Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms"

Transcription

1 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms Daryl Fougnie and René Marois Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Center for Integrative and Cognitive Neuroscience, Vanderbilt University ABSTRACT A hallmark of both visual attention and working memory is their severe capacity limit: People can attentively track only about four objects in a multiple object tracking (MOT) task and can hold only up to four objects in visual working memory (VWM). It has been proposed that attention underlies the capacity limit of VWM. We tested this hypothesis by determining the effect of varying the load of a MOT task performed during the retention interval of a VWM task and comparing the resulting dual-task costs with those observed when a VWM task was performed concurrently with another VWM task or with a verbal working memory task. Instead of supporting the view that the capacity limit of VWM is solely attention based, the results indicate that VWM capacity is set by the interaction of visuospatial attentional, central amodal, and local task-specific sources of processing. A key characteristic shared by both visual attention and working memory is their severe capacity limit. The amount of visual information that can be actively held in visual working memory (VWM) is limited to about four objects (Irwin & Gordon, 1998; Pashler, 1988; Sperling, 1960; Vogel, Woodman, & Luck, 2001). Attention is similarly constrained: People s ability to attentively track a number of randomly moving objects among like distractors is limited to four or five items (Cavanagh & Alvarez, 2005; Culham, Cavanagh, & Kanwisher, 2001; Oksama & Hyönä, 2004; Pylyshyn & Storm, 1988; Scholl, 2001; Sears & Pylyshyn, 2000). The similarity in the number of objects that can be held in mind and attentively tracked has led researchers to suggest that these two abilities rely on a common capacity-limited process Address correspondence to René Marois, Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, 530 Wilson Hall, st Ave S., Nashville, TN 37203, rene.marois@vanderbilt.edu. (e.g., Cowan, 1998, 2001). Studies consistent with this proposal have demonstrated that visuospatial attention and VWM can interfere with each other (Awh, Jonides, & Reuter-Lorenz, 1998; Oh & Kim, 2004; Woodman & Luck, 2004). Because visuospatial working memory depends on an attention-based rehearsal mechanism (Awh et al., 1998), the shared process may be none other than attention. Indeed, it has been proposed that attention is the capacity-limited process that constrains VWM capacity (Cowan, 2001; Rensink, 2000a, 2000b, 2002). However, the fact that attention and working memory interact need not imply that the capacity limit of VWM is reducible to that of attention. The capacity limit of VWM may result from contentspecific stages of information processing rather than from a monolithic attentional capacity (Luck & Vecera, 2002), or it may be a product of the interaction between capacity-limited attentional processes and the independent capacity of distinct feature stores (Delvenne & Bruyer, 2004; Wheeler & Treisman, 2002). In the present dual-task study, we tested the hypothesis that attention underlies the capacity limit of VWM. The basic experimental design consisted of inserting an attention-demanding multiple object tracking (MOT) task during the maintenance period of a VWM task (Fig. 1) and assessing the effect of varying the load of the attention task. If VWM capacity is constrained solely by attention, then not only should increasing attentional load decrease VWM performance, but it should do so to the same extent as a second concurrently executed working memory task would. EXPERIMENT 1 The first experiment assessed whether a VWM change-detection task (Todd & Marois, 2004; Vogel et al., 2001) and a MOT task (Pylyshyn & Storm, 1988) interfere with each other in a loaddependent manner when they are executed concurrently. 526 Copyright r 2006 Association for Psychological Science Volume 17 Number 6

2 Daryl Fougnie and René Marois Fig. 1. Trial design for the dual-task condition of Experiment 1, which involved a visual working memory (VWM) task and a multiple object tracking (MOT) task. In the VWM task, participants memorized the colors and locations of three briefly presented circles. During the MOT task, participants tracked a subset of randomly moving discs (either one or three). Participants then responded to the VWM task (indicating whether a VWM probe item matched the color and location of a circle in the original VWM display) and the MOT task (indicating whether the item selected during the probe phase of the tracking task was a MOT target). See the text for details. Method Twenty-three undergraduate students at Vanderbilt University participated for course credit or monetary reward. VWM Task On each trial of the VWM task, three colored circles (each 0.781) were presented for 400 ms, with each circle located in one of six possible locations along an imaginary ring (3.31 radius from fixation). The colors of the circles were selected from a pool of 11 colors (light blue, dark green, red, black, white, light green, brown, pink, blue, yellow, and purple). Participants memorized both the color and the location of each circle. After a retention period of 10.2 s, a probe display containing one colored circle from the sample display was shown. On half of the trials, this circle was at the same location as in the sample display, and on the other half of the trials, it was at the location previously occupied by one of the other two circles. Participants indicated by button press whether the colored circle was in the correct location. The probe display remained present until response collection. MOT Task A MOT movie selected out of a pool of 40 was shown during the retention interval of each VWM trial, beginning 1.2 s after the offset of the VWM sample display. MOT stimuli were hollow white discs (0.501) that moved around within an area centered at fixation. The speed of each disc varied from /ms to /ms, and the disc s direction of movement had a 10% chance of changing at every refresh (13.3 ms). The discs bounced off the boundaries of the MOT display, other MOT discs, and the six possible locations of the VWM stimuli. Each 9-s MOT movie had three phases: marker, tracking, and probe phases, lasting 3, 4.5, and 1.5 s, respectively. During the marker phase, target discs were surrounded by hollow black squares (0.501). Half of the trials contained one target (low load), and the other half contained three targets (high load). During the tracking phase, the markers disappeared, rendering the targets indistinguishable from other discs. During the probe phase, a single disc was marked, with 50% probability that this disc corresponded to a target. Following the VWM response, a question mark appeared above fixation, notifying participants to indicate by button press whether the MOT probe item corresponded to a target. Articulatory Suppression Task To minimize contamination with verbal working memory (verbal WM), we instructed participants to perform an articulatory suppression task by repeating the word the at a 2-Hz rate, starting 1 s prior to the VWM stimulus display and ending after Volume 17 Number 6 527

