Child Development, November/December 2001, Volume 72, Number 6, Pages

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Child Development, November/December 2001, Volume 72, Number 6, Pages"

Transcription

1 Child Development, November/December 2001, Volume 72, Number 6, Pages Associations between Phenylalanine-to-Tyrosine Ratios and Performance on Tests of Neuropsychological Function in Adolescents Treated Early and Continuously for Phenylketonuria Monica Luciana, Jill Sullivan, and Charles A. Nelson Phenylketonuria (PKU) is a genetic disorder characterized by hyperphenylalaninemia. Treatment involves dietary phenylalanine restriction to prevent mental retardation. Because phenylalanine is involved in tyrosine synthesis and tyrosine is a catecholamine precursor, low tyrosine may lead to brain dopamine deficiencies. Because dopamine is involved in the modulation of prefrontally orchestrated executive functions, deficiencies may lead to executive impairments. Despite treatment, impairments in executive cognitive functions have been reported in young children with PKU. Outcome beyond middle childhood has not been extensively investigated. In this study, PKU-affected adolescents (N 18) with normal-range IQ scores completed neuropsychological tests, and their performance was compared with unaffected peers (N 16) and chronically ill controls (N 17). Results demonstrated that the overall performance of the PKU group did not differ from that of the other two groups, but that performance of the PKU proband was associated with phenylalanine and tyrosine levels, and most strongly with phenylalanine-to-tyrosine ratios at several points in development. These findings provide a preliminary test of the dopamine hypothesis of PKU as it applies to adolescents and young adults. INTRODUCTION Phenylketonuria (PKU) is an inborn disorder of metabolism affecting approximately 1 in 10,000 to 20,000 live births (Benson & Fensom, 1985). PKU is caused by a limited ability to metabolize the amino acid phenylalanine due to the absence or inactivity of the enzyme phenylalanine hydroxylase (PAH). Left untreated, affected individuals experience a rise in serum phenylalanine and subsequent mental retardation. The biochemical mechanisms that explain why high phenylalanine levels result in mental retardation have not been definitively established. However, under normal circumstances, phenylalanine, through the action of PAH, is converted to tyrosine, the precursor enzyme for the production of dopamine and norepinephrine in the central nervous system (McKean & Peterson, 1970). Treated individuals are typically maintained on a diet in which phenylalanine is restricted, but not necessarily entirely eliminated (due to the body s need for protein), and tyrosine is supplemented. Nonetheless, a state of brain tyrosine (and dopamine) depletion is still possible due to (1) the shared competitive mechanism through which both phenylalanine and tyrosine cross the blood brain barrier, and (2) the differential sensitivity of regionally specific brain neurons to modest decreases in tyrosine availability (for a thorough review, see Diamond, Prevor, Callender, & Druin, 1997). Thus, even with treatment, phenylalanine levels in PKU may be elevated in the context of low tyrosine levels. It has been suggested (Diamond et al., 1997) that because of this biochemical state, characterized by moderately high phenylalanine levels in the context of low tyrosine levels, one of the best indicators of dopamine availability in PKU is the phenylalanine-to-tyrosine ratio (Phe:Tyr). This ratio is not typically examined in studies of PKU. Dopamine neurons in the prefrontal cortex and retina, unlike those in other regions, are particularly sensitive to decreased tyrosine availability (Bradberry, Karasic, Deutch, & Roth, 1989; Diamond & Herzberg, 1996; Tam, Elsworth, Bradberry, & Roth, 1990). Reduced levels of the precursor can theoretically occur either when PKU is left untreated, because phenylalanine cannot be converted to tyrosine through PAH activity, or when PKU is treated, if the ratio of phenylalanine to supplemented dietary tyrosine is in the moderate to high range. Under either condition, disruptions in prefrontally mediated behaviors are possible, although specific prefrontal dysfunction is most likely when phenylalanine levels are moderately, but not excessively, elevated. Diffuse damage, extending beyond prefrontal cortex, is more likely when phe by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. All rights reserved /2001/

2 1638 Child Development nylalanine levels are excessively high, as in untreated PKU (Diamond et al., 1997). Definitive evidence of central nervous system dopamine deficiency in PKU has not been demonstrated, although there is limited evidence of reduced tyrosine concentrations in the central nervous systems of phenylketonurics (McKean & Petersen, 1970). Additionally, aberrant levels of dopamine precursors, metabolites, and reduced norepinephrine in the plasma, urine, and cerebrospinal fluid of individuals who have discontinued dietary treatment have been reported (Güttler & Lou, 1986). An inverse relationship between plasma phenylalanine and urinary dopamine excretion also has been found (Krause et al., 1985). In laboratory animals, when dopamine is depleted prenatally through tyrosine depletion or neurochemical lesions, animals exhibit decreased responding to dopamine-dependent rewarding stimuli, as well as impaired acquisition of operant responding (Moy, 1995; Palmour, Ervin, Baker, & Young, 1998; Yokogoshi & Nomura, 1991). Supersensitivity to dopaminergic agents has also been observed (Owasoyo, Neri, & Lamberth, 1992). Moreover, when tyrosine or other dopamine agonists are administered prenatally to pregnant females, their offspring show lifetime alterations in response to dopaminergic agents (Nasello & Ramirez, 1978; Santana, Martin, & Rodrigues, 1994). Acute tyrosine depletions in healthy adult humans have been associated with increases in arousal, depression, and anxiety (McCann et al., 1995), as well as impaired cognitive function (McCann et al., 1992) and decreased responses to the rewarding effects of alcohol (Rammsayer & Vogel, 1995). Alternatively, under stressful conditions, tyrosine administration improves learning and working memory performance in animals (Ahlers, Salander, Shurtleff, & Thomas, 1992; Shukitt-Hale, Stillman, & Lieberman, 1996; Shurtleff, Thomas, Ahlers, & Schrot, 1993) and in humans (Deijen & Orlebeke, 1994; Shurtleff, Thomas, & Schrot, 1994). Although experimental studies of tyrosine depletion have yet to result in an animal analogue of human mental retardation, it is well established that dopamine modulates cognitive functions, particularly those mediated by the prefrontal cortex. Specifically, dopamine in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex facilitates spatial working memory performance (Diamond, 1996; Luciana & Collins, 1997; Sawaguchi & Goldman-Rakic, 1991). Nonhuman primates with depleted prefrontal dopamine exemplify impairments in working memory, and dopamine augmentation under acute conditions improves performance (Brozoski, Brown, Rosvold, & Goldman, 1979). Thus, although mental retardation as a consequence of PKU has been successfully averted through dietary intervention (Williamson, Koch, Azen, & Chang, 1981), it has yet to be determined whether phenylketonurics exemplify dopamine-dependent cognitive deficits. Although language, memory, and basic motor skills appear to be normal, several researchers have reported deficits in reaction times, attention, problem solving, abstract reasoning, and executive abilities in affected groups of all ages (Waisbren, Brown, de Sonneville, & Levy, 1994). Executive functions involve the ability to maintain a problem-solving set that is appropriate for goal acquisition, including formulation, planning, self-monitoring, mental flexibility, and the ability to change strategies in response to new information. Tests of executive functioning typically require information to be held in mind while other pieces of information are simultaneously processed (e.g., multitasking or working memory). Several studies have reported that infants and young children with early treated PKU show deficits in working memory and other high-level reasoning skills, despite the fact that these children have normal-range IQs (Diamond et al., 1997; Pennington, van Doorninck, McCabe, & McCabe, 1985; Spreen, Tupper, & Risser, 1984; Welsh, Pennington, Ozonoff, Rouse, & McCabe, 1990). Outcome in young adults treated since birth has not been reliably ascertained, because findings have been inconsistent in studies of older patients. Mazzocco et al. (1994) studied PKU probands, ages 6 to 13 years, and found no evidence of prefrontal dysfunction, suggesting that the cognitive deficits observed in younger children represent a developmental delay. In contrast, Weglage, Pietsch, Fünders, Koch, and Ullrich (1996) found evidence of dysfunction in PKU subjects aged 8 to 13 years on the Stroop test, a conventionally used executive measure of attention and inhibitory control. Ris and colleagues (Ris, Williams, Hunt, Berry, & Leslie, 1994) compared the neuropsychological performance of adults who had PKU with that of their unaffected siblings and found that the PKU probands were relatively impaired on measures of attention, visual reasoning, and on the Wisconsin Card Sort. Smith and colleagues (Smith, Klim, Mallozzi, & Hanley, 1996) studied the neuropsychological performance of hyperphenylalaninemic adults and found that the hyperphenylalaninemic group performed worse than control subjects on both frontal and temporal/parietal lobe tests. Only performance on the frontal measures, however, was related to concurrently measured phenylalanine levels. Interpretation of these studies is complicated, as has been noted (Stemerdink et al., 1999), because the neuropsychological measures vary widely between

