Visual adaptation to masculine and feminine faces influences generalized preferences and perceptions of trustworthiness B

Similar documents
PERCEPTIONS OF PARTNER FEMININITY PREDICT INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MEN S SENSITIVITY TO FACIAL CUES OF MALE DOMINANCE

Sensation seeking and men's face preferences

British Journal of Psychology (2010), 00, 1 13 q 2010 The British Psychological Society

IN PRESS: PROCEEDINGS OF THE ROYAL SOCIETY OF LONDON, B

Personality and Individual Differences

URL: <

UNCORRECTED PROOF ARTICLE IN PRESS

B.C. Jones a, *, A.C. Little b, L. Boothroyd c, L.M. DeBruine c, D.R. Feinberg c, M.J. Law Smith c, R.E. Cornwell c, F.R. Moore c, D.I.

Adaptation to different mouth shapes influences visual perception of ambiguous lip speech

When facial attractiveness is only skin deep

Sex Differences in Attraction to Familiar and Unfamiliar Opposite-Sex Faces: Men Prefer Novelty and Women Prefer Familiarity

Facial resemblance increases the attractiveness of same-sex faces more than other-sex faces

Preferences for symmetry in faces change across the menstrual cycle


NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Vision Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 January 25.

Viewing attractive or unattractive same-sex individuals changes self-rated attractiveness and face preferences in women

Women s own voice pitch predicts their preferences for masculinity in men s voices

Title: THE FACES OF LEADERS: SEXUAL DIMORPHISM, PERCEIVED TRAITS, AND VOTING IN CONTEXT

Female Condition Influences Preferences for Sexual Dimorphism in Faces of Male Humans (Homo sapiens)

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

Perceived health contributes to the attractiveness of facial symmetry, averageness, and sexual dimorphism

Discrimination of Attractiveness and Health in Men s Faces: the Impact of Color Cues and Variation in Relation to Sex and Age of Rater

Resemblance to self increases the appeal of child faces to both men and women

Evolution and Human Behavior

Impressions of Dominance are Made Relative to Others in the Visual Environment

Sex, beauty, and the relative luminance of facial features

Common neural mechanisms for the evaluation of facial trustworthiness and emotional expressions as revealed by behavioral adaptation

Jillian J. M. O Connor

Relationship satisfaction and outcome in women who meet their partner while using oral contraception

Investigating an imprinting-like phenomenon in humans Partners and opposite-sex parents have similar hair and eye colour

The voice and face of woman: One ornament that signals quality?

Author's personal copy

Does the Interaction Between Cortisol and Testosterone Predict Men s Facial Attractiveness?

Don t look back in anger: the rewarding value of a female face is discounted by an angry expression

Voice pitch influences voting behavior

Judging a Man by the Width of his Face: The Role of Facial Ratios and Dominance in Mate Choice at Speed-Dating Events

Crossing the Uncanny Valley : Adaptation to Cartoon Faces Can Influence Perception of Human Faces

BIRTH CONTROL, KISSING, AND ASSESSMENT OF MATE QULAITY 1. Running head: BIRTH CONTROL, KISSING, AND ASSESSMENT OF MATE

View-Specific Coding of Face Shape Linda Jeffery, 1 Gillian Rhodes, 1 and Tom Busey 2

Running head: STIGMA AND FACIAL JUDGMENT 1

Vision Research 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Gaze Properties of Women Judging the Attractiveness of Masculine and Feminine Male Faces

URL: < >

MENSTRUAL INFLUENCES ON PERSON PERCEPTION: MALE SENSITIVITY TO FLUCTUATING FEMALE FERTILITY

Female Preferences for Male Vocal and Facial Masculinity in Videos

Stimulus Selectivity of Figural Aftereffects for Faces

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Oral Contraceptives Suppress Ovarian Hormone Production

Body Odor Quality Predicts Behavioral Attractiveness in Humans

This version was downloaded from Northumbria Research Link:

Assortative mating for perceived facial personality traits

Concordant preferences for opposite-sex signals? Human pheromones and facial characteristics

RELATIVE DIGIT LENGTHS PREDICT MEN S BEHAVIOR AND ATTRACTIVENESS DURING SOCIAL INTERACTIONS WITH WOMEN

Kin Selection Theory. Overview. Methods of Kin Recognition. The Trust Game. Hypotheses. The Trust Game

Sex-Specificity in the Reward Value of Facial Attractiveness. Amanda C Hahn 1, Lisa M DeBruine 1, Benedict C Jones 1. Corresponding author

Kin recognition: evidence that humans can perceive both positive and negative relatedness

Smelling human sex hormone-like compounds a ects face gender judgment of men

ADAPTATION TO RACIAL CONTENT OF EMERGENT RACE FACES: GRADIENT SHIFT OR PEAK SHIFT?