3 Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Visual Working Memory Fig. 2. Accuracy data for Task 1 (top panels) and Task 2 (bottom panels) of Experiments 1, 2, and 3. In Experiment 1 (a), Task 1 was a visual working memory (VWM) task, and Task 2 was a multiple object tracking (MOT) task. In Experiment 2 (b), both tasks were VWM tasks (VWM1 and VWM2). In Experiment 3 (c), Task 1 was a VWM task, and Task 2 was a verbal working memory (verbal WM) task. Error bars represent within-subjects errors of the mean. responses for both tasks were collected. Performance of this task was monitored by the experimenter via speakers. Design and Procedure Forty-two percent of all trials contained both the VWM and MOT tasks. In the remaining trials, subjects performed either the VWM or the MOT task; this allowed us to assess performance in each task independently. To control for sensory and motor demands across all trial types, we designed each single-task condition to involve visual presentation and a motor response for the other task. Thus, for the VWM-only trials (25% of trials), a MOT movie with no targets replaced the normal MOT task. For the MOT-only trials (33% of trials: 16.5% for each MOT load), the VWM display was replaced by a screen with six black triangles (0.781 each), one at each possible VWM position. For both these kinds of trials, the probe display contained an ampersand above fixation, indicating that the participants should press the space bar. All trial types were randomly intermixed within two blocks of 48 trials. Participants were instructed to maintain fixation throughout each trial, and to emphasize the two tasks equally. Results and Discussion A one-way within-subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) across MOT conditions (no, low, or high load) revealed that VWM performance was impaired by the MOT task, F(2, 21) , p <.0001, 1 o (Fig. 2a, top panel). Planned comparisons using paired t tests showed that accuracy was different across the three load conditions (ps <.05). In addition, a 2 (MOT load: low or high) 2 (VWM condition: task or no task) within-subjects ANOVA revealed that MOT accuracy was reduced with high MOT load, F(1, 22) , p <.001, o More important, MOT accuracy was lower when performed with than without the VWM task, F(1, 22) , p <.005, o (Fig. 2a, bottom panel). Thus, the MOT and VWM tasks interfered with each other: Performing a MOT task reduced VWM accuracy, and vice versa. Furthermore, this interference was load-dependent, as VWM performance decreased as the number of MOT targets increased. To test whether these drops in accuracy in dual-task conditions translate into fewer objects stored or tracked, we used Cowan s (2001) K formula: K 5 [hit rate 1 correct rejection rate 1]N, where K is the estimated number of objects stored or tracked and N is the number of objects or targets presented. Specifically, we compared the observed dual-task capacity (measured by summing the K estimates for the two tasks in the dual-task condition) with the expected dual-task capacity assuming independent task capacities (measured by summing 1 A significant result (p <.05) corresponds to a probability of replication ( p rep ) of.917 or higher (Killeen, 2005). 528 Volume 17 Number 6

4 Daryl Fougnie and René Marois t(22) , p <.001, d (Fig. 3b), indicating that increasing MOT load from one to three objects led to a further decrease in the number of objects that could be stored or tracked under dual-task conditions relative to single-task conditions. Thus, the K and accuracy data both indicate that MOTand VWM interfere with each other in a load-dependent manner. EXPERIMENT 2 Experiment 2 assessed how the MOT-VWM dual-task costs compare with those obtained when the two tasks performed concurrently are both VWM tasks, the dual-task condition that should provide maximal VWM interference. If MOT taps into the same capacity-limited process as VWM, then a VWM-VWM dual task should yield interference costs comparable to those obtained in Experiment 1 at both low and high loads. Fig. 3. Observed versus expected dual-task performance in Experiments 1, 2, and 3. Observed dual-task K is the sum of the K estimates for the two tasks under dual-task conditions; expected K corresponds to the sum of the K estimates for the two tasks under single-task conditions. The load of the second task was either low (a) or high (b). Avisual working memory (VWM) task was paired with a multiple object tracking (MOT) task in Experiment 1, with a second VWM task in Experiment 2, and with a verbal working memory (verbal WM) task in Experiment 3. K estimates for the two tasks in the single-task condition). This analysis was performed separately for each MOT load. For the low MOT load, observed dual-task capacity was lower than the expected dual-task capacity, t(22) , p <.001, d (Fig. 3a), indicating that fewer objects were stored or tracked under dual-task conditions than under single-task conditions. Because similar results at high MOT load could simply be due to a carryover of the difference at low load, the latter interference was subtracted out in the analysis of the highload condition. Nevertheless, the observed dual-task capacity at high load was still lower than the expected dual-task capacity, Method This experiment, which involved 21 participants, was identical to Experiment 1 except that the MOT task was replaced by a second VWM task, so that there were two VWM tasks (VWM1 and VWM2). We used set sizes of 0, 1, and 3 for VWM2 because they yielded single-task performance similar to that for the MOT task. The shapes (circles or squares), colors, and locations of the stimuli were distinct for the two VWM tasks. For each trial, each of two color sets (light blue, dark green, red, white, and purple vs. light green, brown, pink, blue, and yellow) was randomly assigned to either VWM1 or VWM2. The three VWM1 circles could appear at any of the four locations 3.31 from fixation on the horizontal and vertical axes, whereas the VWM2 squares could appear at the corresponding locations of the two 451 diagonal axes. The VWM2 sample display appeared for 400 ms, starting 1.2 s after the offset of the VWM1 sample display. After a retention interval of 9 s, participants responded first to the VWM1 probe display and then to the VWM2 probe display, indicating in each case whether the single colored probe matched a sample item in both its color and its location. A match occurred in 50% of the trials. For the VWM2 task, the probe for nonmatch trials was a new color or a new location for set size 1, and either a new color or location or an incorrect pairing of a sample color and location for set size 3. For trials with no VWM1 (25%) or VWM2 (33%) task, four black triangles (0.781) replaced the color stimuli at the four locations. Results and Discussion VWM1 task performance was impaired by the VWM2 task, F(2, 19) , p <.0001, o (Fig. 2b, top panel). VWM1 accuracy dropped across the three VWM2 load conditions ( ps <.05). VWM2 accuracy was reduced by increasing VWM2 load, F(1, 20) , p <.0001, o , and VWM2 accuracy was lower in dual-task than in single-task trials, F(1, 20) 5 Volume 17 Number 6 529

