Report. A Specialized Area in Limbic Cortex for Fast Analysis of Peripheral Vision

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Report. A Specialized Area in Limbic Cortex for Fast Analysis of Peripheral Vision"

Transcription

1 Current Biology 22, , July 24, 2012 ª2012 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI /j.cub A Specialized Area in Limbic Cortex for Fast Analysis of Peripheral Vision Report Hsin-Hao Yu, 1, * Tristan A. Chaplin, 1 Amanda J. Davies, 1,2 Richa Verma, 1 and Marcello G.P. Rosa 1,2 1 Department of Physiology 2 Monash Vision Group Monash University, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia Summary In primates, prostriata [1 3] is a small area located between the primary visual cortex (V1) and the hippocampal formation. Prostriata sends connections to multisensory and high-order association areas in the temporal, parietal, cingulate, orbitofrontal, and frontopolar cortices [4 14]. It is characterized by a relatively simple histological organization, alluding to an early origin in mammalian evolution. Here we show that prostriata neurons in marmoset monkeys exhibit a unique combination of response properties, suggesting a new pathway for rapid distribution of visual information in parallel with the traditionally recognized dorsal and ventral streams. Whereas the location and known connections of prostriata suggest a high-level association area, its response properties are unexpectedly simple, resembling those found in early stages of the visual processing: neurons have robust, nonadapting responses to simple stimuli, with latencies comparable to those found in V1, and are broadly tuned to stimulus orientation and spatiotemporal frequency. However, their receptive fields are enormous and form a unique topographic map that emphasizes the far periphery of the visual field. These results suggest a specialized circuit through which stimuli in peripheral vision can bypass the elaborate hierarchy of extrastriate visual areas and rapidly elicit coordinated motor and cognitive responses across multiple brain systems. Results Prostriata is a small, distinct area located in a bridge of cortex that links the retrosplenial and parahippocampal regions (Figure 1A). Like these limbic cortices, prostriata lacks the clear six-layered cellular structure that characterizes the majority of the cerebral cortex, and is lightly myelinated (see Figure S1 available online). However, we found that prostriata is unlike other limbic cortices in that its neurons consistently show robust responses to visual stimulation. Prostriata Neurons Have Short Response Latencies but Enormous Receptive Fields We explored the responses of neurons in prostriata to visual stimuli, using single-unit recordings in marmoset monkeys. Recording sites were reconstructed relative to electrolytic lesions and histological boundaries (Figure S1). As expected [15], neurons near the rostral boundary of V1 had receptive fields in the far periphery of the visual field (Figures 1B and 1C). Crossing the histological boundary into prostriata *Correspondence: hhyu00@gmail.com resulted in a clear-cut change in visual response properties. Among the most notable features, the receptive fields became much larger (>30 in length) and much less selective to direction of motion and orientation (Figures S1 and S4). A flashed patch of light was an effective stimulus for the vast majority (>90%) of prostriata neurons, allowing precise mapping of receptive fields. The responses were robust and consistent upon repeated stimulations, showing little sign of adaptation (Figure 2A; Figure S2). Using this stimulation, we compared the response properties of neurons in prostriata (n = 90) and in the far peripheral representation of V1 (n = 78). This analysis yielded three key observations. First, prostriata neurons had higher spontaneous firing rates than V1 neurons (13.9 Hz versus 3.8 Hz; one-sided Mann-Whitney test, U = 6,786, p < 0.01). Second, the median response strength (Figure 2B) was similar in prostriata and V1 (46.7 Hz versus 39.7 Hz; Mann-Whitney test, U = 4,128.5, p = 0.426). Third, the response onset latencies observed in prostriata were comparable to those observed in peripheral V1 under similar conditions (Figure 2C; Figure S2). The distribution of response latencies in prostriata suggested the possibility of two neuronal populations (Dip test of uniformity: D = 0.05, p = 0.034). Nonetheless, the distributions of latencies observed in these areas covered the same range (V1: ms; prostriata: ms) and had similar median values (55 ms versus 56 ms; Mann-Whitney test, U = 3,574, p = 0.840). Whenever tested, cells with receptive fields in the binocular part of the visual field responded to stimulation of either eye. We obtained receptive field maps of 73 prostriata neurons using a flashed square projected to locations on a grid (Figure 3). The receptive fields were typically not circularly symmetrical, and the strongest responses were often obtained away from their geometrical centers. The median width and length of the receptive fields in our sample were 29.9 and 49.1, respectively (Figure 3E); even the smallest receptive field (centered 60.9 from the fovea) was 25.3 long. On average, the area covered by each receptive field corresponded to 10% of the contralateral hemifield (Figure 3F; Figure S3). Notably, unlike in any other primate visual area described to date, there was no systematic relationship between eccentricity and receptive field area (Kendall s rank correlation coefficient t = , p = 0.367). The Representation of the Outer Limits of the Visual Field Is Emphasized in Prostriata In a unique pattern among all primate areas studied so far, most prostriata neurons had receptive fields centers (points of maximal response) at eccentricities (distances from the fovea) greater than 50 (Figure 3F). Nonetheless, when receptive borders were considered, they formed a complete representation of the contralateral hemifield (Figure S3). The representation of polar angle (angular deviation from the horizontal meridian of the visual field) was systematic, and congruent with that found in V1, but the representation of eccentricity was more irregular (Figures 1B and 1C). In general, neurons near the caudal border of prostriata with V1 had receptive field centers located in the far periphery of the visual field, and eccentricities decreased toward its rostral boundary. We

2 Current Biology Vol 22 No Figure 1. Location of Prostriata in the Marmoset Monkey and Its Visuotopic Organization (A) Ventromedial view of the layer-4 surface of the left hemisphere of a marmoset brain, showing the location of prostriata (ProSt) relative to surrounding cortical areas. Prostriata (red) is located near the rostral tip of the calcarine sulcus and is bounded dorsally by the retrosplenial cortex (area 23V, the ventral component of area 23, and area 30 [3]). The insert indicates the magnified region in relation to the rest of the brain. The abbreviations follow a recent review of the organization of the cerebral cortex of the marmoset [36]. (B) The polar angles of the centers of the excitatory receptive fields of 201 neurons (color-coded according to the wheel shown in the inset) recorded in prostriata and V1 in one animal. The recording sites are displayed on a computationally flattened reconstruction of the rostral part of the calcarine sulcus and adjacent retrosplenial cortex. Dashed lines indicate the histological boundaries of V1 and prostriata. The gray level represents the curvature of the cortex. Dark regions are concave (e.g., the fundus of the calcarine sulcus), and bright regions are convex. (C) The eccentricity of the receptive fields is shown in (B). Insert abbreviations: C, caudal; L, lateral; M, medial; R, rostral; D, dorsal; V, ventral. observed a distinct population of neurons (12/85; 14%) that responded to stimulation of two regions in the visual field, typically one central and the other peripheral (Figure 3D). Although it is possible that some of these responses may reflect recordings from two adjacent units, this relatively frequent observation reinforces the impression of an unusual visual topography.