Other Papers. What does facial symmetry reveal about health and personality?

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Cross-cultural perception of trustworthiness: The effect of ethnicity features on evaluation of faces observed trustworthiness across four samples

Preference for Facial Self-Resemblance and Attractiveness in Human Mate Choice

Cognition. Auditory adaptation in vocal affect perception. Patricia E.G. Bestelmeyer a,, Julien Rouger b, Lisa M. DeBruine c, Pascal Belin a

Copyright and moral rights for this work are retained by the author

This manuscript ( Facial scarring enhances men s attractiveness for short-term

Cross-category adaptation: exposure to faces produces gender aftereffects in body perception

Facial appearance affects voting decisions

Probing the Visual Representation of Faces With Adaptation

Personality goes a long way: The malleability of opposite-sex physical attractiveness

RUNNING HEAD: RAPID EVALUATION OF BIG FIVE 1

Published in: Vision Research. DOI: /j.visres Document Version Peer reviewed version

Populational differences in attractiveness judgements of male and female faces: Comparing British and Jamaican samples

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not?

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments

For Non-Commercial Academic Use Only

Are average and symmetric faces attractive to infants? Discrimination and looking preferences

SHORT REPORT Facial features influence the categorization of female sexual orientation

Differential Viewing Strategies towards Attractive and Unattractive Human Faces

An Ambiguous-Race Illusion in Children s Face Memory Kristin Shutts and Katherine D. Kinzler

Hand movements reveal the time-course of shape and pigmentation processing in face categorization

IN PERCEPTION SCIENCE

Gavin Buckingham. Web:

Static and Dynamic Facial Images Cue Similar Attractiveness Judgements

Are human preferences for facial symmetry focused on signals of developmental instability?

Social Perception of Facial Resemblance in Humans

Mixed-ethnicity face shape and attractiveness in humans

Mate retention behavior modulates men's preferences for self-resemblance in infant faces

Sexual behavior and jealousy: An evolutionary perspective

Title: Cross-cultural effects of color, but not morphological masculinity, on perceived attractiveness of men s faces

Using composite images to assess accuracy in personality attribution to faces

Accepted author version posted online: 02 Mar 2015.

THE BIAS OF PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS IN LEADER EMERGENCE: A META-ANALYTIC REVIEW

Fruit over sunbed: Carotenoid skin colouration is found more attractive than melanin colouration

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

I like the way you move : how hormonal changes across the menstrual cycle affect female perceptions of gait

Adaptive face coding and discrimination around the average face

Report. Face Adaptation without a Face

Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attractiveness

Functionality, Parsimony, Discovery, Avoiding Hamartia: How Evolutionary Perspectives are Changing Psychology

A model of parallel time estimation

Transcription:

