Protecting the prominent? A research journey with Paul Mullen

Similar documents
James, D. V., Mullen, P. E., Meloy, J. R., Pathe, M. T., Preston, L., Darnley, B., Scalora, M.

RESEARCH ARTICLE Persistence in stalking: a comparison of associations in general forensic and public figure samples

A) UNDERLYING PRINCIPLES

The Unreasonable, Querulant and Vexatious as Self Represented Litigants

Threat Assessment in Schools (2002). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Secret Service & U.S. Dept. of Education.

Psychiatric Criminals

I N T E R O F F I C E M E M O R A N D UM

Stalked by a patient. Cases That Test Your Skills

The Handbook for Campus Threat Assessment and Management Teams. By Gene Deisinger, Ph.D. Marisa Randazzo, Ph.D. Daniel O Neil Jenna Savage SUMMARY

Incorporating Behavioral Threat Assessment into Emergency Planning. Internal and External Threats to Safety

What does not guilty by reason of insanity mean?

British Psychological Society response to the Home Office. Introducing a stalking protection order

First published on: 12 February 2010

School of Law and Criminology

The Paranoid Patient: Perils and Pitfalls

Violence Risk Assessment

Many investigators. Documenting a Suspect s State of Mind By PARK DIETZ, M.D., M.P.H., Ph.D.

Death Threats and Violence

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY E.G., COMPETENCE TO STAND TRIAL CHILD CUSTODY AND VISITATION WORKPLACE DISCRIMINATION INSANITY IN CRIMINAL TRIALS

Threat Assessment: Behavioral Indicators for Risk of Future Violence

Cognitive Behavioral Treatment of Delusions and Paranoia. Dennis Combs, Ph.D. University of Tulsa

Judgements Made by Judge George Moorhouse Domestic Violence. Ryan Phillips and Ms Samijo Glenton attended a party separately in October 2013.

TRENDS IN LEGAL ADVOCACY: INTERVIEWS WITH LEADING PROSECUTORS AND DEFENCE LAWYERS ACROSS THE GLOBE

Lessard v. Schmidt at 40 Years: Ruling Gone Awry or Sound Public Policy?

Five Mistakes and Omissions That Increase Your Risk of Workplace Violence

Suicide/Homicide Precautions OFFICE OF BEHAVIORAL HEALTH

8/4/2016. Attacks are not impulsive acts Action is Opportunistic Acts are part of a campaign waged in asymmetrical conflict

Does the Metropolitan Police Service, and/or any other security service, have the legal right to conduct themselves in a prejudicial manner?

BOOK YOUR PLACE NOW. DASH Two day training. Domestic Abuse Awareness One day training. Training is free of charge.

The Insanity Defense Not a Solid Strategy. jail card. However, this argument is questionable itself. It is often ignored that in order to apply

Review of Factors and Considerations for Decision-Making Full Disclosure of Excerpts from

Please take time to read this document carefully. It forms part of the agreement between you and your counsellor and Insight Counselling.

Men and Sexual Assault

Fitness to Stand Trial

Contents. Chapter. Coping with Crisis. Section 16.1 Understand Crisis Section 16.2 The Crises People Face. Chapter 16 Coping with Crisis

Safer East Sussex Team Newsletter

Assessing the Risk: Protecting the Child

Targeted Violence: Pre-Attack Behaviors and Indicators. Community College Risk Management Consortium David L. Young

PATIENTS' RIGHTS VS. PUBLIC SAFETY

OREGON STATE POLICE CAPITOL MALL AREA COMMAND

DAPHNE III PROGRAMME

THE HONORABLE SOCIETY OF KINGS INNS

Intersections of Domestic Violence and Sexual Assault ext ext. 17

How to stop Someone who is ADDICTED ENABLING

Equality and Human Rights Commission. Sexual harassment and the law: Guidance for employers

Crisis Intervention Team CIT BY: CORY ELAM

Student Drug Policy Approved GE March 2011 Updated May 2012 and approved GE November 2012 Next Review November 2018 Page 1

Of Mice and Manslaughter: California Manslaughter Law in the Context of John Steinbeck s Of Mice and Men

Assessment and management of risk to others

Preventing Police Suicide

LAW03: Criminal Law (Offences against the Person) Defences: Insanity

Appendix: Brief for the American Psychiatric Association as Amicus Curiae Supporting Petitioner, Barefoot v. Estelle

Herd Behavior By CommonLit Staff 2014

Annual Report 2014/15

SOCIAL ATTITUDES TO HOMELESSNESS. A Student Survey of Cambridge Residents

Querulous Complainants. Dr Gordon Skilling December 2017

Does executive coaching work? The questions every coach needs to ask and (at least try to) answer. Rob B Briner

Schizophrenia. This factsheet provides a basic description of schizophrenia, its symptoms and the treatments and support options available.