3 Visual Function of Area Prostriata 1353 Prostriata Neurons Are Selective to Stimulus Speed, but Not Direction of Motion We used three types of visual stimuli to investigate the tuning properties of prostriata neurons: moving bars, kinetic dot fields, and sinusoidal drifting gratings. This analysis was based on 98 units with receptive fields centered between 40 and 70 eccentricity. Moving bars were the most effective type of stimulus, consistently eliciting higher response rates and more clearly defined tuning curves. We also experimented with other types of stimulus (e.g., looming disks and optical flow fields), but none of these proved more effective than simple moving bars. Prostriata neurons are poorly tuned to the orientation and direction of motion. One of the best-tuned units is illustrated in Figure 4A (see Figure S4 for additional examples). We used circular variance (cv) to quantify orientation selectivity [16]. Only 29.6% (29/98) of the neurons in our sample showed any hint of a bias in their responses to the orientation of moving bars (cv < 0.9). This is in contrast to the neighboring region of V1 (Figure 4B), where 74% of neurons showed orientation bias using the same criterion (median values: prostriata, cv = 0.93; V1, cv = 0.72; Mann-Whitney test, U = 14,213; p < 0.05). Only one prostriata neuron in our sample was strongly direction selective (direction index > 0.8). However, prostriata neurons exhibited clear tuning for the speed of the moving bar, with a distinct preference for very high speeds (Figure 4C; Figure S4). The median value of the optimal bar speed was /s (Figure 4D), and the median half-width half-height bandwidth value was 2.1 octaves. Figure 2. Responses of Prostriata Neurons to Brief Flashes of Uniform, Bright Stimuli (A) Raster diagram of the responses of a prostriata neuron to 80 consecutive flashes. The pink region indicates the time interval (0.2 s) during which the stimulus was present. The response pattern of this cell is summarized in the lower panel, which illustrates the peristimulus time histogram of the responses. The response onset latency (35 ms, indicated by the blue vertical line) was estimated using a maximal likelihood estimator of the change-point from spontaneous firing to stimulus-evoked responses [37]. The receptive field of this neuron (eccentricity 63 ) was 59 in diameter. The responses of several additional neurons to a similar test are illustrated in Figure S2. Prostriata Neurons Respond Well to Relatively Small and Sparse Stimuli We tested the selectivity of prostriata neurons to the length of bars presented at a near-optimal direction of motion and speed. Somewhat surprisingly, given the large receptive fields, the majority of neurons (68.4%, 67/98) showed little response modulation according to bar length. Among the 24 neurons that did show selectivity (24.5%), most increased their firing rate monotonically with length (Figure 4E). Using an integralof-gaussian model, the length summation field of the unit illustrated in Figure 4E was estimated to be 38.6, which was close to the size of the neuron s excitatory receptive field. Note, however, that much shorter bars could reliably activate this cell (half the above-baseline response was achieved using a 12.6 long bar). A small group of cells (7.1%, 7/98) showed statistically significant suppression in response to longer bars (Figure S4). Despite comprehensive tests employing variations of field diameter, direction of motion, speed, and density, kinetic dot fields proved to be less effective than bars, usually eliciting responses that were just above the spontaneous activity. Some prostriata neurons, however, did show selectivity to the density of dot fields (Figure 4F). In this test, the individual dots were 2 in diameter and moved at a speed of 100 /s. The optimal density was found to be as low as 4.2 dots per steradian, which amounted to only two or three dots moving within the entire receptive field at any time. Of 56 units that (B) Comparison of the response strength (peak spike rate in 10 ms bins above spontaneous rate) of neurons in prostriata (light gray) and in far peripheral V1 (dark gray). (C) Comparison of response latency of prostriata (light gray) and far peripheral V1 (dark gray) neurons. Arrowheads above histograms indicate the median values.

4 Current Biology Vol 22 No Figure 3. Quantification of the Geometry of Receptive Fields in Prostriata (A D) Examples of receptive fields mapped by a bright square projected at a grid of locations. The colors represent the mean above-spontaneous firing rates, normalized to the peak values. The contours in (A) (C) illustrate the shape of the receptive fields quantified by a model based on the bivariate skewed-normal distribution (equation 1 in Supplemental Experimental Procedures). The contours represent sensitivity at 30%, 50%, 70%, and 90% of the peak value. (D) An example of a receptive field showing two spatially distinct regions of activation. Longitudes and latitudes on the hemisphere are plotted in 10 intervals. The horizontal and the vertical meridians are indicated by thicker lines. (E) Distribution of the length and width of the receptive fields of 73 units, estimated from the best-fit functions as the region showing responses within 30% of the peak value. (F) Relationship between receptive field size and eccentricity in a sample of units for which quantitative receptive field maps were obtained. The receptive field size is expressed as the ratio of the surface area of the best-fit function and the surface area of the visual hemifield (p). The distributions of receptive field size and eccentricity of the receptive field center are plotted as histograms, with the arrowheads indicating median values. See Figure S3 for additional data. were tested for selectivity to the density of the dot fields, 48.2% (27/56) had clear preferences for very low densities; others showed little modulation (Figure S4). Prostriata Neurons Are Relatively Insensitive to Spatial and Temporal Modulation of the Stimuli Drifting sinusoidal gratings were generally ineffective stimuli. Most prostriata neurons responded transiently to the onset of the gratings but showed weak sustained responses. After removing initial transient responses from the spike trains, only 6 of 76 units (9.2%) were classified as simple using the F1/F0 > 1 criterion [17]. Thirty-nine neurons (51.3%) exhibited selectivity to spatial frequency (Figure S4), with a median preferred value that was much lower than that obtained in the far peripheral representation of V1 under similar conditions (0.03 c/ versus 0.14 c/ ). In addition, 46 neurons (60.5%) showed selectivity to temporal frequency; again, the median optimal value (1.4 Hz) was lower than that observed in V1 (4.0 Hz) [18]. Discussion Although reports of connections with visual areas [4 6] and responses to light [19 21] can be found scattered in the literature, there has been no study of the physiological properties of prostriata neurons. Prostriata has been traditionally regarded as a part of the retrosplenial limbic cortex [3]. Given the involvement of the retrosplenial cortex in memory, navigation, and other cognitive functions [22], it has been natural to expect that prostriata neurons exhibit physiological characteristics of higher-order association areas. This expectation was reinforced by tracer injection studies, which have demonstrated monosynaptic connections to a wide range of cortical systems, including not only visual [4 6], auditory [7, 8], and motor areas [2], but also association areas in the superior temporal [9], cingulate [10], parahippocampal [11], posterior parietal [7], medial prefrontal [12], orbitofrontal [13], and frontopolar [14] regions. Thus, the physiological properties of prostriata neurons revealed by our experiments were unexpected. These results indicate the existence of a significant, and yet unexplored anatomical bypass whereby peripheral vision can exert rapid and widespread effects over behavior and cognition. The response properties of prostriata neurons are highly unusual in light of expectations based on the hierarchical model of the organization of the visual cortex [23]. According to this model, as visual information is transmitted from V1 to areas at successively higher hierarchical levels, receptive fields become larger, response latencies longer, and stimulus selectivities more elaborate. Some of the response