Evolution and Human Behavior 27 (2006) 381 389 Visual adaptation to masculine and feminine faces influences generalized preferences and perceptions of trustworthiness B Gavin Buckingham a, Lisa M. DeBruine b, Anthony C. Little c, Lisa L.M. Welling a, Claire A. Conway a, Bernard P. Tiddeman d, Benedict C. Jones a, 4 a School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, Scotland, UK, AB24 3FX b School of Psychology, University of St Andrews, Scotland, UK, KY16 9AJ c School of Biology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK, L69 7Z8 d School of Computer Science, University of St Andrews, Scotland, UK, KY16 9AJ Initial receipt 8 November 2005; final revision received 10 March 2006 Abstract Although previous studies of individual differences in preferences for masculinity in male faces have typically emphasized the importance of factors such as changes in levels of sex hormones during the menstrual cycle, other research has demonstrated that recent visual experience with faces also influences preferences for sexual dimorphism in faces. Adaptation to either masculine or feminine faces increases preferences for novel faces that are similar to those that were recently seen. Here, we replicate this effect and demonstrate that adaptation to masculine or feminine faces also influences the extent to which masculine faces are perceived as trustworthy. These adaptation effects may reflect a proximate mechanism that contributes to the development of face preferences within individuals, underpins phenomena such as imprinting-like effects and condition-dependent face preferences, and shapes personality attributions to faces that play an important role in romantic partner and associate B LMD was supported by a grant from the U.S. National Science Foundation (0401951), ACL was supported by a Royal Society University Research Fellowship, and GB was supported by a studentship from the University of Aberdeen 6th Century. 4 Corresponding author. Face Research Laboratory, School of Psychology, William Guild Building, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, AB24 2UB, Scotland, UK. Tel.: +44 1224 273933; fax: +44 1224 273426. E-mail address: ben.jones@abdn.ac.uk (B.C. Jones). 1090-5138/06/$ see front matter D 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.03.001

382 G. Buckingham et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 27 (2006) 381 389 choices. Furthermore, our findings also support the proposal that visual exposure alone cannot explain the context specificity of attitudes to self-resembling faces. D 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Visual adaptation; Face aftereffects; Trust; Sexual dimorphism 1. Introduction Face preferences and perceptions influence important social interactions including partner and associate choices, hiring decisions, and voting behavior (e.g., Langlois et al., 2000; Todorov, Mandisodza, Goren, & Hall, 2005). Furthermore, many researchers have highlighted the importance of identifying the factors that influence the development of these attitudes (DeBruine, 2005; Little, DeBruine, & Jones, 2005; Perrett et al., 2002; Rhodes, Jeffery, Watson, Clifford, & Nakayama, 2003). Although factors such as changes in levels of sex hormones influence attitudes to faces (e.g., DeBruine, Jones, & Perrett, 2005; Jones, Little, Boothroyd, DeBruine, et al., 2005; Jones, Perrett, Little, et al., 2005; Penton-Voak et al., 1999), visual experience also plays an important role in shaping how we perceive others (Little et al., 2005; Perrett et al., 2002; Rhodes et al., 2003; Webster, Kaping, Mizokami, & Duhamel, 2004). Adaptation to faces following visual experience (i.e., weakened responses of neurons that code a given type of stimulus following habituation to that type of stimulus; Loffler, Yourganov, Wilkinson, & Wilson, 2005; Winston, Henson, Fine-Goulden, & Dolan, 2004) influences perceptions of faces (Leopold, O Toole, Vetter, & Blanz, 2001; Leopold, Rhodes, Müller, & Jeffery, 2005; Little et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2003, 2004; Webster et al., 2004). Adaptation to faces varying in race, sex, sexual dimorphism of shape, identity, eye spacing, and expanded or compressed features has been shown to influence subsequent perceptions of the normality (Leopold et al., 2001, 2005; Little et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2003, 2004; Webster et al., 2004) and attractiveness (Little et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2003) of faces such that faces similar to those seen previously are judged more normal and attractive than would otherwise be the case. Many researchers have emphasized the possible direct and indirect benefits of imprinting-like effects on mate preferences in both humans and nonhumans (as opposed to relatively inflexible innate preferences; for reviews, see Little, Burt, Penton-Voak, & Perrett, 2001 and Perrett et al., 2002), and visual adaptation has been identified as a plausible proximate mechanism for imprinting-like effects on face preferences (Little et al., 2005; Perrett et al., 2002). bface aftereffectsq (Leopold et al., 2001, 2005; Little et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2003, 2004; Webster et al., 2004) reflect changes in the responses of neurons that code faces (Loffler et al., 2005; Winston et al., 2004) and cannot be explained by retinal adaptation because they are robust to changes in the size and retinal location of the stimuli between adaptation and test phases (Leopold et al., 2001, 2005). Furthermore, bigger face aftereffects occur when the orientation or sex of the faces shown in the adaptation and test phases are congruent than when they are incongruent, suggesting that different neural populations code different subcategories of faces (Little et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2004).