TARGETED VIOLENCE, THREAT ASSESSMENTS & THREAT MANAGEMENT

My name is Todd Elliott and I graduated from the University of Toronto, Factor- Inwentash Faculty of Social Work, in 1999.

WORKPLACE AND ON CAMPUS VIOLENCE GUIDE

issue. Some Americans and criminal justice officials want to protect inmates access to

1. I am a citizen of the United States and a resident of the State of Alaska. I am

Scottish Parliament Region: North East Scotland. Case : Tayside NHS Board. Summary of Investigation

Preventive detention as a measure to keep sentences short. Randi Rosenqvist Oslo University hospital and Ila prison

Violence Risk and Threat Assessment: What Clinicians Need to Know

Connected Communities

London Division 22nd November 2016

Dusty L Humes, Ph.D., Clinical & Forensic Services 2201 San Pedro NE, Building Albuquerque, NM

OREGON STATE POLICE CAPITOL MALL AREA COMMAND. Oregon State Police 900 Court St Rm 60C Salem, Or (503) Ver

What are the connections drawn between mental illness and dangerousness? certain criteria are satisfied. Note consent is not required.

PLEASE COMPLETE THIS PAGE FOR ALL STUDENTS. Examiner 1 Examiner 2 Hospital, Date: Time: Clinical Interview F P- P P+ F P- P P+ F P- P P+ F P- P P+

What is Social Cognition?

Basic Risk Assessment. Kemshall, H., Mackenzie, G., Wilkinson, B., (2011) Risk of Harm Guidance and Training Resource CD Rom, De Montfort University

Legal 2000 and the Mental Health Crisis in Clark County. Lesley R. Dickson, M.D. Executive Director, Nevada Psychiatric Association

How can you prevent workplace violence? March 21, 2017

Substance Misuse and Domestic Abuse: Dual Issues, an Integrated Response A study in County Durham. Frazer Hill Gail Murphy

Name: Date: /100 % DIRECTIONS: ACT I Scene i 1. Where does the play open? 2. Describe the Duke s feelings about love.

ENTITLEMENT ELIGIBILITY GUIDELINE SCHIZOPHRENIA

Who we are and what we do?

Officer-Involved Shooting Investigation. Things to think about ahead of time

Herd Behavior By CommonLit Staff 2014

RAPE AND ASSAULT BY PENETRATION: INFORMATION FOR SURVIVORS OF SEXUAL VIOLENCE

Violence Risk and Threat Assessment: What Clinicians Need to Know. J. Reid Meloy, Ph.D., ABPP Dept. of Psychiatry Yale University March 17, 2015

Mental Health Strategy. Easy Read

PROFESSIONALISM THE ABC FOR SUCCESS

Women and Their Children Living with Domestic and Family Violence: Principles; Problems and Possibilities

Forensic Mental Health and Prison Risk Management

Victim, Survivor, or Accuser? SAR Language Policy Offers Guidance

Dr. J. Doe, Ltd. 1. Consulting and Clinical Forensic Psychologist. Psychological Assessment Report on Mr. Timothy Smith (DOB: April 1st, 1970)

THE SEXUAL OFFENCES (SCOTLAND) ACT 2009

How to have a restorative conversation with victims and people working with victims

PREVENTING WORKPLACE VIOLENCE PRESENTED BY THE SOLUTIONS GROUP

Self-Defense and Female-Perpetrated Violence. Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford

Do Violent Offenders With Schizophrenia Who Attack Family Members Differ From Those With Other Victims?

