline. Definition. Distress* Deviance 10/31/2012. What is abnormal? Statistical infrequency. Personal suffering. Culturally-based.

Transcription:

Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 7 14 www.elsevier.com/locate/schres Affective reactivity of speech and emotional experience in patients with schizophrenia Alex S. Cohen*, Nancy M. Docherty Department of Psychology, Kent State University, PO Box 119, Kent, OH 44240, USA Received 17 September 2002; received in revised form 22 January 2003; accepted 7 February 2003 Available online 22 May 2003 Abstract Communication disturbances are exacerbated by stress in patients with schizophrenia, a phenomenon known as affective reactivity of speech. However, a subset of these patients, those with the deficit syndrome, is characterized by a diminished capacity to experience emotion. We examined affective reactivity in the natural speech of schizophrenia patients with and without the deficit syndrome, with the expectation that deficit syndrome patients would evidence lower levels of affective reactivity. Two 10-min conversational speech samples were collected from each of 13 deficit syndrome patients, 22 nondeficit patients and 26 healthy control subjects. One speech sample from each participant was on the topic of emotionally negative, stressful memories, and the other was on emotionally positive, nonstressful memories. The audiotaped speech samples were analyzed blindly for frequencies of referential communication failure. All three groups showed significantly higher frequencies of communication disturbances in the emotionally negative speech sample than the positive speech sample. Nondeficit patients showed greater affective reactivity of speech than either deficit patients or controls. Conversely, deficit patients speech was not more reactive to emotion than the speech of the control group. These results suggest that emotion-related variables mediate the relationship between stress and language symptom exacerbations in at least some patients with schizophrenia. D 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Language; Exacerbations; Schizophrenia 1. Introduction Schizophrenia is a heterogeneous disorder with substantial differences among patients in presenting symptoms, pathophysiology and clinical course. One approach used to create more homogeneous groups for empirical study involves subtyping patients based on differences in symptoms and behaviors that are * Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-330-672-2372 x80914. E-mail address: acohen1@kent.edu (A.S. Cohen). believed to reflect differences in underlying pathological process (e.g., Crow, 1985; Liddle et al., 1989; Kirkpatrick et al., 2001). Some, but not all, patients with schizophrenia demonstrate marked symptom exacerbations in response to arousal of negative affect. This affective reactivity of symptoms may reflect a pathophysiological process present in a subgroup of patients (Docherty, 1996). Of particular note, some patients speech becomes significantly more disorganized during laboratory stress-induction paradigms (Docherty et al., 1994, 1998; Burbridge and Barch, 2002). There is evidence to suggest that 0920-9964/03/$ - see front matter D 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/s0920-9964(03)00069-0

8 A.S. Cohen, N.M. Docherty / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 7 14 affective reactivity of speech reflects a distinct process that is present in some schizophrenia patients, and not others. It has been associated with other putative process discriminators, including reactivity of right ear advantage to emotionally laden stimuli in fused word dichotic listening tasks (Rhinewine and Docherty, 2002), higher than normal levels of startle reactivity to acoustic stimuli (Docherty and Grillon, 1995) and greater familiality of illness (Docherty et al., 1998). However, the processes underlying affective reactivity of speech are not well understood. The present study examined whether the presence or absence of the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia, which involves a diminished capacity to experience emotions, was related to patients level of affective reactivity of speech. Nuechterlein and Dawson (1984) have hypothesized that symptom exacerbations in patients with schizophrenia are influenced by certain mediating vulnerability factors. In the context of a stress diathesis model of schizophrenia pathology (for a review, see Fowles, 1992), mediating vulnerability factors may mediate the relationship between external stressors and subsequent exacerbations of psychotic symptoms. Accordingly, patients with higher levels of these mediating vulnerability factors are more likely to relapse under conditions of stress (e.g., Rosenfarb et al., 