The Impact of Advance Letters on Cellphone Response in a Statewide Dual-Frame Survey

Similar documents
The Effectiveness of Advance Letters for Cell Telephone Samples

Item Nonresponse and the 10-Point Response Scale in Telephone Surveys 1

The Impact of Cellphone Sample Representation on Variance Estimates in a Dual-Frame Telephone Survey

Item-Nonresponse and the 10-Point Response Scale in Telephone Surveys

Using Soft Refusal Status in the Cell-Phone Nonresponse Adjustment in the National Immunization Survey

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, December, 2014, Perceptions of Job News Trend Upward

NEW JERSEY RESIDENTS DON T KNOW OR LIKE MUCH ABOUT COMMON CORE

Comparing Dual Phone Type Respondents from Cell Sample vs. Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System (BRFSS)

Reducing Non Response in Longitudinal Studies: What Can We Do Instead of Increasing Monetary Incentives?

The Effects of Introductory Scripts and Respondent Incentives on Overall Participation and Subgroup Participation in an RDD Telephone Survey

Attitudes about Opioids among North Carolina Voters

NEW JERSEY: SUPPORT FOR LEGAL WEED STAYS HIGH

Geographical Accuracy of Cell Phone Samples and the Effect on Telephone Survey Bias, Variance, and Cost

Executive Summary. Lupus Awareness Survey. October 2012

ASSESSING WITHIN-HOUSEHOLD SELECTION METHODS IN HOUSEHOLD MAIL SURVEYS

SURVEY TOPIC INVOLVEMENT AND NONRESPONSE BIAS 1

EVALUATING THE IMPACT OF A NEW CATI SCREENER IN THE NATIONAL IMMUNIZATION SURVEY

Nonresponse Bias in a Longitudinal Follow-up to a Random- Digit Dial Survey

Adjustment for Noncoverage of Non-landline Telephone Households in an RDD Survey

NEW JERSEY: LEGAL WEED SEEN AS ECONOMIC BOON

Survey Research and Cell Phones: Is There a Problem?

Vermonters Choose Healthy Eating Habits: Children and Time Impact Eating Choices Most Vermonter Poll March, 2008 Michele C.

2017 TOBACCO USAGE COMMUNITY SURVEY. Tobacco-Free Action of Columbia Greene

Technologies is funded by a 5 year grant from the U.S. Department of Education s National Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research.

Oral Health in Children in Iowa

Testing for non-response and sample selection bias in contingent valuation: Analysis of a combination phone/mail survey

Pre-Conception & Pregnancy in Ohio

A Survey of Public Opinion on Secondhand Smoke Related Issues in Bourbon County, KY

Lewis & Clark National Estimation and Awareness Study

CELLULAR-ONLY SUBSTITUTION IN THE UNITED STATES AS LIFESTYLE ADOPTION IMPLICATIONS FOR TELEPHONE SURVEY COVERAGE

Oral Health in Children in Iowa: An Overview From the 2010 Iowa Child and Family Household Health Survey

THE LAST-BIRTHDAY SELECTION METHOD & WITHIN-UNIT COVERAGE PROBLEMS

Health Information Technology: Comprehensive Telehealth Interventions to Improve Diet Among Patients with Chronic Diseases

An Examination of Visual Design Effects in a Self- Administered Mail Survey

Public Attitudes and Knowledge about HIV/AIDS in Georgia Kaiser Family Foundation

Funding Health and Long-Term Care: A Survey on Increasing North Carolina's Cigarette and Alcohol Taxes

Following a Telephone Survey with a Mail Survey

Huber, Gregory A. and John S. Lapinski "The "Race Card" Revisited: Assessing Racial Priming in

Survey Errors and Survey Costs

WHAT S MISSING FROM NATIONAL LANDLINE RDD SURVEYS? THE IMPACT OF THE GROWING CELL-ONLY POPULATION

Report 2. Data Collection Methods. -Short Report- CHIS 2015 Methodology Report Series. August 17, 2017

