A Critical Review of Self-perceptions and the Positive Illusory Bias in Children with ADHD

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "A Critical Review of Self-perceptions and the Positive Illusory Bias in Children with ADHD"

Transcription

1 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10: DOI /s A Critical Review of Self-perceptions and the Positive Illusory Bias in Children with ADHD Julie Sarno Owens Æ Matthew E. Goldfine Æ Nicole M. Evangelista Æ Betsy Hoza Æ Nina M. Kaiser Published online: 28 September 2007 Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007 Abstract Despite significant functional problems in multiple domains, children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) unexpectedly provide extremely positive reports of their own competence in comparison to other criteria reflecting actual competence. This counterintuitive phenomenon is known as the positive illusory bias (PIB). This article provides a comprehensive and critical review of the literature examining the self-perceptions of children with ADHD and the PIB. Specifically, we analyze methodological and statistical challenges associated with the investigation of the phenomenon, the theoretical basis for the PIB, and the effects of sample heterogeneity on selfperception patterns. We conclude by discussing the implications of this work and providing recommendations for advancing research in this area. J. S. Owens (&) N. M. Evangelista Ohio University, Porter Hall 200, Athens, OH 45701, USA owensj@ohio.edu N. M. Evangelista ne298698@ohio.edu M. E. Goldfine West Virginia University, 2210 Life Sciences Bldg, Morgantown, WV , USA Matthew.Goldfine@mail.wvu.edu B. Hoza University of Vermont, Dewey Hall Rm 226, Burlington, VT 05405, USA Betsy.Hoza@uvm.edu N. M. Kaiser University of California, Box 0984, LPPI, CPT, San Francisco, CA , USA nina.kaiser@ucsf.edu Keywords ADHD Self-perceptions Positive illusions Positive illusory bias Children As one of the most prevalent childhood mental health disorders, Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) affects an estimated 3 7% of all school-aged children (American Psychiatric Association 2000). Diagnosed considerably more often in males, ADHD is characterized by developmentally inappropriate hyperactive/impulsive and/or inattentive symptoms that result in impairment in multiple settings (American Psychiatric Association 2000). Children with ADHD consistently have been found to display academic difficulties (LeFever et al. 2002), social deficits (Pelham and Bender 1982), and behavior problems (Barkley 1990). Within the academic domain, children with ADHD typically have poorer academic achievement scores (Hinshaw 1992), lower grades (Cantwell and Satterfield 1978) and less persistence on school-related tasks than their non-impaired peers (Hoza et al. 2001). Within the social domain, children with ADHD have fewer close friendships (Bagwell et al. 2001), are more rejected in the classroom (Hodgens et al. 2000; Hoza et al. 2005), and have more negative interactions with their mothers as compared to their peers without ADHD (Cunningham and Barkley 1979). Within the behavioral domain, the most noticeable deficits include aggression (Loney and Milich 1982) and noncompliance with adult commands (Danforth et al. 1991). Given the above-described deficits, this population is at increased risk for school dropout, drug abuse, and juvenile delinquency (Shaffer 1994). Despite these findings of chronic functional problems in academic, social, and behavioral domains, many children

2 336 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10: with ADHD tend to underreport the presence of these problems. Indeed, a growing body of literature suggests that many children with ADHD actually overestimate their own competence in comparison to other criteria reflecting actual competence (e.g., Hoza et al. 2004; Hoza et al. 2002). In other words, within this population, children s self-perceptions frequently do not correspond with objective measures of performance or with parent and teacher ratings of competence. This phenomenon has been termed the positive illusory bias (PIB) and is operationally defined as a disparity between self-report of competence and actual competence such that self-reported competence is substantially higher than actual competence (Hoza et al. 2002). Interestingly, social psychology research has documented the existence of similar positive illusions in the general population. For example, multiple studies have produced consistent evidence of the better than average effect (e.g., Alicke and Govorun 2005). When adults are asked to compare themselves to a hypothetical average target, self-evaluations are typically more positive than mathematically possible. In addition, a review by Taylor and Brown (1988) suggested that mentally healthy individuals hold unrealistically positive self-evaluations, inflated beliefs of power over the environment, and exaggerated optimism about the future. Thus, moderately positive illusory beliefs appear to be somewhat normative. However, the PIB found in children with ADHD differs from the overly positive cognitions found in the general population in three ways. First, the discrepancy between perceived and actual competence typically is of greater magnitude in samples of children with ADHD as compared to children without ADHD (Hoza et al. 2002; Owens and Hoza 2003), and thus deviates from normative positive cognitions. That is, children with ADHD report positive illusory levels of self-competence that exceed those of similarly aged, non-diagnosed peers. Indeed, past researchers have suggested that excessively inflated self-perceptions may differ qualitatively from the moderately enhanced self-evaluations exhibited in the general population (e.g., Baumeister 1989; Taylor and Armor 1996). Second, Taylor and Brown (1988) suggested that moderate positive illusions are adaptive in that they enhance motivation, performance, and task persistence. This does not seem to be true for the positive illusions held by children with ADHD. Despite demonstrating positive illusory self-perceptions, children with ADHD tend to give up more frequently on tasks and perform worse than their non-adhd peers (Hoza et al. 2001; O Neill and Douglas 1991). Third, within the context of other theoretical models, the PIB in children with ADHD seems to be a unique and counterintuitive phenomenon. For example, Harter s model of motivation (1981) purports that children s self-perceptions of competence and control contribute to their motivational orientation. More specifically, children who experience success following task engagement develop an enhanced sense of self-efficacy. As a result, these children are motivated to engage in future challenging tasks and are subsequently reinforced by additional success. Conversely, children who experience failure on a given task develop a lower sense of self-competence and thus are less likely to participate in future challenging tasks. Since most children with ADHD have been found to experience failure in multiple domains, Harter s (1981) model would suggest that these children are at risk for developing a low sense of self-competence. However, most studies that have examined self-perceptions in children with ADHD have found evidence inconsistent with Harter s model, suggesting that there is something unique about the self-system of children with ADHD. Specifically, past results imply that children with ADHD have unrealistically high self-views of skills and competencies, despite histories marked with failure in numerous domains (e.g., Hoza et al. 2002). On one hand, such positive illusory self-views may render children with ADHD more susceptible to failure, as these views likely prevent children from recognizing the need for improvement, acknowledging negative feedback, and altering their approach to task completion (Milich and Okazaki 1991). On the other hand, if children with ADHD do not perceive their academic and social failures, this lack of awareness may spare their self-confidence, protect their self-esteem, and ward off negative affect. As this review article highlights, additional research examining the relative adaptive or maladaptive consequences of positive illusory selfperceptions upon social-emotional functioning and task performance and persistence is needed before firm conclusions can be drawn in this area. We acknowledge that overly positive self-evaluations are not exclusive to children with ADHD and have been found in other non-clinic samples (e.g., Gresham et al. 1998; Heath and Glen 2005; Hymel et al. 1993). Nonetheless, for several reasons, the current article will primarily focus on the positive illusory self-perceptions of children with ADHD. First, the pattern of differences between children with ADHD and asymptomatic control children has been replicated across multiple studies and laboratories. These studies represent a rich and coherent literature that warrants synthesis and critical analysis. Since this literature is focused on a well-defined sample, recommendations produced by such an analysis can be focused and directive. Second, a closer examination of the studies that have found a similar pattern of positive illusory self-concepts in non-clinic samples of children reveals a common behavioral characteristic. Although the focus of these studies may have been children with poor social