3 Luciana, Sullivan, and Nelson 1639 studies, and several of the available reports (e.g., Ris et al., 1994; Smith et al., 1996) focused on samples of young adult PKU probands who had been early but not continuously treated. Most recently, Stemerdink et al. (1999) assessed neuropsychological function in early and continuously treated PKU probands, ages 8 to 20, relative to an age-matched control group. The cognitive measures utilized involved a test of visual contrast sensitivity, the Wisconsin Card Sort, the Tower of Hanoi planning test, and the Corsi Milner test of recency judgments. Several control measures were also employed to assess nonexecutive functions. These authors reported that their PKU sample was impaired relative to normal controls on a composite index of the prefrontal measures and specifically on the Wisconsin Card Sort and the test of recency judgments. Performance on nonexecutive measures was indistinct between the groups. Moreover, concurrent levels were related to task performance in the predicted direction within the youngest (e.g., age 8 years) and oldest (age 20 years) subjects, although findings were not consistent across the tasks employed. In the intermediate age groups, concurrent phenylalanine levels were related to task performance in a direction opposite to that predicted. That is, high concurrent phenylalanine levels were associated with better task performance. There have been several reports of associations between concurrent phenylalanine levels and composite indices of prefrontal function (Diamond et al., 1997; Ris et al., 1994; Weglage et al., 1996; Welsh et al., 1990), although the relationship between phenylalanine levels and performance on specific tests of executive function has been inconsistent. None of these studies of adolescent and young adult probands has examined the relationship between Phe:Tyr ratios and neuropsychological performance, despite its potential importance for the study of dopamine availability in PKU. This literature raises several questions relevant to the longer term outcome of adolescents and young adults with PKU. Not only does it remain uncertain as to whether early and continuous restriction of phenylalanine prevents cognitive dysfunction, particularly with respect to behaviors mediated by the prefrontal cortex, but evidence for a dopaminergic mechanism underlying the problems that have been reported is lacking. In this study, we compared the neurocognitive performance of adolescents and young adults who had early and continuously treated PKU with that of two comparison groups one consisting of age-matched peer-nominated controls and the other an age-matched group of individuals with chronic illnesses. All individuals completed measures of prefrontally mediated executive function including spatial working memory, set shifting and look-ahead planning. The measures of executive function used in this study are sensitive to alterations in catecholamine neurotransmission (Coull, Middleton, Robbins, & Sahakian, 1995; Dias, Robbins, & Roberts, 1996; Downes et al., 1989; Lange et al., 1992; Morris et al., 1988) and have been used to detect cognitive impairments in developmentally disordered populations (Hughes, Russell, & Robbins, 1994; Luciana, Lindeke, Georgieff, Mills, & Nelson, 1999; Ozonoff, 2001). It was predicted that relative to controls, individuals with PKU would show impairments in executive functions such as spatial working memory and set shifting that are modulated by dopamine activity in the frontal cortex. It was also predicted that individual differences in the cognitive performance of PKU probands on these tests of executive function would be associated with the Phe:Tyr ratio measured at various time points during individuals lifetimes. A high ratio score, indicating low tyrosine in the context of high phenylalanine levels, was expected to be associated with relatively poor cognitive performance. METHODS Participants PKU probands were recruited from the patient database at the Fairview-University Medical Center s Department of Pediatrics at the University of Minnesota. All had received treatment at the hospital s PKU Clinic during childhood, had been diagnosed with classic PKU (i.e., based on the clinic s criteria of serum phenylalanine concentrations 20 mg/dl on an unrestricted diet, normal or low tyrosine levels, and increased phenylketones present in urine), and had begun treatment within the first 3 months of life. This clinic serves Minnesota and several surrounding states, as well as southern central Canada. The clinic database was screened for individuals between the ages of 14 and 25 who had measured full-scale IQ scores above 80 on the age-appropriate version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales. A total of 58 probands were identified as possible study participants, and all were sent letters of invitation. Letters were followed by phone calls from a study representative. Recruitment was complicated by the fact that many families were currently living out of state or in Canada. Of the initial pool of 58 possible participants, 19 could not be contacted, 11 refused to participate, and 28 expressed some interest in the study. Of those who expressed interest, 10 participants initially agreed to participate but did not make

4 1640 Child Development their scheduled appointments even when multiple visits were scheduled. The final number of PKU probands who agreed to participate (N 18) were asked (but not required) to nominate a friend of the same age and gender to co-participate in the study. These peer controls (N 16) were contacted separately and invited to participate in a study of adolescent health. In an attempt to address whether the PKU probands who had agreed to participate in this study were representative of the group as a whole, the IQ scores and phenylalanine levels of the 18 PKU probands who participated in this study were compared with those of 30 affected individuals who were identified as possible participants but could not be contacted or declined to participate. The nonparticipants had phenylalanine, tyrosine, and IQ data available through medical chart review. Although scores remained within the normal range, there was a trend toward lower full-scale IQ scores in the nonparticipating (M 97.2, SD 11.3) versus participating (M 104.8, SD 10.9) PKU sample, F(1, 37) 2.76, p.10. Phenylalanine levels did not differ between the groups, F(1, 37) 1.14, ns. In order to control for the nonspecific effects of chronic illness on behavioral functioning, a group of age-matched controls with lifelong illnesses was also recruited. This comparison group was comprised of patients with cystic fibrosis (n 6) and juvenile onset diabetes (n 4) identified through records of the Pediatric Pulmonary and the Pediatric Endocrinology Clinics at the Fairview-University Medical Center, as well as asthmatic patients (n 7) from the Pulmonology Clinic at Children s Health Care in St. Paul, MN. Of 95 potentially recruitable chronically ill patients, 73% (N 32) of patients with cystic fibrosis, 73% (N 22) of patients with diabetes, and 64% (N 14) of patients with asthma declined to participate. Eleven patients agreed to participate but either cancelled or failed to attend their scheduled visits. The 17 individuals who agreed to participate were also invited to nominate a peer to participate with them. There was no difference between the PKU and chronic illness groups in their willingness to recruit peers, 2 (1, N 37) 2.45, p 12. Participants under the age of 18 years (65%) were significantly more likely to recruit peers that were older participants, 2 (1, N 37) 8.40, p.01. The final study sample consisted of 18 PKU probands, 16 peer controls, and 17 patients with other chronic illnesses. All individuals were assessed on an outpatient basis. Demographic characteristics of the groups are presented in Table 1. As indicated, the three samples were comparable in terms of gender representation, age, F(2, 51) 1.61, ns, ethnicity, dwelling (rural versus urban settings), marital status, and living arrangements. As would be expected, age at initial diagnosis was significantly different between PKU probands (M days, SD days) and chronically ill participants (M 4.74 years, SD 4.13 years). All but one chronically ill participant was currently participating in daily medical and/or dietary treatment; the majority of these participants (N 13) reported that they complied with treatment as prescribed at least 75% of the time, with no significant differences in rates of compliance between the chronically ill subgroups, 2 (2, N 17) 5.42, p.49. Socioeconomically, the groups were similar in terms of their completed years of education, F(2, 51) 1.18, ns, level of education of their parents, and their annual median incomes (their own if living independently; parental if living with parents). The three groups did not differ in terms of history of academic difficulties, F(2, 51) 1.05, ns, the number of individuals who repeated a grade in school, F(2, 51).26, ns, nor in the number of current clinical scale elevations on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory Adolescent Version (MMPI-A; Butcher et al., 1992), F(2, 51).64, ns. There was a group difference in the number of individuals who had received past psychiatric treatment, with approximately equal numbers of PKU and chronically ill individuals reporting such histories. No one in the peer control group reported a psychiatric treatment history, F(2, 51) 6.31, p.01. Cognitive Assessment Tests of Executive Function Three tasks were selected to represent executive functions mediated by the prefrontal cortex. These included a test of spatial working memory (Owen, Downes, Sahakian, Polkey, & Robbins, 1990), the Tower of London planning task (Shallice, 1982), and a set-shifting task, analogous to the Wisconsin Card Sort (Downes et al., 1989). All tasks were administered through the use of a touch-screen computer (Fray, Robbins, & Sahakian, 1996). Spatial working memory. This self-ordered searching task measures working memory for spatial stimuli and requires the participant to use mnemonic information to work toward a goal (Owen et al., 1990; Owen, Evans, & Petrides, 1996; Petrides & Milner, 1982). Two variables of interest are computed. The first is a measure of strategy whereby a high score represents poor strategy use. The second consists of mnemonic errors for searches of two, three, four, six, and eight items.