5 Visual Function of Area Prostriata 1355 Figure 4. Tuning Characteristics of Prostriata Neurons (A) Direction tuning curves of a prostriata neuron, measured using a moving bar (solid line) and a kinetic dot field (dashed line). The speed of both stimuli was 100 /s. The curve measured with the moving bar is plotted in polar coordinates in the inset. This particular neuron (eccentricity was 70 ) was more selective than most of our samples (circular variance = 0.89). Tuning curves from eight additional cells are shown in Figure S4 (top row). The green bar indicates the estimated optimal direction. (B) The distributions of circular variance of prostriata (light gray) and V1 (dark gray) neurons. Larger value is less selective. (C) Speed tuning curve of a prostriata neuron. The optimal speed for this cell was /s (see Figure S4, second row, for additional examples). The interval between the two vertical green lines is the half-width-half-height bandwidth, which was estimated to be 2.2 octaves. (D) Distribution of the preferred speed of 87 prostriata neurons, measured with moving bars (median = /s). (E) Spatial summation curves of the same neuron shown in (A), illustrating the relationship between neuronal response and bar length (solid lines) or diameter of the dot field (dashed lines). Both the bar and the dot field moved at the speed of 100 /s. The green bars represent bar lengths at which maximal response, as well as half the maximal response, were elicited; see Figure S4 (third row) for other examples. (F) Turning curve of a neuron in response to variations of the density of the kinetic dot fields. The dots were 2 in diameter and moved at 100 /s. The field diameter was kept constant across the tests, being sufficient to cover the entire receptive field of the neuron. Dot density is expressed as the number of dots per steradian (dots/sr), which is equivalent to the number of dots on a circular patch 65.5 in diameter (see also Figure S4, fourth row). In all graphs, error bars represent SEM, arrowheads indicate spontaneous firing rates, and blue curves are best-fit functions. characteristics we observed in prostriata are usually associated with very early stages of the visual system, such as the superior colliculus, the lateral geniculate nucleus, and the granular layer of V1. These include high spontaneous activity, robust and short latency responses to visual stimuli, lack of adaptation to repeated stimulation, lack of selectivity to orientation and direction of motion, and broadband responses to spatial and temporal frequency [24, 25]. However, the receptive fields of neurons in prostriata are comparable in size to, or larger than, those found in high-level visual areas of the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex [26, 27]. At the same time, this area shows unique physiological features, such as emphasis in peripheral vision, and receptive fields of constant size across the visual field. Thus, prostriata neurons exhibit few of the defining characteristics of either the dorsal or the ventral streams of visual areas [28, 29], and