5 Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Visual Working Memory 61.32, p <.0001, o (Fig. 2b, bottom panel). The interaction between VWM2 load and VWM1 condition, F(1, 20) , p <.001, indicates that VWM2 performance suffered mostly under high load and dual-task conditions. Finally, observed dual-task capacity was lower than expected dual-task capacity for both low load, t(20) , p <.05, d , and high load, t(20) , p <.001, d (Fig. 3). Thus, the accuracy and K results indicate that two concurrently executed VWM tasks interfere with each other in a load-dependent manner. The K scores cannot be directly used to compare dual-task costs between Experiments 1 and 2 because K units may not equate to the same demands across tasks. That is, the K capacity under single-task conditions may not be identical for the VWM and MOT tasks. We therefore developed a normalized K score, which expressed for each task the dual-task costs relative to the K in the single-task condition. This proportional measure of dual-task cost was calculated separately for each of the two tasks of a given experiment and then summed across the two tasks to provide a combined measure of interference that is robust to performance trade-offs. These computations were carried out separately for the low-load and high-load conditions. Thus, DK low assessed the extent to which the number of objects stored or tracked in tasks i and j decreased under dual-task, low-load conditions relative to their respective single-task, low-load conditions: ð Þ K low ð duali Þ DK low ¼ þ K low single i K low ð singlei Þ K low singlej ð Þ K! lowðdual j Þ K low ð singlej Þ ð1þ DK high assessed whether increasing task load resulted in interference additional to that observed at low load: DK high ¼ K highðsingle i Þ K high ð duali Þ K lowðsingle i Þ K low ð duali Þ K high ð singlei Þ K low ð singlei Þ K low singlej " K high ð singlej Þ K! highðdual j Þ ð Þ K!# lowðdual j Þ þ K high ð singlej Þ K low ð singlej Þ ð2þ Figure 4 shows that although the combined dual-task costs are equivalent for Experiments 1 (VWM-MOT) and 2 (VWM-VWM) at low load, t < 1, they are much higher for Experiment 2 than for Experiment 1 at high load, t(41) , d These results suggest that under low-load conditions, VWM performance is equally affected by concurrently performing another VWM task or a MOT task, but that when the load is increased, VWM performance is much more affected by another VWM task than by a MOT task. Thus, although VWM and MOT interfere with each other in a load-dependent manner, this interference is smaller than the interference observed under VWM-VWM dual-task conditions. Fig. 4. DK scores in Experiments 1, 2, and 3. Larger DK scores imply greater dual-task interference. The load of the second task was either low (left) or high (right). In each experiment, a visual working memory (VWM) task was paired with a multiple object tracking (MOT) task, a second VWM task, or a verbal working memory (verbal WM) task. Error bars represent between-subjects errors of the mean. EXPERIMENT 3 Even though MOT and VWM tasks do not interfere with each other to the same extent that two VWM tasks do, concurrent performance of a MOT task and a VWM task still instilled substantial and load-dependent dual-task costs. Did these costs arise from interference in visuospatial attention, or did they originate from central sources of dual-task limitations? Experiment 3 addressed this issue by determining whether verbal WM, generally considered to be independent from visuospatial attention and VWM (Baddeley, 1986; Logan, 1979; Vogel et al., 2001), would interfere with VWM to the same extent as a MOT task. If so, this would provide evidence for a central, amodal source for the VWM-MOT interference. Method Experiment 3, which involved 22 participants, was identical to Experiment 1 except that a verbal WM task replaced the MOT task. The verbal WM task began 1.2 s after the offset of the VWM display, with presentation of either two (low load) or eight (high load) spoken consonants over headphones. Each consonant stimulus was presented for 360 ms and separated from the next consonant by a 400-ms interstimulus interval. For each trial, the consonants were chosen randomly with no repeats from the following set: F, G, K, N, P, Q, R, S, T, X, Y, orz. Participants repeated the stimuli aloud at a 2-Hz rate for 8.4 s, after which a spoken probe consonant was presented for 360 ms, followed 600 ms later by the VWM probe. Following response to that probe, a question mark above fixation instructed the participants to indicate by key press whether the spoken probe matched a 530 Volume 17 Number 6

6 Daryl Fougnie and René Marois consonant from the sample set (50% of the trials were match trials). In the 25% of the trials with no verbal WM task, the second probe screen contained an ampersand, indicating that the participants should press the space bar. Participants performed articulatory suppression only in those trials. Results and Discussion VWM accuracy was not affected by the verbal WM task, F(2, 20) , p >.2, o (Fig. 2c, top panel). VWM performance did not differ between the conditions with no and low verbal WM load (p 5.81), and there was a marginal difference in VWM performance between the conditions with low and high verbal WM load (p 5.07). However, verbal WM accuracy was reduced not only by increased verbal load, F(1, 21) , p <.001, o (Fig. 2c, bottom panel), but also by concurrent performance of the VWM task, F(1, 21) , p <.005, o Subjects observed and expected dual-task capacities were comparable under low verbal WM load, t(21) , p 5.85, d (Fig. 3a). Furthermore, DK low for the verbal task was not only lower than DK low for the MOT task, t(37) , p <.05, d , but was also indistinguishable from 0, t < 1 (Fig. 4). These results suggest that verbal WM and VWM did not interfere with each other under low verbal WM load. By contrast, the observed K was lower than the expected K under high verbal WM load, t(21) , p <.005, d (Fig. 3b). Furthermore, under high-load conditions, the verbal WM-VWM costs were just as large as the VWM-MOT costs in Experiment 1, t < 1 (Fig. 4), although they were smaller than the VWM-VWM costs in Experiment 2, t(39) , p <.005, d Thus, whereas the low-load results reveal that VWM and MOT share a process that is not accessed by verbal WM, the high-load results suggest that the increased interference between the VWM and MOT tasks relative to the low-load results may be accounted for by central sources of dual-task interference. EXPERIMENTS 4 THROUGH 7 We carried out additional experiments to investigate the source of the interference between the attention and working memory tasks. The first two examined the importance of featural overlap between the MOT and VWM tasks. The MOT task involved monitoring the motion of colorless objects, whereas the VWM task required holding in mind the location of colored objects. We reasoned that perhaps greater interference would be obtained if the two tasks overlapped more in task-relevant attributes. In Experiment 4, which involved 20 participants, we assessed whether VWM-MOT interference would increase if both tasks involved monitoring colored stimuli. The experiment was identical to Experiment 1, except that the MOT discs were colored. Distinct color sets were used for the two tasks, as in Experiment 2. In trials with one MOT target, the target shared a color with three distractors, and two sets of four distractors shared other colors. In trials with three targets, each target had a distinct color shared with three distractors. Both matching and nonmatching probes were the same color as a target. The resulting DK scores (DK low 5.236, SE 5.119; DK high 5.195, SE 5.125) were indistinguishable from the DK scores in Experiment 1 ( ps >.5). Additionally, DK high was still lower than in Experiment 2 ( p <.01). These results suggest that interference between the MOT and VWM tasks is not strongly affected by whether or not both tasks involve color processing. Given that interference between attention and VWM tasks depends on whether they both require visuospatial information (e.g., Awh et al., 1998; Oh & Kim, 2004; Woodman & Luck, 2004; Woodman, Vogel, & Luck, 2001), Experiment 5 examined whether a purely spatial VWM task could produce greater dualtask interference between VWM and MOT tasks, because the latter task involves tracking objects moving in space. This experiment, which involved 17 participants, was identical to Experiment 1 except that the VWM task consisted of the sequential presentation of four black discs (0.781) that marked four spatial positions along an imaginary circle (3.31 radius). Each disc was presented for 200 ms and followed by a 200-ms gap. After the retention interval, participants indicated whether the probe disc occupied one of the memorized locations (50% of trials were match trials). Not only were dual-task costs (DK low 5.253, SE 5.103; DK high 5.01, SE 5.188) statistically indistinguishable from those in Experiment 1 (both ps >.1), but more important, the DK score was still lower than in Experiment 2 (VWM-VWM; p <.01). Nearly identical dual-task costs were also obtained in Experiment 6 (n 5 18), which was identical to Experiment 1 except that the VWM task did not require spatial working memory because probe items were presented at fixation. Thus, manipulations that affected the spatial demands of the VWM task failed to increase the MOT-VWM costs to the level obtained under VWM-VWM conditions. More generally, these spatial experiments, together with the color experiment, suggest that the difference in dual-task costs between MOT-VWM and VWM- VWM conditions does not likely originate at the featural level. Last, Experiment 7, which involved 17 participants, assessed whether the source of the MOT-VWM interference is common to other visual attention tasks. Specifically, we paired the VWM task of Experiment 1 with a target detection task involving multiple rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) streams (Fig. 5). The attentional load of the RSVP task was manipulated by varying the number of streams to be attended (one or three). Thus, whereas K estimated the number of objects tracked successfully in the MOT task, in the RSVP task K estimated the number of streams attended successfully. A pilot experiment (n 5 15) established that in single-task conditions, participants monitoring capacity leveled off at three to four RSVP streams. The VWM task was the same as in Experiment 1, except that the stimuli were colored diamonds (pink, dark green, light blue, Volume 17 Number 6 531