5 Luciana, Sullivan, and Nelson 1641 Table 1 Patients Demographic Characteristics of Phenylketonuria (PKU) Probands, Peer Controls, and Chronically Ill PKU Probands Peer Controls Chronically Ill Patients Age Gender (male:female) 9:9 8:8 11:6 Ethnicity (% White) 100% 98% 100% Years of education Number with reported history of academic difficulties 35 2 Number who repeated a grade in school Number with history of psychiatric treatment Average number of MMPI clinical scale elevations Dwelling Urban Rural Marital status Single Married Living arrangements With parents or guardian Dormitory Rented apartment Homeowner Employment status Student Student, plus part-time employment Part-time employment Full-time employment Unemployed Mothers education 12 years years a or GED Some college b College degree 34 3 Advanced degree Unknown Fathers education 12 years years a or GED Some college b College degree Advanced degree 0 32 Unknown 4 30 Incomes c Parental $40 $50,000 $40 $50,000 $40 $50,000 Own $20 $30,000 $20 $30,000 $20 $30,000 Note: MMPI Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory; GED Graduate Equivalency Diploma. a Indicates completion of high school degree. b Includes trade school. c Participants reported personal income if financially dependent, otherwise parental income was used. d Two participants refused to provide financial information. Intradimensional/extradimensional set shifting. One of the most popularly used measures for the experimental assessment of frontal lobe dysfunction is the Wisconsin Card Sort Test (Milner, 1964). Deficient performance is typically attributed to the inability to shift response set between conceptual categories. However, the precise nature of the cognitive deficit underlying task failure has yet to be determined, be-

6 1642 Child Development cause the task actually requires several abilities. To dissociate the various abilities underlying set-shifting performance, we utilized a task called the Intradimensional/Extradimensional Set-Shift task (Dias, Robbins, & Roberts, 1996; Downes et al., 1989). This task measures discrimination and reversal learning under conditions that require the subject to shift attention to changing patterns of visual stimuli. Briefly, this task progresses along a series of nine stages of increasing difficulty. The first two stages involve simple discrimination and reversal learning. The next three stages involve extending the discrimination and reversal learning to instances in which a distractor stimulus is present. The sixth stage demands an attentional shift. Termed the intradimensional (ID) shift stage, novel or never-seen exemplars of each of the two prior dimensions are introduced, and the participant generalizes the rule from previous learning in order to achieve correct responses. Subsequently, another extradimensional (ED) attentional shift is required. Novel exemplars of each stimulus dimension are presented, and the subject must shift response set from the previously relevant dimension to the previously irrelevant dimension. This stage is analogous to the between-category shifts required by the Wisconsin Card Sort (Milner, 1964). Variables coded for each participant included the stage reached, the trials to criterion, and number of errors for each completed stage. Tower of London. This task measures planning and behavioral inhibition (Shallice, 1982). The participant views two displays on a computer screen, one of which is the target display and one that comprises a workspace. The participant must move colored balls on the screen to make the workspace display appear like the target. There are a minimum number of moves in which to complete a trial perfectly, and the participant is told at the start of a trial that the trial should be completed in that minimum number of moves. The following variables were computed for each problem set representing four levels of problem difficulty (2-move, 3-move, 4-move, and 5-move problems): average number of moves to complete each set, planning time, and total number of problems perfectly completed in the minimum number of moves. Performance expectations for PKU probands. Relative to comparison groups, we anticipated the performance of PKU probands on measures of executive function to be consistent with frontal lobe impairment. Specifically, the PKU group was expected to demonstrate increased mnemonic errors and higher strategy scores (indicative of poor use of strategy) on the spatial working memory task, an inability to proceed through all nine stages of the ID/ED set-shift task, and fewer perfect solutions in completing Tower of London problems. Tests of Nonexecutive Function Three additional cognitive measures were included to represent nonexecutive functions related to psychomotor speed and accuracy, sequencing ability, and recognition memory. Motor screening task. This test measures psychomotor speed and accuracy. The participant s task is to quickly and accurately touch visual targets that are presented one at a time on the computer screen. Accuracy and response latency are recorded. Spatial span task. This task measures memory for a figural sequence and is a computerized analog of the Corsi block task (Milner, 1971). It yields a measure of the participant s nonverbal memory span, the number of items that can be accurately remembered in a sequence. Pattern/spatial recognition. This delayed-match-tosample task is a measure of recognition memory for visual patterns and spatial locations. First, the participant views geometric patterns presented consecutively on screen. After a brief pause, the participant is presented with two geometric patterns. One of the two designs is from the previously viewed list, while the other is novel. The participant indicates the pattern that has already been seen. Accuracy and response latency are recorded. An additional series of trials comprises a delayed-match-to-location task. The participant attends to the locations of five empty boxes presented at various locations on the screen. Then the participant views two empty boxes, one of which is in a location that was just targeted, and touches the box that represents a previously targeted location. Notably, performance on these recognition memory tasks is highly sensitive to temporal and parietal lobe lesions in adult patients and appears to be unaffected by frontal lobe pathology (B. J. Sahakian, personal communication, April 16, 1998). Participants completed cognitive testing in the course of one 3-hour testing session. In addition, each participant completed the age-appropriate version of the MMPI-2/A (Butcher et al., 1992), the Tennessee Self- Concept Scale (Fitts & Warren, 1996), and a structured clinical interview (the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents Revised Adolescent Version for DSM-IV: Reich, Leacock, & Shanfeld, 1995; or the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Non-Patient Version: First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1994), Cognitive testing took place during either the first or second hour of the assessment. The cognitive tasks were administered in the same order for all participants.

7 Luciana, Sullivan, and Nelson 1643 Chart review of medical records maintained by the Department of Pediatrics and the Biomedical Genetics Laboratory were utilized to obtain IQ and phenylalanine and tyrosine levels for PKU probands. IQ scores for PKU probands were obtained from each participant s most recent neuropsychological assessment (N 3, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised; N 15, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Revised, 3rd edition). The average IQ obtained (M 104.8, SD 10.9) was not significantly different from the normal population mean (M 100, SD 15). IQ was not measured in the comparison groups. Additionally, each participant s lifetime history of recorded phenylalanine and tyrosine levels was recorded and used in subsequent data analyses. Over the course of their lifetimes, PKU probands each contributed an average of assessments (M 58.67, SD 36.06, range 1 120) to this analysis. From each assessment, consisting of a phenylalanine and a tyrosine level, the following values were computed for each of the PKU probands: 1. The most recently obtained phenylalanine and tyrosine levels obtained within 6 months prior to the cognitive assessment. All probands had values available. 2. Yearly grand averages of phenylalanine and tyrosine for each year of life; the grand average computed for the first year of life always excluded the first value recorded subsequent to the proband s birth (e.g., the value from which the diagnosis of PKU was made). From these yearly averages, five variables were constructed separately for phenylalanine and tyrosine, representing grand averages for the years (a) 0 to 2 years, (b) 3 to 4 years, (c) 5 to 8 years, (d) 9 to 13 years, and (e) 14 to 15 years. The period encompassing ages 16 to 18 was not included in the analysis because only a small percentage of the PKU sample (n 10 or 55%) was old enough to contribute these values. These age ranges were selected based on developmental research using cognitive tasks identical or similar to the ones used in this study (Diamond et al., 1997; Luciana & Nelson, 1998). 3. For each of these five time periods, the average Phe:Tyr ratio was computed. This method of obtaining phenylalanine and tyrosine levels from medical records has been used in other PKU studies (Diamond et al., 1997). To assess whether the most recently obtained levels were representative of each participant s lifetime pattern of dietary control, correlations between these levels and lifetime averages were computed. These findings are presented in Table 2. As indicated in Table 2, the most Table 2 Intercorrelations among Lifetime and Most Recent Phenylalanine (Phe) and Tyrosine (Tyr) Levels Most Recent Phe Level Most Recent Tyr Level Lifetime Phe Level Most recent Tyr level.502 Lifetime Phe level.840**.600* Lifetime Tyr level **.338 Note: Values represent Pearson correlations. * p.05; ** p.01; p.10. recent and lifetime levels of phenylalanine and tyrosine were significantly intercorrelated. RESULTS Data were analyzed using SPSS for Windows, version 10.0 (SPSS, 1999). Two primary questions were addressed in the data analyses: (1) Do PKU probands differ from the two comparison groups with respect to cognitive functions in general, and, specifically, with respect to executive functions? and (2) In individuals with PKU, what is the relationship between lifetime measures of phenylalanine and tyrosine levels and cognitive function? Group Differences in Cognitive Function Cognitive task variables were compared between groups using one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA). When group differences were found, post hoc comparisons using Tukey s honestly significant difference were evaluated to determine the nature of significant group differences. As indicated in Table 3, the cognitive performance of adolescents with PKU was comparable with that exhibited by the two age-matched comparison groups. There were no statistically significant differences noted between groups on any cognitive task variables. Following the methodology of Welsh et al. (1990), a composite score representing executive function (EF) was derived by converting raw scores on the frontal lobe measures to Z scores, using the total sample mean and standard deviation. After the Z score conversions, the following variables, each of which is the best representative of frontal lobe dysfunction for the particular task, were averaged to obtain composite EF scores: spatial working memory strategy score, number of minimum-move Tower of London solutions, and ID/ED stage reached. Composites were computed so that a Z score above the mean reflected below-average performance. As a discriminant measure, a second composite