3 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10: status, learning disabilities, or aggressive behavior (Gresham et al. 1998; Heath and Glen 2005; Hymel et al. 1993), disruptive behavior problems are a common factor linking all of these samples. Thus, to date extant research does not specify the degree to which positive illusions are specific to any one population. However, most research suggests that the PIB is linked to characteristics associated with ADHD. Given the coherence of the literature focused on this disorder, we will contain this review to samples of children with ADHD or clearly defined ADHD symptoms. Finally, ADHD is a disorder for which evidence-based behavioral treatments have been identified. There is suggestive evidence that awareness of one s own deficits may serve a motivating function in behavioral treatment (Hoza and Pelham 1995), whereas inaccurate estimations of selfcompetence may interfere with treatment progress. Thus, a better understanding of the self-system of children with ADHD may have implications for future enhancements to evidence-based treatment for this population. The goal of this article is to provide a comprehensive and critical review of the literature examining the selfperceptions of children with ADHD. Our synthesis and analysis of the literature was guided by three overarching questions: Is the PIB present in the self-perceptions of children with ADHD? What is the function (or explanation) of the PIB in children with ADHD? And, is the PIB adaptive or maladaptive in children with ADHD? Our understanding of the latter two questions will guide our interpretation of the data associated with the first question. That is, simply identifying the existence of the PIB is not enough. Research in this area must strive to understand the underlying mechanism or function of the bias, as well as the short- and long-term consequences of this pattern of self-perceptions; both of these issues might have important implications for treatment modifications. Thus, first we review the extant literature that provides substantial empirical support for the presence of the PIB in children with ADHD. In doing so, we highlight critical methodological and statistical challenges associated with the investigation of the phenomenon. Next, to create a conceptual context for stimulating new ideas and advancing research regarding the underlying mechanisms of the PIB, we review the data that explore the possible functions of the phenomenon, as well as the effects of sample heterogeneity on patterns of self-perceptions. To date, the adaptiveness of the PIB is unclear. Thus, in the absence of longitudinal studies examining the implications of the short- and long-term effects of this perceptual style, we can only speculate with regard to possible consequences of the PIB and suggest how future studies can explore this issue. Thus, we conclude by discussing the implications of this work and offering recommendations for future studies. We hope that this review will ignite interest and provide guidance in research that attempts to obtain a better conceptualization of self-perceptual patterns in children with ADHD. Self-perceptions and Children with ADHD The methodology used to examine the self-perceptions of children with ADHD has evolved over the last decade. Early research compared ratings of competence on selfperception measures (i.e., absolute rating scale scores), predictions of performance on various tasks, and postperformance self-evaluation ratings among children with and without ADHD. These early studies highlighted unique patterns in children s self-perceptions and provided preliminary evidence to suggest that the self-perceptions of children with ADHD were less congruent with actual performance than were those of control children. In order examine the self-perceptions within this population, more recent studies have investigated discrepancies between children s self-perceived competence and competence as indexed by standardized achievement measures or parent and teacher ratings. These studies provide additional support for the existence of a PIB in children with ADHD. More recently, researchers have begun to examine the role of ADHD subtype, comorbidity, and gender in the selfperception patterns of children with ADHD. Below, we synthesize the findings of these methodological approaches in order of methodological strength (i.e., studies with weaker methodology are reviewed first), including studies that make use of (a) absolute self-perception scores (mean scores) in the absence of a criterion for comparison, (b) pre-task prediction and/or post-task performance evaluation ratings, and (c) discrepancy analyses utilizing a criterion to calculate the discrepancy between childreported competence and actual competence. See Table 1 for a summary of all studies examining the PIB in children with ADHD. Following this analysis, we further discuss methodological and statistical challenges associated with examining the PIB in children with ADHD. Absolute Self-perceptions In one of the first studies to compare the self-perceptions of boys with and without ADHD, Hoza et al. (1993) utilized the Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPPC; Harter 1985) to assess children s self-perceptions of scholastic competence, social acceptance, behavioral conduct, athletic competence, physical appearance, and global selfworth. After controlling for comorbid internalizing symptoms, Hoza et al. found that the self-perceptions of boys with ADHD were not significantly different from those of

4 338 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10: Table 1 Summary of studies examining the positive illusory bias in children with ADHD Citation Age Sample size % Male Comorbidity Type of analyses Diener and Milich (1997) 8 11 years ADHD = 30; CTL = None SPH Evangelista et al. (2007) 3 5 grades ADHD = 67; CTL = None DIS (T) Horn et al. (1989) 7 9 years ADHD = 54; CTL = None ABS Hoza et al. (2004) 7 10 years ADHD = 487; CTL = CP, DEP, LOW ACH DIS (P, T) Hoza et al. (2002) 7 13 years ADHD = 195; CTL = AGG, DEP, LOW ACH ABS, DIS (T) Hoza et al. (1993) 8 13 years ADHD = 27; CTL = None ABS Hoza et al. (2001) 7 13 years ADHD = 83; CTL = None PP Hoza et al. (2000) 7 13 years ADHD = 120; CTL = None PP Ialongo et al. (1994) 7 11 years ADHD = 48; CTL = None ABS Milich and Okazaki (1991) 9 11 years ADHD = 23; CTL = None PP Ohan and Johnston (2002) 7 12 years ADHD = 45; CTL = None SPH O Neill and Douglas (1991) 3 7 grades ADHD = 20; CTL = 20; LD = None PP Owens and Hoza (2003) 9 12 years IA = 38; HICB = 59; CTL = None ABS, DIS (T, A) Treuting and Hinshaw (2001) 7 12 years ADHD = 114; CTL = AGG ABS Whalen et al. (1991) 7 13 years ADHD = 15; CTL = None PP Note. Comorbidity column indicates if analyses included subgroupings within the ADHD group. CTL = control; CP = conduct problems; DEP = depression; LOW ACH = low achievement; AGG = aggression; LD = learning disability; IA = ADHD, Inattentive Type; HICB = ADHD, Hyperactive/Impulsive or Combined Type; ABS = absolute analyses; DIS (P) = discrepancy analyses with parent as criterion; DIS (T) = discrepancy analyses with teacher as criterion; DIS (A) = discrepancy analyses with actual achievement scores as criterion; SPH = test of the self-protective hypothesis; PP = pre-task prediction and/or post-performance ratings boys without ADHD across all domains, with the exception of athletic competence in which boys with ADHD provided more positive self-evaluations than boys without ADHD. Notably, the boys with ADHD in this sample were enrolled in intensive day treatment and thus presumably demonstrating significant functional problems. Consequently, the authors interpreted these findings as evidence of selfenhancement on the part of the boys with ADHD. Data from more recent studies with larger sample sizes have replicated this pattern (e.g., Hoza et al. 2002). Conversely, two studies (Horn et al. 1989; Ialongo et al. 1994) found that children with ADHD reported significantly lower self-concepts than did control children. However, there are several possible explanations for the inconsistency between these findings and those of Hoza et al. (2002, 1993). First, Horn et al. (1989) and Ialongo and colleagues (1994) utilized the Piers-Harris Self-Concept Scale (Piers 1969), whereas most other studies (e.g., Hoza et al. 2002, 1993) utilized the SPPC (Harter 1985). It may be that between-group differences in self-perceptions are better detected when a more sensitive dimensional assessment method (e.g., SPPC) is used compared to a dichotomous assessment method (e.g., Piers-Harris Self- Concept Scale). Second, Horn and colleagues (1989) only administered the anxiety, happiness, and popularity subscales of the Piers-Harris Self-Concept Scale, excluding more salient domains such as behavior and academics. Finally, Horn et al. (1989) did not control for internalizing symptoms, whereas Hoza et al. (1993) did control for internalizing symptoms. Importantly, there is some evidence to support the idea that comorbid mood problems (and authors decisions about how to assess these symptoms) are quite relevant to findings about self-perceptions; we discuss this issue in detail later in the article. 1 In sum, studies examining the absolute self-perceptions of children with ADHD have generated mixed results. However, these studies provide the weakest support for the PIB because without a basis for comparison, the congruence between children s perceptions and actual competence is unknown. In an attempt to advance the literature, other studies have utilized children s predictions of their future task performance and post-task performance evaluations in order to determine the presence of a PIB. Pre-task Prediction and Post-performance Ratings Many of the early studies examining pre-task predictions and post-task performance evaluations made by children with ADHD revealed a glaring optimism in both arenas. For example, Whalen et al. (1991) found that 80% of boys 1 Treuting and Hinshaw (2001) also failed to find evidence of higher self-reported self-perceptions in children with ADHD as compared to controls. However, the pattern of findings in this study may be better explained through sample characteristics other than ADHD (i.e., depressive symptoms). Thus, Treuting and Hinshaw s study is discussed in more detail later in the article (see Effects of Sample Heterogeneity section).