6 Current Biology Vol 22 No it is conceivable that this area operates in parallel with these better-characterized information processing pathways. One of the main questions raised by the present results is the origin of the short-latency visual activation of prostriata neurons. One possibility is that these responses derive from cortical areas such as V1 and the middle temporal area, MT [4, 6]. However, given the lack of selectivity of prostriata neurons to spatiotemporal features of the stimulus, this would require an unusual circuit, whereby its receptive fields are generated by nonselective convergence of many neurons with different response properties. Indeed, it is worth entertaining the possibility of an independent subcortical visual pathway to this area, as suggested by previous results in the tree shrew [30]. However, results of retrograde tracer injections in prostriata have never been reported. Prostriata s histological characteristics suggest that it is an evolutionarily ancient area, likely to be shared by most mammals. Whereas visually responsive areas adjacent to peripheral V1 have been described in other species [31 33], their exact homology with prostriata remains uncertain. In cats and flying foxes, lightly myelinated, limbic-type cortices adjoin a much larger proportion of the perimeter of V1 [21, 33]. However, visual responses are not observed throughout this region, suggesting additional areas. In addition, information about physiological response properties is available only for the cat splenial visual area, where neurons differ from those in prostriata by showing sharp orientation selectivities [31]. The present findings are likely to provide a much-needed basis for comparative studies and may prompt noninvasive studies of human brain activity in response to stimulation of peripheral visual fields. Our results demonstrate that cells in prostriata have clearly defined (albeit large) receptive fields and respond well to relatively small, fast-moving objects. This argues against the notion that prostriata cells are simply measuring luminous flux. Features such as the large, uniformly sized receptive fields, emphasis on peripheral representation, and lack of selectivity to spatiotemporal features of the stimulus also seem to exclude a role in high acuity vision. On the other hand, these same features, together with the short-latency activation and widespread connectivity, are compatible with a function in monitoring peripheral visual space for new, unexpected stimuli, and perhaps triggering coordinated defensive responses and shifts in the focus of attention. The observed topographic sampling of the visual field, together with the known connections with areas involved in auditory localization [7, 8] and motor control, particularly of the head and upper limb musculature [2], seem compatible with this idea. Indeed, an interesting possibility suggested by recent studies is that disturbances of a cortical circuit involving prostriata are related to the higher sensitivity to peripheral visual stimulation observed in patients with panic disorder and agoraphobia [34]. Thus, we suggest that prostriata may provide a limbic pathway by which visual information can provide relatively coarse but fast spatial information to command coordinated responses across multiple cortical systems, in situations that demand rapid action. This hypothesis may prove a fruitful guide for future studies exploring its role in behavior and mapping its full network of connections. Experimental Procedures Extracellular recordings were obtained from six anesthetized adult marmoset monkeys. Details of the preparation, visual stimuli, methods of analysis, and histological criteria are given in Supplemental Experimental Procedures. Briefly, tungsten microelectrodes (w1 MU) were directed toward the anterior end of V1 and prostriata. Given the need to explore the far periphery of the visual field, we used panoramic stimuli projected to a hemispherical screen [35]. This method uses a mathematical transformation to project geometrically undistorted stimuli across a wide expanse of visual field, allowing quantitative assessment of receptive field properties (Movie S1). At the end of each experiment, the brains were prepared for histology, and series of sections stained for cell bodies, myelin, and cytochrome oxidase were used to reconstruct the location of every recording site relative to histological boundaries and electrolytic lesions (Figure S1). The experiments were conducted in accordance with the Australian Code of Practice for the Care and Use of Animals for Scientific Purposes. All procedures were approved by the Monash University Animal Ethics Experimentation Committee. Supplemental Information Supplemental Information includes four figures, Supplemental Experimental Procedures, and one movie and can be found with this article online at doi: /j.cub Acknowledgments We thank Katrina Worthy for histological and technical support, Rowan Tweedale for comments on the manuscript, and Janssen-Cilag Pty for the donation of sufentanil citrate. This work was funded by the Australian Research Council (ARC, DP ) and National Health and Medical Research Council (Grant ). The Monash Vision Group is funded by the ARC Special Research Initiative in Bionic Vision and Technology. Received: April 2, 2012 Revised: May 14, 2012 Accepted: May 14, 2012 Published online: June 14, 2012 References 1. Sanides, F. (1972). Representation in the cerebral cortex and its areal lamination patterns. In The Structure and Function of the Nervous Tissue, Volume 5, G.F. Bourne, ed. (New York: Academic Press), pp Morecraft, R.J., Rockland, K.S., and Van Hoesen, G.W. (2000). Localization of area prostriata and its projection to the cingulate motor cortex in the rhesus monkey. Cereb. Cortex 10, Kobayashi, Y., and Amaral, D.G. (2000). Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex: I. three-dimensional and cytoarchitectonic organization. J. Comp. Neurol. 426, Sousa, A.P.B., Piñon, M.C.G.P., Gattass, R., and Rosa, M.G.P. (1991). Topographic organization of cortical input to striate cortex in the Cebus monkey: a fluorescent tracer study. J. Comp. Neurol. 308, Gattass, R., Sousa, A.P.B., Mishkin, M., and Ungerleider, L.G. (1997). Cortical projections of area V2 in the macaque. Cereb. Cortex 7, Palmer, S.M., and Rosa, M.G.P. (2006). A distinct anatomical network of cortical areas for analysis of motion in far peripheral vision. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, Rockland, K.S., and Ojima, H. (2003). Multisensory convergence in calcarine visual areas in macaque monkey. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 50, Falchier, A., Schroeder, C.E., Hackett, T.A., Lakatos, P., Nascimento- Silva, S., Ulbert, I., Karmos, G., and Smiley, J.F. (2010). Projection from visual areas V2 and prostriata to caudal auditory cortex in the monkey. Cereb. Cortex 20, Seltzer, B., and Pandya, D.N. (1994). Parietal, temporal, and occipital projections to cortex of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey: a retrograde tracer study. J. Comp. Neurol. 343, Morecraft, R.J., Cipolloni, P.B., Stilwell-Morecraft, K.S., Gedney, M.T., and Pandya, D.N. (2004). Cytoarchitecture and cortical connections of the posterior cingulate and adjacent somatosensory fields in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 469, Blatt, G.J., Pandya, D.N., and Rosene, D.L. (2003). Parcellation of cortical afferents to three distinct sectors in the parahippocampal gyrus of the rhesus monkey: an anatomical and neurophysiological study. J. Comp. Neurol. 466,

7 Visual Function of Area Prostriata Barbas, H. (1988). Anatomic organization of basoventral and mediodorsal visual recipient prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 276, Cavada, C., Compañy, T., Tejedor, J., Cruz-Rizzolo, R.J., and Reinoso- Suárez, F. (2000). The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. A review. Cereb. Cortex 10, Burman, K.J., Reser, D.H., Yu, H.-H., and Rosa, M.G.P. (2011). Cortical input to the frontal pole of the marmoset monkey. Cereb. Cortex 21, Fritsches, K.A., and Rosa, M.G.P. (1996). Visuotopic organisation of striate cortex in the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus). J. Comp. Neurol. 372, Ringach, D.L., Shapley, R.M., and Hawken, M.J. (2002). Orientation selectivity in macaque V1: diversity and laminar dependence. J. Neurosci. 22, Skottun, B.C., De Valois, R.L., Grosof, D.H., Movshon, J.A., Albrecht, D.G., and Bonds, A.B. (1991). Classifying simple and complex cells on the basis of response modulation. Vision Res. 3, Yu, H.-H., Verma, R., Yang, Y., Tibballs, H.A., Lui, L.L., Reser, D.H., and Rosa, M.G.P. (2010). Spatial and temporal frequency tuning in striate cortex: functional uniformity and specializations related to receptive field eccentricity. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, Cuénod, M., Casey, K.L., and MacLean, P.D. (1965). Unit analysis of visual input to posterior limbic cortex. I. Photic stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 28, Van Essen, D.C., Newsome, W.T., and Maunsell, J.H.R. (1984). The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability. Vision Res. 24, Rosa, M.G.P., Casagrande, V.A., Preuss, T., and Kaas, J.H. (1997). Visual field representation in striate and prestriate cortices of a prosimian primate (Galago garnetti). J. Neurophysiol. 77, Vann, S.D., Aggleton, J.P., and Maguire, E.A. (2009). What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, Felleman, D.J., and Van Essen, D.C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 1, Marrocco, R.T., and Li, R.H. (1977). Monkey superior colliculus: properties of single cells and their afferent inputs. J. Neurophysiol. 40, Forte, J.D., Hashemi-Nezhad, M., Dobbie, W.J., Dreher, B., and Martin, P.R. (2005). Spatial coding and response redundancy in parallel visual pathways of the marmoset Callithrix jacchus. Vis. Neurosci. 22, Desimone, R., and Gross, C.G. (1979). Visual areas in the temporal cortex of the macaque. Brain Res. 178, Motter, B.C., and Mountcastle, V.B. (1981). The functional properties of the light-sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex studied in waking monkeys: foveal sparing and opponent vector organization. J. Neurosci. 1, Ungerleider, L.G., and Haxby, J.V. (1994). What and where in the human brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 4, Goodale, M.A. (2008). Action without perception in human vision. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 25, Conrad, C.D., and Stumpf, W.E. (1975). Direct visual input to the limbic system: crossed retinal projections to the nucleus anterodorsalis thalami in the tree shrew. Exp. Brain Res. 23, Kalia, M., and Whitteridge, D. (1973). The visual areas in the splenial sulcus of the cat. J. Physiol. 232, Montero, V.M., Rojas, A., and Torrealba, F. (1973). Retinotopic organization of striate and peristriate visual cortex in the albino rat. Brain Res. 53, Rosa, M.G.P. (1999). Topographic organization of extrastriate areas in the flying fox: implications for the evolution of mammalian visual cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 441, Caldirola, D., Teggi, R., Bondi, S., Lopes, F.L., Grassi, M., Bussi, M., and Perna, G. (2011). Is there a hypersensitive visual alarm system in panic disorder? Psychiatry Res. 187, Yu, H.-H., and Rosa, M.G.P. (2010). A simple method for creating widefield visual stimulus for electrophysiology: mapping and analyzing receptive fields using a hemispheric display. J. Vis. 10, Paxinos, G., Watson, C., Petrides, M., Rosa, M.G.P., and Tokuno, H. (2012). The marmoset brain in stereotaxic coordinates (New York, NY: Academic Press). 37. Friedman, H.S., and Priebe, C.E. (1998). Estimating stimulus response latency. J. Neurosci. Methods 83,