8 1644 Child Development Table 3 Cognitive Task Performance among Phenlyketonuria (PKU) Probands, Peer Controls, and Chronically Ill Patients Task Variable PKU Probands Peer Controls Chronically Ill Patients F Ratio p Value Motor error ns Motor latency Memory span ns Spatial working memory Easy-item errors ns Hard-item errors ns Strategy score ns Tower of London planning Minimum move solutions ns Average moves to complete 3-move problems ns 4-move problems ns 5-move problems ns Planning times 3-move problems move problems ns 5-move problems ns ID/ED set shifting Stage reached ns ID errors ID trials ns ED errors ns ED trials ns Reversal errors ns Recognition memory Patterns: % correct ns Spatial: % correct ns Executive function composite ns Nonexecutive function composite ns Note: ID intradimensional; ED extradimensional. score was similarly derived to represent performance on tasks measuring nonexecutive functions (NEF). This measure was derived from the following variables: motor screening accuracy, motor screening latency, spatial memory span, pattern recognition percent correct performance, and spatial recognition percent correct performance. The composite was scored such that a high score represented poor performance. EF and NEF composite scores were compared among the three groups in a one-way ANOVA yielding no significant main effect for group for the EF nor for the NEF composite. Because this pattern of findings was inconsistent with what has been observed in younger samples of PKU probands, we considered whether performance in the comparison groups accurately represented normal cognitive performance. Akin to the notion of assortative mating, it may be that individuals with PKU select friends who are similar in having some level of cognitive deficit. As indicated in Table 1 (items 4 8), the three groups did not differ in terms of average educational level, whether school services had ever been received in the past, or in the average number of clinical scale elevations obtained on the MMPI. A more thorough analysis of the emotional functioning of these groups has indicated that the three groups were generally healthy with respect to current psychopathology (Sullivan, in press). However, each group consisted of some individuals who experienced difficulties in one or more of these areas, as indicated in Table 1. Accordingly, to further address the representativeness of the two comparison samples (peers and chronically ill), their cognitive performance was compared with that of a third group (n 19) of age-matched normal controls whose performance on the same battery of tests was measured for a separate study. This sample was recruited from the undergraduate population of students at the University of Minnesota and from a private area high school. Their data have been reported elsewhere (Luciana & Nelson, 1998). No one in this additional sample had a history of school difficulty, special

9 Luciana, Sullivan, and Nelson 1645 education, chronic illness, or psychological disorder. There were no significant differences in cognitive performance on any task variable among the peernominated controls, chronically ill adolescents, and this third normative sample of young adults. Thus, we have no reason to conclude that our comparison groups were not representative of individuals in this age range. Associations between Biochemical Variables and Cognitive Outcome Associations between phenylalanine levels, tyrosine levels, the Phe:Tyr ratio and measures of executive and nonexecutive function were examined. Several tables are included to describe these data: Table 4 presents correlations between biochemical variables at various points in development and the PKU probands ages and IQs, Table 5 presents correlations between measures of executive function and biochemical variables, and Table 6 presents correlations between measures of nonexecutive function and biochemical variables. Phenylalanine and Cognitive Function Of the 18 PKU probands studied, 77% (14/18) indicated by self-report that they were currently complying with dietary restrictions. However, the range of most recently obtained phenylalanine levels varied in this group from 5.3 to 28.8 mg/dl (M 16.3, SD 6.8). Since adequate control of phenylalanine levels is represented by a value of less than 10 to 15 mg/dl (Diamond et al., 1997), many of these participants were either not maintaining the diet adequately or were not maximally benefiting from phenylalanine restriction. The correlation between the most recently obtained phenylalanine level and IQ was in the predicted direction, with high phenylalanine levels associated with lower full-scale IQ scores. Additionally, older participants tended to exhibit higher phenylalanine levels (see Table 4). As indicated in Tables 5 and 6, there were significant associations between phenylalanine levels obtained at various points in development and several cognitive task variables. Executive function. With respect to spatial working memory performance, phenylalanine levels at 0 to 2 years of age were related to the total number of mnemonic errors, but in a manner opposite to that predicted. High phenylalanine levels were associated with lower numbers of errors. However, poor use of strategy was associated with high phenylalanine levels at ages 3 to 5 years, 9 to 13 years, 14 to 15 years, and in the past 6 months. In terms of set-shifting performance, high phenylalanine levels at ages 3 to 4, 5 to 8, and 9 to 13 years were associated with a decreased stage reached on the task. Tower of London performance, as assessed by a high number of items solved in the minimum number of moves, was associated with higher phenylalanine levels at 0 to 2 years, a finding similar to what was found for spatial working memory errors and counter to prediction. High phenylalanine levels at 9 to 13 years of age were associated with poor overall performance as represented by the EF composite. Nonexecutive function. Increased motor latency was marginally associated with higher phenylalanine levels at ages 9 to 13 and 14 to 15. Low memory spans were associated with high phenylalanine levels at ages 9 to 13, 14 to 15, and in the past 6 months. High phenylalanine levels at ages 9 to 13 also related to cur- Table 4 Associations among Age, IQ, and Biochemical Indices Age When Blood Levels of Phe and Tyr Were Recorded 0 2 Years 3 4 Years 5 8 Years 9 13 Years Years Past 6 Months Age Phe/age *.58**.64*.61* Tyr/age **.25.44*.31 Ratio/age **.74**.75**.74** IQ Phe/IQ.49*.47*.71**.75**.56**.69* Tyr/IQ Ratio/IQ.39.57*.52*.54*.48*.39 Note: Age and IQ correlations are Pearson correlations. Bold-faced values indicate correlations that conform to the expectations that high phenylalanine (Phe), low tyrosine (Tyr), and/or a high Phe:Tyr ratio will be associated with lower IQs and increased age. Note that values recorded during ages 0 2 exclude those used to diagnose phenylketonuria. * p.05; ** p.01; p.10.

10 1646 Child Development Table 5 Associations among Executive Function (EF) Task Performance, Phenylalanine (Phe), and Tyrosine (Tyr) Age When Blood Phe and Tyr Levels Were Recorded 0 2 Years 3 4 Years 5 8 Years 9 13 Years Years Past 6 Months EF composite Phe/EF * Tyr/EF.76** Ratio/EF.67* *.01 Spatial working memory Strategy/Phe *.71** Strategy/Tyr * Strategy/ratio.89** Errors/Phe Errors/Tyr.81** * Errors/ratio Set shifting Stage/Phe *.57*.03### Stage/Tyr ### Stage/ratio **.67**.40 ### Tower of London Perfect solutions/phe.54* Perfect solutions/tyr.92**.51* * Perfect solutions/ratio.58* * Note: Values are partial correlation coefficients, covarying for age and IQ. Bold-faced values indicate correlations that conform to the study s predictions that cognitive function will be impaired by high Phe, low Tyr, and/or a high Phe:Tyr ratio. * p.05; ** p.01; p.10; ### coefficient cannot be determined because of lack of variance. rent spatial recognition memory performance. Otherwise, correlations between phenylalanine levels and measures of nonexecutive function were nonsignificant. Tyrosine and Cognitive Outcome The range of most recently obtained tyrosine levels varied from.50 to 1.7 mg/dl (M 1.05, SD.36). The reference range for normal tyrosine levels in individuals above the age of 12 years is from.6 to 2.4 mg/dl according to the University of Minnesota s Department of Biomedical Genetics, where the assays were conducted. Tyrosine levels were correlated with all cognitive task variables, partialing out the effects of both IQ and age on performance. These findings are represented in Tables 5 and 6. Executive function. Spatial working memory impairment, as measured by a high number of mnemonic errors and poor use of strategy, was associated with low tyrosine levels at ages 0 to 2 years. However, counter to prediction, higher tyrosine levels were associated with poor performance on this task at ages 5 to 8 (increased mnemonic errors) and 9 to 13 (increased errors and high strategy scores). Stage reached on the set-shifting task was unrelated to tyrosine levels. A high number of Tower of London problems solved in the minimum number of moves was associated with higher tyrosine levels at ages 0 to 2 years and in the past 6 months. Counter to prediction, high tyrosine levels at ages 3 to 4 and 5 to 8 were associated with poorer Tower of London performance. The EF composite was most related to tyrosine levels at ages 0 to 2 years, with low tyrosine levels associated with poorer current performance. Nonexecutive function. In terms of nonexecutive performance, longer motor response latencies were associated with high tyrosine levels at ages 0 to 2 years. A relatively higher spatial memory span was related to high tyrosine levels at ages 0 to 2 years. Finally, better pattern recognition memory was associated with higher tyrosine levels in the most recent period. The Phe:Tyr Ratio and Cognitive Outcome As indicated in Table 2, phenylalanine and tyrosine levels measured within the past 6 months were inversely correlated to a moderate degree, r(17).50. To address Diamond and colleagues (1997) hypothesis that the ratio between phenylalanine and tyrosine leads to dopamine deficiency in the prefrontal cortex and associated cognitive dysfunction, ratio scores were correlated with all tasks, covarying once again for IQ