7 Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Visual Working Memory Fig. 5. Dual-task trial design for Experiment 7, which paired a visual working memory (VWM) task with a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task. The VWM task involved memorizing the color and location of three briefly presented diamonds. During the RSVP task, participants monitored one or three locations for the appearance of target items (red squares). Participants then responded to the VWM task (indicating whether a VWM probe item matched the color and location of a diamond in the original VWM display) and the RSVP task (indicating the number of targets that appeared). or orange) that appeared at any of the four locations 2.51 from fixation along the two 451 diagonal axes. Starting 1.2 s after the offset of the VWM display, white or black boxes (0.71) appeared at four locations 2.5 o from fixation along the vertical and horizontal axes, remaining present for the 7.5-s duration of the RSVP streams. Black boxes indicated the streams to be attended for targets, and white boxes indicated the streams to be ignored. Trials without the RSVP task contained four white boxes that informed participants not to perform the search task. The RSVP streams started 1.5 s after the appearance of the boxes and consisted of the synchronous presentations of colored shapes (0.51) in all four boxes every 300 ms (each shape was presented for 200 ms and followed by a 100-ms blank). Because stimuli included the target (red square), red distractors (circle, triangle, cross, hexagon), and nonred square distractors (blue, brown, yellow, purple), the task required subjects to attend to both color and shape at the cued streams. There were zero, one, or two targets per trial, with the targets appearing only in cued streams. After the response to the VWM task (see Experiment 1), a number sign indicated that the participants should press a key to indicate the number of targets seen in the RSVP task. For the 25% of trials with no RSVP task, an ampersand, rather than a number sign, appeared, and participants pressed the space bar in response. Only zero- and one-target trials were used for analysis because attention may not have been engaged for the entire RSVP sequence in two-target trials. The results revealed not only that DK scores for the low-load (DK 5.362, SE 5.153, p 5.6) and high-load (DK 5.272, SE Volume 17 Number 6

8 Daryl Fougnie and René Marois.137, p 5.86) conditions were indistinguishable from DK scores in Experiment 1 (VWM-MOT), but also that DK high was still lower than in Experiment 2 (VWM-VWM; p <.05). Thus, even though the RSVP and MOT tasks are very distinct attentional tasks, they yielded comparable dual-task costs when paired with a VWM task. These results strongly suggest that the interference between MOT and VWM generalizes across other visual attention tasks. Furthermore, given that the RSVP and VWM tasks involved substantial featural overlap (the RSVP task requiring attention to location, color, and shape, and the VWM task requiring attention to location and color) and yet yielded much less dual-task interference than the VWM-VWM condition, these results provide further evidence against the possibility that the difference between MOT-VWM and VWM-VWM dualtask costs is primarily due to differences in task-relevant features. GENERAL DISCUSSION Although our results suggest that attention and VWM are intertwined cognitive operations, they are not consistent with the view that the capacity limit of VWM is reducible to that of attention (e.g., Cowan, 2001; Rensink, 2000a), because the dualtask costs in the MOT-VWM condition (Experiment 1) and the VWM-VWM condition (Experiment 2) were not comparable. However, the data also suggest that VWM storage capacity is not a property that is entirely specific to this cognitive subsystem, because we observed interference between verbal WM and VWM, much as did Morey and Cowan (2004, 2005). Thus, our results are consistent with VWM capacity being a product of three cognitive operations: visuospatial attention; a central, amodal supervisory process; and local stage-specific operations. The dual-task costs observed under low-load conditions corroborate previous findings of an interaction between visuospatial attention and VWM (Awh & Jonides, 2001; Awh et al., 1998; Oh & Kim, 2004; Woodman & Luck, 2004), but not between verbal WM and VWM (Todd & Marois, 2004; Vogel et al., 2001). The interference between the MOT and RSVP tasks and the VWM task may have originated from the common use of visuospatial attention. Focused attention to the VWM positions may have been necessary either to maintain spatial location information during the retention interval (Awh & Jonides, 2001) or to bind featural information in memory (Wheeler & Treisman, 2002). Because execution of the MOT task necessitated shifts of visuospatial attention (e.g., Culham et al., 2001), attention may have been withdrawn from the tagged sites, thereby disrupting the memory. Two principal conclusions can be drawn from the dual-task costs observed under high load. The first is that attention and verbal WM tasks produce comparable dual-task interference with VWM. The most parsimonious account of this finding is that the interference arises from a shared capacity-limited process that impedes the concurrent execution of two distinct cognitive tasks under high demands. Although this process remains to be fully characterized (Baddeley, Chincotta, & Adlam, 2001; Morey & Cowan, 2004, 2005; Ruthruff & Pashler, 2001; Tombu & Jolicoeur, 2003), it is evidently of central, amodal origin, and may correspond to a supervisory attentional system responsible for goal and task setting (Norman & Shallice, 1986). The second important conclusion from the high-load results is that dual-task costs are much higher when two VWM tasks are performed concurrently than when a VWM task is paired with an attentional or verbal task. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that VWM involves stage-specific capacity-limited processes (Luck & Vecera, 2002). Indeed, VWM may contain several distinct subprocesses, each with its own processing capacities, just as MOT likely taps into its own content-specific processes. Evidently, it is through the complex interactions between these local processes and global attentional processes that the vivid but constrained mental representations of the visual world emerge. Acknowledgments This work was supported by National Science Foundation Grant and National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01 MH70776 to R.M. REFERENCES Awh, E., & Jonides, J. (2001). Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, Awh, E., Jonides, J., & Reuter Lorenz, P.A. (1998). Rehearsal in spatial working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, England: Clarendon. Baddeley, A., Chincotta, D., & Adlam, A. (2001). Working memory and the control of action: Evidence from task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, Cavanagh, P., & Alvarez, G.A. (2005). Tracking multiple targets with multifocal attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, Cowan, N. (1998). Visual and auditory working memory capacity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, Culham, J.C., Cavanagh, P., & Kanwisher, N.G. (2001). Attention response functions: Characterizing brain areas using fmri activation during parametric variations of attentional load. Neuron, 32, Delvenne, J.F., & Bruyer, R. (2004). Does visual short-term memory store bound features? Visual Cognition, 11, Irwin, D.E., & Gordon, R.D. (1998). Eye movements, attention and trans-saccadic memory. Visual Cognition, 5, Killeen, P.R. (2005). An alternative to null-hypothesis significance tests. Psychological Science, 16, Logan, G.D. (1979). On the use of a concurrent memory load to measure attention and automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, Luck, S.J., & Vecera, S.P. (2002). Attention. In H. Pashler (Ed.-in- Chief) & S. Yantis (Vol. Ed.), Stevens handbook of experimental psychology: Vol. 1. Sensation and perception (3rd ed., pp ). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Volume 17 Number 6 533