5 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10: with ADHD predicted perfect performance on a wordsearch task compared to 43% of boys without ADHD. Another study demonstrated that despite similar performance (i.e., no significant group difference) on a story recall task, children with ADHD reported higher pre-task predictions than control children (O Neill and Douglas 1991). Additionally, boys with ADHD studied for less time, appeared to exert less effort, and utilized less effective strategies relative to control boys. Milich and Okazaki (1991) obtained similar results when evaluating the predictions of boys with and without ADHD prior to solving a set of find-a-word puzzles. Boys with ADHD predicted better self-performance, yet completed considerably fewer puzzles than controls. Boys with ADHD also reported more frustration and gave up more frequently than boys without ADHD. Similarly, findings from several other studies employing success-failure manipulation tasks imply that children with ADHD are overly optimistic in their post-task performance evaluations. Hoza et al. (2000) examined children s selfevaluations of performance in a success-failure manipulation task within the social domain using a get-acquainted task. Boys with and without ADHD were instructed to get a child confederate to like them and to convince the confederate to come to a camp (or school). Performance was manipulated with the help of the confederate, such that each child participated in a successful and unsuccessful social interaction. Coders who were unaware of group status evaluated boys with ADHD as less socially competent than boys without ADHD across both success and failure conditions. Importantly, despite these objective indices of relative incompetence at the task, boys with ADHD evaluated their own task performance as significantly better than did control boys. This overestimation was most evident after experiencing a failed social interaction. Hoza et al. (2001) expanded upon the previous study (Hoza et al. 2000) by examining children s self-evaluations of performance in the context of success and failure experiences in the academic domain (i.e., find-a-word puzzles). Despite the fact that boys with ADHD solved fewer test puzzles, gave up more often, and were rated by objective evaluators to be less effortful, their post-task evaluations were not significantly different from those of control children. These findings suggest that the selfevaluation ratings of children with ADHD were not commensurate with their actual performance. More specifically, boys with ADHD provided overly optimistic reports of their own performance. Clearly, the studies described in this section represent a methodological improvement over using absolute selfperceptions scores in the absence of a criterion. As such, future research should continue to explore critical questions related to the PIB in the context of such methodology (i.e., using performance on a specific task as the basis for children s ratings of competence and the competence criterion). In the next section, we discuss another methodological improvement over the use of absolute selfperceptions; the use of discrepancy scores that index the congruence between the child s perceptions of competence and other estimates of actual competence across multiple domains. Discrepancy and Criterion Analysis Recent studies have utilized discrepancy analyses in which difference scores are calculated by subtracting a criterion score (e.g., teacher report) from the child s self-report of competence. Difference scores are then compared between children with and without ADHD. Across studies, mother, father, and teacher reports of competence across multiple domains have been used as comparison criteria, and scores on standardized academic achievement tests. Hoza et al. (2002) examined the self-perceptions of boys with and without ADHD by comparing the boys selfreport on the SPPC (Harter 1985) against teacher report on the parallel teacher version (Teacher Report of Child s Actual Behavior; Harter 1985). Relative to teacher report, boys with ADHD overestimated their academic, behavioral, and social abilities to a greater degree than did control boys. In a more recent study, Hoza et al. (2004), further advanced the literature by demonstrating that the PIB was present in both boys and girls with ADHD and that the bias was observed regardless of the informant ratings used as the criterion (i.e., mothers, fathers, and teachers), ruling out potential rater bias on the part of teachers as an explanation for the phenomenon. Interestingly, two studies (Hoza et al. 2004, 2002) have provided evidence that children with ADHD demonstrate the greatest overestimation of competence in their domain of greatest deficit. To date, Owens and Hoza s (2003) study is the only investigation of the role of ADHD subtype (Predominantly Inattentive Type [IA] versus Hyperactive/Impulsive and/or Combined Types [HICB]) in self-perceptions of competence in the academic domain. The effect of ADHD subtype will be discussed in more detail in the Effects of Sample Heterogeneity section; however, it is noteworthy that Owens and Hoza employed a discrepancy analysis with teacher report and standardized achievement tests used as the criterion. Boys and girls in the HICB group overestimated their scholastic competence more than control boys when math and reading achievement scores were used as the criterion. Boys and girls in the IA group, however, provided estimates of their own academic competence that were more congruent with the criterion.

6 340 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10: Moreover, regression analyses indicated that greater overestimation of scholastic competence was associated with more severe hyperactive/impulsive symptoms, but not more severe inattentive symptoms, suggesting that children in the IA group were not simply overlooking (e.g., failing to perceive due to attentional difficulties) their underperformance. Measurement, Methodological, and Statistical Issues Related to the PIB Over time, studies investigating the PIB in children with ADHD have made methodological improvements. Nonetheless, methodological and statistical limitations remain. In this section, we discuss challenges in assessing the PIB in children with ADHD, delineate current methodological and statistical approaches, and highlight the strengths and weaknesses of each approach. We conclude by making suggestions that may guide future inquiry in this area. Rater Bias in Competence Measurement As previously discussed, due to the methodological limitations of analyzing absolute self-perception scores (e.g., Gresham et al. 1998; Hoza et al. 2002) and as an attempt to better evaluate the accuracy of self-perceptions, researchers have examined self-perceptions in comparison to criteria that presumably provide more objective indices of children s actual competencies (Hoza et al. 2002). Many studies examining the PIB have relied on teacher report as a criterion against which to compare children s self-perceptions. One might argue that the overly inflated selfperceptions of children with ADHD are a result of rater bias on the part of the teacher providing the criterion rating. That is, teachers may provide overly negative evaluations given the difficulties that teachers experience with children with ADHD; consequently, the self-perceptions of children with ADHD would appear inflated. However, a recent study addressed this concern by obtaining ratings of competence from teachers, mothers, and fathers (Hoza et al. 2004). The PIB was found in children with ADHD across all raters, but was not evident for control children. Hoza et al. (2004) findings provide greater confirmation that the self-perceptions of children with ADHD are positive illusory and suggest that the PIB is not simply an artifact of rater bias on the part of the teacher. It is also possible that both parents and teachers perceptions could be negatively biased, leading to overestimation on the part of the child with ADHD. However, additional studies comparing children s self-perceptions to more objective criteria such as achievement scores or lab task performance also report positive illusions in ADHD samples (e.g., Ohan and Johnston 2002; Owens and Hoza 2003). These findings confirm that rater bias alone (whether on the part of a teacher or parent) is not an adequate explanation for these findings. Nonetheless, future research that incorporates objective criteria, including standardized tests, actual task performance, and competency estimates made by more objective raters (i.e., observers who are unaware of diagnostic status) would be helpful in confirming this point. Challenges to Difference Score Approaches To date, the majority of research investigating the PIB in children with ADHD employs statistical difference scores that index the discrepancy between children s perceptions of competence and actual competence. Studies investigating the PIB have utilized both unstandardized and standardized difference scores (e.g., Owens and Hoza 2003). We acknowledge that difference scores are not without limitations. Criticisms of the use of difference scores center around two primary themes. First, the reliability of a difference score typically is substantially lower than are the reliabilities of the variables employed to construct the discrepancy as a result of combined measurement error (Edwards 2001). The low reliability of difference scores then results in an increased likelihood of making a Type II error, or failure to detect in the sample the presence of a meaningful relationship between the discrepancy score and relevant outcome variables that exist in the population (Edwards 2001). Second, difference scores have a tendency to be strongly and systematically correlated with their components (e.g., Cronbach 1958; Zuckerman and Knee 1996). These correlations, then, may result in the detection of significant relations between difference scores and dependent variables that actually are reflective of a relation between one of the difference score s component variables and the dependent variables, rather than being indicative of any predictive ability of the discrepancy score itself (Cronbach 1958). For this reason, the use of difference scores may make it difficult to piece apart the meaning of statistically significant relationships. For example, Griffin et al. (1999) argue that correlations between difference scores and other variables may reflect any number of underlying patterns among component and outcome variables. Many researchers have further contended that difference scores are linearly related to their components, such that any difference score is no more than the sum of its constituent parts (Zuckerman and Knee 1996). Based on this argument, critics of the discrepancy construct have maintained that utilizing component variables is more parsimonious than using difference scores (Cronbach 1958).

7 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10: A more simplistic limitation of the difference score is that children with ADHD are mathematically more likely to overestimate their competence than are control children simply as a function of their lower levels of actual competence. In other words, due to the true impairments on the part of children with ADHD, the criterion scores (e.g., actual achievement scores) almost certainly will be much lower for children with ADHD than for control children. As a result, it is much easier for children with ADHD to overestimate their competence compared to control children (i.e., the potential gap is much larger for children with ADHD). Similarly, there may be a ceiling effect for control children such that they may not be able to mathematically overestimate their competence if their score on the competence criterion is already high. Despite these limitations of difference scores, there are several marked advantages to their use. Difference score advocates have addressed concerns about the validity of the discrepancy score construct by arguing for the importance of conceptual validity over that of statistical validity. Researchers endorsing this viewpoint contend that discrepancy scores represent constructs that are theoretically distinct from the constructs represented by the component variables (Tisak and Smith 1994a, b). Colvin et al. (1996) asserted that for this reason, construction of difference scores facilitates a different type of parsimony than that advocated by opponents of the difference score construct. They noted that difference scores provide researchers with the opportunity to investigate the concepts that might otherwise be awkward to assess, comprehend, and evaluate (Colvin et al. 1996, p. 1253). This certainly seems to be the case when examining the relative accuracy of children s self-perceptions. In addition, Tisak and Smith (1994a) contended that although difference scores as a category may be prone to low reliability, reliability should be evaluated on a case-bycase basis. Rogosa et al. (1984) noted that even difference scores with low reliability might still have utility. In fact, it has been argued that if difference scores do indeed possess low reliability, the effect of this reduced reliability will be to attenuate statistical correlations with other variables. Statistically significant results therefore would serve as conservative estimates of relations between difference scores and other variables (Colvin et al. 1996). Alternative Statistical Methodologies Given the challenges and limitations associated with the use of difference scores, alternative statistical methodologies warrant discussion and exploration. Other researchers have employed residual difference scores to investigate similar constructs. These scores are generated through regression analyses that employ one informant (i.e., the child) as a predictor of the other informant (i.e., teacher, parent, or objective criterion assessing actual competence). A residual difference score indexing the difference between the score predicted by the independent variable (child) and that provided by the dependent variable (criterion) is generated by the regression analysis, and this score typically is converted into a z-score (see Chi and Hinshaw 2002; De Los Reyes and Kazdin 2004). However, we note that the drawback of this statistical approach is that the best-fitting regression line divides the sample roughly in half, with approximately 50% of cases lying above the line and 50% lying below it. Hence, this approach will always result in a distribution of scores composed of approximately 50% underestimators and 50% overestimators. However, in samples for which this assumed equal distribution of overestimators and underestimators does not hold (for example, children with ADHD), residual difference scores consequently may misrepresent the data. A recent review (De Los Reyes and Kazdin 2004) examined the most commonly used methods for comparing informant ratings in child psychopathology; these authors recommended the use of standardized difference scores. More specifically, the authors evaluated three informant discrepancy methods, including unstandardized difference scores among two informants ratings, standardized difference scores among two informants ratings, and the residual difference between two informants ratings. The standardized difference score measure was the only method that was uniformly correlated with the ratings from which it was computed. More specifically, standardization of the component scores prior to computing the discrepancy associates the discrepancy score with both component parts, which alleviates concerns about construct validity. Future studies should investigate other analyses that may best evaluate the accuracy of self-perceptions while minimizing methodological limitations. Although there are limitations to using discrepancy analyses, the alternatives also have significant limitations. Thus, we concur with De Los Reyes and Kazdin (2004) in recommending that researchers employ standardized discrepancy scores when conducting evaluations of children s self-perceptions. Theoretical Explanations for the Positive Illusory Bias The PIB in the self-perceptions of children with ADHD has been well documented; however, the function and causes of this phenomenon are unclear. Hypothesized explanations for the PIB include cognitive immaturity (Milich 1994), neuropsychological deficits (Owens and Hoza 2003), ignorance of incompetence (Hoza et al. 2002), and selfprotection (Ohan and Johnston 2002). We acknowledge