11 Luciana, Sullivan, and Nelson 1647 Table 6 Associations among Nonexecutive Function (NEF) Task Performance, Phenylalanine (Phe), and Tyrosine (Tyr) Age When Phe and Tyr Levels Obtained 0 2 Years 3 4 Years 5 8 Years 9 13 Years Years Past 6 Months NEF composite Phe/NEF Tyr/NEF Ratio/NEF.64* Motor screening Accuracy/Phe Accuracy/Tyr Accuracy/ratio Latency/Phe Latency/Tyr.59* Latency/ratio **.64 Memory span Span/Phe **.93** Span/Tyr Span/ratio ** ### Recognition memory Patterns/Phe Patterns/Tyr ** Patterns/ratio *.32 Locations/Phe Locations/Tyr ** Locations/ratio Note: Values are partial correlation coefficients, covarying for age and IQ. Bold-faced values indicate correlations that conform to the study s predictions that cognitive function will be impaired by high Phe, low Tyr, and/or a high Phe:Tyr ratio. * p.05; ** p.01; p.10; ### coefficient cannot be determined because of lack of variance. and age. The findings, represented in Tables 5 and 6, suggest that the Phe:Tyr ratio is moderately to strongly associated with several aspects of cognitive function. Executive function. Increased errors on the spatial working memory task were associated with increased Phe:Tyr ratios at ages 0 to 2 and 14 to 15. Poor use of strategy was also associated with a higher ratio score at these ages, as well as within the past 6 months. A decreased stage reached on the ID/ED set-shifting task was associated with higher Phe:Tyr ratios at ages 3 to 4, 5 to 8, 9 to 13, and 14 to 15. Similarly, a decrease in the number of Tower of London problems solved in the minimum number of moves was related to increased ratio scores at ages 0 to 2, 14 to 15, and in the past 6 months. As expected from this pattern of findings within individual tasks, a high current EF composite score was associated with increased ratio scores at ages 0 to 2, 5 to 8, 9 to 13, and 14 to 15 years. Nonexecutive function. With respect to nonexecutive functions, increased motor response latencies were associated with higher ratio scores at ages 14 to 15 and in the past 6 months. Higher ratio scores at ages 14 to 15 were also associated with poor pattern recognition memory performance. In the age range from 0 to 2 years, high ratio scores were associated with better current NEF performance. However, high ratio scores at ages 9 to 13 and 14 to 15 were associated with poorer NEF performance, as indicated by the composite index. Implications of high versus low ratio scores. To better understand the implications of high Phe:Tyr ratio scores for current levels of cognitive function, the median ratio score (represented by a value of 14.6) at ages 14 to 15 years was used to group PKU probands into those with high versus low scores. This age range was selected because all PKU probands had values available for this time period and because it was significantly associated with performance on a variety of tasks. The performance of these two groups was compared once again with that of the control samples. Findings indicated that PKU probands with high ratio scores performed significantly worse than the control sample in terms of the NEF composite, F(2, 43) 3.62, p.05, and marginally worse in terms of the EF composite, F(2, 43) 2.49, p.10. With respect to specific tasks, the high-ratio PKU sample performed worse than controls in terms of the maximum stage reached on the set-shifting task, F(2, 43) 3.29, p

12 1648 Child Development.05. All other specific task variables yielded nonsignificant differences between the high-ratio PKU, lowratio PKU, and control samples. A large number of correlations have been presented in these analyses, and control for multiple correlations has not been stringently applied. In Tables 5 and 6, there are a total of 194 correlations reported. We utilized one-tailed tests of significance and have highlighted p values less than.10. Hence, we would expect 10% of these correlations to be significant by chance. In Table 5, which represents 87 correlations between measures of executive function and three biochemical indices (phenylalanine, tyrosine, and the Phe:Tyr ratio), 31 correlations (or 36%) were significant in the predicted direction, a number that exceeds chance expectancies. Moreover, 48% of the correlations that were significant in the expected direction represent associations that involved the Phe:Tyr ratio. Table 6 presents 107 correlations between measures of nonexecutive function and the same biochemical indices. Here, 16 of 107 correlations (15%) were significant in what might loosely be described as the predicted direction (e.g., poor cognitive performance associated with high phenylalanine and low tyrosine levels). Of these, 38% involved the ratio score. This was, admittedly, a crude analysis. Biochemical levels obtained at one age are highly associated with those obtained at past and subsequent ages, and the ratio score is related to each of the two indices that comprised it. Moreover, the cognitive task variables, particularly those derived from the same task, are significantly intercorrelated. A larger study sample would permit the use of factor-analytic techniques to reduce the data into smaller components, but data reduction techniques were not appropriate with this sample. Thus, because of the number of comparisons reported, we stress that these findings await replication, although the observed pattern of effects was clearly in the predicted direction. DISCUSSION Most studies of cognitive function in early and continuously treated PKU have included participants who are infants or preschoolers (Diamond et al., 1997). Long-term cognitive outcome is uncertain, particularly in adolescents and young adults who have achieved normal IQ scores. The results of the current study suggest that with continuous dietary treatment, individuals with PKU who have achieved normal IQ scores do not differ substantially from their peers or from an age-matched group of individuals with other chronic illnesses in terms of cognitive performance. The cognitive measures used in this study are particularly sensitive to frontostriatal dysfunction, as evidenced by both clinical and functional neuroimaging studies (Baker et al., 1996; Fray et al., 1996; Morris, Ahmed, Syed, & Toone, 1993; Owen, Doyon, Petrides, & Evans, 1996; Owen, Evans, & Petrides, 1996; Owen et al., 1992; Owen, Morris, Sahakian, Polkey, & Robbins, 1996; Owen, Roberts, Pokley, Sahakian, & Robbins, 1991; Veale, Sahakian, Owen, & Marks, 1996). Although relatively low scores on tasks that index frontal lobe function have been reported in younger samples of PKU-affected individuals, it cannot be generally concluded on the basis of the current data that adolescents and young adults with PKU will necessarily demonstrate a deficit level of performance on measures of executive function. Despite what appears to be a relatively positive outcome for the PKU sample as a whole, dietary control of phenylalanine and tyrosine levels nevertheless appears to contribute to individual differences in cognitive function. Diamond and colleagues (Diamond et al., 1997) have discussed the importance of the Phe:Tyr ratio as an index of the level of tyrosine that will be available to the brain for dopamine synthesis. A high ratio indicates that phenylalanine levels are in a range that would theoretically impede the transport of available tyrosine across the blood brain barrier, resulting in a decreased availability of tyrosine for dopamine synthesis. The findings of this study support the hypothesis that in individuals with PKU who may have decreased tyrosine availability, frontal lobe functions that are dopamine dependent may be compromised. High Phe:Tyr ratio scores in the past 6 months, at ages 14 to 15 and at ages 0 to 2 years were associated with relatively poor current spatial working memory performance (both in terms of number of errors and use of strategy) and a relatively poor ability to solve Tower of London problems with maximal efficiency. Relatively poor set-shifting performance was associated with high ratio scores in all age ranges between 3 and 15 years of age. Moreover, below-average scores on a composite index of EF performance were associated with high Phe:Tyr ratios at ages 0 to 2, 5 to 8, 9 to 13, and 14 to 15 years. However, counter to prediction, high ratio scores, particularly at ages 9 to 13 and 14 to 15, were also associated with relatively poor nonexecutive function performance. Indeed, individuals with high ratio scores at ages 14 to 15 performed significantly worse than control participants in terms of the NEF composite. If these findings can be replicated, then the specificity of dopamine s role in mediating performance on tests of executive function, particularly in the context of what is hypothesized to be a lifetime dopamine deficit, should be re-evaluated. In addition, to better examine the significance of

Emotional Outcome of Adolescents and Young Adults With Early and Continuously Treated Phenylketonuria

Emotional Outcome of Adolescents and Young Adults With Early and Continuously Treated Phenylketonuria Journal of Pediatric Psychology, Vol. 26, No. 8, 2001, pp. 477 484 Emotional Outcome of Adolescents and Young Adults With Early and Continuously Treated Phenylketonuria Jill E. Sullivan, PhD University

More information

NEUROCOGNITIVE, OUTCOMES IN PKU: IT S TIME TO RAISE THE BAR

NEUROCOGNITIVE, OUTCOMES IN PKU: IT S TIME TO RAISE THE BAR NEUROCOGNITIVE, OUTCOMES IN : IT S TIME TO RAISE THE BAR KEY POINTS 1. High Phenylalanine (Phe) levels harm the brain.. Traditional therapies do not completely protect individuals with. 3. New approaches

More information

The Context and Purpose of Executive Function Assessments: Beyond the Tripartite Model

The Context and Purpose of Executive Function Assessments: Beyond the Tripartite Model The Context and Purpose of Executive Function Assessments: Beyond the Tripartite Model Janina Eberhart & Sara Baker Neuroscience & Education Conference 4 th June 2018 Executive Function (EF) EF is important

More information

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 703 708 Test review Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., 2002 1. Test description The Trail Making Test

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Curr Dir Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Curr Dir Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009 February 1; 18(1): 48 52. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01604.x. Phenylketonuria in Children and Mothers:

More information

Chapter Three BRIDGE TO THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES

Chapter Three BRIDGE TO THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES Chapter Three BRIDGE TO THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES Developmental Psychopathology: From Infancy through Adolescence, 5 th edition By Charles Wenar and Patricia Kerig When do behaviors or issues become pathologies?