9 Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Visual Working Memory Morey, C.C., & Cowan, N. (2004). When visual and verbal memories compete: Evidence of cross-domain limits in working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, Morey, C.C., & Cowan, N. (2005). When do visual and verbal memories conflict? The importance of working-memory load and retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, Norman, D., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R.J. Davidson, G.E. Schwartz, & D.E. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 4, pp. 1 18). New York: Plenum. Oh, S.H., & Kim, M.S. (2004). The role of spatial working memory in visual search efficiency. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, Oksama, L., & Hyönä, J. (2004). Is multiple object tracking carried out automatically by an early vision mechanism independent of higher-order cognition? An individual difference approach. Visual Cognition, 11, Pashler, H. (1988). Familiarity and visual change detection. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, Pylyshyn, Z.W., & Storm, R.W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision, 3, Rensink, R.A. (2000a). The dynamic representation of scenes. Visual Cognition, 7, Rensink, R.A. (2000b). Visual search for change: A probe into the nature of attentional processing. Visual Cognition, 7, Rensink, R.A. (2002). Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, Ruthruff, E., & Pashler, H. (2001). Perceptual and central interference in dual-task performance. In K. Shapiro (Ed.), The limits of attention: Temporal constraints in human information processing (pp ). New York: Oxford University Press. Scholl, B.J. (2001). Objects and attention: The state of the art. Cognition, 80, Sears, C.R., & Pylyshyn, Z.W. (2000). Multiple object tracking and attentional processing. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74, Todd, J.J., & Marois, R. (2004). Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature, 428, Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, Vogel, E.K., Woodman, G.F., & Luck, S.J. (2001). Storage of features, conjunctions, and objects in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, Wheeler, M.E., & Treisman, A.M. (2002). Binding in short-term visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, Woodman, G.F., & Luck, S.J. (2004). Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, Woodman, G.F., Vogel, E.K., & Luck, S.J. (2001). Visual search remains efficient when visual working memory is full. Psychological Science, 12, (RECEIVED 5/20/05; REVISION ACCEPTED 9/9/05; FINAL MATERIALS RECEIVED 9/16/05) 534 Volume 17 Number 6

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms Daryl Fougnie and René Marois Vanderbilt

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article Research Article VISUAL SEARCH REMAINS EFFICIENT WHEN VISUAL WORKING MEMORY IS FULL Geoffrey F. Woodman, Edward K. Vogel, and Steven J. Luck University of Iowa Abstract Many theories of attention have

More information

Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation

Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (1), 154-158 Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation JOO-SEOK HYUN AND STEVEN J. LUCK University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa In mental rotation, a

More information

Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration

Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:1694 1709 DOI 10.3758/s13414-011-0135-5 Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration Tomonari Shimomura & Takatsune Kumada Published

More information

Visual Working Memory Represents a Fixed Number of Items Regardless of Complexity Edward Awh, Brian Barton, and Edward K. Vogel

Visual Working Memory Represents a Fixed Number of Items Regardless of Complexity Edward Awh, Brian Barton, and Edward K. Vogel PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Visual Working Memory Represents a Fixed Number of Items Regardless of Complexity University of Oregon ABSTRACT Does visual working memory represent a fixed number

More information

Binding across space and time in visual working memory

Binding across space and time in visual working memory Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (3), 292-303 doi:10.3758/mc.38.3.292 Binding across space and time in visual working memory PAUL JOHAN KARLSEN University of Tromsø, Tromsø, Norway RICHARD J. ALLEN University

More information

Finding Memory in Search: The Effect of Visual Working Memory Load on Visual Search. 1 Department of Psychology, University of Toronto

Finding Memory in Search: The Effect of Visual Working Memory Load on Visual Search. 1 Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Finding Memory 1 Running head: FINDING MEMORY IN SEARCH This is a preprint of an article submitted for consideration in the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 2010 Experimental Psychology Society;

More information

The association of color memory and the enumeration of multiple spatially overlapping sets

The association of color memory and the enumeration of multiple spatially overlapping sets Journal of Vision (2013) 13(8):6, 1 11 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/8/6 1 The association of color memory and the enumeration of multiple spatially overlapping sets Sonia Poltoratski Yaoda

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Remembering Complex Objects in Visual Working Memory: Do Capacity Limits Restrict Objects or Features? Kyle O. Hardman and Nelson Cowan

More information

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 3, 580 584 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027885 OBSERVATION Templates

More information

Retrieval from long-term memory reduces working memory representations for visual features and their bindings

Retrieval from long-term memory reduces working memory representations for visual features and their bindings Mem Cogn (2015) 43:237 246 DOI 10.3758/s13421-014-0468-0 Retrieval from long-term memory reduces working memory representations for visual features and their bindings Amanda E. van Lamsweerde & Melissa

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Change Detection Performance in Naturalistic Scenes: The Influence of Visual Working Memory for Identity and Spatial Locations

Change Detection Performance in Naturalistic Scenes: The Influence of Visual Working Memory for Identity and Spatial Locations Current Research in Psychology 3 (2): 49-59, 2012 ISSN: 1949-0178 2012 Science Publication doi:10.3844/crpsp.2012.49.59 Published Online 3 (2) 2012 (http://www.thescipub.com/crp.toc) Change Detection Performance

More information

Tracking unique objects 1 Makovski & Jiang. The role of visual working memory in attentive tracking of unique objects. Tal Makovski Yuhong V.