Nature and antecedents of psychotic patients crimes

Other significant mental health complaints

SEPTEMBER 2010 Recovery discussion Topic Boundaries

Suggested Questions and Recommendations for Universal Periodic Review of the United States

Blue Ridge Legal Services

Transcription:

Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health 20: 242 250 (2010) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).769 Protecting the prominent? A research journey with Paul Mullen DAVID V. JAMES, MA, FRCPsych, Consultant Forensic Psychiatrist, Fixated Threat Assessment Centre, London SW1E 6JP, UK ABSTRACT Recent research from the United Kingdom has called into question entrenched assumptions from the USA about public figure threat assessment, in particular the low importance given to mental illness and to the making of threats. This article reviews some of the evidence, concluding that the high prevalence of psychotic illness in threateners and harassers of public figures and the frequency of warning behaviours determines that these factors should be accorded a central role in the assessment and management of such cases. It is noted that data in US studies do not differ greatly from those in UK studies, but rather their interpretation. The practical benefits of attending to the factors in question are illustrated by reference to the activities of the United Kingdom s Fixated Threat Assessment Centre. The overlap is noted between the fi elds of public figure threat assessment and stalking, which have hitherto developed separately. The issue of public figure threat assessment is used to illustrate some of the attitudes and personal qualities which have typifi ed the career and achievements of Paul Mullen in forensic psychiatry research. Copyright 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Paul Mullen, questioned as to which part of his work had given him most satisfaction, replied: I have enjoyed research that aims to derail the juggernauts of official wisdom and entrenched assumption, though he feared that his time in psychiatry had dulled the outraged iconoclasm which was my most honest attribute (Mullen, 2003). The official wisdom and entrenched assumptions in the area of public figure protection can be summed up as follows: mental illness is of little importance in attacks on public figures; threats do not matter in fact, they are protective ; and the field of protecting the prominent is separate from that of stalking its main focus is on the policing of a behavioural pathway to violence.

Protecting the prominent? A research journey with Paul Mullen 243 There are two highly influential studies in this field. The fi rst, by Dietz and colleagues (1991), examined writers of letters to members of the US Congress. It compared 43 writers who also physically approached members with 43 who did not. The conclusion that was widely drawn from the results was that those who threaten do not go on to approach. No comment was made in the paper on the issue of mental illness. The second study was by Fein and Vossekuil (1998, 1999), known as the Exceptional Case Study. This set out to examine the characteristics of those who attacked or assassinated public figures. To obtain a sufficiently large sample, the authors took a 50-year period. They included businessmen and film stars, as well as politicians, and expanded the concept of attack to include near lethal approaches, by which was meant someone found in the vicinity of the prominent person with a weapon. There were 34 cases of attack or assassination. Their two central conclusions were that mental illness only rarely plays a part in assassination behaviours (Fein and Fossekuil, 1998: p. 182) and that threats are not of central importance: persons who pose threats most often do not make threats (Fein and Fossekuil, 1998: p. 183). If this is the entrenched wisdom, are there any reasons to be suspicious of it? Taking the Dietz paper fi rst, two things stand out. First, the authors fail to mention that 72% of people in their sample had a psychotic illness. This is discoverable from their lengthy unpublished report to the National Institute of Justice (Dietz and Martell, 1989), from which the data in their paper were extracted. It is difficult to understand why such a central item of information should have been omitted from their paper. Second, the conclusion that threats are of little importance has been over-interpreted. In fact, their paper states: Subjects who sent threats to a member of Congress were significantly less likely to pursue a face-to-face encounter with him or her (p. 1466). This may be statistically accurate, but disguises the fact that 33% of cases who made a direct threat subsequently approached. The widespread interpretation that threats are somehow protective, which the authors do not appear to have discouraged, is quite erroneous. There is also reason to be suspicious of the entrenched wisdom represented by the Exceptional Case Study (Fein and Vossekuil, 1998, 1999). Let us examine the two central conclusions quoted above. First, that mental illness is of little importance in attacks on public figures. Examining the detail of their study, 61% had a psychiatric history, 43% had a history of delusional ideas and 10% of violent command hallucinations. This is so different from prevalence rates in the general population that it seems impossible to square with their conclusion. It also is difficult to square with earlier observations that approximately 90 per cent of all persons the Secret Service presently consider dangerous gave some indication of mental disorder (Takeuchi et al., 1981: p. 28). Then, there is the assertion that threats are not of major importance. This sits uncomfortably with Fein and Vossekuil s reports that 77% of their cases had a history of verbal or written communication about the target and that 63% had a history of indirect or conditional