G. Buckingham et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 27 (2006) 381 389 383 Although previous experiments have established that adaptation to faces increases subsequent attraction to novel similar faces (Little et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2003), it is unclear if face aftereffects also influence attributions of other prosocial traits that are important for social interactions (e.g., trustworthiness; see DeBruine, 2005 and Perrett et al., 1998). Although the bbeauty is goodq stereotype is a prevalent way to explain how humans attribute personality characteristics such as trustworthiness to faces (e.g., Langlois et al., 2000), judgments of faces in the contexts of mate choice and general social interaction may have different functional explanations and may be affected in different ways by different aspects of facial appearance (e.g., age and cues of kinship). For example, facial selfresemblance increases perceptions of the trustworthiness of opposite-sex faces but decreases perceptions of these same faces sexual attractiveness and does not influence attractiveness judgments made in the context of long-term relationships (which arguably reflect a mixture of sexual attraction and prosocial regard, DeBruine, 2005; see Little et al., 2001 and Little, Jones, Penton-Voak, Burt, & Perrett, 2002 for further discussion of the distinction between face preferences expressed in the contexts of short- and long-term relationships). For this reason, it is not obvious that factors affecting the attractiveness of faces will affect their perceived trustworthiness in the same way. To investigate the extent to which adaptation to faces has similar effects on attraction to novel similar faces and perceptions of their trustworthiness, here we compared the effect of adaptation to faces varying in sexual dimorphism of 2D shape on attraction with sexual dimorphism in male faces and attributions of the trustworthiness of masculine and feminine male faces. Male faces were used because attraction to sexual dimorphism in male faces is known to be variable (see Fink and Penton-Voak, 2002 and Penton-Voak and Perrett, 2000 for reviews), and therefore, ceiling effects on preferences are less likely to mask face aftereffects than if female faces were used, and because sex-contingent face aftereffects have previously been reported (Little et al., 2005). Sexual dimorphism of 2D shape was manipulated using computer graphic methods (prototype-based transformations; Rowland and Perrett, 1995; Tiddeman, Burt, & Perrett, 2001) that have been previously used to investigate attraction (Jones, Little, Boothroyd, DeBruine, et al., 2005; Little et al., 2001, 2002; Penton-Voak et al., 1999) and attributions of personality characteristics (Perrett et al., 1998) to masculine and feminine faces as well as to manufacture stimuli for studies of face aftereffects (Little et al., 2005). The adaptation paradigm we used was a slightly modified version of that used by Little et al. (2005) to demonstrate that adaptation to faces varying in sexual dimorphism of 2D shape influences attraction to masculine and feminine faces. 2. Methods 2.1. Stimuli Male and female prototype face images were manufactured by averaging the shape and color information from 20 young (17 23 years old) adult male faces (to create a male prototype face image) and 20 young (18 23 years old) adult female faces (to create a female prototype face

384 G. Buckingham et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 27 (2006) 381 389 image) using methods reported in Perrett et al. (1998) and elsewhere (Rowland and Perrett, 1995; Tiddeman et al., 2001). Following Little et al. (2005), the 20 male face images were then transformed by +50% of the differences in 2D shape between the male and female prototypes to create masculinized versions and by 50% of the differences in shape to create feminized versions (see Rowland and Perrett, 1995 and Tiddeman et al., 2001 for technical details of this method and Jones, Little, Boothroyd, DeBruine, et al., 2005, Little et al., 2001, 2002, 2004, and Penton-Voak et al., 1999 for other studies that have used this method to manipulate sexual dimorphism in faces). This procedure generates masculinized and feminized versions of the male faces that differ equally from average, differ only in 2D shape and are matched in terms of identity (see Little et al., 2005 and Perrett et al., 1998 for discussions of these issues). An example of a masculinized and feminized face used in the experiment is shown in Fig. 1. Itis noteworthy that while other methods for manipulating masculinity of faces have been used in some other studies (e.g., manipulating facial appearance using composites of faces perceived to be high or low in terms of masculinity; Johnston et al., 2001), these methods produce effects on face perceptions that are equivalent to those produced using the methods used in our current study (DeBruine et al., 2006). 2.2. Participants Fifty-five participants (mean age=22.46 years, S.D.=3.22 years, 34 females) took part in the experiment. 2.3. Procedure To assess attraction or attributions of trust to masculinity in male faces prior to the adaptation phase, we showed participants five pairs of male face images where each pair Fig. 1. Examples of masculinized (left) and feminized (right) versions of a male face image used in the experiment. Note that the faces differ in sexual dimorphism of 2D shape but are matched in other regards (e.g., identity).