8 342 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10: that these hypotheses may not be mutually exclusive. Indeed, because the mechanisms underlying the PIB are not yet well understood, and thus not yet well-articulated, there is overlap in the various constructs that past researchers have proposed to explain the function of the PIB. Given the existing data (or lack thereof), the goal of this section is not to provide definitive answers, but rather to provide a conceptual context for stimulating new ideas and advancing research regarding the underlying mechanisms of the PIB. The following discussion presents current empirical evidence supporting or disconfirming each of the possible explanations presented in the literature till date. Cognitive Immaturity Compared to older children, younger children tend to overestimate their skills in various academic tasks, and overestimate their future performance, and purportedly this overestimation serves as an adaptive purpose (Bjorklund and Green 1992). For example, by definition, young children experience frequent failure as they continuously encounter novel tasks. Young children s optimistic beliefs about their ability to succeed allow them to try new activities and persist on challenging tasks. Since children with ADHD often are characterized as being behaviorally and cognitively immature (Whalen 1989), it has been suggested that cognitive immaturity may explain their positive illusory self-perceptions (Milich 1994). That is, the PIB found in children with ADHD may be analogous to the overestimation of self-competence evident in young children because both types of children have immature cognitive functions. This hypothesis, thus, implies that children with ADHD, like the general population of young children, will outgrow this bias in cognitions. Direct empirical support for this hypothesis is quite limited (Milich 1994). Upto date, no study has directly examined the cognitive immaturity hypothesis in the positive illusory self-perceptions of children with ADHD. Moreover, no study has examined the presence of the PIB in a longitudinal sample or in a cross-sectional sample with ages that span early childhood to young adulthood. As such, the role of development in the expression of the PIB is largely unknown. In order to sufficiently examine the cognitive immaturity hypothesis, both longitudinal and cross-sectional studies are needed. Thus, conclusions about the cognitive immaturity hypothesis can only be drawn indirectly from extant data. For example, if the PIB in children with ADHD was analogous to the optimism of young children, such optimism would result in continued persistence during challenging tasks. In contrast, several studies suggest that optimistic self-perceptions in children with ADHD do not result in continued persistence (Hoza et al. 2001; Milich 1994). Instead, Milich (1994) characterized the performance behavior of children with ADHD as being consistent with learned helplessness, despite the presence of optimistic assessments of self-competence. This indirect evidence challenges the viability of cognitive immaturity as the best explanation for the PIB. Neuropsychological Deficits Neuroimaging studies suggest that there are four main neural regions associated with ADHD. These regions include the (a) prefrontal cortices, (b) basal ganglia, (c) cerebellum, and (d) corpus callosum (Nigg 2006). There is preliminary evidence that children with ADHD have abnormal patterns of brain activation during challenging tasks that require the use of these four structures. The four neural regions listed above all also are related to executive functioning, and not surprisingly, neuropsychological research suggests that children with ADHD demonstrate executive functioning deficits (e.g., Murphy et al. 2001; Swanson et al. 1998; Tannock 1998). Anosognosia is a neurologically based lack of awareness of personal errors (Stuss and Benson 1987) and impairments in self-awareness (Ownsworth et al. 2002; Starkstein et al. 2006) that is linked to frontal lobe damage and executive dysfunction. Patients who have frontal lobe damage and executive dysfunction are unable to selfmonitor and regulate their behavior (Stuss and Benson 1987) and often overestimate their abilities, despite maintaining the ability to evaluate the competence of their spouses (e.g., Duke et al. 2002; Kaszniak and Christensen 1995). Taken together, it is possible that the unique pattern of self-perceptions, self-monitoring, and self-awareness found in children with ADHD may be the result of some form of an anosognosia-like condition, stemming from executive dysfunction. If this were the case, consistent with research on anosognosia in adult patients (e.g., Duke et al. 2002), children with ADHD would have difficulty accurately evaluating their own competence, but would be capable of accurately evaluating the competence of others. Further, this pattern of competence evaluation would be more extreme in children with ADHD who demonstrate greater executive functioning deficits (as assessed by standardized tests of executive functioning) than those with less impairment in these areas. Unfortunately, no study has examined the relation between executive dysfunction and the PIB. Given the current state of knowledge, we cannot determine the relation among executive dysfunction, the PIB, and ADHD. However, the prominent role of executive dysfunction in the manifestation of ADHD makes this an area worthy of future research.

9 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10: Ignorance of Incompetence Hypothesis According to Kruger and Dunning (1999), individuals who are incompetent in a given domain suffer a dual burden: not only do they reach erroneous conclusions and make unfortunate choices, but their incompetence robs them of their ability to realize it (p. 1121). This phenomenon has been labeled ignorance of incompetence (Dunning et al. 2003; Kruger and Dunning 1999). Since children with ADHD demonstrate chronic and significant deficits (e.g., Barkley 1990; Pelham and Bender 1982; LeFever et al. 2002), it might be argued that these children are incompetent across multiple domains. As such, ignorance of incompetence may be a plausible explanation for the overly optimistic self-assessments of children with ADHD. If applied to children with ADHD, this hypothesis would suggest that overly inflated self-perceptions of children with ADHD might be a result of their inability to recognize their deficits precisely because they are incompetent and lack skills. For instance, a child with ADHD who lacks social skills will not be able to accurately evaluate why he or she could not maintain a friendship. However, in contrast to the executive function hypothesis (anosognosia) in which the child with ADHD would have difficulty accurately evaluating their own competence, but would be capable of accurately evaluating the competence of others, the ignorance of incompetence hypothesis implies that the child would be a poor evaluator of both self-competence and the competence of others. Upto date, no studies have directly tested the ignorance of incompetence hypothesis (i.e., replicated the methodology of Kruger and Dunning 1999) in children with ADHD. Nonetheless, we present some preliminary confirming and disconfirming evidence for this hypothesis as an explanation for the PIB in children with ADHD. In support of the ignorance of incompetence hypothesis, Hoza et al. (2004, 2002) have found that children with ADHD overestimate their competence the most in their domain(s) of greatest deficit. For example, children with ADHD and comorbid aggression tended to overestimate their competence the most in the social acceptance and behavioral conduct domains; whereas children with ADHD and comorbid low achievement tended to overestimate their competence the most in the scholastic competence domain. As such, Hoza et al. (2002) speculated that children with ADHD may not have the knowledge to evaluate competence if they are significantly impaired in a given domain (i.e., if these children do not have the skills necessary to succeed in the specified domain, they also do not possess the skills necessary to evaluate their competence in that domain). Conversely, in Owens and Hoza s (2003) study, HICB children were no more impaired in the academic domain (i.e., as assessed by standardized achievement tests) than were IA children; yet HICB children overestimated their academic self-competence significantly more than IA children. If the ignorance of incompetence hypothesis was applicable to the PIB in ADHD, it would be expected that IA children would also demonstrate inflated perceptions of scholastic competence because IA children were as equally impaired academically as HICB children. Further, performance estimates of children with ADHD have been shown to decrease following positive feedback in the social domain (Diener and Milich 1997; Ohan and Johnston 2002). The ignorance of incompetence hypothesis cannot explain why the performance estimates of children with ADHD become more aligned with actual performance after receiving positive feedback. Lastly, Evangelista et al. (2007) found that children with ADHD were accurate (i.e., not significantly different from control children) in evaluating other children s competence in success and failure situations in both academic and social domains; yet, children with ADHD overestimated their own competence relative to teachers perceptions across multiple domains significantly more than the control children did. These findings suggest that children with ADHD likely have the skills to evaluate the competence of others across multiple domains, even in domains in which they experience significant deficits. According to the ignorance of incompetence hypothesis, individuals who are incompetent in a given domain and lack the skills to evaluate competency in this domain that should not be able to evaluate the competency of themselves or others (Dunning et al. 2003). As such, Evangelista and colleagues (2007) study provides the most convincing evidence that disconfirms the ignorance of incompetence hypothesis as the most viable explanation for the PIB in children with ADHD. Future studies should replicate the methodology used by Kruger and Dunning (1999) to further explore this hypothesis. Self-protective Hypothesis The self-protective hypothesis posits that, when children with ADHD are threatened by a challenging task, they attempt to hide their incompetence and prevent feelings of failure or inadequacy by inflating reports of self-competence (Diener and Milich 1997). In other words, this hypothesis suggests that children with ADHD overestimate their competence as a coping mechanism that presents a confident front to others and allows children to protect their self-esteem. This explanation is consistent with Hoza et al. (2004, 2002) findings that children with ADHD overestimated their competence the most in the domain of greatest deficit. Evangelista et al. (2007) findings also provide