More information

Phenylketonuria: No Specific Frontal Lobe-Dependent Neuropsychological Deficits of Early-Treated Patients in Comparison with Diabetics

Phenylketonuria: No Specific Frontal Lobe-Dependent Neuropsychological Deficits of Early-Treated Patients in Comparison with Diabetics 0031-3998/02/5106-0761 PEDIATRIC RESEARCH Vol. 51, No. 6, 2002 Copyright 2002 International Pediatric Research Foundation, Inc. Printed in U.S.A. Phenylketonuria: No Specific Frontal Lobe-Dependent Neuropsychological

More information

Hill, Elisabeth L Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), pp ISSN [Article]

Hill, Elisabeth L Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), pp ISSN [Article] Hill, Elisabeth L.. 2004. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), pp. 26-32. ISSN 13646613 [Article] http://research.gold.ac.uk/2558/ The version presented here may differ

More information

1 Cognitive, Psychological and Behavioral Assessment Based Evidence

1 Cognitive, Psychological and Behavioral Assessment Based Evidence 1 Cognitive, Psychological and Behavioral Assessment Based Evidence Preface Current medical evidence has led experts on PKU to suspect that current therapy for PKU may leave individuals with an increased

More information

FRONTO-STRIATAL COGNITIVE DEFICITS AT DIFFERENT STAGES OF PARKINSON'S DISEASE

FRONTO-STRIATAL COGNITIVE DEFICITS AT DIFFERENT STAGES OF PARKINSON'S DISEASE Brain (1992). 115, 1727-1751 FRONTO-STRIATAL COGNITIVE DEFICITS AT DIFFERENT STAGES OF PARKINSON'S DISEASE by A. M. OWEN,' M. JAMES, 2 P. N. LEIGH, 3 B. A. SUMMERS, 3 C. D. MARSDEN, 4 N. P. QUINN, 4 K.

More information

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch. The Frontal Lobes Readings: KW Ch. 16 Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions E.L. Highly organized college professor Became disorganized, showed little emotion, and began to miss deadlines Scores on intelligence

More information

Relationship between socioeconomic status and quality of life of children with phenylketonuria

Relationship between socioeconomic status and quality of life of children with phenylketonuria Relationship between socioeconomic status and quality of life of children with phenylketonuria Lacramioara Aurelia Brinduse, MD, PhD Prof. Dana Galieta Minca, MD, PhD Michaela Iuliana Nanu MD, PhD Phenylketonuria

More information

Factors related to neuropsychological deficits in ADHD children

Factors related to neuropsychological deficits in ADHD children Factors related to neuropsychological deficits in ADHD children MD S. DRUGĂ Mindcare Center for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department, Bucharest, Romania Clinical Psychologist

More information

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance in Above Average and Superior School Children: Relationship to Intelligence and Age

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance in Above Average and Superior School Children: Relationship to Intelligence and Age Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 13, No. 8, pp. 713 720, 1998 Copyright 1998 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/98 $19.00.00 PII S0887-6177(98)00007-9

More information

Working Memory in Schizophrenia: Transient "Online" Storage Versus Executive Functioning

Working Memory in Schizophrenia: Transient Online Storage Versus Executive Functioning Working Memory in Schizophrenia: Transient "Online" Storage Versus Executive Functioning William Perry, Robert K. Heaton, Eric Potterat, Tresa Roebuck, Arpi Minassian, and David L. Braff Abstract Working

More information

Cue Saliency and Age as Factors Affecting Performance in a Card Sorting Task

Cue Saliency and Age as Factors Affecting Performance in a Card Sorting Task The Huron University College Journal of Learning and Motivation Volume 52 Issue 1 Article 6 2014 Cue Saliency and Age as Factors Affecting Performance in a Card Sorting Task Rebecca Herbert Huron University

More information

Neurocognitive and Behavioral Issues in PKU

Neurocognitive and Behavioral Issues in PKU Neurocognitive and Behavioral Issues in PKU Susan Waisbren, PhD Associate Professor of Psychology Harvard Medical School Children s Hospital Boston Boston, Massachusetts Content developed in conjunction

More information

2. Area of the brain affected by the seizures.

2. Area of the brain affected by the seizures. Learning Through Storms When discussing learning, we sometimes refer to cognition, or one s ability to think, learn and use information. Seizures can impact cognition, learning and behaviour in a variety

More information

PEG TAPPING. Description of the Instrument:

PEG TAPPING. Description of the Instrument: PEG TAPPING Description of the Instrument: The rules for the task were as follows: Immediately after the experimenter tapped once with a wooden dowel (6 inches long, ¼ inch in diameter), the child was

More information

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation. REVIEW OF FRONTAL LOBE STRUCTURES Main organization of frontal cortex: 1. Motor area (precentral gyrus). 2. Premotor & supplementary motor areas (immediately anterior to motor area). Includes premotor,

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Sun LS, Li G, Miller TLK, et al. Association between a single general anesthesia exposure before age 36 months and neurocognitive outcomes in later childhood. JAMA. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.6967

More information

Nafisa Cassimjee University of Pretoria, South Africa. Raegan Murphy University College Cork, Ireland

Nafisa Cassimjee University of Pretoria, South Africa. Raegan Murphy University College Cork, Ireland Impulsive and rigid temperament subtypes and executive functioning an exploratory study of temperament configurations and neuropsychological performance Raegan Murphy University College Cork, Ireland Nafisa

More information

Screening for Phenylketonuria: A Literature Update for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force

Screening for Phenylketonuria: A Literature Update for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force Screening for Phenylketonuria: A Literature Update for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force Prepared by: Iris Mabry-Hernandez, MD, MPH Tracy Wolff, MD, MPH Kathy Green, MD, MPH Corresponding Author:

More information

Received: 12 November 2003 / Accepted: 29 March 2004 / Published online: 28 April 2004 # Springer-Verlag 2004

Received: 12 November 2003 / Accepted: 29 March 2004 / Published online: 28 April 2004 # Springer-Verlag 2004 Psychopharmacology (2004) 176: 331 342 DOI 10.1007/s00213-004-1899-2 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Mitul A. Mehta. Facundo F. Manes. Gianna Magnolfi. Barbara J. Sahakian. Trevor W. Robbins Impaired set-shifting

More information

Difference between ADHD and Executive Functioning. Dr. Josette Abdalla

Difference between ADHD and Executive Functioning. Dr. Josette Abdalla Difference between ADHD and Executive Functioning Dr. Josette Abdalla DSM-5 Definition of ADHD The DSM-5 diagnoses 3 types of ADHD: ADHD Predominantly Inattentive type ADHD Predominantly Hyperactive-Impulsive

More information

The Brain on ADHD. Ms. Komas. Introduction to Healthcare Careers

The Brain on ADHD. Ms. Komas. Introduction to Healthcare Careers The Brain on ADHD Ms. Komas Introduction to Healthcare Careers Ms. Komas Period 9/2/2016 Komas 1 HOOK: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) plagues between 5% and 7% of children and less than

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

Nutritional factors affecting serum phenylalanine concentration during pregnancy for identical twin mothers with phenylketonuria

Nutritional factors affecting serum phenylalanine concentration during pregnancy for identical twin mothers with phenylketonuria Nutritional factors affecting serum phenylalanine concentration during pregnancy for identical twin mothers with phenylketonuria By: C. Fox, J. Marquis, D.E. Kipp This is the accepted version of the following

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

CANTAB Test descriptions by function CANTAB Test descriptions by function The 22 tests in the CANTAB battery may be divided into the following main types of task: screening tests visual memory tests executive function, working memory and

More information

Research Article The Association between EEG Abnormality and Behavioral Disorder: Developmental Delay in Phenylketonuria