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Fani Deligianni B. A. Wandell, et al. Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex, Neuron, 56(2):366-383, 2007 Retinotopy Visual Cortex organised in visual field maps: Nearby neurons have

More information

The Visual System. Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004

The Visual System. Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004 The Visual System Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004 Phone: 343-4538 Email: vivien.casagrande@mcmail.vanderbilt.edu Office: T2302 MCN Required Reading Adler s Physiology of the Eye Chapters

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

Adventures into terra incognita

Adventures into terra incognita BEWARE: These are preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. Chapter VI. Adventures into terra incognita In primary visual cortex there are neurons

More information

Parallel streams of visual processing

Parallel streams of visual processing Parallel streams of visual processing RETINAL GANGLION CELL AXONS: OPTIC TRACT Optic nerve Optic tract Optic chiasm Lateral geniculate nucleus Hypothalamus: regulation of circadian rhythms Pretectum: reflex

More information

Neuronal responses to plaids

Neuronal responses to plaids Vision Research 39 (1999) 2151 2156 Neuronal responses to plaids Bernt Christian Skottun * Skottun Research, 273 Mather Street, Piedmont, CA 94611-5154, USA Received 30 June 1998; received in revised form

More information

Functional Response Properties of Neurons in the Dorsomedial Visual Area of New World Monkeys (Callithrix jacchus)

Functional Response Properties of Neurons in the Dorsomedial Visual Area of New World Monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) Cerebral Cortex February 2006;16:162-177 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi094 Advance Access publication April 27, 2005 Functional Response Properties of Neurons in the Dorsomedial Visual Area of New World Monkeys

More information

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization THE VISUAL SYSTEM Higher Visual Processing Martin Paré Assistant Professor Physiology & Psychology Most of the cortex that covers the cerebral hemispheres is

More information

Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the Macaque

Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the Macaque Cerebral Cortex March 2008;18:477-499 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm061 Advance Access publication June 4, 2007 Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the Macaque Leslie G. Ungerleider 1, Thelma W. Galkin 2, Robert

More information

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception 1 Virginia R. de Sa Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Lecture 5: LGN and V1: Magno and Parvo streams Chapter 3 Course Information 2 Class web page: http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Functional Specialization of Seven Mouse Visual Cortical Areas

Functional Specialization of Seven Mouse Visual Cortical Areas Article Functional Specialization of Seven Mouse Visual Cortical Areas James H. Marshel, 1,2,3 Marina E. Garrett, 1,2,3 Ian Nauhaus, 1 and Edward M. Callaway 1,2, * 1 Systems Neurobiology Laboratories,

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision Biological Bases of Behavior 6: Vision Sensory Systems The brain detects events in the external environment and directs the contractions of the muscles Afferent neurons carry sensory messages to brain

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones Photoreceptors Rods Cones 120 000 000 Dim light Prefer wavelength of 505 nm Monochromatic Mainly in periphery of the eye 6 000 000 More light Different spectral sensitivities!long-wave receptors (558 nm)

More information

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye: Problems confronting the visual system: Visual Physiology image contains a huge amount of information which must be processed quickly. image is dim, blurry and distorted. Light levels vary enormously.

More information

Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (1) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (1) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang 580.626 Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (1) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang Laboratory of Auditory Neurophysiology Department of Biomedical Engineering Johns

More information

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) What happens beyond the retina? What happens in Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)- 90% flow Visual cortex Information Flow Superior colliculus 10% flow Slide 2 Information

More information

The Eye. Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. Today s goals. 5 From eye to brain. Today s reading

The Eye. Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. Today s goals. 5 From eye to brain. Today s reading Cognitive Neuroscience of Language 5 From eye to brain Today s goals Look at the pathways that conduct the visual information from the eye to the visual cortex Marielle Lange http://homepages.inf.ed.ac.uk/mlange/teaching/cnl/

More information

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I Monocular Sensory Processes of Vision: Color Vision Mechanisms of Color Processing . Neural Mechanisms of Color Processing A. Parallel processing - M- & P- pathways B. Second

More information

Multiple Visual Areas in the Caudal Superior Temporal Sulcus of the Macaque

Multiple Visual Areas in the Caudal Superior Temporal Sulcus of the Macaque THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 248:164-189 (1986) Multiple Visual Areas in the Caudal Superior Temporal Sulcus of the Macaque ROBERT DESIMONE AND LESLIE G. UNGERLEIDER Laboratory of Neuropsychology,

More information

Neuroscience Tutorial

Neuroscience Tutorial Neuroscience Tutorial Brain Organization : cortex, basal ganglia, limbic lobe : thalamus, hypothal., pituitary gland : medulla oblongata, midbrain, pons, cerebellum Cortical Organization Cortical Organization

More information

Nature Neuroscience doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Characterization of viral injections.

Nature Neuroscience doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Characterization of viral injections. Supplementary Figure 1 Characterization of viral injections. (a) Dorsal view of a mouse brain (dashed white outline) after receiving a large, unilateral thalamic injection (~100 nl); demonstrating that

More information

Third tier ventral extrastriate cortex in the New World monkey, Cebus apella

Third tier ventral extrastriate cortex in the New World monkey, Cebus apella Exp Brain Res (2000) 132:287 305 Digital Object Identifier (DOI) 10.1007/s002210000344 RESEARCH ARTICLE Marcello G.P. Rosa Maria Carmen Piñon Ricardo Gattass Aglai P. B. Sousa Third tier ventral extrastriate

More information

1. The responses of on-center and off-center retinal ganglion cells

1. The responses of on-center and off-center retinal ganglion cells 1. The responses of on-center and off-center retinal ganglion cells 2. Responses of an on-center ganglion cell to different light conditions 3. Responses of an on-center ganglion cells to different light

More information

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited by law: any public performance or display,

More information

Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons

Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons Vincent Bonin *, Valerio Mante and Matteo Carandini Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute 2318 Fillmore Street San Francisco, CA 94115, USA Institute of Neuroinformatics

More information

PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study. Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision

PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study. Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision 1. Explain what a hierarchically organised neural system is, in terms of physiological response properties of its neurones.