2000). There is reason to suspect that individual differences in the degree to which patients experience emotions, i.e., their level of emotionality, might be a mediating vulnerability factor. Thus, patients level of emotionality might influence the level of affective reactivity of symptoms that they exhibit. Differences among patients in emotionality have been documented and have been used to help define a distinct syndrome within schizophrenia, denoted the deficit syndrome (Carpenter, 1988). The deficit syndrome is diagnosed based on the presence of two of the following six primary and enduring negative symptoms: flat affect, diminished capacity to experience emotions, poverty of speech, curbing of interests, diminished sense of purpose and diminished social drive (Kirkpatrick et al., 1989). Patients with the deficit syndrome have reported lower levels of stress than schizophrenia patients without the deficit syndrome following a laboratory stress-induction paradigm (Cohen et al., submitted for publication), and have evidenced lower levels of many negative emotions including anxiety, guilt, hostility (Kirkpatrick et al., 1993; Tek et al., 2001) and depression (Kirkpatrick et al., 1993, 1994, 1996). The deficit syndrome also has been associated with a lower severity of certain symptoms that are considered to be relatively reactive to external stressors, such as suspiciousness (Kirkpatrick et al., 1996), delusions and hallucinations (Kirkpatrick et al., 1993, 1996; Buchanan et al., 1994; Cohen and Docherty, submitted for publication; but see also Buchanan et al., 1997). In summary, because patients with the deficit syndrome experience lower levels of emotion, they may be less susceptible to symptom exacerbations in response to affectively negative circumstances. The present study examined whether patient s capacity to experience emotions was related to the degree to which their speech becomes more disordered under conditions of stress. Three groups, including patients with the deficit syndrome, patients without the deficit syndrome and healthy controls, were compared on measures of communication disturbance in speech samples elicited during separate positive and negative affectively valenced narrative conditions. Affective reactivity of speech was determined by comparing levels of communication disturbance in the two affective conditions. We hypothesized that all three groups would exhibit some degree of affective reactivity of speech, but that patients with the deficit syndrome and control subjects would evidence significantly lower levels of affective reactivity of speech than patients with nondeficit schizophrenia. 2. Method 2.1. Participants This study was part of a multi-faceted research project investigating language disorder in schizophrenia. Participants who presented histories suggestive of organic impairment were excluded from the study, as were participants with a primary language other than English. Furthermore, participants who met criteria from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Edition (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) for substance abuse or dependence

A.S. Cohen, N.M. Docherty / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 7 14 9 also were excluded. All participants received monetary compensation for participation in the present study. The participants demographic and descriptive information are presented in Table 1. 2.1.1. Patients The patient group originally consisted of 51 stable outpatients with DSM-IV schizophrenia. However, 35 of these cases were included in the majority of the analyses used in the present study (see Section 2.2.2 for elaboration). Diagnoses were made by a clinical psychologist with diagnostic expertise (N.M. Docherty) based on information obtained using the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia Lifetime Version (SADS-L; Endicott and Spitzer, 1978). In order to provide some assurance that participants could comply with the study protocol, participants who had Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF, DSM-III-R, American Psychiatric Association, 1987) scores below 35 were excluded from this study. Of the 35 patients, 8 were being prescribed typical antipsychotic medication, 19 were being prescribed atypical antipsychotics, 4 patients were prescribed both and 4 were not being prescribed any antipsychotics at the time of testing. Ten patients in the present study were being prescribed anticholinergic medications. 2.1.2. Controls The control group consisted of 26 volunteers who were matched to the patients on age and parents socioeconomic status using the Socioeconomic Index (SEI; Hauser and Warren, 1997). Controls were recruited from university support staff and were excluded if they had any history of psychotic symptoms or if they met criteria for an axis I substance abuse or mood disorder based on information obtained from a SADS-L interview. 