G. Buckingham et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 27 (2006) 381 389 385 consisted of a masculinized and feminized version of the same identity. Twenty-six of the participants were asked to choose the face in each pair that they considered more attractive and to indicate the strength of this preference by choosing from the options bmuch more attractive,q bmore attractive,q bsomewhat more attractive,q and bslightly more attractive.q The remaining 29 participants were shown the same pairs of faces and were asked to choose the face in each pair that they considered more trustworthy and to indicate the strength of this preference by choosing from the options bmuch more trustworthy,q bmore trustworthy,q bsomewhat more trustworthy,q and bslightly more trustworthy.q Pairs of faces were presented in a random order, and the side of the screen on which any particular image was shown was also randomized. Following this preadaptation test phase, participants viewed a slideshow of 15 face images where each face was shown twice and for 2 s on each occasion. Participants were instructed to bwatch the faces and try to remember them.q Twenty-eight of the participants viewed masculinized versions of the face images, and the remainder viewed feminized versions of the same face images. After this adaptation phase, participants repeated the preadaptation test. Twelve participants viewed feminized faces in the adaptation phase and made attractiveness judgements, 15 participants viewed feminized faces in the adaptation phase and made trustworthiness judgements, 14 participants viewed masculinized faces in the adaptation phase and made attractiveness judgements, and 14 participants viewed masculinized faces in the adaptation phase and made trustworthiness judgements. 2.4. Initial processing of data For participants who made attractiveness judgments in the pre- and postadaptation phases of the experiment, responses were recoded using the following eight-point scale (with high scores indicating a strong preference for the type of face seen in the adaptation phase and low scores indicating a relatively weak preference for the type of face seen in the adaptation phase): 0=The type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated much more attractive than the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase. 1=The type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated more attractive than the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase. 2=The type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated somewhat more attractive than the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase. 3=The type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated slightly more attractive than the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase. 4=The type of face viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated slightly more attractive than the type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase. 5=The type of face viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated somewhat more attractive than the type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase.

386 G. Buckingham et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 27 (2006) 381 389 6=The type of face viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated more attractive than the type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase. 7=The type of face viewed in the adaptation phase (i.e., masculine or feminine) was rated much more attractive than the type of face that was not viewed in the adaptation phase. The mean strength of preference for the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase was calculated for each participant and calculated separately for the pre- and postadaptation tests. These values were then converted to a percentage of the maximum possible strength of preference for the type of face that was seen in the adaptation phase. Corresponding values were calculated for participants who judged the trustworthiness of faces in the pre- and postadaptation phases. 3. Results Responses were analyzed using a mixed-design ANOVA [dependent variable: strength of preference for the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase; within-subject factor: phase (preadaptation, postadaptation); between-subject factors: sex of the participant (male, female), judgement made by the participant (attractiveness, trustworthiness), type of face shown in adaptation phase (masculine, feminine)]. This analysis revealed a main effect of phase ( F =5.52, df =1,47, p =.023). Preferences for the type of face viewed in the adaptation phase were stronger in the postadaptation test phase (mean percentage of the maximum possible strength of preference for the type of face that was seen in the adaptation phase=52.9, S.E.=1.6) than in the preadaptation test phase (mean=50.5, S.E.=1.6). There were no significant effects (all F b2.515, all pn.12) other than an interaction between the type of judgement made by a participant and the type of face shown in the adaptation phase ( F =5.83, df=1,47, p=.020). Because this interaction did not qualify the main effect of phase we observed (i.e., was not at all related to the experimental manipulation) and was, therefore, most likely to reflect individual differences in attitudes to sexual dimorphism in male faces (see Fink and Penton-Voak, 2002 and Penton-Voak and Perrett, 2000 for a discussion of this), we did not analyze it further. 4. Discussion Exposure to masculine or feminine male faces increased both attraction to faces of the type seen in the adaptation phase and attributions of trustworthiness to these faces relative to preadaptation phase tests. It is unsurprising that the effects of visual adaptation to faces were relatively subtle because the duration of the adaptation period was short and the duration of adaptation is positively and logarithmically related to the magnitude of face aftereffects (Leopold et al., 2005). Thus, effects of adaptation on face perceptions that occur as a consequence of longer real-life social interactions are likely to be considerably more pronounced. The effect of adaptation to masculine and feminine faces on subsequent