IMMEDIATE RELEASE THURSDAY, JUNE 18, 2015

Response Scales in CATI Surveys. Interviewers Experience

What is your age? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative Percent. Under 20 years to 37 years

Report 1. Sample Design. -Short Report- CHIS 2015 Methodology Report Series. December 13, 2016

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, February 2015, 83% Say Measles Vaccine Is Safe for Healthy Children

Department of Mathematics and Statistics

Social Issues in Nonmetropolitan Nebraska: Perceptions of Social Stigma and Drug and Alcohol Abuse: 2018 Nebraska Rural Poll Results

Berrien County 2011 Behavioral Risk Factor Survey. A report on the status of health outcomes and their related behaviors in Berrien County

Changes in Prevalence of Parent-reported Autism Spectrum Disorder in School-aged U.S. Children: 2007 to

Within-Household Selection in Mail Surveys: Explicit Questions Are Better Than Cover Letter Instructions

Report 1. Sample Design

BY Lee Rainie and Cary Funk

Survey research. Survey. A data collection method based on: Survey. Survey. It also comes with shortcomings. In general, survey offers:

Survey of Pathways to Diagnosis and Services

October CHIS 2017 Methodology Report Series. Report 1. Sample Design

WEDNESDAY JUNE 20, 2018

How Errors Cumulate: Two Examples

Annenberg Science Knowledge Survey: Zika August 18-August 22, 2016 Florida Oversample/Non-Florida (Week 28, N=1,472) Appendix

BLACK RESIDENTS VIEWS ON HIV/AIDS IN THE DISTRICT OF COLUMBIA

A Comparison of Listed and Unlisted Households on Nonresponse and Measurement Error

Evaluating Questionnaire Issues in Mail Surveys of All Adults in a Household

Using Telephone Survey Data to Predict Participation in an. Internet Panel Survey

Methodology for the VoicesDMV Survey

Panel Attrition and its Effects on Results from a Longitudinal Study:

Adult overweight and obesity

Surveying alcohol and other drug use through telephone sampling: a comparison of landline and mobile phone samples

CRACIUN RESEARCH. Alaska Injury Prevention Center CHA. September 7, 2014

USE OF COGNITIVE SHORTCUTS IN LANDLINE AND CELL PHONE SURVEYS

Noncoverage Adjustments in a Single-Frame Cell-Phone Survey: Weighting Approach to Adjust for Phoneless and Landline-Only Households

Comparison of Estimates From An Address-Based Mail Survey And A RDD Telephone Survey

Running Head: PARENTAL ATTITUDES & CHILDHOOD VACCINES 1

2016/17 SEASONAL INFLUENZA VACCINE COVERAGE IN CANADA

Does Voice Matter for Youth Reports of Tobacco Use? An Interactive Voice Response Experiment

Future research should conduct a personal magnetic-field exposure survey for a large sample (e.g., 1000 persons), including people under 18.

Howard Memorial Hospital. Community Needs Assessment Summary

2. Could you insert a reference, proving your statement on p. 5, l. 66/67?

Special Cancer Behavioral Risk Factor Survey, 2008

Do People Care What s Done with Their Biobanked Samples?

Two-in-three are dieting or exercising In the Battle of the Bulge, More Soldiers Than Successes

Does Voice Matter for Youth Reports of Tobacco Use? An Interactive Voice Response Experiment

Assessing Outpatient Drug Abuse

American Addiction Centers Outcomes Study Long-Term Outcomes Among Residential Addiction Treatment Clients. Centerstone Research Institute

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, October 2014, Most Are Confident in Government s Ability to Prevent Major Ebola Outbreak in U.S.

ASTHMA BY RACE AND ETHNICITY

Center for Urban Initiatives and Research Wisconsin Public Health Survey December 2011 N=626. Frequency Tables (Weighted)

I. Survey Methodology

DMA will take your dental practice to the next level

Media Advisory. Alameda County Public Health Department. acphd.org (510)

Introduction and Methodology

Variable Data univariate data set bivariate data set multivariate data set categorical qualitative numerical quantitative

APPLES TO ORANGES OR GALA VERSUS GOLDEN DELICIOUS? COMPARING DATA QUALITY OF NONPROBABILITY INTERNET SAMPLES TO LOW RESPONSE RATE PROBABILITY SAMPLES

Estimates of Influenza Vaccination Coverage among Adults United States, Flu Season

National Survey of Teens and Young Adults on HIV/AIDS

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Psychiatr Serv. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 October 01.