2.2. Procedure 2.2.1. Symptom rating scales The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS; Lukoff et al., 1986) was used to measure patients symptomatology. Symptom ratings were made by graduate student level researchers who had attained acceptable levels of interrater reliability (all intraclass correlations >0.69, most >0.90). 2.2.2. Deficit syndrome score Deficit syndrome scores were computed using the Proxy for Deficit Schedule (PDS; Kirkpatrick et al., 1993), a measure that derives deficit scores directly from BPRS ratings. Although PDS scores are derived from cross-sectional symptom ratings, they have been Table 1 Demographics and clinical variables for deficit (Def), nondeficit (Nondef) and healthy control subjects (Con) Deficit (N = 13) Nondeficit (N = 22) Controls (N = 26) Group comparisons (post hoc = Scheffe) Age 34 F 8 37F 11 39 F 8 Def = Nondef = Con Education 12.6 F 1 12.1 F 2 14.8 F 2 Def = Nondef < Con* GAF 53 F 9 49F 14 86 F 5 Def = Nondef < Con* Parents SEI 72.7 F 32.1 54.4 F 16.6 64.6 F 26.3 Def = Nondef = Con Gender Male 10 11 12 Def = Nondef = Con Female 3 11 14 Ethnicity Cauc. 11 16 21 Def = Nondef = Con Afr Am. 2 6 4 Other 0 0 1 IQ 91 F 15 85 F 14 105 F 9 Def = Nondef < Con* Psychosis 8.4 F 5.2 13.7 F 5.1 Def < Nondef* GAF = Global Assessment of Functioning, Scale 0 90 (American Psychiatric Association, 1987). SEI = Socioeconomic Index rating; calculated as combined fathers and mothers SEI rating. Cauc. = Caucasian, Afr. Am. = African American, Other = Asian/Hispanic/other. IQ derived from the Shipley Institute of Living (Frisch and Jessop, 1989). Psychosis scores calculated using the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (Ventura et al., 2000). * p < 0.05.

10 A.S. Cohen, N.M. Docherty / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 7 14 found to correspond highly with ratings on the Schedule for Deficit Syndrome (SDS; Kirkpatrick et al., 1989), the gold standard measure of the deficit syndrome. In essence, PDS scores are based on two deficit symptoms: blunted affect (measured by the BPRS blunted affect scale ratings) and diminished emotionality (measured by the AFFSCALE score, which is a summation of BPRS depression, anxiety, guilt and hostility scale ratings). The PDS score is computed by subtracting the AFFSCALE score from the blunted affect rating. Deficit/nondeficit group assignment was determined following the method of Kirkpatrick et al. (1996) using the available cut-points in the present dataset that fell closest to the ideal recommended values. Patients in the highest 25% range of PDS scores (PDS score > 4, n = 13) were included in the deficit group, while the nondeficit group was composed of all cases in the lowest 43% range of PDS scores (PDS score < 6, n = 22). The remaining 32% (PDS score = 4, 5, n = 16) of cases were excluded from the between group analyses. 2.2.3. Positive syndrome scores Severity of psychosis was measured using the positive syndrome score described by Ventura et al. (2000), which is a summation of five BPRS items: bizarre behavior, disorganization, hallucinatory behavior, suspiciousness and unusual thought content. A psychosis score could not be calculated for one of the nondeficit patients due to missing data. This case was excluded from the analyses examining psychotic symptomatology. 2.2.4. Ratings of affective reactivity Participants were asked to produce three separate, 10-min-long narratives. During the first narrative, the participants talked about affectively neutral topics (i.e., hobbies, daily routine, etc.). This neutral condition was administered with the sole purpose of reducing the situational stress associated with speaking and being tape-recorded and to familiarize the participants with the procedures. The second and third narratives consisted of the participants talking about affectively valenced memories. In one of the two conditions, participants were asked to recount nonstressful, good memories from their lives (the affectively positive condition), and in the other condition, stressful, bad memories (the affectively negative condition). Patients were expected to do most of the talking; however, interviewers kept them on task as needed and elicited elaboration of condition-appropriate memories. The positive and negative narratives were collected on separate days to avoid carryover effects, and the order was counterbalanced. The narratives were transcribed and carefully proofread to ensure accuracy. The transcribed narratives were then analyzed using the Communication Disturbance Index (CDI; Docherty et al., 1994). The CDI is designed to assess disturbances