Tracking unique objects 1 Makovski & Jiang. The role of visual working memory in attentive tracking of unique objects. Tal Makovski Yuhong V. Tracking unique objects 1 Makovski & Jiang The role of visual working memory in attentive tracking of unique objects Tal Makovski Yuhong V. Jiang Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Sciences,

More information

Visual working memory for simple and complex visual stimuli

Visual working memory for simple and complex visual stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 005, (6), 7-33 Visual working memory for simple and complex visual stimuli HING YEE ENG, DIYU CHEN, and YUHONG JIANG Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts Does the

More information

When visual and verbal memories compete: Evidence of cross-domain limits in working memory

When visual and verbal memories compete: Evidence of cross-domain limits in working memory Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (2), 296-301 When visual and verbal memories compete: Evidence of cross-domain limits in working memory CANDICE C. MOREY and NELSON COWAN University of Missouri,

More information

The Role of Attention in the Maintenance of Feature Bindings in Visual Short-Term Memory

The Role of Attention in the Maintenance of Feature Bindings in Visual Short-Term Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2008, Vol. 34, No. 1, 41 55 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.1.41

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

3. Title: Within Fluid Cognition: Fluid Processing and Fluid Storage?

3. Title: Within Fluid Cognition: Fluid Processing and Fluid Storage? Cowan commentary on Blair, Page 1 1. Commentary on Clancy Blair target article 2. Word counts: Abstract 62 Main text 1,066 References 487 (435 excluding 2 references already in the target article) Total

More information

Discrete Resource Allocation in Visual Working Memory

Discrete Resource Allocation in Visual Working Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 5, 1359 1367 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015792 Discrete Resource

More information

Proactive interference from items previously stored in visual working memory

Proactive interference from items previously stored in visual working memory Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (1), 43-52 doi: 10.3758/MC.36.1.43 Proactive interference from items previously stored in visual working memory Tal Makovski and Yuhong V. Jiang Harvard University, Cambridge,

More information

The Relation between Visual Working Memory and Attention: Retention of Precise Color Information. in the Absence of Effects on Perceptual Selection

The Relation between Visual Working Memory and Attention: Retention of Precise Color Information. in the Absence of Effects on Perceptual Selection 1 The Relation between Visual Working Memory and Attention: Retention of Precise Color Information in the Absence of Effects on Perceptual Selection Andrew Hollingworth and Seongmin Hwang Department of

More information

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Background Maintenance: Modality specificity and buffers Interference resolution: Distraction and

More information

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1 The attentional window configures to object boundaries University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 2 ABSTRACT When

More information

Competition in visual working memory for control of search

Competition in visual working memory for control of search VISUAL COGNITION, 2004, 11 6), 689±703 Competition in visual working memory for control of search Paul E. Downing and Chris M. Dodds University of Wales, Bangor, UK Recent perspectives on selective attention

More information

Demonstrations of limitations in the way humans process and

Demonstrations of limitations in the way humans process and Visual memory needs categories Henrik Olsson* and Leo Poom Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Box 1225, SE-751 42 Uppsala, Sweden Edited by Anne Treisman, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ,

More information

Orienting Attention in Visual Working Memory Reduces Interference From Memory Probes

Orienting Attention in Visual Working Memory Reduces Interference From Memory Probes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2008, Vol. 34, No. 2, 369 380 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.369

More information

Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items

Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items Journal of Vision (2003) 3, 6-21 http://journalofvision.org/3/1/2/ 6 Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items Jun Saiki PRESTO, JST, Kawaguchi, Japan; and Graduate School

More information

Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel

Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel 1 This is an in press article to appear in Psychological Science, summer 2006 Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel Justin Halberda, Johns Hopkins University Sean F. Sires,

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

The allocation of visual short-term memory capacity: Evidence for a flexible storage mechanism

The allocation of visual short-term memory capacity: Evidence for a flexible storage mechanism The allocation of visual short-term memory capacity: Evidence for a flexible storage mechanism George A. Alvarez 1 & Steven L. Franconeri 2 (1) Massachusetts Institute of Technology (2) University of British

More information

The flicker paradigm provides converging evidence for a 3-item limit of visual working memory. Justin Halberda Johns Hopkins University

The flicker paradigm provides converging evidence for a 3-item limit of visual working memory. Justin Halberda Johns Hopkins University The flicker paradigm provides converging evidence for a 3-item limit of visual working memory Justin Halberda Johns Hopkins University Daniel J. Simons University of Illinois Jeffrey Wetherhold Harvard

More information

Selective storage and maintenance of an object s features in visual working memory

Selective storage and maintenance of an object s features in visual working memory Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (1), 223-229 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.1.223 Selective storage and maintenance of an object s features in visual working memory GEOFFREY F. WOODMAN Vanderbilt University,

More information

The Role of Working Memory Representations in the Control of Attention

The Role of Working Memory Representations in the Control of Attention Cerebral Cortex 2007;17:i118--i124 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm065 The Role of Working Memory Representations in the Control of Attention Geoffrey F. Woodman 1, Steven J. Luck 2 and Jeffrey D. Schall 1 1 Vanderbilt

More information

Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied

Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (2), 269-274 Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied GEOFFREY F. WOODMAN Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee and STEVEN J. LUCK

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists. International authors and editors

We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists. International authors and editors We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists 4,000 116,000 120M Open access books available International authors and editors Downloads Our

More information

Binding global and local object features in visual working memory

Binding global and local object features in visual working memory Atten Percept Psychophys (2016) 78:94 106 DOI 10.3758/s13414-015-1008-0 Binding global and local object features in visual working memory Justin M. Ericson 1,2 & Melissa R. Beck 1 & Amanda E. van Lamsweerde

More information

Memory-Based Attention Capture when Multiple Items Are Maintained in Visual Working. Memory. Andrew Hollingworth and Valerie M.