More information

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences Marc Pomplun 1, Yueju Liu 2, Julio Martinez-Trujillo 2, Evgueni Simine 2, and John K. Tsotsos 2 1 Department

More information

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J. The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem By Andrew Laguna, S.J. Outline I. Introduction II. The Visual System III. What is the Binding Problem? IV. Possible Theoretical Solutions

More information

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!)

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!) Introduction to Physiological Psychology Vision ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ /~ksweeney/psy260.html Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy (the part that s visible to us!)

More information

Privileged Processing of the Straight-Ahead Direction in Primate Area V1

Privileged Processing of the Straight-Ahead Direction in Primate Area V1 Article Privileged Processing of the Straight-Ahead Direction in Primate Area V1 Jean-Baptiste Durand, 1,2 Yves Trotter, 1,2 and Simona Celebrini 1,2, * 1 Université de Toulouse; UPS; Centre de Recherche

More information

Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe

Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe How many extrastriate areas are there? LOTS!!! Macaque monkey flattened cortex Why? How do we know this? Topography Functional properties Connections

More information

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling. Lect21-22: Vision Central Pathways

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling. Lect21-22: Vision Central Pathways CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect02: BOSS Discrete Event Simulator Lect21-22: Vision Central Pathways Chapter 12 of Purves et al., 4e Larry Wittie Computer Science, StonyBrook

More information

Area MT Neurons Respond to Visual Motion Distant From Their Receptive Fields

Area MT Neurons Respond to Visual Motion Distant From Their Receptive Fields J Neurophysiol 94: 4156 4167, 2005. First published August 24, 2005; doi:10.1152/jn.00505.2005. Area MT Neurons Respond to Visual Motion Distant From Their Receptive Fields Daniel Zaksas and Tatiana Pasternak

More information

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Baran Çürüklü Department of Computer Science and Engineering Mälardalen University Västerås S-721 23, Sweden Abstract. In this paper

More information

Reappraisal of DL/V4 Boundaries Based on Connectivity Patterns of Dorsolateral Visual Cortex in Macaques

Reappraisal of DL/V4 Boundaries Based on Connectivity Patterns of Dorsolateral Visual Cortex in Macaques Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published September 30, 2004 Reappraisal of DL/V4 Boundaries Based on Connectivity Patterns of Dorsolateral Visual Cortex in Macaques Iwona Stepniewska, Christine E. Collins

More information

Exploring the pulvinar path to visual cortex

Exploring the pulvinar path to visual cortex C. Kennard & R.J. Leigh (Eds.) Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 171 ISSN 0079-6123 Copyright r 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved CHAPTER 5.14 Exploring the pulvinar path to visual cortex Rebecca A.

More information

Effects of Attention on MT and MST Neuronal Activity During Pursuit Initiation

Effects of Attention on MT and MST Neuronal Activity During Pursuit Initiation Effects of Attention on MT and MST Neuronal Activity During Pursuit Initiation GREGG H. RECANZONE 1 AND ROBERT H. WURTZ 2 1 Center for Neuroscience and Section of Neurobiology, Physiology and Behavior,

More information

9.14 Classes #21-23: Visual systems

9.14 Classes #21-23: Visual systems 9.14 Classes #21-23: Visual systems Questions based on Schneider chapter 20 and classes: 1) What was in all likelihood the first functional role of the visual sense? Describe the nature of the most primitive

More information

Spatial Summation, End Inhibition and Side Inhibition in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT)

Spatial Summation, End Inhibition and Side Inhibition in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT) J Neurophysiol 97: 1135 1148, 2007. First published November 15, 2006; doi:10.1152/jn.01018.2006. Spatial Summation, End Inhibition and Side Inhibition in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT) Leo L. Lui,

More information

Visual Brain: The Neural Basis of Visual Perception!

Visual Brain: The Neural Basis of Visual Perception! Visual Brain: The Neural Basis of Visual Perception!?! Human Brain: Amazing Machine! Cerebral cortex! Highest level of all sensory integration Highest level of somatic motor control Memory, association

More information

Required Slide. Session Objectives

Required Slide. Session Objectives Vision: CNS 2018 Required Slide Session Objectives Visual system: CNS At the end of this session, students will be able to: 1. Understand how axons from the eyes travel through the optic nerves and tracts

More information

2/3/17. Visual System I. I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex

2/3/17. Visual System I. I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex 1 Visual System I I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex 2 1 2/3/17 Window of the Soul 3 Information Flow: From Photoreceptors

More information

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1 Announcements exam 1 this Thursday! review session: Wednesday, 5:00-6:30pm, Meliora 203 Bryce s office hours: Wednesday, 3:30-5:30pm, Gleason https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zdw7pvgz0um M Cells M cells

More information

Supplementary Information. Gauge size. midline. arcuate 10 < n < 15 5 < n < 10 1 < n < < n < 15 5 < n < 10 1 < n < 5. principal principal

Supplementary Information. Gauge size. midline. arcuate 10 < n < 15 5 < n < 10 1 < n < < n < 15 5 < n < 10 1 < n < 5. principal principal Supplementary Information set set = Reward = Reward Gauge size Gauge size 3 Numer of correct trials 3 Numer of correct trials Supplementary Fig.. Principle of the Gauge increase. The gauge size (y axis)

More information

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m. Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Wednesday, 16 March

More information

Neuroanatomy, Text and Atlas (J. H. Martin), 3 rd Edition Chapter 7, The Visual System, pp ,

Neuroanatomy, Text and Atlas (J. H. Martin), 3 rd Edition Chapter 7, The Visual System, pp , Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: RETINA and CENTRAL VISUAL PATHWAYS P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Friday, 20

More information

Visual area MT responds to local motion. Visual area MST responds to optic flow. Visual area STS responds to biological motion.