in communication of meaning, rather than thought disorder as such, or disorder of language structure. The CDI measures six conceptually distinct types of referential disturbances, including vague references, confused references, missing information references, ambiguous word meanings, wrong word references and structural unclarities. Instances of each type of disturbance in the speech samples are counted. In order to control for individual differences in verbosity, numbers of instances are then divided by the amount of speech (hundreds of words) in the narrative. Total CDI scores, which were used in the present study, are computed by summing the six referential disturbance scores. The CDI and its validation are described more fully in an earlier publication (Docherty et al., 1994). Affective reactivity of speech was assessed by comparing CDI ratings in the positive vs. negative speech samples. 2.2.5. IQ Estimated IQ scores were derived from the Shipley Institute of Living scales (Shipley; Zachary et al., 1985). The Shipley-derived IQ score has been found to correlate highly with the Wechsler Adult Intelligent Scale Revised full scale IQ score (Frisch and Jessop, 1989). 2.2.6. Analyses The analyses were conducted in two parts. First, a two-way (group condition) analysis of variance (ANOVA) compared the deficit, nondeficit and control groups on CDI ratings in the affectively negative vs. positive conditions. These analyses determined whether (1) participants as a group demonstrated affective reactivity of speech, (2) groups differed on levels of communication disturbances across the two

conditions and (3) there was a group by condition interaction, with the three groups differing on degree of reactivity of speech. Second, residualized reactivity scores were computed by regressing the CDI ratings in the affectively positive condition from the CDI ratings in the affectively negative condition. A oneway ANOVA was employed to compare the three participant groups on these reactivity scores. This analysis provided a second comparison of the three groups on level of affective reactivity of speech, a comparison that took into account baseline differences in communication disturbances. All results reported are two tailed. A.S. Cohen, N.M. Docherty / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 7 14 11 3. Results Table 1 contains descriptive information for the deficit, nondeficit and control groups. These three groups did not differ in age, male/female ratio or ethnic composition. Patient groups had significantly lower levels of education, IQ and GAF scores than the control group. The deficit and nondeficit groups did not differ on level of education, IQ or GAF, but the deficit syndrome group showed significantly less severe psychotic symptoms t[32] = 2.94, p <0.01 compared to the nondeficit group. 3.1. CDI ratings compared in the deficit, nondeficit and control groups Table 2 contains the means and standard deviations of the CDI ratings in the affectively positive and negative conditions for the deficit, nondeficit and control groups. These ratings were compared by means of a two-way (group condition) repeated Table 2 Means, standard deviations and ranges for the Communications Disturbance Inventory ratings in the affective positive and negative conditions for the deficit, nondeficit and control groups Group Affective condition Positive Negative Means F S.D. Range Means F S.D. Range Deficit 2.6 F 1.7 0.6 7.0 3.7 F 2.0 2.1 8.2 Nondeficit 1.7 F 0.9 0.4 4.2 3.5 F 1.7 1.0 9.9 Control 0.4 F 0.3 0.1 1.1 0.9 F 0.6 0.2 2.8 Mean totals 1.4 F 1.3 2.4 F 2.0 Fig. 1. Communication Disturbance Index (CDI) ratings in the positive (Pos) and negative (Neg) affectively valenced conditions for the deficit, nondeficit and control groups. measures ANOVA. There was a significant main effect for group, F[2,57] = 27.62, p < 0.001, indicating that the three groups differed on total CDI ratings across conditions. There was a significant main effect for condition, F[1,56] = 58.91, p < 0.001, indicating that subjects across groups exhibited higher CDI ratings in the affectively negative condition compared to the affectively positive condition. Most importantly, the interaction effect was significant, F[2,57] = 9.618, p < 0.001, indicating that there were significant differences among the groups with respect to degree of affective reactivity of speech. Three paired t-tests were then used to compare CDI ratings in the affectively positive and negative conditions for each group separately. The deficit, nondeficit and control groups each evidenced higher CDI ratings in the affectively negative condition (t[12] = 4.497, p = 0.001; t[20] = 5.365, p < 0.001 and t[25] = 4.102, p < 0.001, respectively), indicating that all three groups demonstrated significant levels of affective reactivity of speech. These results are summarized in Fig. 1. 3.2. Affective reactivity of speech compared between the deficit, nondeficit and control groups Residualized affective reactivity scores were computed by regressing the CDI ratings in the affectively