NCVS- KENTUCKY S ISSUES

THE 2009 NEW JERSEY HOUSEHOLD SURVEY ON DRUG USE AND HEALTH

2015 Wyoming Adult Tobacco Survey

Appendix I: Methodology

Transcription:

Vol. 11, Issue 2, 2018 The Impact of Advance Letters on Cellphone Response in a Statewide Dual-Frame Survey Eva Aizpurua *, Ki H. Park, Mitchell Avery, Jill Wittrock *, Rodney Muilenburg, Mary E. Losch * * Institution: University of Northern Iowa (IA) Department: Center for Social & Behavioral Research Institution: University of Northern Iowa (IA) Department: Center for Social & Behavioral Research Institution: University of Northern Iowa (IA) Department: Center for Social & Behavioral Research Institution: University of Northern Iowa (IA) Department: Center for Social & Behavioral Research Institution: University of Northern Iowa (IA) Department: Center for Social & Behavioral Research Institution: University of Northern Iowa (IA) Department: Center for Social & Behavioral Research

Abstract Many studies have illustrated the effectiveness of prenotification letters to increase response rates for telephone surveys. However, most studies assessing the impact of advance letters have been carried out using landlines, so the evidence of the effectiveness of prenotification letters on cellphones is scarce. This is important because the proportion of cellphone numbers used in survey research continues to rise. In this study, we assess the impact of advance letters on response rates from a cellphone sample of a statewide dual-frame survey about perceptions of and experiences with healthcare. In a dual-frame telephone survey of a Midwestern state s residents, half of cellphones with addresses were randomly assigned to receive advance letters (n = 600) and the other half (n = 600) did not receive prenotices. Cellphone results are similar to previous studies using landlines, showing a positive effect of advance letters on cellphone response and cooperation rates (9.1 and 10.3 percentage point gain, respectively). Despite this positive effect on responses, the letters seemed to affect the sample composition by increasing the presence of respondents who were white, with higher education and income. The impact of advance letters on calling effort varied; it reduced the number of call attempts needed to reach completions while increasing the attempts required that ultimately resulted in refusals. Implications of the results for the use of advance letters prior to calling cellphones in dual-frame telephone surveys are discussed. Survey Practice 1

Introduction The percentage of cellphone-only households has grown steadily in recent years. At the end of 2006, 15.8% of American homes had only wireless phones but, by the end of 2016, that percentage had risen to 50.8% (Blumberg and Luke 2007; Blumberg and Luke 2017). The increase in cellphone-only households and the notable decline in response rates (e.g., Dutwin and Lavrakas 2016) have led researchers to adapt their methods to the realities of telephone surveys in the 21st century. The use of dual-frame telephone surveys that include landline and cellphone numbers, rather than just landline numbers, and prenotification or advance letters are methods that have been used to combat declining response rates. With landline random digit dialing (RDD) and list-based studies, advance letters have been shown to be effective at increasing response and cooperation rates (de Leeuw et al. 2007; Richardson, 2011). However, most studies assessing the impact of advance letters have been carried out using landlines; thus, the evidence in dual-frame samples is scarce and inconclusive to date. An experiment carried out in Norway showed that advance letters resulted in higher response rates and lower refusals in a dual-frame telephone sample combining landline and cellphone numbers. Specifically, response rates increased by 8% and refusals decreased by 27% (Kluch, Tortora and Kluch 2015). The aim of this study is to assess the impact of advance letters on cellphone responses in a statewide dual-frame survey examining perceptions and experiences with healthcare. Based on findings from landline RDD and list-based studies, we hypothesize that advance letters would increase cellphone response and cooperation rates (de Leeuw et al. 2007). We also hypothesize that advance letters would reduce the calling effort required to complete a survey (Hembroff et al. 2005). Finally, we examine whether advance letters impact the volume of missing data and the sample composition in order to determine whether any increase in response rates results in a reduction in data quality (as measured by item nonresponse) and/or an increase in nonresponse bias. Survey Practice 2