G. Buckingham et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 27 (2006) 381 389 387 preferences for sexual dimorphism in male faces complements attractiveness aftereffects observed for adaptation to sexual dimorphism of 2D shape (Little et al., 2005), identity (Little et al., 2005), and expanded or compressed features (Rhodes et al., 2003) and is consistent with the proposal that face preferences are recalibrated according to recent visual experience with faces (Little et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2003). The effect of adaptation to masculine and feminine faces on subsequent attributions of trustworthiness, however, demonstrates that face aftereffects also influence attributions of trustworthiness and (potentially) attributions of other personality characteristics that are also important for social interactions and mate choice. Furthermore, that visual adaptation to faces elicits identical effects on attributions of trustworthiness and attraction to novel similar faces supports the proposal that visual exposure alone cannot explain the context specificity of attitudes to self-resembling faces (see DeBruine, 2005). While DeBruine (2005) demonstrated that self-resembling faces were perceived as more trustworthy, but less attractive, than other-resembling faces, we show, in this study, that adaptation to faces produces equivalent effects for attraction and trustworthiness judgments. This supports DeBruine s proposal that attitudes to self-resembling faces are not simply a by-product of visual adaptation (DeBruine, 2004). While previous studies of variations in attitudes to masculine faces have emphasized the importance of hormone-mediated attraction to masculinity in male faces (Jones, Little, Boothroyd, DeBruine, et al., 2005; Penton-Voak et al., 1999) and attributions of male-sex typical personality characteristics (Macrae, Alnwick, Milne, & Schloerscheidt, 2002), here we emphasize the role of recent visual experience with masculine and feminine faces in shaping these preferences and attitudes. It has previously been noted that adaptation effects are likely to play an important role in the development of face preferences within individuals (Little et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2003) and may be the proximate mechanism that underpins phenomena such as attraction to parental characteristics (e.g., imprinting-like effects; Little, Penton-Voak, Burt, & Perrett, 2003; Perrett et al., 2002) and conditiondependent face preferences (e.g., matching for attractiveness in face preference tests where there is no possibility of actual rejection; Jones, Little, Boothroyd, Feinberg, et al., 2005; Little et al., 2001). In other words, positive regard for parental characteristics may be a consequence of individual differences in the type of faces individuals are adapted to (e.g., individuals with older parents are likely to have more visual experience of older faces than individuals with relatively younger parents will, Perrett et al., 2002), and matching for attractiveness on face preference tests may be a consequence of attractive individuals being more likely to associate with other attractive individuals than are relatively unattractive individuals (see Feingold, 1988 for evidence of this). Furthermore, our findings indicate that adaptation to faces also influences the development of social stereotypes within individuals that are also important for partner and associate choices. For example, while Perrett et al. (1998) reported that masculine faces are generally perceived to be less trustworthy than feminine faces, the effect of adaptation to faces on perceptions of trustworthiness that was observed in the current study indicates that the extent to which masculine faces are perceived as untrustworthy is, to some extent, dependent on recent visual experience with masculine faces.