12 A.S. Cohen, N.M. Docherty / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 7 14 positive condition from the CDI ratings in the affectively negative condition. A one-way ANOVA was conducted to compare the deficit, nondeficit and control groups on these scores. The ANOVA yielded a significant effect ( F[2, 58] = 7.74, p = 0.001), indicating that the three groups differed on level of affective reactivity of speech. A Scheffe post hoc analysis indicated that (1) the nondeficit group demonstrated significantly higher levels of affective reactivity of speech than either the deficit syndrome group ( p < 0.01) or the control group ( p < 0.01), and (2) the deficit syndrome and control groups did not differ significantly from each other on level of affective reactivity. This indicates that the nondeficit patients exhibited significantly elevated levels of affective reactivity of speech compared to both deficit syndrome patients and controls, even when differences between groups in baseline levels of communication disturbances were controlled statistically. Bivariate correlations also were calculated between patients deficit syndrome severity scores (using the PDS as a continuous measure) and affective reactivity scores to determine whether severity of deficit symptomatology was related to affective reactivity of speech. All 51 patients were included in this analyses, including the 16 patients who were ambiguous with respect to deficit/nondeficit group assignment. Deficit syndrome scores were inversely correlated with affective reactivity scores (r[51] = 0.321, p < 0.05), indicating that higher levels of deficit syndrome symptomatology were associated with lower levels of affective reactivity. 3.3. Controlling for level of psychosis, medication and gender in the deficit and nondeficit patient groups In order to control for differences in level of psychotic symptomatology between the deficit and nondeficit groups, a univariate ANOVA was conducted comparing the deficit and nondeficit patients on level of affective reactivity of speech, covarying out psychosis scores. The deficit and nondeficit groups remained significantly different, F[1,31] = 4.79, p < 0.05, indicating that the difference in level of affective reactivity of speech between the deficit and nondeficit patients was not due solely to differences in psychosis severity. Deficit and nondeficit patients did not differ significantly on the type of medications being prescribed. In order to examine medication effects on level of affective reactivity of speech, a one-way ANOVA was conducted comparing four groups of patients: those who were receiving typical, atypical, both typical and atypical or no antipsychotic medications. There were no significant group differences on CDI ratings from the stressful or nonstressful conditions, or residualized affective reactivity scores. Patients who were receiving anticholinergic medications then were compared with those who were not. Again, there were no group differences on the CDI ratings or affective reactivity scores. Finally, there were no significant differences between males and females on CDI ratings or levels of affective reactivity in any of the three groups. 4. Discussion The deficit, nondeficit and control groups each exhibited increased levels of disorder in their speech in the affectively negative condition as compared to the affectively positive condition. However, nondeficit patients demonstrated markedly and significantly higher levels of affective reactivity of speech than either patients with the deficit syndrome or healthy controls. Although patients with the deficit syndrome evidenced higher levels of baseline communication disturbances than controls, they resembled control participants in degree of affective reactivity of speech. Given that the patient groups did not differ with respect to level of education, IQ or psychosocial functioning, and that controlling for psychotic symptoms did not significantly change the results, it is unlikely that the present findings are due to generalized deficit effects. In summary, the results of this study support the idea that the relationship between negative affect/stress and speech disturbances is mediated, at least in part, by patients capacity to experience emotions. Hence, differences among patients in their capacity for emotional experience may help to explain previous findings that some patients exhibit marked affective reactivity of speech while others do not. Alternatively, it is possible that both a diminished capacity for emotional experience and diminished levels of affective reactivity of speech are phenomena that are specific to the deficit syndrome, and that nondeficit patients tend to exhibit high levels of affective reactivity of speech regardless of their level