Methods Study Design Data for the experiment were collected from January 13 to February 17, 2017, as part of a statewide dual-frame survey of adults in a Midwestern state regarding their perceptions and experiences with healthcare. The random sample (provided by Marketing Systems Group) included cellphone numbers for which corresponding addresses were available. To determine the impact of advance letters on cellphone response rates and data quality, the sample was randomly split into two groups. As shown in Figure 1, respondents in the treatment group (n = 600) were sent advance letters, while respondents in the control group (n = 600) did not receive prenotices. Figure 1 Research design. Between three and eleven days prior to the first call attempts, staff at the Center for Social & Behavioral Research sent advance letters with first-class postage. Cellphone respondents' letters were addressed to individuals registered as the phones' users. The advance letter text is included in the supplemental materials. The letters invited potential respondents to take part in the study, outlined its purpose and usefulness, and mentioned the importance of their participation. The letters also specified the institutions carrying out the study and provided contact information. Finally, they indicated the number from which they would receive the calls and the estimated dates of contact. All of the letters were in English and were not personalized. No incentives were offered. Survey Practice 3

Variables and Analysis The effect of prenotification letters was analyzed on six different measures: response rates (RR3, American Association for Public Opinion Research [AAPOR] 2016); cooperation rates (COOP3, AAPOR 2016); contact rates (CON3, AAPOR 2016); number of call attempts needed to reach completions and refusals (range 1 to 11); nonsubstantive responses ("Refused" and "Don't know") and sample composition (i.e., demographic characteristics). Chi-square tests were used to compare response, contact, and cooperation rates between the groups. Differences in call attempts and item nonresponse between the groups were tested using parametric (t-tests) and nonparametric tests (Mann-Whitney tests) and calculating effect sizes (Cohen's d). In accordance with previous research (Koopman et al. 2013), only items for which nonresponse exceeded 5% were compared. Comparisons between the groups on selected demographics were performed using chi-square tests and t-tests. Data presented here are unweighted. Findings Response, Cooperation, and Contact Rates The RR3 was 9.1 percentage points higher for the treatment group (31.3%) compared to the control group (22.2%), and this difference was statistically significant (X 2 [1] = 12.865, p <.01). Next, we calculated contact and cooperation rates. We did so to determine whether this difference was due to the fact that the participants in the letter group were more reachable or, being equally reachable, more willing to complete the survey. Only cooperation rates were significantly different between the groups (X 2 [1] = 1.316, p =.339; and X 2 [1] = 22.356, p <.01, respectively). The CON3 was comparable across groups (97.2% vs. 98.2%); however, the COOP3 was 10.3 points higher in the treatment group (87.8% vs. 77.5%) when compared to the control group. Hence, participants in the treatment group were equally reachable but were more willing to cooperate after being contacted than were participants in the control group. In summary, our results show the positive effect of advance letters on response and Survey Practice 4