244 James threats about or to the target. It seems likely that the message that others have drawn from their data is not one that involved a detailed reading of their papers. In 2003, Paul and I were commissioned to research risks to public fi gures from lone individuals, and we formed the Fixated Research Group. The research was in several parts. First, there were visits to security agencies in other countries. The second part of the project comprised an exhaustive, 3-year evaluation of 5000 files from the archives of the Royalty Protection Division of the Metropolitan Police Service. Finally, we undertook a detailed examination of attacks on Western European politicians and an historical review of attacks on the British royal family. How then did Paul s efforts with our group fare in terms of research that aims to derail the juggernauts of official wisdom and entrenched assumption? Various published studies answer this. The fi rst, published in Psychological Medicine, was based on examination of 5000 files held by Royalty Protection Police on people who had made threatening, harassing or inappropriate communications to members of the royal family, their residences or public appearances (James et al., 2009). Initially, the files were examined for evidence of serious mental illness, motivation and type of behaviour. Eighty-three per cent of cases showed clear evidence of serious mental illness, according to a strict operational defi nition. This placed mental illness at the heart of the matter. It also pointed to a direct overlap between the issue of protecting public figures and the interests of public health and the health of the individual. Dealing with inappropriate attentions involves obtaining treatment for seriously ill people who have fallen through the care net. Scattered amongst people with florid psychosis were petitions from persistent complainants and querulents, whose initial local grievances had expanded to become pressing matters of national import and whose strange personality traits had fi nally crossed the border into delusional disorder. This was a group that Paul had previously studied (Mullen and Lester, 2006). Our later study, just described (James et al., 2009) looked at associations between motivation, mental state and behaviour. There were no episodes of violence to a royal family member in the case series, although there had been to a number against protection staff, so proxy markers for assault were studied instead (James et al., in press a). It transpired that it was the group of aggrieved individuals on highly personal quests for justice, which was highly significantly associated with the three main proxies studied breaching security barriers, achieving close proximity to a royal family member, and carrying a weapon. So much for proxies, but were there any actual attacks? Were these associated with mental disorder? We undertook our own study of attacks on politicians, specifically in Western Europe, between the years 1990 and 2004 (James et al., 2007). We excluded terrorist attacks, which meant excluding Spain and the UK assassination of Ian Gow in 1990 by the Provisional Irish Republican Army. There were 24 attacks in all. Two were mass killings involving local assemblies in which 22 people were killed and 37 injured. The remaining cases involved

Protecting the prominent? A research journey with Paul Mullen 245 presidents, prime ministers, ministers, heads of parties and the mayors of Vienna and Paris. There were 3 deaths, 8 serious injuries and 11 cases where no injury was sustained. Death and serious injury were significantly associated with the presence of mental disorder (p = 0.012, φ = 0.57) and of psychosis (p = 0.036, φ = 0.49), and those responsible were pursuing particular personal quests or grievances. Twelve of the 24 cases in the sample had engaged in warning behaviours and these had the same associations as death and serious injury, in other words the presence of mental disorder (p = 0.000, φ = 0.77) and of psychosis (p = 0.003, φ = 0.65). The warning behaviours were not subtle. They comprised chaotic deluded letters to politicians and police, threatening letters to politicians, paranoid contacts with MPs, lawsuits against the government, attempted selfimmolation, newspaper advertisement, posters, leafleting and telling friends. There are many more illustrative cases than can be considered in the space available. Three will suffice. In April 1990, Oskar Lafontaine, a leading politician in Germany s Social Democratic Party, was stabbed in the neck at a political rally in front of his protection officers. The assailant, a middle-aged woman, attacked him with a knife hidden in a bunch of flowers, as he signed his autograph for her. For years, she had made repeated representations to the authorities about underground killing factories. She believed that citizens were being abducted from their beds at night, and chopped up to make robots: anything left over was being turned into sausages and sold off in butchers shops. Desperate to stop this and with her pleas unheard, she decided that she had to attack the fi rst politician who came to town. This happened to be Mr Lafontaine. In the same year, the German interior minister, Wolfgang Schäubler, was shot as he left a political rally in the company of his bodyguards. He was paralysed from the waist down. The attacker believed that the German government was using transmitters to beam intense pain and feelings of lust into his body. For 7 years, he made numerous complaints to official bodies and to his member of parliament, and twice tried to take the government to court, but all to no avail. He eventually decided that his only way out was to take matters into his own hands. A similar case occurred in the United Kingdom in 2001. The then Liberal Democrat MP, Nigel Jones, was attacked by a constituent with a samurai sword. His friend and aide, Andrew Pennington, who intervened to protect him, was run through and killed. The perpetrator was a man who had developed complex persecutory delusions after losing his job. These involved conspiracies by freemasons and others to ruin him. He wrote numerous letters to the authorities about the plot over a 2-year period and initiated a string of legal actions. He became increasingly angry about the lack of redress. He visited the MP s surgery, so it came out at his trial, between 50 and 100 times, with the same rambling paranoid grievances. None of these cases was predictable, but all were potentially preventable, had a system been in place to evaluate bizarre and hostile complaints. The Fixated Research Group also studied historical attacks on the British royal family over the period 1778 to 2003 (James et al., 2008). The task was complicated