A.S. Cohen, N.M. Docherty / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 7 14 13 of emotional experience. In support of this formulation, when the nondeficit patient group was examined alone, deficit syndrome scores were not at all related to affective reactivity scores (r[22] = 0.074, ns). This finding adds some support to the idea that the deficit syndrome reflects a categorically defined subgroup of patients with a distinct underlying pathological process (see Kirkpatrick et al., 2001). However, further research will be required to determine whether the deficit syndrome truly reflects a categorical or continuous construct, and whether lower levels of affective reactivity occur exclusively in patients with deficit symptomatology. Whether or not affective reactivity of speech is related to emotional capacity only in patients with the deficit syndrome, the capacity for emotional experience appears to be a promising construct for discriminating subtypes or distinct processes within schizophrenia. The present findings add further support to the idea that the diminished emotionality seen in the deficit syndrome may have beneficial or protective value (see Cohen et al., submitted for publication). Generally speaking, patients with the deficit syndrome are considered to be more severely ill than nondeficit patients in terms of overall psychopathology. However, deficit syndrome patients were more nearly normal with respect to affective reactivity of speech and consistent with some prior research had less severe psychotic symptomatology than nondeficit patients (Kirkpatrick et al., 1993; Buchanan et al., 1994, but see Buchanan et al., 1997). While conclusions are premature, these results suggest that in some cases, positive and negative/deficit symptom severity may be inversely related. Further research will be required to determine whether diminished emotionality in patients is associated with lower affective reactivity of other reactive positive symptoms such as disorganized behavior and clarify how positive symptoms such as hallucinations and delusions differ between deficit and nondeficit patients. Several limitations of the present project warrant mention here. First, although there were no obvious medication effects, the methodology used in the present study did not permit a full analysis of the potential impact of medications on symptomatology or levels of affective reactivity of speech. Second, the use of a cross sectional case identification tool for deficit syndrome group assignment is not ideal because it does not assess the degree of stability of the putative deficit symptoms. Nonetheless, the PDS has been found to correspond highly to the SDS, a longitudinal measure of deficit syndrome. Furthermore, because PDS scores are based almost exclusively on affective symptoms from the BPRS, the PDS may actually be a better measure of diminished emotionality than the SDS. One drawback to using the PDS for group assignment is that it requires removal of the patients with mid range scores. This procedure reduced the sample size considerably in the patient group. However, the between-group findings were significant despite the relatively small sample size. 4.1. Conclusions The present study found that patients with the deficit syndrome evidenced significantly lower levels of affective reactivity of speech than patients with nondeficit schizophrenia. These results suggest that emotion-related variables mediate the relationship between stress and language symptom exacerbations in at least some patients. Further research is required to determine whether other symptoms besides communication disturbances are similarly related to level of emotionality, and whether patients level of emotionality is a mediating vulnerability factor specific to the deficit syndrome, or whether it is a factor in schizophrenia more generally. Acknowledgements This research was supported by NIMH grants MH58783 and MH57151 to Dr. Docherty. References American Psychiatric Association, 1987. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders III Revised. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC. American Psychiatric Association, 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC. Buchanan, R.W., Strauss, M.E., Kirkpatrick, B., Holstein, C., Breier, A., Carpenter Jr., W.T., 1994. Neuropsychological impairments in deficit vs. nondeficit forms of schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51, 804 811.