Visual area MT responds to local motion. Visual area MST responds to optic flow. Visual area STS responds to biological motion. Visual area responds to local motion MST V3a V3 Visual area MST responds to optic flow MST V3a V3 Visual area STS responds to biological motion STS Macaque visual areas Flattening the brain What is a visual

More information

Vision II. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota

Vision II. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota Vision II Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota 1 Ganglion Cells The axons of the retinal ganglion cells form the optic nerve and carry visual information into the brain. 2 Optic

More information

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey 3.6.QH Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey Versnel, Huib; Zwiers, Marcel; Van Opstal, John Department of Biophysics University of Nijmegen Geert Grooteplein 655

More information

The processing of feature discontinuities for different cue types in primary visual cortex

The processing of feature discontinuities for different cue types in primary visual cortex BRAIN RESEARCH 1238 (2008) 59 74 available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report The processing of feature discontinuities for different cue types in primary visual

More information

Pathway from the eye to the cortex

Pathway from the eye to the cortex Vision: CNS 2017 Pathway from the eye to the cortex Themes of this lecture Visual information is analyzed in more complicated ways than in the retina. One major pathway from the eye leads to the striate

More information

Behavioral generalization

Behavioral generalization Supplementary Figure 1 Behavioral generalization. a. Behavioral generalization curves in four Individual sessions. Shown is the conditioned response (CR, mean ± SEM), as a function of absolute (main) or

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

Supplementary Material for

Supplementary Material for Supplementary Material for Selective neuronal lapses precede human cognitive lapses following sleep deprivation Supplementary Table 1. Data acquisition details Session Patient Brain regions monitored Time

More information

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience Levels of organization Central Nervous System 1m 10 11 neurons Neural systems and neuroanatomy Systems 10cm Networks 1mm Neurons 100μm 10 8 neurons Professor Daniel Leeds

More information

LISC-322 Neuroscience. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation

LISC-322 Neuroscience. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation LISC-3 Neuroscience THE VISUAL SYSTEM Central Visual Pathways Each eye sees a part of the visual space that defines its visual field. The s of both eyes overlap extensively to create a binocular. eye both

More information

Cortical Control of Movement

Cortical Control of Movement Strick Lecture 2 March 24, 2006 Page 1 Cortical Control of Movement Four parts of this lecture: I) Anatomical Framework, II) Physiological Framework, III) Primary Motor Cortex Function and IV) Premotor

More information

Supplementary Information for Correlated input reveals coexisting coding schemes in a sensory cortex

Supplementary Information for Correlated input reveals coexisting coding schemes in a sensory cortex Supplementary Information for Correlated input reveals coexisting coding schemes in a sensory cortex Luc Estebanez 1,2 *, Sami El Boustani 1 *, Alain Destexhe 1, Daniel E. Shulz 1 1 Unité de Neurosciences,

More information

A Single Mechanism Can Explain the Speed Tuning Properties of MT and V1 Complex Neurons

A Single Mechanism Can Explain the Speed Tuning Properties of MT and V1 Complex Neurons The Journal of Neuroscience, November 15, 2006 26(46):11987 11991 11987 Brief Communications A Single Mechanism Can Explain the Speed Tuning Properties of MT and V1 Complex Neurons John A. Perrone Department

More information

Neurophysiology of systems

Neurophysiology of systems Neurophysiology of systems Motor cortex (voluntary movements) Dana Cohen, Room 410, tel: 7138 danacoh@gmail.com Voluntary movements vs. reflexes Same stimulus yields a different movement depending on context

More information

Models of Attention. Models of Attention

Models of Attention. Models of Attention Models of Models of predictive: can we predict eye movements (bottom up attention)? [L. Itti and coll] pop out and saliency? [Z. Li] Readings: Maunsell & Cook, the role of attention in visual processing,

More information

Localization of visually evoked cortical activity in humans.

Localization of visually evoked cortical activity in humans. Page 1 of 5 Journal List > J Physiol > v.360; Mar 1985 J Physiol. 1985 March; 360: 233 246. PMCID: PMC1193458 Copyright notice R Srebro This article has been cited by other articles in PMC. Abstract The

More information

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material Supplemental Material Recording technique Multi-unit activity (MUA) was recorded from electrodes that were chronically implanted (Teflon-coated platinum-iridium wires) in the primary visual cortex representing

More information

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner Cerebral Cortex 1 Sarah Heilbronner heilb028@umn.edu Want to meet? Coffee hour 10-11am Tuesday 11/27 Surdyk s Overview and organization of the cerebral cortex What is the cerebral cortex? Where is each

More information

Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning

Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning Maximilian Riesenhuber Tomaso Poggio Center for Biological and Computational Learning and Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences Massachusetts

More information

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15 s PSY 200 Greg Francis Lecture 06 How do you recognize your grandmother? Action potential With enough excitatory input, a cell produces an action potential that sends a signal down its axon to other cells

More information

Key questions about attention

Key questions about attention Key questions about attention How does attention affect behavioral performance? Can attention affect the appearance of things? How does spatial and feature-based attention affect neuronal responses in

More information

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception 1 Virginia R. de Sa Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Lecture 6: Beyond V1 - Extrastriate cortex Chapter 4 Course Information 2 Class web page: http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/

More information

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus Central Visual Pathways V1/2 NEUR 3001 dvanced Visual Neuroscience The Lateral Geniculate Nucleus () is more than a relay station LP SC Professor Tom Salt UCL Institute of Ophthalmology Retina t.salt@ucl.ac.uk

More information

Lecture overview. What hypothesis to test in the fly? Quantitative data collection Visual physiology conventions ( Methods )

Lecture overview. What hypothesis to test in the fly? Quantitative data collection Visual physiology conventions ( Methods ) Lecture overview What hypothesis to test in the fly? Quantitative data collection Visual physiology conventions ( Methods ) 1 Lecture overview What hypothesis to test in the fly? Quantitative data collection

More information

The Visual Parietal Areas in the Macaque Monkey: Current Structural Knowledge and Ignorance

The Visual Parietal Areas in the Macaque Monkey: Current Structural Knowledge and Ignorance NeuroImage 14, S21 S26 (2001) doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0818, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on The Visual Parietal Areas in the Macaque Monkey: Current Structural Knowledge and Ignorance Carmen

More information

Quantitative Analysis of the Corticocortical Projections to the Middle Temporal Area in the Marmoset Monkey: Evolutionary and Functional Implications

Quantitative Analysis of the Corticocortical Projections to the Middle Temporal Area in the Marmoset Monkey: Evolutionary and Functional Implications Cerebral Cortex September 2006;16:1361--1375 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj078 Advance Access publication November 16, 2005 Quantitative Analysis of the Corticocortical Projections to the Middle Temporal Area

More information

To Accompany: Thalamic Synchrony and the Adaptive Gating of Information Flow to Cortex Wang, Webber, & Stanley

To Accompany: Thalamic Synchrony and the Adaptive Gating of Information Flow to Cortex Wang, Webber, & Stanley SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL To Accompany: Thalamic Synchrony and the Adaptive Gating of Information Flow to Cortex Wang, Webber, & Stanley Supplementary Note 1: Parametric fits of spike count distributions

More information

Chapter 7: First steps into inferior temporal cortex

Chapter 7: First steps into inferior temporal cortex BEWARE: These are preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. Chapter 7: First steps into inferior temporal cortex Inferior temporal cortex (ITC)

More information

Selectivity and Spatial Distribution of Signals From the Receptive Field Surround in Macaque V1 Neurons

Selectivity and Spatial Distribution of Signals From the Receptive Field Surround in Macaque V1 Neurons J Neurophysiol 88: 2547 2556, 2002; 10.1152/jn.00693.2001. Selectivity and Spatial Distribution of Signals From the Receptive Field Surround in Macaque V1 Neurons JAMES R. CAVANAUGH, 2 WYETH BAIR, 1,2

More information

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch!