10 344 Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev (2007) 10: support for the self-protective hypothesis. Namely, children with ADHD inflated reports of their own competence, but not the competence of others (i.e., consistent with the idea that there is no need for self-protection when rating another person s competence). Taken together, these findings (Evangelista et al. 2007; Hoza et al. 2004, 2002) lend preliminary support to the self-protective hypothesis. Additional research that has directly tested the selfprotective hypothesis offers more substantive confirming evidence. For example, Diener and Milich (1997) tested the self-protective hypothesis in boys with ADHD within the social domain. Diener and Milich reasoned that boys with ADHD who receive positive feedback about their social performance would lower their performance estimates, as they would no longer feel the need to bolster their selfperceptions (i.e., self-protect) once positive feedback was received. Boys with ADHD and non-impaired control boys engaged in a social interaction task with an unfamiliar peer. Boys were randomly assigned to either a positive feedback or no feedback condition. Results supported the hypothesis such that boys with ADHD who received positive feedback lowered their self-perceptions of social performance, whereas boys with ADHD who received no feedback increased their self-perceptions of social performance. The opposite pattern was observed in non-impaired control boys. More specifically, after positive feedback, nonimpaired control boys increased their self-perceptions of social performance, whereas after no feedback, nonimpaired control boys decreased their self-perceptions of social performance. These results suggested that boys with ADHD do not feel the need to enhance their reports of selfcompetence once positive feedback is given (i.e., they have proven their competence and/or warded off feelings of inadequacy), lending support to the self-protective hypothesis. Ohan and Johnston (2002) expanded on the previous study s methodology by testing the self-protective hypothesis in boys with ADHD in both academic and social domains. Similar to Diener and Milich (1997), Ohan and Johnston hypothesized that boys with ADHD would lower their reports of self-competence after receiving positive feedback about their performance on a maze task with a confederate (i.e., teacher). Performance on the maze task represented the academic domain and the interaction with the confederate teacher represented performance in the social domain. Participants were assigned to positive feedback, no feedback, or average feedback conditions. Consistent with Diener and Milich (1997), results revealed that boys with ADHD lowered their self-perceptions of social performance following positive feedback; whereas boys without ADHD did not (Ohan and Johnston 2002). Conversely, there were no differences in self-perceptions of social performance between boys with and without ADHD who received average or no feedback. Unexpectedly, there was a different pattern of results for the academic domain. More specifically, both groups of boys increased their self-perceptions after receiving positive feedback. Thus, results provide additional support for the self-protective hypothesis in the social domain; however, such support was not replicated in the academic domain. It is possible that the mechanism behind the PIB in children with ADHD varies depending on the domain. In other words, children with ADHD may believe that social competence is more important than academic competence; as a result, they may feel the need to self-protect to a greater extent in the social domain. However, it is also possible that methodological limitations in the Ohan and Johnston (2002) study contributed to the inconsistency of findings across domains. For example, it is arguable that the academic task utilized in the study (i.e., the maze task) was not viewed by the children as an ecologically valid academic task. In addition, performance representing the academic and social domains was drawn from one situation (i.e., the student teacher interaction occurred in the context of maze completion), rather than from independent situations (i.e., an academic task that does not include a social interaction and vice-versa). This overlap may have created confounding effects, making it difficult to adequately assess the self-protective hypothesis in the academic domain. Despite the limitations of these studies (Diener and Milich 1997; Ohan and Johnston 2002), they offer insight into the mechanism that may be responsible for the PIB in children with ADHD. Upto date, the self-protective hypothesis has garnered more empirical support than any other explanation for the PIB in children with ADHD. Nonetheless, because inconsistencies (e.g., support is found in the social but not academic domain) and methodological limitations remain, additional investigation and extension to other domains of competence are warranted. In sum, the function of the PIB in children with ADHD is unclear. Cognitive immaturity, neuropsychological deficits, ignorance of incompetence, and self-protection are possible explanations; however, as discussed, most of these hypotheses have not been directly tested. Further, these hypotheses may not be mutually exclusive. For example, cognitive immaturity and ignorance of incompetence may be difficult to distinguish; in fact, these two hypotheses may compliment each other. That is, because children with ADHD may be cognitively immature, they may also be deemed incompetent in a given domain. At this time, the self-protective hypothesis has accrued the most empirical support and appears to provide the most viable explanation for the PIB. Additional research is needed to confirm or disconfirm the other hypotheses and elucidate the function of the PIB in children with ADHD.

THE POSITIVE ILLUSORY BIAS: AN EXAMINATION OF SELF-PERCEPTIONS IN ADULTS WITH ADHD SYMPTOMATOLOGY. A thesis presented to.

THE POSITIVE ILLUSORY BIAS: AN EXAMINATION OF SELF-PERCEPTIONS IN ADULTS WITH ADHD SYMPTOMATOLOGY. A thesis presented to. THE POSITIVE ILLUSORY BIAS: AN EXAMINATION OF SELF-PERCEPTIONS IN ADULTS WITH ADHD SYMPTOMATOLOGY A thesis presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In partial fulfillment

More information

Impact of Comorbidities on Self-Esteem of Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Impact of Comorbidities on Self-Esteem of Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder The International Journal of Indian Psychology ISSN 2348-5396 (e) ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) Volume 3, Issue 3, No.1, DIP: 18.01.011/20160303 ISBN: 978-1-365-03416-9 http://www.ijip.in April - June, 2016 Impact

More information

Accuracy of self-perceptions in children and adults with ADHD

Accuracy of self-perceptions in children and adults with ADHD Zurich Open Repository and Archive University of Zurich Main Library Strickhofstrasse 39 CH-8057 Zurich www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2011 Accuracy of self-perceptions in children and adults with ADHD Rizzo, P

More information

Emotional or Behavioral Disorders

Emotional or Behavioral Disorders Ekornås Self-Perception of Social Acceptance Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, Vol. 30, No. 6, 2011, pp. 570-582 Primary School Children s Peer Relationships: Discrepancies in Self-Perceived Social

More information

Functioning Hypothesis. A dissertation presented to. the faculty of. the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University. In partial fulfillment

Functioning Hypothesis. A dissertation presented to. the faculty of. the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University. In partial fulfillment The Positive Illusory Bias in Children with ADHD: An Examination of the Executive Functioning Hypothesis A dissertation presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In

More information

Asperger s Syndrome (AS)

Asperger s Syndrome (AS) Asperger s Syndrome (AS) It is a psychological disorder that falls under the umbrella of autism spectrum disorder The Effectiveness of a Model Program for Children with Asperger s Syndrome (Smith, Maguar,

More information

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Risk factors for the development and outcome of childhood psychopathology SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Chapter 147 In this chapter I present a summary of the results of the studies described in this thesis followed

More information

Positive Illusory Bias in Adolescents with ADHD: Prevalence, Stability, and Accuracy of Reporters

Positive Illusory Bias in Adolescents with ADHD: Prevalence, Stability, and Accuracy of Reporters Virginia Commonwealth University VCU Scholars Compass Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2016 Positive Illusory Bias in Adolescents with ADHD: Prevalence, Stability, and Accuracy of Reporters Elizaveta

More information

Negative Life Events, Self-Perceived Competence, and Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults

Negative Life Events, Self-Perceived Competence, and Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults Cogn Ther Res (2007) 31:773 783 DOI 10.1007/s10608-006-9101-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Negative Life Events, Self-Perceived Competence, and Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults Dorothy J. Uhrlass Æ Brandon E. Gibb

More information

Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury. Seth Warschausky, PhD Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation University of Michigan

Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury. Seth Warschausky, PhD Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation University of Michigan Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury Seth Warschausky, PhD Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation University of Michigan Modules Module 1: Overview Module 2: Cognitive and Academic Needs Module

More information

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity Measurement & Variables - Initial step is to conceptualize and clarify the concepts embedded in a hypothesis or research question with

More information

When the Evidence Says, Yes, No, and Maybe So

When the Evidence Says, Yes, No, and Maybe So CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE When the Evidence Says, Yes, No, and Maybe So Attending to and Interpreting Inconsistent Findings Among Evidence-Based Interventions Yale University ABSTRACT

More information

Testing Hypotheses Regarding the Causes of Comorbidity: Examining the Underlying Deficits of Comorbid Disorders

Testing Hypotheses Regarding the Causes of Comorbidity: Examining the Underlying Deficits of Comorbid Disorders Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 114, No. 3, 346 362 0021-843X/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.346 Testing Hypotheses Regarding

More information

The Role of Executive Functions in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Learning Disabilities

The Role of Executive Functions in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Learning Disabilities Journal April 2000 Volume 10, No. 2 (Reprinted with permission of Editor) Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) appears to be a disorder of self-control or executive functions. The executive

More information

EFFECTS OF ADHD ON EARLY LEARNING AND ACDEMIC PERFORMANCE 1

EFFECTS OF ADHD ON EARLY LEARNING AND ACDEMIC PERFORMANCE 1 EFFECTS OF ADHD ON EARLY LEARNING AND ACDEMIC PERFORMANCE 1 The Effects of ADHD on Learning and Academic Performance in the Pre- and Elementary School Years. Christopher Kalogeropoulos November 19, 2012

More information

Peer rejection is pervasive among children with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity

Peer rejection is pervasive among children with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity Development and Psychopathology 22 (2010), 785 802 # Cambridge University Press 2010 doi:10.1017/s0954579410000465 Developmental processes in peer problems of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

More information

Wonder Girls to Wonder Women: Lessons Learned from a Longitudinal Study of Girls with ADHD

Wonder Girls to Wonder Women: Lessons Learned from a Longitudinal Study of Girls with ADHD Wonder Girls to Wonder Women: Lessons Learned from a Longitudinal Study of Girls with ADHD Christine Zalecki, Ph.D. Associate Clinical Professor, UC San Francisco Director, Berkeley Girls with ADHD Longitudinal

More information

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT Study Focus Empathy s impact on: O Human Interactions O Consequences of those interactions O Empathy Development Study Purpose to examine the empathetic

More information

Running Head: THE INTERPLAY BETWEEN INSOMNIA AND DEPRESSION 1

Running Head: THE INTERPLAY BETWEEN INSOMNIA AND DEPRESSION 1 Running Head: THE INTERPLAY BETWEEN INSOMNIA AND DEPRESSION 1 The Interplay Between Insomnia and Depression Parker A. Dreves East Tennessee State University 2 Depression and insomnia are two psychiatric

More information

Regression Discontinuity Analysis

Regression Discontinuity Analysis Regression Discontinuity Analysis A researcher wants to determine whether tutoring underachieving middle school students improves their math grades. Another wonders whether providing financial aid to low-income

More information

SIBLINGS OF CHILDREN WITH INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY 1

SIBLINGS OF CHILDREN WITH INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY 1 SIBLINGS OF CHILDREN WITH INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY 1 Development of Siblings of Children with Intellectual Disability Brendan Hendrick University of North Carolina Chapel Hill 3/23/15 SIBLINGS OF CHILDREN

More information

Author's response to reviews

Author's response to reviews Author's response to reviews Title:Mental health problems in the 10th grade and non-completion of upper secondary school: the mediating role of grades in a population-based longitudinal study Authors:

More information

Assessment of ADHD and Co-Morbidities

Assessment of ADHD and Co-Morbidities Assessment of ADHD and Co-Morbidities Sam Goldstein Ph.D University of Utah www.samgoldstein.com info@samgoldstein.com The Mindset of the Effective Evaluator Evaluate for intervention. Appreciate and stay

More information

QUT Digital Repository:

QUT Digital Repository: QUT Digital Repository: http://eprints.qut.edu.au/ Dray, Stacey M. and Campbell, Marilyn A. and Gilmore, Linda A. (2006) Why are girls with ADHD invisible?. In Connections, 23(2) pages pp. 2-7, Queensland

More information

Title: The Relationship between Locus of Control and Academic Level and Sex of Secondary School Students

Title: The Relationship between Locus of Control and Academic Level and Sex of Secondary School Students Title: The Relationship between Locus of Control and Academic Level and Sex of Secondary School Students Author: Marvin W. Ross, University of Ottawa and Maurice C. Taylor, Algonquin College Complete text:

More information

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders: Are Children Being Overmedicated?

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders: Are Children Being Overmedicated? U.S. Department of Health and Human Services National Institutes of Health National Institute of Mental Health September 26, 2002 NOTE TO WRITERS AND EDITORS: Dr. Richard Nakamura, Acting Director of the

More information

Piers Harris Children s Self-Concept Scale, Second Edition (Piers-Harris 2)

Piers Harris Children s Self-Concept Scale, Second Edition (Piers-Harris 2) Piers Harris Children s Self-Concept Scale, Second Edition (Piers-Harris 2) WPS TEST REPORT Copyright 2002 by Western Psychological Services 12031 Wilshire Blvd., Los Angeles, California 90025-1251 Version

More information

ADHD and Comorbid Conditions A Conceptual Model

ADHD and Comorbid Conditions A Conceptual Model ADHD and Comorbid Conditions A Conceptual Model Thomas E. Brown PhD. Assistant Clinical Professor of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine and Associate Director of the Yale Clinic for Attention

More information

The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories

The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories Wai Keen Vong (waikeen.vong@adelaide.edu.au) Andrew T. Hendrickson (drew.hendrickson@adelaide.edu.au) Amy Perfors (amy.perfors@adelaide.edu.au)

More information

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES Correlational Research Correlational Designs Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are

More information

WORKING WITH ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER (ADHD) OPPOSITIONAL DEFIANT DISORDER CONDUCT DISORDER

WORKING WITH ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER (ADHD) OPPOSITIONAL DEFIANT DISORDER CONDUCT DISORDER COURSES ARTICLE - THERAPYTOOLS.US WORKING WITH ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER (ADHD) OPPOSITIONAL DEFIANT DISORDER CONDUCT DISORDER WORKING WITH ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER (ADHD)

More information

Recent advances in the study of biological alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD)

Recent advances in the study of biological alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) Psychiatr Clin N Am 25 (2002) xi xv Preface Recent advances in the study of biological alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) Rachel Yehuda, PhD Guest Editor This issue of The Psychiatric

More information

peer relations; psychological adjustment; context; methodology; adolescent development

peer relations; psychological adjustment; context; methodology; adolescent development Contextual Variations in Negative Mood and State Self-Esteem What Role Do Peers Play? Bridget M. Reynolds Rena L. Repetti University of California, Los Angeles Journal of Early Adolescence Volume 28 Number

More information

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H PSYC H O PAT H O LO G Y : PSYC H I AT R I C P R O B LEMS A N D T H E A S SO C I

More information

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H

More information

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS CHAPTER 5 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS 5.1. Summary Childhood cognitive and behavioral problems categorized as disorders of attention, impulsiveness and hyperactivity, present a challenge for the therapist.