14 A.S. Cohen, N.M. Docherty / Schizophrenia Research 69 (2004) 7 14 Buchanan, R.W., Strauss, M.E., Breier, A., Kirkpatrick, B., Carpenter Jr., W.T., 1997. Attentional impairments in deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 363 370. Burbridge, J.A., Barch, D.M., 2002. Emotional valence and reference disturbance in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 111, 186 191. Carpenter Jr., W.T., 1988. Deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: the concept. Am. J. Psychiatry 145, 578 583. Cohen, A., Docherty, N., 2003. Deficit vs. negative syndrome of schizophrenia: prediction of attentional impairment. Submitted for publication. Cohen, A., Docherty, N., Dinzeo, T., Nienow, T., 2003. Stress and the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Submitted for publication. Crow, T.J., 1985. The two-syndrome concept: origins and current status. Schizophr. Bull. 11, 471 486. Docherty, N.M., 1996. Affective reactivity of symptoms as a process discriminator in schizophrenia. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 184, 535 541. Docherty, N.M., Grillon, C., 1995. Affective reactivity of language and the startle response in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 38, 68 70. Docherty, N.M., Evans, I.M., Sledge, W.H., Seibyl, J.P., Krystal, J.H., 1994. Affective reactivity of language in schizophrenia. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 182, 98 102. Docherty, N.M., Rhinewine, J.P., Labhart, R.P., Gordinier, S.W., 1998. Communication disturbances and family psychiatric history in parents of schizophrenic patients. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 186, 761 768. Endicott, J., Spitzer, R.L., 1978. A diagnostic interview: the schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 35, 837 844. Fowles, D.C., 1992. Schizophrenia: diathesis stress revisited. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 43, 303 336. Frisch, M., Jessop, N., 1989. Improving WAIS-R estimates with the Shipley Hartford and Wonderlic personnel tests: need to control for reading ability. Psychol. Rep. 65, 923 928. Hauser, R., Warren, J., 1997. Socioeconomic Indexes of Occupational Status: A Review, Update and Critique. Sociological Methodology. In: Raftery, A. (Ed.), Blackwell Publishers, Cambridge, pp. 177 298. Kirkpatrick, B., Buchanan, R.W., McKenney, P.D., Alphs, L.D., Carpenter Jr., W.T., 1989. The Schedule for the Deficit syndrome: an instrument for research in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 30, 119 123. Kirkpatrick, B., Buchanan, R.W., Breier, A., Carpenter Jr., W.T., 1993. Case identification and stability of the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 47, 47 56. Kirkpatrick, B., Buchanan, R.W., Breier, A., Carpenter Jr., W.T., 1994. Depressive symptoms and the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 182, 452 455. Kirkpatrick, B., Amador, X.F., Yale, S.A., Bustillo, J.R., Buchanan, R.W., Tohen, M., 1996. The deficit syndrome in the DSM-IV Field Trial: Part II. Depressive episodes and persecutory beliefs. Schizophr. Res. 20, 79 90. Kirkpatrick, B., Buchanan, R.W., Ross, D.E., Carpenter Jr., W.T., 2001. A separate disease within the syndrome of schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 58, 165 171. Liddle, P.F., Barnes, T.R., Morris, D., Haque, S., 1989. Three syndromes in chronic schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry, Suppl., 119 122. Lukoff, D., Nuechterlein, K., Ventura, J., 1986. Manual for the expanded brief psychiatric rating scale. Schizophr. Bull. 12, 594 602. Nuechterlein, K.H., Dawson, M.E., 1984. A heuristic vulnerability/ stress model of schizophrenic episodes. Schizophr. Bull. 10, 300 312. Rhinewine, J.P., Docherty, N.M., 2002. Affective reactivity of language and right-ear advantage in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 53, 181 186. Rosenfarb, I.S., Nuechterlein, K.H., Goldstein, M.J., Subotnik, K.L., 2000. Neurocognitive vulnerability, interpersonal criticism, and the emergence of unusual thinking by schizophrenic patients during family transactions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 1174 1179. Tek, C., Kirkpatrick, B., Buchanan, R.W., 2001. A five-year followup study of deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 49, 253 260. Ventura, J., Nuechterlein, K.H., Subotnik, K.L., Gutkind, D., Gilbert, E.A., 2000. Symptom dimensions in recent-onset schizophrenia and mania: a principal components analysis of the 24-item Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychiatry Res. 97, 129 135. Zachary, R., Paulson, M., Gorsuch, R., 1985. Estimating WAIS IQ from the Shipley Institute of Living Scale using continuously adjusted age norms. J. Clin. Psychol. 41, 820 831.