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch! Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch! How do we perceive objects held in the hand?! Touch receptors deconstruct objects to detect local features! Information is transmitted in parallel

More information

Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex

Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex David Whitney,* 1 Herbert C. Goltz, 2 Christopher G. Thomas, 1 Joseph S. Gati, 2 Ravi S. Menon, 2 Melvyn A. Goodale 1 1 The Department

More information

London CYVlE 6BT. animals, by plotting receptive field positions for different recording sites. Results

London CYVlE 6BT. animals, by plotting receptive field positions for different recording sites. Results J. Physiol. (1978), 277, pp. 193-226 193 With 3 plates and 14 text-figures Printed in Great Britain THE TOPOGRAPHIC ORGANIZATION OF RHESUS MONKEY PRESTRIATE CORTEX BY D. C. VAN ESSEN* AND S. M. ZEKIt From

More information

The detection of visual signals by macaque frontal eye field during masking

The detection of visual signals by macaque frontal eye field during masking articles The detection of visual signals by macaque frontal eye field during masking Kirk G. Thompson and Jeffrey D. Schall Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Department of Psychology, Wilson Hall, Vanderbilt

More information

The Neuroscience of Vision II

The Neuroscience of Vision II Striate Cortex (V1) is not Sufficient for Seeing The Neuroscience of Vision II Hubel and Wiesel conclude their 1968 paper by Where else is Visual Processing Performed? Beyond V1 An important step in identifying

More information

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Overview Explain the significance of the turn to the brain in cognitive science Explain Mishkin and Ungerleider s hypothesis that there are two distinct visual systems Outline

More information

Reward prediction based on stimulus categorization in. primate lateral prefrontal cortex

Reward prediction based on stimulus categorization in. primate lateral prefrontal cortex Reward prediction based on stimulus categorization in primate lateral prefrontal cortex Xiaochuan Pan, Kosuke Sawa, Ichiro Tsuda, Minoro Tsukada, Masamichi Sakagami Supplementary Information This PDF file

More information

THE INFLUENCE OF THE ANGLE OF GAZE UPON THE EXCITABILITY OF THE LIGHT-SENSITIVE NEURONS OF THE POSTERIOR PARIETAL CORTEX1

THE INFLUENCE OF THE ANGLE OF GAZE UPON THE EXCITABILITY OF THE LIGHT-SENSITIVE NEURONS OF THE POSTERIOR PARIETAL CORTEX1 0270~6474830303-0532$02.00O Copyright 0 Society for Neuroscience Printed in U.S.A. The Journal of Neuroscience Vol. 3, No. 3, pp. 532-548 March 1983 THE INFLUENCE OF THE ANGLE OF GAZE UPON THE EXCITABILITY

More information

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Neuron, Volume 70 Supplemental Information Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Luke J. Chang, Alec Smith, Martin Dufwenberg, and Alan G. Sanfey Supplemental Information

More information

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing C Contextual Influences in Visual Processing TAI SING LEE Computer Science Department and Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Synonyms Surround influence;

More information

Decoding a Perceptual Decision Process across Cortex

Decoding a Perceptual Decision Process across Cortex Article Decoding a Perceptual Decision Process across Cortex Adrián Hernández, 1 Verónica Nácher, 1 Rogelio Luna, 1 Antonio Zainos, 1 Luis Lemus, 1 Manuel Alvarez, 1 Yuriria Vázquez, 1 Liliana Camarillo,

More information

Single cell tuning curves vs population response. Encoding: Summary. Overview of the visual cortex. Overview of the visual cortex

Single cell tuning curves vs population response. Encoding: Summary. Overview of the visual cortex. Overview of the visual cortex Encoding: Summary Spikes are the important signals in the brain. What is still debated is the code: number of spikes, exact spike timing, temporal relationship between neurons activities? Single cell tuning

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention Cognitive Neuroscience Attention There are many aspects to attention. It can be controlled. It can be focused on a particular sensory modality or item. It can be divided. It can set a perceptual system.

More information

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1 Vision and Action Our ability to move thru our environment is closely tied to visual perception. Simple examples include standing one one foot. It is easier to maintain balance with the eyes open than

More information

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 05 Why do you need bright light to read? Rods and cones Photoreceptors are not evenly distributed across the retina 1 Rods and cones Cones are most dense in

More information

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings J. Besle, P. Lakatos, C.A. Schevon, R.R. Goodman, G.M. McKhann, A. Mehta, R.G. Emerson, C.E.

More information

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System Lecturer: Jitendra Malik Scribe: Ryan White (Slide: layout of the brain) Facts about the brain:

More information

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the Some transformations of color information from lateral geniculate nucleus to striate cortex Russell L. De Valois*, Nicolas P. Cottaris, Sylvia D. Elfar*, Luke E. Mahon, and J. Anthony Wilson* *Psychology

More information

Pre-Attentive Visual Selection

Pre-Attentive Visual Selection Pre-Attentive Visual Selection Li Zhaoping a, Peter Dayan b a University College London, Dept. of Psychology, UK b University College London, Gatsby Computational Neuroscience Unit, UK Correspondence to

More information

Time Course and Time Distance Relationships for Surround Suppression in Macaque V1 Neurons

Time Course and Time Distance Relationships for Surround Suppression in Macaque V1 Neurons 7690 The Journal of Neuroscience, August 20, 2003 23(20):7690 7701 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Time Course and Time Distance Relationships for Surround Suppression in Macaque V1 Neurons Wyeth Bair, 1,2

More information

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017 V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8 Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017 Topography: mapping of objects in space onto the visual cortex contralateral representation

More information

Physiological Correlates of Perceptual Learning in Monkey V1 and V2

Physiological Correlates of Perceptual Learning in Monkey V1 and V2 J Neurophysiol 87: 1867 1888, 2002; 10.1152/jn.00690.2001. Physiological Correlates of Perceptual Learning in Monkey V1 and V2 GEOFFREY M. GHOSE, TIANMING YANG, AND JOHN H. R. MAUNSELL Division of Neuroscience

More information