246 James by the paucity of historical records on some cases. It was also necessary to set aside a prevailing belief in Victorian times that attacking the royal family was in itself proof positive of mental illness. For instance, the judge sentencing Pate after his attack on Queen Victoria in 1850 stated: I would fain believe it is the privilege and boast of the country that no man but a madman would attack the most gracious Sovereign of this country. I believe it is as great a proof of insanity as it is possible for a person to give (R. v. Pate, 8 St. Tr. N.S. 2). The popular versifier, McGonagall, expressed similar sentiments in rhyme after a further attack on the Queen in 1882: Maclean must be a madman, Which is obvious to be seen, Or else he wouldn t have tried to shoot, Our most beloved Queen (McGonagall, 1890). Despite these methodological complications, sufficient evidence was found to conclude that, out of 23 attackers, 11 (48%) clearly had psychosis and one possibly so, two had depressive illnesses and one exhibited features of anti-social personality disorder. Ten attackers (44%) were known to have engaged in forms of warning behaviour. The common themes in these studies, then, were the importance of mental illness and the significance of warning behaviours. To students of British forensic psychiatry, these cases may have a familiar ring. Delusionally driven individuals, pursuing idiosyncratic quests for justice, were responsible for the killings of the Prime Minister Spencer Perceval in 1812 (Wilson, 1812; Hanrahan, 2008) and Edward Drummond, the private secretary to the Prime Minister, in 1843 the Daniel McNaughton case, which became a legal landmark (West and Walk, 1977). In both cases, what we now understand as clear warning behaviours had been apparent for several years. It is worth noting that later research has established the particular importance of delusional drive with respect to more rather than less serious violence (Taylor, 1985; Taylor et al., 1998). The importance of motivation in assessing risk was also a consistent theme of our studies (Mullen et al., 2009b). In short, the risk of violence was significantly higher in the resentful (i.e. querulants and those with pathological quests for justice or high-risk delusions). The risk was low in those seeking intimacy or personal relationships for whatever reason. This contrasts with the entrenched assumptions in the risk assessment literature, which is typified by Calhoun and Weston (2009: p. 21), in a book otherwise full of practical experience, where they stress the importance in threat assessment and management of concentrating on behaviour and the need to avoid trying to plumb the minds and motives of cases, preferring to leave that chore to forensic psychologists and psychiatrists. The third official wisdom or entrenched assumption, which links to this, is that the field of public figure protection is separate from that of stalking and harassment of the general population, its main focus being on the policing of a behavioural pathway to violence. The latter, it is contended, involves a logical sequence from conception, through planning, practical preparations, and then dry runs, followed by action. Paul s group looked for evidence for such a pathway