cooperation rates, even when only 58.9% of respondents who were sent a letter remembered receiving it and 82.2% of those who remembered receiving the letter reported reading it. This percentage is slightly lower than the rates obtained in landline samples (de Leeuw et al. 2007), suggesting that cellphone addresses may be less accurate or that cellphone-only households are less likely to open and/or read mail from unknown senders. Number of Call Attempts Needed to Reach a Final Disposition We also compared the number of call attempts needed to obtain completions and refusals. On average, the number of calls required to reach a completion was lower in the treatment group (M = 2.72, SD = 1.71), than in the control group (M = 2.88, SD = 1.87). Although this difference did not reach statistical significance (t[208] = 0.665, p =.507), a small effect size was found (Cohen's d = 0.09). Because the distribution of this variable was skewed, we supplemented this analysis with nonparametric tests that yielded similar results. The median number of call attempts was 2 on the treatment condition and 2.50 on the control condition (Mann-Whitney = 5136.000, z = -0.464, p =.643). Among cases that resulted in refusals, the average number of calls required was higher in the treatment group (M = 4.88, SD = 2.94) than in the control group (M = 4.39, SD = 2.59). However, the difference in the means between the two groups was not statistically significant (t[37] = 0.543, p =.591), and the effect size was small (Cohen's d = 0.18). A Mann-Whitney test was performed taking into consideration the non-normal distribution of the variable. Similar to the findings obtained when comparing the means, the Mann-Whitney test indicated that those in the treatment group had a higher, although nonsignificant, number of call attempts (Mdn = 5.50) than those in the control group (Mdn = 4, Mann-Whitney = 199.500, z = -0.447, p =.662). In summary, prenotification letters did not significantly reduce the number of calls to completion or final refusal, although a small effect was observed in both cases. Survey Practice 5

Nonsubstantive Responses We explored the possibility that advance letters could have an effect on nonsubstantive responses. In general, these responses were very low (ranging from 0% and 3% for all of the questions except one). The only item for which "don't know" and "refusal" exceeded 5% was income (the question was worded as "What is your annual gross household income from all sources before taxes?"). We found no significant difference between the groups (X 2 [1] = 0.748, p =.475) as the percentage of these answers was similar in the treatment group (8.1%) and the control group (11.6%). Respondents' Characteristics: acteristics: Are There Differences Between the Groups? The characteristics of the respondents who completed the survey in both groups are shown in Table 1. No statistically significant differences were found between the groups in four of the five sociodemographic variables (age, sex, education, and income level). However, there was a significant difference in race (X 2 [1] = 4.847, p =.05), with the treatment group having a higher percentage of whites (97.6% vs. 90.7%) than the control group. While not statistically different, the treatment group profile indicated a lower percentage of individuals with lower income levels (34.2% vs. 44.7%) and educational attainment (19.4% vs. 24.4%). Survey Practice 6

Table 1 Respondents characteristics acteristics by group. No letter (86) Letter (124) Average age [SD]] (t[207] = 0.131, p =.90) 50.34 [17.09] 50.65 [16.69] Sex (X 2 [1] = 0.000, p = 1.00) 1 Male 45.9% (39) 46.0% (57) Female 54.1% (46) 54.0% (67) Race (X 2 [1] = 4.847, p =.051) White 90.7% (78) 97.6% (121) Non-White 9.3% (8) 2.4% (3) Education (X 2 [2] = 0.818, p =.66) High school, GED or less 24.4% (21) 19.4% (24) Some college/ technical school 36.0% (31) 37.1% (46) College graduate and more 39.5% (34) 41.9% (54) Income (X 2 [2] = 4.405, p =.11) <$50,000 44.7% (34) 34.2% (39) $50,000 - $100,000 36.8% (28) 34.2% (39) >$100,000 18.4% (14) 31.6% (36) 1 Fisher s exact test Discussion and Conclusions Our results are consistent with previous studies using landline RDD and list-based samples showing the positive effects of advance letters to reduce nonresponse (de Leeuw et al. 2007). The response and cooperation rates were 9.1 and 10.3 percentage points higher for the treatment group compared to the control group. In accordance with the results obtained in most landline samples (Richardson, 2011; see Von der Lippe, Schmich and Lang 2011 for an exception), we found no effect of advance letters on contact rates indicating that respondents in the treatment group were as likely to pick up the phone as those in the control group. Despite this increase in participation, the results do not allow us to rule out the possibility that advance letters may have introduced sample bias in dual-frame telephone surveys. Although only racial composition yielded significant differences between the groups with a greater percentage of white participants in the treatment group compared to the control group, our results suggest a greater presence of highly educated and high-income respondents in the treatment group. Although these differences were not statistically significant, they are consistent with the trend observed in previous research (Link and Mokdad 2005) in which advance Survey Practice 7