More information

A Longitudinal Study of the Achievements Progress and Attitudes of Severely Inattentive, Hyperactive and Impulsive Young Children

A Longitudinal Study of the Achievements Progress and Attitudes of Severely Inattentive, Hyperactive and Impulsive Young Children A Longitudinal Study of the Achievements Progress and Attitudes of Severely Inattentive, Hyperactive and Impulsive Young Children Christine Merrell and Peter Tymms, CEM Centre, Durham University. Contact:

More information

Module 2/3 Research Strategies: How Psychologists Ask and Answer Questions

Module 2/3 Research Strategies: How Psychologists Ask and Answer Questions Myers PSYCHOLOGY 8 th Edition in Modules Module 2/3 Research Strategies: How Psychologists Ask and Answer Questions 1 The Need for Psychological Science Psychologists, like all scientists, use the scientific

More information

Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz

Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz This study presents the steps Edgenuity uses to evaluate the reliability and validity of its quizzes, topic tests, and cumulative

More information

Child and Parent Predictors of Perceptions of Parent Child Relationship Quality

Child and Parent Predictors of Perceptions of Parent Child Relationship Quality Journal of Attention Disorders Vol. 11(1):37-48 (2007) ISSN: 1087-0547 doi: 10.1177/1087054706295664 This is a peer reviewed pre-print version of the following article: Child and Parent Predictors of Perceptions

More information

A Clinical Translation of the Research Article Titled Antisocial Behavioral Syndromes and. Additional Psychiatric Comorbidity in Posttraumatic Stress

A Clinical Translation of the Research Article Titled Antisocial Behavioral Syndromes and. Additional Psychiatric Comorbidity in Posttraumatic Stress 1 A Clinical Translation of the Research Article Titled Antisocial Behavioral Syndromes and Additional Psychiatric Comorbidity in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder among US Adults: Results from Wave 2 of the

More information

Chapter 4. Outsiders and Defenders Bullying-related Behavioral Reputation Versus. Daily Self-reported Provictim Intervention Behavior

Chapter 4. Outsiders and Defenders Bullying-related Behavioral Reputation Versus. Daily Self-reported Provictim Intervention Behavior Outsiders and Defenders Bullying-related Behavioral Reputation Versus Daily Self-reported Provictim Intervention Behavior Submitted for Publication as: Pronk, J., Nishina, A., Olthof, T., & Goossens, F.

More information

Development of a New Fear of Hypoglycemia Scale: Preliminary Results

Development of a New Fear of Hypoglycemia Scale: Preliminary Results Development of a New Fear of Hypoglycemia Scale: Preliminary Results Jodi L. Kamps, 1 PHD, Michael C. Roberts, 2 PHD, ABPP, and R. Enrique Varela, 3 PHD 1 Children s Hospital of New Orleans, 2 University

More information

Media Campaigns and Perceptions of Reality

Media Campaigns and Perceptions of Reality 2820 Media Campaigns and Perceptions of Reality Media Campaigns and Perceptions of Reality Rajiv N. Rimal Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health Humans act, at least partly, on the basis of how

More information

T. Rene Jamison * and Jessica Oeth Schuttler

T. Rene Jamison * and Jessica Oeth Schuttler Jamison and Schuttler Molecular Autism (2015) 6:53 DOI 10.1186/s13229-015-0044-x RESEARCH Open Access Examining social competence, self-perception, quality of life, and internalizing and externalizing

More information

Chapter 3. Psychometric Properties

Chapter 3. Psychometric Properties Chapter 3 Psychometric Properties Reliability The reliability of an assessment tool like the DECA-C is defined as, the consistency of scores obtained by the same person when reexamined with the same test

More information

Ambiguous Data Result in Ambiguous Conclusions: A Reply to Charles T. Tart

Ambiguous Data Result in Ambiguous Conclusions: A Reply to Charles T. Tart Other Methodology Articles Ambiguous Data Result in Ambiguous Conclusions: A Reply to Charles T. Tart J. E. KENNEDY 1 (Original publication and copyright: Journal of the American Society for Psychical

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences

Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences Intelligence and reasoning are not one and the same Ira A. Noveck and Jérôme Prado Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences Cognitives, CNRS-Université de Lyon,

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Jónsdóttir, S. (2006). ADHD and its relationship to comorbidity and gender. s.n.

Citation for published version (APA): Jónsdóttir, S. (2006). ADHD and its relationship to comorbidity and gender. s.n. University of Groningen ADHD and its relationship to comorbidity and gender Jónsdóttir, Sólveig IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite

More information

McCloskey Executive Functions Scale (MEFS) Report

McCloskey Executive Functions Scale (MEFS) Report McCloskey Executive Functions Scale (MEFS) Report Student's Name: Amy Date of Birth: Gender: Female Date of Ratings: Grade: 7 Age: 3 years, month School: Middle Rater s Name: Teacher Executive Functions

More information

MCAS Equating Research Report: An Investigation of FCIP-1, FCIP-2, and Stocking and. Lord Equating Methods 1,2

MCAS Equating Research Report: An Investigation of FCIP-1, FCIP-2, and Stocking and. Lord Equating Methods 1,2 MCAS Equating Research Report: An Investigation of FCIP-1, FCIP-2, and Stocking and Lord Equating Methods 1,2 Lisa A. Keller, Ronald K. Hambleton, Pauline Parker, Jenna Copella University of Massachusetts

More information

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) 1 VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) Thank you for providing us with the opportunity to revise our paper. We have revised the manuscript according to the editors and

More information

SAMPLE. Conners 3 Self-Report Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

SAMPLE. Conners 3 Self-Report Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. Conners 3 Self-Report Assessment Report This Assessment report is intended for use by qualified assessors only, and is not to be shown or presented to the respondent or any other

More information

Learning with Rare Cases and Small Disjuncts

Learning with Rare Cases and Small Disjuncts Appears in Proceedings of the 12 th International Conference on Machine Learning, Morgan Kaufmann, 1995, 558-565. Learning with Rare Cases and Small Disjuncts Gary M. Weiss Rutgers University/AT&T Bell

More information

ADHD in the Classroom

ADHD in the Classroom ADHD in the Classroom What Educators Need and How to Make it Happen Sam Goldstein, Ph.D. Assistant Clinical Professor University of Utah School of Medicine Clinical Director Neurology, Learning and Behavior

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

Correlations Among Symptoms of ADHD and Peer Relationships, Academic Performance, and Self-Image

Correlations Among Symptoms of ADHD and Peer Relationships, Academic Performance, and Self-Image CORRELATIONS AMONG SYMPTOMS 1 Professional Issues in Counseling 2007, Volume 7, Article 1, p. 1-6 Correlations Among Symptoms of ADHD and Peer Relationships, Academic Performance, and Self-Image Correlations

More information

Alternative Explanations for Changes in Similarity Judgments and MDS Structure

Alternative Explanations for Changes in Similarity Judgments and MDS Structure Cornell University School of Hotel Administration The Scholarly Commons Articles and Chapters School of Hotel Administration Collection 8-1990 Alternative Explanations for Changes in Similarity Judgments

More information

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing? Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC How are we doing? Prepared by Maura McCool, M.S. Office of Research, Evaluation and Assessment Metropolitan Community Colleges Fall 2003 1 General Education Assessment

More information

CHAPTER LEARNING OUTCOMES

CHAPTER LEARNING OUTCOMES EXPERIIMENTAL METHODOLOGY CHAPTER LEARNING OUTCOMES When you have completed reading this article you will be able to: Define what is an experiment Explain the role of theory in educational research Justify

More information

Child Planning: A Treatment Approach for Children with Oppositional Disorder

Child Planning: A Treatment Approach for Children with Oppositional Disorder COURSES ARTICLE - THERAPYTOOLS.US Child Planning: A Treatment Approach for Children with Oppositional Disorder A Treatment Approach for Children with Oppositional Disorder. Duration: 3 hours Learning Objectives:

More information

Correlation between Emotional Competence and Behavioral Problems in Elementary School Students with ADHD

Correlation between Emotional Competence and Behavioral Problems in Elementary School Students with ADHD Correlation between Emotional Competence and Behavioral Problems in Elementary School Students with ADHD Chihlan Chang, National Kaohsiung Normal University, Taiwan ShiSen Shyu, National Kaohsiung Normal

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information

1983, NUMBER 4 (WINTER 1983) THOMAS H. OLLENDICK, DONNA DAILEY, AND EDWARD S. SHAPIRO

1983, NUMBER 4 (WINTER 1983) THOMAS H. OLLENDICK, DONNA DAILEY, AND EDWARD S. SHAPIRO JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1983, 16. 485-491 NUMBER 4 (WINTER 1983) VICARIOUS REINFORCEMENT: EXPECTED AND UNEXPECTED EFFECTS THOMAS H. OLLENDICK, DONNA DAILEY, AND EDWARD S. SHAPIRO VIRGINIA

More information

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder How to manage these disorganized and inattentive children.