Memory-Based Attention Capture when Multiple Items Are Maintained in Visual Working. Memory. Andrew Hollingworth and Valerie M. 1 Running head: VWM guidance Memory-Based Attention Capture when Multiple Items Are Maintained in Visual Working Memory Andrew Hollingworth and Valerie M. Beck Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences,

More information

Creation of visual long-term memory

Creation of visual long-term memory Perception & Psychophysics 2007, 69 (6), 904-912 Creation of visual long-term memory DANKO NIKOLIć AND WOLF SINGER Max Planck Institute for Brain Research, Frankfurt am Main, Germany and Frankfurt Institute

More information

Limitations of Object-Based Feature Encoding in Visual Short-Term Memory

Limitations of Object-Based Feature Encoding in Visual Short-Term Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 458 468 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.458

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL Visual working memory load disrupts template-guided attentional selection during visual search Nick Berggren* and Martin Eimer Department

More information

Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck

Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 729-734 Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck PAUL E. DUX AND RENÉ MAROIS Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee The attentional blink

More information

Attentional Blink Paradigm

Attentional Blink Paradigm Attentional Blink Paradigm ATTENTIONAL BLINK 83 ms stimulus onset asychrony between all stimuli B T D A 3 N P Z F R K M R N Lag 3 Target 1 Target 2 After detection of a target in a rapid stream of visual

More information

On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink

On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 723-728 On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink Pau l E. Dux Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee and Irina M. Harris University

More information

On perfect working-memory performance with large numbers of items

On perfect working-memory performance with large numbers of items Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:958 963 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0108-7 BRIEF REPORT On perfect working-memory performance with large numbers of items Jonathan E. Thiele & Michael S. Pratte & Jeffrey N. Rouder

More information

Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects

Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects Roy Luria 1, Paola Sessa 1, Alex Gotler 2, Pierre Jolicœur 3, and Roberto DellʼAcqua 1 Abstract Does the capacity of visual short-term memory

More information

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Masters of Science in Psychology By Quan Ying Gao University of Canterbury

More information

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Limits to Iconic Memory 0 Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Ronald A. Rensink Departments of Psychology and Computer Science University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4 Canada Running Head: Limits

More information

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making Matthew A J Apps 1,2, Laura Grima 2, Sanjay Manohar 2, Masud Husain 1,2 1 Nuffield Department of Clinical Neuroscience,

More information

First published on: 10 November 2010

First published on: 10 November 2010 This article was downloaded by: [VUL Vanderbilt University] On: 10 November 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 917420668] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

The role of figure ground segregation in change blindness

The role of figure ground segregation in change blindness Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (2), 254-261 The role of figure ground segregation in change blindness ROGIER LANDMAN Graduate School of Neurosciences, Amsterdam, The Netherlands and The Netherlands

More information

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Psychon Bull Rev (2017) 24:1651 1657 DOI 10.3758/s13423-017-1226-7 BRIEF REPORT Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Sébastien De Schrijver 1 & Pierre Barrouillet 1 Published

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? University of Iowa Honors Theses University of Iowa Honors Program Spring 2017 Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? Ann Walsh University of Iowa Follow this and additional works at:

More information

Dual n-back training increases the capacity of the focus of attention

Dual n-back training increases the capacity of the focus of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:135 141 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0335-6 BRIEF REPORT Dual n-back training increases the capacity of the focus of attention Lindsey Lilienthal & Elaine Tamez & Jill Talley Shelton

More information

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory Alejandro Lleras, 1 Ronald A. Rensink, 2 and James T. Enns

More information

Fractionating Working Memory

Fractionating Working Memory PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Fractionating Working Memory Consolidation and Maintenance Are Independent Processes Geoffrey F. Woodman and Edward K. Vogel Vanderbilt University and University

More information

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Running Head: Visual Working Memory and the CDA Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Nick Berggren and

More information

Out with the old? The role of selective attention in retaining targets in partial report

Out with the old? The role of selective attention in retaining targets in partial report Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79:117 137 DOI 10.3758/s13414-016-1214-4 Out with the old? The role of selective attention in retaining targets in partial report Dakota R. B. Lindsey 1 & Claus Bundesen

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1040-1050 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1040 Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching ERINA SAEKI AND SATORU

More information

Tracking planets and moons: mechanisms of object tracking revealed with a new paradigm

Tracking planets and moons: mechanisms of object tracking revealed with a new paradigm Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:738 750 DOI 10.3758/s13414-010-0060-z Tracking planets and moons: mechanisms of object tracking revealed with a new paradigm Michael Tombu & Adriane E. Seiffert Published

More information

The Neural Fate of Task-Irrelevant Features in Object-Based Processing

The Neural Fate of Task-Irrelevant Features in Object-Based Processing 14020 The Journal of Neuroscience, October 20, 2010 30(42):14020 14028 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive The Neural Fate of Task-Irrelevant Features in Object-Based Processing Yaoda Xu Department of Psychology,

More information

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Nick Berggren and Martin Eimer Abstract During the retention of

More information

Working Memory (Goal Maintenance and Interference Control) Edward E. Smith Columbia University

Working Memory (Goal Maintenance and Interference Control) Edward E. Smith Columbia University Working Memory (Goal Maintenance and Interference Control) Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Goal Maintenance Interference resolution: distraction, proactive interference, and directed forgetting

More information

Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience

Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1465-8 Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience Daniel B. Vatterott 1 & Michael C. Mozer 2 & Shaun P. Vecera

More information

Stressing the mind: The effect of cognitive load and articulatory suppression on attentional guidance from working memory

Stressing the mind: The effect of cognitive load and articulatory suppression on attentional guidance from working memory Perception & Psychophysics 2008, 70 (5), 924-934 doi: 10.3758/PP.70.5.924 Stressing the mind: The effect of cognitive load and articulatory suppression on attentional guidance from working memory DAVID

More information

The role of location and motion information in the tracking and recovery of moving objects

The role of location and motion information in the tracking and recovery of moving objects Perception & Psychophysics 2007, 69 (4), 567-577 The role of location and motion information in the tracking and recovery of moving objects DAVID E. FENCSIK Brigham and Women s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Grouping by similarity is mediated by feature selection: evidence from the failure of cue combination

Grouping by similarity is mediated by feature selection: evidence from the failure of cue combination Psychon Bull Rev (2015) 22:1364 1369 DOI 10.3758/s13423-015-0801-z BRIEF REPORT Grouping by similarity is mediated by feature selection: evidence from the failure of cue combination Liqiang Huang Published