388 G. Buckingham et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 27 (2006) 381 389 Acknowledgments This research was carried out in the Face Research Laboratory at the University of Aberdeen and was supervised by BCJ and LMD. References DeBruine, L. M. (2004). Facial resemblance increases the attractiveness of same-sex faces more than other-sex faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 271, 2085 2090. DeBruine, L. M. (2005). Trustworthy but not lust-worthy: Context-specific effects of facial resemblance. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 272, 919 922. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L. G., Perrett, D. I., Penton-Voak, I. S., Cooper, P. A., Penke, L., Feinberg, D. R., & Tiddeman, B. P. (2006). Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner s masculinity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, [Epub ahead of press; http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2005.3445]. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Women s attractiveness judgments of self-resembling faces change across the menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 47, 379 383. Feingold, A. (1988). Matching for attractiveness in romantic partners and same-sex friends: A meta-analysis and theoretical critique. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 226 235. Fink, B., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2002). Evolutionary psychology of facial attractiveness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 154 158. Johnston, V. S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M., Fink, B., & Grammer, K. (2001). Male facial attractiveness: Evidence for a hormone mediated adaptive design. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 251 267. Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L. G., DeBruine, L. M., Feinberg, D. R., Law Smith, M. J., Cornwell, R. E., Moore, F. R., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high. Hormones and Behavior, 48 (3), 283 290. Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L. G., Feinberg, D. R., Cornwell, R. E., DeBruine, L. M., Roberts, S. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Law Smith, M. J., Moore, F. R., Davis, H. P., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Women s physical and psychological condition independently predict their preference for apparent health in faces. Evolution and Human, 26. Jones, B. C., Perrett, D. I., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L., Cornwell, R. E., Feinberg, D. R., Tiddeman, B. P., Whiten, S., Pitman, R. M., Hillier, S. G., Burt, D. M., Stirrat, M. R., Law Smith, M. J., & Moore, F. R. (2005). Menstrual cycle, pregnancy and oral contraceptive use alter attraction to apparent health in faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 272, 347 354. Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390 423. Leopold, D. A., O Toole, A. J., Vetter, T., & Blanz, V. (2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 89 94. Leopold, D. A., Rhodes, G., Müller, K. M., & Jeffery, L. (2005). The dynamics of visual adaptation to faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 272, 897 904. Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Self perceived attractiveness influences human preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 268, 39 44. Little, A. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2005). Sex-contingent face aftereffects suggest distinct neural populations code male and female faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 272, 1 5. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape. Proceedings of the Royal Society London, Series B, 269, 1095 1100.

G. Buckingham et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 27 (2006) 381 389 389 Little, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2003). Investigating an imprinting-like phenomenon in humans: Partners and opposite-sex parents have similar hair and eye colour. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 24, 43 51. Loffler, G., Yourganov, G., Wilkinson, F., & Wilson, H. R. (2005). fmri evidence for the neural representation of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1386 1391. Macrae, C. N., Alnwick, K. A., Milne, A. B., & Schloerscheidt, A. M. (2002). Person perception across the menstrual cycle: Hormonal influences on social cognitive functioning. Psychological Science, 13, 532 536. Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I. (2000). Female preference for male faces changes cyclically Further evidence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 39 48. Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., Kobayashi, T., Burt, D. M., Murray, L. K., & Minamisawa, R. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature, 399, 741 742. Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I. S., Rowland, D. R., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., Henzi, S. P., Castles, D. L., & Akamatsu, S. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, 884 887. Perrett, D. I., Penton-Voak, I. S., Little, A. C., Tiddeman, B. P., Burt, D. M., Oxley, R., & Barrett, L. (2002). Facial attractiveness judgements reflect learning of parental age characteristics. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 269, 873 880. Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Watson, T. L., Clifford, C. W. G., & Nakayama, K. (2003). Fitting the mind to the world: Face adaptation and attractiveness aftereffects. Psychological Science, 14, 558 566. Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Watson, T. L., Jaquet, E., Winkler, C., & Clifford, C. W. G. (2004). Orientation-contingent face aftereffects and implications for face-coding mechanisms. Current Biology, 14, 2119 2123. Rowland, D. A., & Perrett, D. I. (1995). Manipulating facial appearance through shape and color. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, 15, 70 76. Tiddeman, B. P., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Prototyping and transforming facial textures for perception research. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, 21, 42 50. Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308, 1623 1626. Webster, M. A., Kaping, D., Mizokami, Y., & Duhamel, P. (2004). Adaptation to natural face categories. Nature, 428, 557 561. Winston, J. S., Henson, R. N. A., Fine-Goulden, M. R., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). fmri-adaptation reveals dissociable neural representations of identity and expression in face perception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 92, 1830 1839.