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder How to manage these disorganized and inattentive children. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder How to manage these disorganized and inattentive children. One of the leading authorities on Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, Russell Barkley, PhD., defines

More information

(CORRELATIONAL DESIGN AND COMPARATIVE DESIGN)

(CORRELATIONAL DESIGN AND COMPARATIVE DESIGN) UNIT 4 OTHER DESIGNS (CORRELATIONAL DESIGN AND COMPARATIVE DESIGN) Quasi Experimental Design Structure 4.0 Introduction 4.1 Objectives 4.2 Definition of Correlational Research Design 4.3 Types of Correlational

More information

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1 INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1 5.1 Clinical Interviews: Background Information The clinical interview is a technique pioneered by Jean Piaget, in 1975,

More information

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework Modified from Organizing Scientific Thinking Using the QuALMRI Framework Written by Kevin Ochsner and modified by others. Based on a scheme devised by

More information

Understanding Students with Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder

Understanding Students with Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder Understanding Students with Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder At the end of this presentation you should be able to: Define and identify the characteristics of students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

More information

EPSE 594: Meta-Analysis: Quantitative Research Synthesis

EPSE 594: Meta-Analysis: Quantitative Research Synthesis EPSE 594: Meta-Analysis: Quantitative Research Synthesis Ed Kroc University of British Columbia ed.kroc@ubc.ca March 14, 2019 Ed Kroc (UBC) EPSE 594 March 14, 2019 1 / 39 Last Time Synthesizing multiple

More information

A Study Looking at How Youth s Self- Perception is Affected by Their Temperament

A Study Looking at How Youth s Self- Perception is Affected by Their Temperament East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University Undergraduate Honors Theses Student Works 5-2017 A Study Looking at How Youth s Self- Perception is Affected by Their Temperament

More information

The Brain on ADHD. Ms. Komas. Introduction to Healthcare Careers

The Brain on ADHD. Ms. Komas. Introduction to Healthcare Careers The Brain on ADHD Ms. Komas Introduction to Healthcare Careers Ms. Komas Period 9/2/2016 Komas 1 HOOK: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) plagues between 5% and 7% of children and less than

More information

The Significance of Empirical Reports in the Field of Animal Science

The Significance of Empirical Reports in the Field of Animal Science The Significance of Empirical Reports in the Field of Animal Science Addison Cheng ABSTRACT Empirical reports published in Animal Science journals have many unique features that may be reflective of the

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing This document contains 4 examples of writing and feedback comments from Level 2A lab reports, and 4 steps to help you apply

More information

Context of Best Subset Regression

Context of Best Subset Regression Estimation of the Squared Cross-Validity Coefficient in the Context of Best Subset Regression Eugene Kennedy South Carolina Department of Education A monte carlo study was conducted to examine the performance

More information

The relationship between self-esteem, and problem behaviour, social and academic competence

The relationship between self-esteem, and problem behaviour, social and academic competence 1 The relationship between self-esteem, and problem behaviour, social and academic competence Introduction. The data that are presented in this paper is a part of an extensive survey to 2164 students in

More information

Gender Sensitive Factors in Girls Delinquency

Gender Sensitive Factors in Girls Delinquency Gender Sensitive Factors in Girls Delinquency Diana Fishbein, Ph.D. Research Triangle Institute Transdisciplinary Behavioral Science Program Shari Miller-Johnson, Ph.D. Duke University Center for Child

More information

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE skills assessment: technical report

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE skills assessment: technical report OnlineAssessments EISA EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE skills assessment: technical report [ Abridged Derek Mann ] To accompany the Emotional Intelligence Skills Assessment (EISA) by Steven J. Stein, Derek Mann,

More information

Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1. Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him. Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2

Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1. Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him. Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2 Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1 Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2 1 University of California, Berkeley 2 Cornell University Author Note

More information

March 10, Group 1 presentation 2. Personality and emotional development, and Gender (pwr. pnt. slides from Mar. 3, 2010) 4.

March 10, Group 1 presentation 2. Personality and emotional development, and Gender (pwr. pnt. slides from Mar. 3, 2010) 4. March 10,2010 1. Group 1 presentation 2. Personality and emotional development, and Gender (pwr. pnt. slides from Mar. 3, 2010) 3. Self-regulation 4. ADHD-DVD The Development of Self-regulation Self-regulation

More information

Academic Achievement Over 8 Years Among Children Who Met Modified Criteria for Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder at 4 6 Years of Age

Academic Achievement Over 8 Years Among Children Who Met Modified Criteria for Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder at 4 6 Years of Age J Abnorm Child Psychol (2008) 36:399 410 DOI 10.1007/s10802-007-9186-4 Academic Achievement Over 8 Years Among Children Who Met Modified Criteria for Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder at 4 6 Years

More information

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are highly extraverted people less afraid of rejection

More information

Module 2: Types of Groups Used in Substance Abuse Treatment. Based on material in Chapter 2 of TIP 41, Substance Abuse Treatment: Group Therapy

Module 2: Types of Groups Used in Substance Abuse Treatment. Based on material in Chapter 2 of TIP 41, Substance Abuse Treatment: Group Therapy Module 2: Types of Groups Used in Substance Abuse Treatment Based on material in Chapter 2 of TIP 41, Substance Abuse Treatment: Group Therapy U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES Substance Abuse

More information

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module 2090 The PedsQL in Pediatric Cancer Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module James W. Varni, Ph.D. 1,2

More information

Letter of Intent. Understanding the Chaos: What Counsellors Need to Know About Adult Attention Deficit. Channone Danychuk

Letter of Intent. Understanding the Chaos: What Counsellors Need to Know About Adult Attention Deficit. Channone Danychuk Running head: ADULT ATTENTION DEFICIT Letter of Intent Understanding the Chaos: What Counsellors Need to Know About Adult Attention Deficit Channone Danychuk Campus Alberta Master of Counselling Program

More information

In this chapter we discuss validity issues for quantitative research and for qualitative research.

In this chapter we discuss validity issues for quantitative research and for qualitative research. Chapter 8 Validity of Research Results (Reminder: Don t forget to utilize the concept maps and study questions as you study this and the other chapters.) In this chapter we discuss validity issues for

More information

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Honors Scholar Theses Honors Scholar Program Spring 5-1-2008 Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible Juemei Yang

More information

SITUATIONAL STRUCTURE AND INDIVIDUAL SELF-ESTEEM AS DETERMINANTS OF THREAT-ORIENTED REACTIONS TO POWER. Arthur R» Cohen

SITUATIONAL STRUCTURE AND INDIVIDUAL SELF-ESTEEM AS DETERMINANTS OF THREAT-ORIENTED REACTIONS TO POWER. Arthur R» Cohen SITUATIONAL STRUCTURE AND INDIVIDUAL SELF-ESTEEM AS DETERMINANTS OF THREAT-ORIENTED REACTIONS TO POWER by Arthur R» Cohen To be presented at the meetings of the American Psychological Association Cleveland,

More information

Externalizing Disorders

Externalizing Disorders Externalizing Disorders Psychology 311 Abnormal Psychology Listen to the audio lecture while viewing these slides 1 Background Many types ADHD Oppositional Defiant Disorder Tourette Disorder Others Includes

More information

Diagnostic tests for autism may miss many girls

Diagnostic tests for autism may miss many girls NEWS Diagnostic tests for autism may miss many girls BY DEBORAH RUDACILLE 27 JUNE 2011 1 / 8 2 / 8 3 / 8 4 / 8 Lonely child: Girls with Asperger syndrome show an interest in socializing, but have difficulty

More information

Russell Barkley, PhD. Internationally recognized authority on ADHD in children and adults

Russell Barkley, PhD. Internationally recognized authority on ADHD in children and adults Russell Barkley, PhD Internationally recognized authority on ADHD in children and adults Dr. Barkley has created a straightforward guide of best practices all educators should use with youth with ADHD.

More information

Depressive disorders in young people: what is going on and what can we do about it? Lecture 1

Depressive disorders in young people: what is going on and what can we do about it? Lecture 1 Depressive disorders in young people: what is going on and what can we do about it? Lecture 1 Professor Alasdair Vance Head, Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics University of Melbourne

More information

Originally appeared in Autism Spectrum News Exploring Educational Challenges Issue Vol. 5, No. 3 Winter Lynda Geller, Ph.D.

Originally appeared in Autism Spectrum News Exploring Educational Challenges Issue Vol. 5, No. 3 Winter Lynda Geller, Ph.D. Evidence-Based Interventions for Students on the Autism Spectrum Originally appeared in Autism Spectrum News Exploring Educational Challenges Issue Vol. 5, No. 3 Winter 2013 Lynda Geller, Ph.D. Spectrum

More information

ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, PHYSICAL HEALTH, AND LIFESTYLE IN OLDER ADULTS

ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, PHYSICAL HEALTH, AND LIFESTYLE IN OLDER ADULTS CHAPTER 5 ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, PHYSICAL HEALTH, AND LIFESTYLE IN OLDER ADULTS J. AM. GERIATR. SOC. 2013;61(6):882 887 DOI: 10.1111/JGS.12261 61 ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER,

More information

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Children and Adults Advances in Psychotherapy

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Children and Adults Advances in Psychotherapy Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Children and Adults Advances in Psychotherapy Questions from chapter 1 1) For a diagnosis of ADHD, some symptoms that cause impairment must be present before

More information

Implicit Theories Relate to Youth Psychopathology, But How? A Longitudinal Test of Two Predictive Models

Implicit Theories Relate to Youth Psychopathology, But How? A Longitudinal Test of Two Predictive Models Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2016) 47:603 617 DOI 10.1007/s10578-015-0595-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Implicit Theories Relate to Youth Psychopathology, But How? A Longitudinal Test of Two Predictive Models Jessica

More information

Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorders (ADHD) definition

Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorders (ADHD) definition Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorders (ADHD) definition: a pervasive pattern of inattention, impulsivity, and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity that is more frequent and severe than is typically observed

More information