More information

New objects do not capture attention without a sensory transient

New objects do not capture attention without a sensory transient Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (5), 1298-1310 doi:10.3758/app.72.5.1298 New objects do not capture attention without a sensory transient Andrew Hollingworth University of Iowa, Iowa City,

More information

The impact of item clustering on visual search: It all depends on the nature of the visual search

The impact of item clustering on visual search: It all depends on the nature of the visual search Journal of Vision (2010) 10(14):24, 1 9 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/14/24 1 The impact of item clustering on visual search: It all depends on the nature of the visual search Yaoda Xu Department

More information

Perceptual grouping in change detection

Perceptual grouping in change detection 1 Perceptual grouping in change detection Yuhong Jiang Massachusetts Institute of Technology Marvin M. Chun Yale University Ingrid R. Olson University of Pennsylvania [In Press: Perception & Psychophysics,

More information

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load Psychonomic Bulletin & Review In press Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load JOSHUA D. COSMAN AND SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Attentional capture by abrupt onsets can be

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

The effect of spatial organization of targets and distractors on the capacity to selectively memorize objects in visual short-term memory

The effect of spatial organization of targets and distractors on the capacity to selectively memorize objects in visual short-term memory The effect of spatial organization of targets and distractors on the capacity to selectively memorize objects in visual short-term memory Aymen Ben Abbes 1, Emmanuelle Gavault 2, and Thierry Ripoll 2 1

More information

This is a repository copy of Executive control of stimulus-driven and goal-directed attention in visual working memory.

This is a repository copy of Executive control of stimulus-driven and goal-directed attention in visual working memory. This is a repository copy of Executive control of stimulus-driven and goal-directed attention in visual working memory. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/101629/

More information

Sensory Memory Systems. Visual Store. PDF created with pdffactory trial version

Sensory Memory Systems. Visual Store. PDF created with pdffactory trial version Sensory Memory Systems Visual Store Jevons (1871) estimate the number of beans. Accurate estimations up to 9 beans (span of apprehension). Averbach(1963) Tachistoscopic display Estimate how many dots are

More information

Attention and Scene Perception

Attention and Scene Perception Theories of attention Techniques for studying scene perception Physiological basis of attention Attention and single cells Disorders of attention Scene recognition attention any of a large set of selection

More information

Perceptual grouping in change detection

Perceptual grouping in change detection Perception & Psychophysics 2004, 66 (3), 446-453 Perceptual grouping in change detection YUHONG JIANG Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts MARVIN M. CHUN Yale University, New

More information

Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles

Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (5), 945-949 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.5.945 Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles Xingshan Li and Gordon D. Logan Vanderbilt

More information

2/27/2017. Modal Model of Memory. Executive Attention & Working Memory. Some Questions to Consider (over the next few weeks)

2/27/2017. Modal Model of Memory. Executive Attention & Working Memory. Some Questions to Consider (over the next few weeks) Executive Attention & Working Memory Memory 1 Some Questions to Consider (over the next few weeks) Why can we remember a telephone number long enough to place a call, but then we forget it almost immediately?

More information

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1 Current Biology 24, 193 198, January 20, 2014 ª2014 The Authors http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.001 Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects Report Martin

More information

Task Specificity and the Influence of Memory on Visual Search: Comment on Võ and Wolfe (2012)

Task Specificity and the Influence of Memory on Visual Search: Comment on Võ and Wolfe (2012) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 6, 1596 1603 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0030237 COMMENTARY Task Specificity

More information

Consolidating working memory: Enhancing cognitive performance through effective encoding

Consolidating working memory: Enhancing cognitive performance through effective encoding Consolidating working memory: Enhancing cognitive performance through effective encoding Donna Bayliss Neurocognitive Development Unit School of Psychology University of Western Australia 1 Consolidation

More information

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Rui Cao (caorui.beilia@gmail.com) Thomas A. Busey (busey@indiana.edu) Robert M. Nosofsky (nosofsky@indiana.edu) Richard

More information

The Representation of Simple Ensemble Visual Features Outside the Focus of Attention

The Representation of Simple Ensemble Visual Features Outside the Focus of Attention The Representation of Simple Ensemble Visual Features Outside the Focus of Attention George A. Alvarez and Aude Oliva Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology The

More information

An Ideal Observer Model of Visual Short-Term Memory Predicts Human Capacity Precision Tradeoffs

An Ideal Observer Model of Visual Short-Term Memory Predicts Human Capacity Precision Tradeoffs An Ideal Observer Model of Visual Short-Term Memory Predicts Human Capacity Precision Tradeoffs Chris R. Sims Robert A. Jacobs David C. Knill (csims@cvs.rochester.edu) (robbie@bcs.rochester.edu) (knill@cvs.rochester.edu)

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

Overcoming the three-item limit: Gestalt grouping principles explain increases in change detection capacity. Justin Halberda Johns Hopkins University

Overcoming the three-item limit: Gestalt grouping principles explain increases in change detection capacity. Justin Halberda Johns Hopkins University Overcoming the three-item limit: Gestalt grouping principles explain increases in change detection capacity Justin Halberda Johns Hopkins University Daniel J. Simons University of Illinois Jeffrey Wetherhold

More information

Perceptual organization influences visual working memory

Perceptual organization influences visual working memory Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2003, 10 (1), 80-87 Perceptual organization influences visual working memory GEOFFREY F. WOODMAN, SHAUN P. VECERA, and STEVEN J. LUCK University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Previous

More information

VISUAL MEMORY. Visual Perception

VISUAL MEMORY. Visual Perception VISUAL MEMORY Visual Perception Memory is unqiue Other aspects of visual perception Bombard us with stimuli at every instance Memory Helps us to make sense from chain of such instances Slide 2 Two Theories

More information

Constructing Visual Representations of Natural Scenes: The Roles of Short- and Long-Term Visual Memory

Constructing Visual Representations of Natural Scenes: The Roles of Short- and Long-Term Visual Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2004, Vol. 30, No. 3, 519 537 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.3.519

More information

Delineating the Neural Signatures of Tracking Spatial Position and Working Memory during Attentive Tracking

Delineating the Neural Signatures of Tracking Spatial Position and Working Memory during Attentive Tracking The Journal of Neuroscience, January 12, 2011 31(2):659 668 659 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Delineating the Neural Signatures of Tracking Spatial Position and Working Memory during Attentive Tracking

More information

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1996, 3 (2), 222 226 Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli ROBERT M. NOSOFSKY Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana and THOMAS J. PALMERI Vanderbilt University,

More information