Research Article The Association between EEG Abnormality and Behavioral Disorder: Developmental Delay in Phenylketonuria International Scholarly Research Network ISRN Pediatrics Volume 2012, Article ID 976206, 4 pages doi:10.5402/2012/976206 Research Article The Association between EEG Abnormality and Behavioral Disorder:

More information

Process of a neuropsychological assessment

Process of a neuropsychological assessment Test selection Process of a neuropsychological assessment Gather information Review of information provided by referrer and if possible review of medical records Interview with client and his/her relative

More information

Neuropsychology and Metabolic Conditions: The Neurocognitive Profile of FOD/OAA and the benefits of neuropsychological assessment

Neuropsychology and Metabolic Conditions: The Neurocognitive Profile of FOD/OAA and the benefits of neuropsychological assessment Neuropsychology and Metabolic Conditions: The Neurocognitive Profile of FOD/OAA and the benefits of neuropsychological assessment Christopher Boys, PhD, LP Pediatric Neuropsychologist Associate Professor

More information

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EXAMINATION A method of examining the brain; abnormal behavior is linked to

More information

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1983, Vol. 51, No. 5, 721-725 Copyright 1983 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation

More information

Biological Risk Factors

Biological Risk Factors Biological Risk Factors Ms Angelina Crea Provisional Psychologist Academic Child Psychiatry Unit Royal Children s Hospital Professor Alasdair Vance Head Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics

More information

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 6.1 Research Design Research is an organized, systematic, data based, critical, objective, scientific inquiry or investigation into a specific problem, undertaken with the

More information

Molecular Genetics and Metabolism

Molecular Genetics and Metabolism Molecular Genetics and Metabolism 101 (2010) 110 114 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Molecular Genetics and Metabolism journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ymgme Sapropterin therapy increases

More information

L-Dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction

L-Dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction Psychopharmacology (1992) 17:394M4 Psychopharmacology (~) Springer-Verlag 1992 L-Dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction

More information

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Ronna Fried, Ed.D. Director of Neuropsychology Clinical & Research Programs in Pediatric Psychopharmacology and Adult ADHD Massachusetts

More information

Use of the Booklet Category Test to assess abstract concept formation in schizophrenic disorders

Use of the Booklet Category Test to assess abstract concept formation in schizophrenic disorders Bond University epublications@bond Humanities & Social Sciences papers Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences 1-1-2012 Use of the Booklet Category Test to assess abstract concept formation in schizophrenic

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

Chapter 3. Psychometric Properties

Chapter 3. Psychometric Properties Chapter 3 Psychometric Properties Reliability The reliability of an assessment tool like the DECA-C is defined as, the consistency of scores obtained by the same person when reexamined with the same test

More information

2015 PKU Patient Survey Results. National PKU Alliance PO Box 501, Tomahawk, WI

2015 PKU Patient Survey Results. National PKU Alliance PO Box 501, Tomahawk, WI 2015 PKU Patient Survey Results National PKU Alliance PO Box 501, Tomahawk, WI 54487 www.npkua.org www.adultswithpku.org The NPKUA Patient Survey demonstrates that strict dietary therapy and treatment

More information

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC) Neural Computation Overview of Lecture Models of prefrontal cortex function Dr. Sam Gilbert Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience University College London E-mail: sam.gilbert@ucl.ac.uk Prefrontal cortex

More information

An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample

An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21 (2006) 495 501 Abstract An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample Jeremy R. Sullivan a,, Cynthia A. Riccio b

More information

BOROUGH OF MANHATTAN COMMUNITY COLLEGE City University of New York Department of Social Sciences

BOROUGH OF MANHATTAN COMMUNITY COLLEGE City University of New York Department of Social Sciences BOROUGH OF MANHATTAN COMMUNITY COLLEGE City University of New York Department of Social Sciences Developmental Psychology: PSY 240-1704 and 1403 Prof. Zorn, Adjunct Lecturer-Spring 2016 Extra Credit Voluntary

More information

Supplementary appendix

Supplementary appendix Supplementary appendix This appendix formed part of the original submission and has been peer reviewed. We post it as supplied by the authors. Supplement to: van Spronsen FJ, van Wegberg AMJ, Ahring K,

More information

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist M------- P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION Name: Date of Birth: Date of Evaluation: 05-28-2015 Tests Administered: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth

More information

What s Wrong With My Client: Understanding Psychological Testing in Order to Work Effectively With Your Expert

What s Wrong With My Client: Understanding Psychological Testing in Order to Work Effectively With Your Expert What s Wrong With My Client: Understanding Psychological Testing in Order to Work Effectively With Your Expert Common Standardized, Norm Referenced Psychological Tests: Diagnostic ( Personality ) Tests:

More information

A systematic review of cognitive functioning in early treated adults with phenylketonuria

A systematic review of cognitive functioning in early treated adults with phenylketonuria Hofman et al. Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases (2018) 13:150 https://doi.org/10.1186/s13023-018-0893-4 REVIEW Open Access A systematic review of cognitive functioning in early treated adults with phenylketonuria

More information

Supplementary Online Content 2

Supplementary Online Content 2 Supplementary Online Content 2 Bieleninik Ł, Geretsegger M, Mössler K, et al; TIME-A Study Team. Effects of improvisational music therapy vs enhanced standard care on symptom severity among children with

More information

Cognitive Functioning in Children with Motor Impairments

Cognitive Functioning in Children with Motor Impairments Cognitive Functioning in Children with Motor Impairments Jan P. Piek School of Psychology & Speech Pathology Curtin Health Innovation Research Institute (CHIRI) Curtin University Perth Western Australia

More information

Genetic Counselors role in maternal PKU: Patient and Genetic Counselor perspectives. Master s Thesis. Presented to

Genetic Counselors role in maternal PKU: Patient and Genetic Counselor perspectives. Master s Thesis. Presented to Genetic Counselors role in maternal PKU: Patient and Genetic Counselor perspectives Master s Thesis Presented to The Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences Brandeis University Department of

More information

The Scientific Basis for Chelation: Animal Studies and Lead Chelation

The Scientific Basis for Chelation: Animal Studies and Lead Chelation The Scientific Basis for Chelation: Animal Studies and Lead Chelation Donald Smith, PhD Microbiology and Environmental Toxicology University of California Santa Cruz, CA ACMT Conference, Atlanta, 212 Molecular

More information

APPENDIX 11: CASE IDENTIFICATION STUDY CHARACTERISTICS AND RISK OF BIAS TABLES

APPENDIX 11: CASE IDENTIFICATION STUDY CHARACTERISTICS AND RISK OF BIAS TABLES APPENDIX 11: CASE IDENTIFICATION STUDY CHARACTERISTICS AND RISK OF BIAS TABLES 1 Study characteristics table... 3 2 Methodology checklist: the QUADAS-2 tool for studies of diagnostic test accuracy... 4

More information

A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function

A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function Todd S. Braver Dept. of Psychology Carnegie Mellon Univ. Pittsburgh, PA 15213 Jonathan D. Cohen Dept. of Psychology Carnegie Mellon Univ. Pittsburgh,

More information

Managing ADHD. Strategies and Modifications for Counselors and Teachers

Managing ADHD. Strategies and Modifications for Counselors and Teachers Managing ADHD Strategies and Modifications for Counselors and Teachers F Russell Crites, Jr., M.S., L.P.C., L.M.F.T, L.S.S.P. Director Crites Psychoeducational Consultants www.critescounseling.com 972-506-7111

More information

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp 1288-1290 Improved executive functioning following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation [Brief Communications] Moser,

More information

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Background Maintenance: Modality specificity and buffers Interference resolution: Distraction and

More information

What do we know about how children develop when they have a parent with BPD?