Protecting the prominent? A research journey with Paul Mullen 247 in the cohort of Royalty Protection Police cases. We compared the characteristics of those that had engaged in certain behaviours: unsuccessful breaching of security perimeters; successful breaching; achieving close proximity to a family member; carrying a weapon and offering evidence of homicidal ideation (James et al., in press-a). In terms of a pathway, one might surmise that these groups would form a chain of subsets of each other, each being successively smaller. However, although there were associations between each form of behaviour and specific patterns of motivation and mental state, no evidence of a pathway phenomenon could be elucidated in this sample. Next, a direct comparison was made between the Royalty Protection sample and the stalker sample collected by Paul s team in Melbourne, which has formed the basis of many of his studies in this area. Re-framing the defi nition of stalking for the public figure arena (Mullen et al., 2009a: p. 197) and excluding cases in which the victim had slept with the perpetrator, it became evident that risk factors for public figure and general population victims were similar in the areas studied (James et al., 2010b). The conclusion to be drawn is an important one. Whereas public figure threat assessment and stalking have developed as entirely separate fields of research, they are essentially dealing with the same phenomenon. This enables the adoption into the public figure arena of the various insights into risk from the general stalking literature, and, potentially, vice versa. In particular, it enabled the adoption by Fixed Threat Assessment Centre (James et al., 2010a) of the Stalking Risk Profile (MacKenzie et al., 2009), which is a manualised assessment of different domains of risk, following a structured professional judgement model, which points to treatment options in individual cases. This approach separates risk into different domains, for which risk factors differ (persistence, escalation, recurrence, psychosocial damage to stalker, disruption and violence) and recognises that risks in each domain vary according to motivational group. This broadens the arena of public figure threat to include other more common types of risk than that of violence, which has dominated the risk assessment literature hitherto. There are other papers produced in this research effort which there is no space to do more than mention here (James et al., in press-b; Meloy et al., 2004; Meloy et al., in press). All were prepared with another of Paul s maxims in mind, which is that there is little point in undertaking research which cannot be translated into some advance in practice. The main practical outcome of all this work was the establishment of the Fixated Threat Assessment Centre (FTAC), the fi rst joint police/nhs unit in the United Kingdom, to assess and manage risks to public figures. FTAC proved able to catalyse suitable health outcomes for its cases. This both reduced risk to public figures and benefited patients (James et al., 2010a). Of the fi rst 100 people assessed by unit staff, 86% suffered from psychotic illness. Following FTAC intervention, 57% were admitted to hospital by local psychiatric services, and 26% taken on by community psychiatric teams. Assessed threat levels were reduced to low through FTAC interventions in 80% of cases.

248 James Objections have been raised in some quarters that FTAC is providing a service only to the privileged, rather than to the general public. This is to misunderstand the effects of FTAC s interventions. Those most directly exposed to the attentions of people who harass or threaten public figures are not the public figures themselves, but rather members of the families of those individuals and the general public. Furthermore, much of the threat engendered by these people arises in consequence of the misery or terror caused by their symptoms. They are seeking relief, albeit in misguided and dangerous ways: FTAC provides this for them where other services have not done so. It is the suffering individuals and their families who almost certainly benefit most from FTAC s work. Here, we come to the crux of the matter in terms of Paul s stamp on proceedings. In effect, the aims set by the funders, ostensibly designed to protect the privileged, had been redefi ned by Paul so as to benefit the primary sufferers and the general populace as well. Considering the initial tasks set, the aim research the risks to prominent public figures from lone individuals had been redefi ned as use inappropriate attentions to the prominent as a new tool to identify untreated serious mental illness in the community. Devise and effect a mechanism by which such risks could be reduced had become establish a new unit which can catalyse the treatment of those suffering from serious mental illness who have fallen through the care net. Finally, thereby help protect a range of prominent people (which one might term the establishment ) had been turned into thereby protect those sick people and the public, as well as the prominent. In this particular research journey, Paul had succeeded in derailing the juggernauts of official wisdom and entrenched assumption about public figure protection. He had done so in a way which helped reshape the field and which immediately gave rise to practical change. One can only applaud Paul s enraged iconoclasm and the part that it has played, not only here, but throughout his career, in improving the lot of the mentally ill and in educating those that have been privileged enough to journey with him. References Calhoun FS, Weston SW (2009) Threat Assessment and Management Strategies. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Dietz P, Matthews D, Martell D, Stewart T, Hrouda D, Warren J (1991). Threatening and otherwise inappropriate letters to members of the United States Congress. Journal of Forensic Sciences 36: 1445 1468. Dietz PE, Martell DA (1989) Mentally Disordered Offenders in Pursuit of Celebrities and Politicians. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Fein RA, Vossekuil B (1998) Preventing attacks on public officials and public figures: A Secret Service perspective. In Reid Meloy J (ed.) The Psychology of Stalking. San Diego, CA: Academic Press pp. 175 191. Fein RA, Vossekuil B (1999) Assassination in the United States: An operational study of recent assassins, attackers and near-lethal approachers. Journal of Forensic Science 44(2): 321 333.