letters helped to recruit respondents that are already easier to interview such as older, better educated, and less racially diverse individuals. However, further research is needed to explore the reliability of this possible bias. In addition to increasing response and cooperation rates, our results provide evidence that sending advance letters in dual-frame surveys can help to reduce the calling effort needed to complete the survey. However, this effect was small and the consideration of time and money necessary to send the letters is a factor to consider when adopting this approach, especially taking into account that advance letters increased the number of call attempts required to reach final refusals by roughly 0.5 attempts which also increases overall survey costs. Finally, we did not find an effect of the advance letters on nonsubstantive responses. This finding corroborates previous evidence from mail surveys (Koopman et al. 2013) suggesting that the increase in participation does not come at the cost of a reduction in data quality. Future studies may be able to expand on these findings analyzing other indicators of data quality in addition to item nonresponse (e.g., acquiescence, non-differentiation). In conclusion, our findings build on previous research showing the positive effects that advance letters can have on response and cooperation rates in dual-frame telephone surveys. This result is useful for survey practitioners given the widespread use of phone surveys that combine cellphones and landlines and the scarcity of studies conducted with this sample frame. Although advance letters do not seem to affect nonsubstantive responses, their effect on sample composition should continue to be studied in future work. Acknowledgement The authors would like to thank Suzanne Bentler and Peter Damiano at the University of Iowa Public Policy Center who facilitated the project by allowing us to conduct this experiment while implementing the study. Survey Practice 8

References American Association for Public Opinion Research. 2016. Standard definitions: final dispositions of case codes and outcome rates for surveys (9th ed.). American Association for Public Opinion Research, Ann Arbor, MI. Blumberg, S.J. and J.V. Luke. 2007. Wireless substitution: early release of estimates from the National Health Interview Survey, July-December 2006. National Center for Health Statistics. Available at https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhis/releases.htm#wirele ss Blumberg, S.J. and J.V. Luke. 2017. Wireless substitution: early release of estimates from the National Health Interview Survey, July-December 2016. National Center for Health Statistics. Available at https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhis/releases.htm#wirele ss de Leeuw, E.D., M. Callegaro, J. Hox, E. Korendijk and G. Lensvelt-Mulders. 2007. The influence of advance letters on response in telephone surveys: a meta-analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly 71 (3):413-443. doi:10.1093/poq/nfm014. Dutwin, D. and P.J. Lavrakas. 2016. Trends in telephone outcomes, 2008-2015. Survey Practice 9(3):1-9. Hembroff, L.A., D. Rusz, A. Rafferty, H. McGee and N. Ehrlich. 2005. The cost-effectiveness of alternative advance mailings in a telephone survey. Public Opinion Quarterly 69(2):232-245. doi:10.1093/poq/nfi021. Kluch, S.P., R. Tortora and K. Kluch. 2015. Impact of pre-notices on response rate in a national RDD study in Norway. Presented at the American Association for Public Opinion Research Conference, Hollywood, FL. Koopman, L., L. Donselaar, J.J. Rademakers and M. Hendriks. 2013. A prenotification letter increased initial response, whereas sender did not affect response rates. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 66(3):340-348. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2012.09.012. Survey Practice 9

Link, M.W. and A. Mokdad. 2005. Advance letters as a means of improving respondent cooperation in random digit dial studies: a multistate experiment. Public Opinion Quarterly 69(4):572-587. doi:10.1093/poq/nfi055. Richardson, A. 2011. Are advance postcards or letters better? An experiment with advance mailing types. Survey Practice 4(4):1-4. Von der Lippe, E., P. Schmich and C. Lange. 2011. Advance letters as a way of reducing non-response in a national health telephone survey: differences between listed and unlisted numbers. Survey Research Methods 5(3):103-116. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.18148/ srm/2011.v5i3.4657. Survey Practice 10

Figures, Tables, and Supplementary Materials Supplemental materials: Advance letter Download: http://app.scholasticahq.com/api/v1/attachments/9550/download Survey Practice 11