What do we know about how children develop when they have a parent with BPD? What do we know about how children develop when they have a parent with BPD? Maureen Zalewski, Ph.D. University of Oregon Presentation for NEA-BPD on March 4, 2018 1 A little bit about me. Assistant professor

More information

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Grant L. Iverson, Ph.D, Professor Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation Harvard Medical School & Red Sox

More information

Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and

Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and private study only. The thesis may not be reproduced elsewhere

More information

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module 2090 The PedsQL in Pediatric Cancer Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module James W. Varni, Ph.D. 1,2

More information

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES (2018-19) PSYCHOLOGY Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology OUTLINE OF THE CHAPTER (i) Scientific Methods in Psychology -observation, case study, surveys, psychological tests, experimentation

More information

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style 2009 Technical Update Donald J. Treffinger Center for Creative Learning This update reports the results of additional data collection and analyses for the VIEW

More information

0-3 DEVELOPMENT. By Drina Madden. Pediatric Neuropsychology 1

0-3 DEVELOPMENT. By Drina Madden. Pediatric Neuropsychology   1 0-3 DEVELOPMENT By Drina Madden DrinaMadden@hotmail.com www.ndcbrain.com 1 PHYSICAL Body Growth Changes in height and weight are rapid in the first two years of life. Development moves from head to tail

More information

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1 Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1 Please use the following citation when referencing this work: McGill, R. J. (2013). Test review: Children s Psychological Processing Scale (CPPS). Journal of Psychoeducational

More information

Technical Specifications

Technical Specifications Technical Specifications In order to provide summary information across a set of exercises, all tests must employ some form of scoring models. The most familiar of these scoring models is the one typically

More information

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD Using Neuropsychological Experts Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD Prepared for Advocate. Arizona Association for Justice/Arizona Trial Lawyers Association. September, 2011 Neurocognitive Associates 9813 North

More information

Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed

Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed Manard et al. BMC Neuroscience 2014, 15:7 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed Marine Manard 1,2, Delphine Carabin

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

Down Syndrome Cognition Research: Memory and Sleep

Down Syndrome Cognition Research: Memory and Sleep Down Syndrome Cognition Research: Memory and Sleep Shon Christy Committee Member LuMind Foundation Dr. Jamie Edgin Assistant Professor of Psychology University of Arizona Today s Agenda Welcome Overview

More information

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc Evaluating Educational Services for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders Erik Mayville, Ph.D., BCBA-D The Institute for Educational Planning, LLC Connecticut Center for Child Development, Inc. & mayville@iepinc.org

More information

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H PSYC H O PAT H O LO G Y : PSYC H I AT R I C P R O B LEMS A N D T H E A S SO C I

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

IQ Tests, IQ Augmenting Technologies & How To Join Mensa

IQ Tests, IQ Augmenting Technologies & How To Join Mensa IQ Tests, IQ Augmenting Technologies & How To Join Mensa Mark Ashton Smith, Ph.D. 2015 HRP Lab, UK Chapter 2. IQ Augmenting Technologies Computerized Cognitive Training (CCT) Generally what we understand

More information

Final Report of Activity February 21 st, 2006 to April 30 th, 2006 CHEO Grant 052

Final Report of Activity February 21 st, 2006 to April 30 th, 2006 CHEO Grant 052 Final Report of Activity February 21 st, 2006 to April 30 th, 2006 CHEO Grant 052 1- Title of Study: The prevalence of neuropsychiatric disorders in children and adolescents on an inpatient treatment unit:

More information

Classes of Neurotransmitters. Neurotransmitters

Classes of Neurotransmitters. Neurotransmitters 1 Drugs Outline 2 Neurotransmitters Agonists and Antagonists Cocaine & other dopamine agonists Alcohol & its effects / Marijuana & its effects Synthetic & Designer Drugs: Ecstasy 1 Classes of Neurotransmitters

More information

Longitudinal Study of Executive Function and Low Birth Weight

Longitudinal Study of Executive Function and Low Birth Weight Longitudinal Study of Executive Function and Low Birth Weight Patricia M Blasco, Sybille Guy, Serra Acar The Research Institute at Western Oregon University Sage N Saxton Oregon Health & Science University

More information

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers!

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers! Marisa Filipe Dezembro de 2013 pdpsi10020@fpce.up.pt EXCL/MHC-LIN/0688/2012 Summary Introduction: Prosodic Abilities in Children and Young Adults with Typical & Non-Typical Development Prosodic abilities

More information

The Neuropsychology of

The Neuropsychology of The Neuropsychology of Stroke Tammy Kordes, Ph.D. Northshore Neurosciences Outline What is the Role of Neuropsychology Purpose of Neuropsychological Assessments Common Neuropsychological Disorders Assessment

More information

ACDI. An Inventory of Scientific Findings. (ACDI, ACDI-Corrections Version and ACDI-Corrections Version II) Provided by:

ACDI. An Inventory of Scientific Findings. (ACDI, ACDI-Corrections Version and ACDI-Corrections Version II) Provided by: + ACDI An Inventory of Scientific Findings (ACDI, ACDI-Corrections Version and ACDI-Corrections Version II) Provided by: Behavior Data Systems, Ltd. P.O. Box 44256 Phoenix, Arizona 85064-4256 Telephone:

More information

CONVERGENT VALIDITY OF THE MMPI A AND MACI SCALES OF DEPRESSION 1

CONVERGENT VALIDITY OF THE MMPI A AND MACI SCALES OF DEPRESSION 1 Psychological Reports, 2009, 105, 605-609. Psychological Reports 2009 CONVERGENT VALIDITY OF THE MMPI A AND MACI SCALES OF DEPRESSION 1 ERIN K. MERYDITH AND LeADELLE PHELPS University at Buffalo, SUNY

More information

CHAPTER 3 METHODOLOGY, DATA COLLECTION AND DATA ANALYSIS

CHAPTER 3 METHODOLOGY, DATA COLLECTION AND DATA ANALYSIS CHAPTER 3 METHODOLOGY, DATA COLLECTION AND DATA ANALYSIS TABLE OF CONTENTS Titles Page Nos. CHAPTER 3: METHODOLOGY, DATA COLLECTION 88 97 AND DATA ANALYSIS 3.1 Objectives of the Study 89 3.2 Research Design

More information

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Ronna Fried, Ed.D. Director of Neuropsychology in the Clinical and Research Programs in Pediatric Psychopharmacology and Adult ADHD, Massachusetts

More information

CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT

CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT Informal Tes3ng Personality Tes3ng Assessment Procedures Ability Tes3ng The Clinical Interview 3 Defining Clinical Assessment The process of assessing the client through

More information

Familial Mental Retardation

Familial Mental Retardation Behavior Genetics, Vol. 14, No. 3, 1984 Familial Mental Retardation Paul L. Nichols ~ Received 18 Aug. 1983--Final 2 Feb. 1984 Familial patterns of mental retardation were examined among white and black

More information

Abstract. Keywords: episodic memory, memory retrieval, working memory, episodic buffer, executive functions, verbal, spatial, child development.

Abstract. Keywords: episodic memory, memory retrieval, working memory, episodic buffer, executive functions, verbal, spatial, child development. Developmental changes in the engagement of episodic retrieval processes and their relationship with working memory during the period of middle childhood. Sinead M. Rhodes, Derek Murphy, Peter J.B. Hancock

More information

Committee Approval Date: August 15, 2014 Next Review Date: September 2015

Committee Approval Date: August 15, 2014 Next Review Date: September 2015 Medication Policy Manual Policy No: dru152 Topic: Kuvan, sapropterin Date of Origin: March 18, 2008 Committee Approval Date: August 15, 2014 Next Review Date: September 2015 Effective Date: October 1,

More information

Classification of Inhibitory Function

Classification of Inhibitory Function Classification of Inhibitory Function Noriaki Tsuchida Behavioral regulation functions can intentionally initiate or terminate behavior (Luria, ). This study investigated the inhibitory function, a behavioral

More information

Introduction about Nutritional Assessment methods. Outlines 2/25/2012. Yasmeen Bashmil 1

Introduction about Nutritional Assessment methods. Outlines 2/25/2012. Yasmeen Bashmil 1 Introduction about Nutritional Assessment methods Outlines Introduction. Nutritional assessment systems: surveys, surveillance, screening, intervention and assessment systems in the clinical setting. Nutritional

More information

Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2

Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2 Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2 Gherdai Hassel a, Jeffrey John Milroy a, and Muhsin Michael Orsini a Adolescents who engage in regular physical

More information

Competition vs. Collaboration Test Proposal. By: Kaya, Adam, Johana, and Mike

Competition vs. Collaboration Test Proposal. By: Kaya, Adam, Johana, and Mike Competition vs. Collaboration Test Proposal By: Kaya, Adam, Johana, and Mike The Overarching Question: Is there a higher rate of occurrence of neural synchrony during competition or collaboration? Our

More information

24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037)

24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037) 24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037) Psychology is introduced as an elective subject at the higher secondary stage of school education. As a discipline, psychology specializes in the study of experiences, behaviours,

More information

24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037)

24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037) 24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037) Psychology is introduced as an elective subject at the higher secondary stage of school education. As a discipline, psychology specializes in the study of experiences, behaviours,

More information

Recommended Assessment Tools for Children and Adults with confirmed or suspected FASD

Recommended Assessment Tools for Children and Adults with confirmed or suspected FASD Recommended Assessment Tools for Children and Adults with confirmed or suspected FASD 2001 Teresa Kellerman, revised October 2005 Thousands of children are born with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders (FASD),

More information

Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury. Seth Warschausky, PhD Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation University of Michigan

Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury. Seth Warschausky, PhD Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation University of Michigan Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury Seth Warschausky, PhD Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation University of Michigan Modules Module 1: Overview Module 2: Cognitive and Academic Needs Module

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER Kesler, S., Hadi Hosseini, S. M., Heckler, C., Janelsins, M., Palesh, O., Mustian, K., & Morrow, G. (2013). Cognitive training for improving executive function in chemotherapy-treated

More information