Protecting the prominent? A research journey with Paul Mullen 249 Hanrahan DC (2008) The Assassination of the Prime Minister: John Bellingham and the Murder of Spencer Percival. Stroud, UK: Stratton. James DV, Mullen PE, Meloy JR, Pathé MT, Farnham FR, Preston L, Darnley B (2007) The role of mental disorder in attacks on European politicians 1990 2004. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 116: 334 344. James DV, Mullen PE, Pathé MT, Meloy JR, Farnham FR, Preston L, Darnley B (2008) Attacks on the British royal family: The role of psychotic illness. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 36: 59 67. James DV, Mullen PE, Pathé MT, Meloy JR, Preston LF, Darnley B, Farnham F (2009) Stalkers and harassers of royalty: The role of mental illness and motivation. Psychological Medicine 39(9): 1479 1490. James DV, Kerrigan T, Forfar R, Farnham F, Preston L (2010a) The Fixated Threat Assessment Centre: Preventing harm and facilitating care. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology DOI: 10.1080/14789941003596981. James DV, Mullen PE, Pathé MT, Meloy JR, Farnham FR, Preston L, Darnley B (in press a) Stalkers and harassers of royalty: An exploration of proxy behaviours for violence. Behavioural Sciences and the Law. James DV, McEwan TE, MacKenzie RD, Meloy JR, Mullen PE, Pathé MT, Farnham F, Preston LF, Darnley B (2010b) Persistence in stalking: A comparison of associations in general forensic and public figure samples. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology. DOI: 10.1080/14789940903388994. James DV, Meloy JR, Mullen PE, Pathé MT, Preston LF, Darnley B, Farnham F (in press-b) Abnormal attentions towards the British Royal Family: Factors associated with approach and escalation. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law. MacKenzie RD, McEwan TE, Pathé MT, James DV, Ogloff JRP, Mullen PE (2009) The Stalking Risk Profi le: Guidelines for the Assessment and Management of Stalkers. Melbourne, Victoria: Stalkinc. & Centre for Forensic Behavioural Science. McGonagall W (1890) Poetic Gems. Trowbridge and Esher. (Republished 1964. London & Dundee: Duckworth). Meloy JR, James DV, Farnham FR, Mullen PE, Pathé M, Darnley B, Preston L (2004) A research review of public figure threats, approaches, attacks, and assassinations in the United States. Journal of Forensic Sciences 49(5): 1086 1093. Meloy JR, James DV, Mullen PE, Pathé MT, Farnham FR, Darnley B, Preston L (in press) Factors associated with escalation and problematic approaches toward public figures. Journal of Forensic Sciences. Mullen PE (2003) E-interview. Psychiatric Bulletin 27: 320. Mullen PE, Lester G (2006) Vexatious litigants and unusually persistent complainants and petitioners: From querulous paranoia to querulous behaviour. Behavioural Sciences and the Law 24: 333 349. Mullen PE, Pathé M, Purcell R (2009a) Stalkers and Their Victims (2nd edn). New York: Cambridge University Press. Mullen PE, James DV, Meloy JR, Pathé MT, Farnham FR, Preston L, Darnley B, Berman J (2009b) The fi xated and the pursuit of public figures. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology 20: 33 47. Takeuchi J, Solomon F, Menninger WW (eds) (1981) Behavioral Science and the Secret Service: Towards the Prevention of Assassination. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press. Taylor PJ (1985) Motives for offending amongst violent and psychotic men. British Journal of Psychiatry 147: 491 498. Taylor PJ, Leese M, Williams D, Butwell M, Daly R, Larkin E (1998) Mental disorder and violence: A special hospital study. British Journal of Psychiatry 172: 218 226. West DJ, Walk A (1977) Daniel McNaughton: His Trial and the Aftermath. London: Gaskell.

250 James Wilson D (1812) The substance of a conversation with John Bellingham, the assassin of the late Right Hon. Spencer Perceval, on Sunday May 17, 1812, the day previous to his execution, together with some general remarks. London: Hatchard. Address correspondence to: David V. James, MA, FRCPsych, Consultant Forensic Psychiatrist, Fixated Threat Assessment Centre, 4 5 Buckingham Gate, London SW1E 6JP, UK. Email: david.james@behmht.nhs.uk