NeuroImage 49 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "NeuroImage 49 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:"

Transcription

1 NeuroImage 49 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: A common functional brain network for autobiographical, episodic, and semantic memory retrieval Hana Burianova a,b,, Anthony R. McIntosh a,b, Cheryl L. Grady a,b,c a Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada b Rotman Research Institute at Baycrest, Toronto, Ontario, Canada c Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada article info abstract Article history: Received 22 April 2009 Revised 9 August 2009 Accepted 31 August 2009 Available online 8 September 2009 The objective of this study was to delineate a common functional network that underlies autobiographical, episodic, and semantic memory retrieval. We conducted an event-related fmri study in which we utilized the same pictorial stimuli, but manipulated retrieval demands to extract autobiographical, episodic, or semantic memories. To assess this common network, we first examined the functional connectivity of regions identified by a previous analysis of task-related activity that were active across all three tasks. Three of these regions (left hippocampus, left lingual gyrus, and right caudate nucleus) appeared to share a common pattern of connectivity. This was confirmed in a subsequent functional connectivity analysis using these three regions as seeds. The results of this analysis showed that there was a pattern of functional connectivity that characterized all three seeds and that was common across the three retrieval conditions. Activity in inferior frontal and middle temporal cortex bilaterally, left temporoparietal junction, and anterior and posterior cingulate gyri was positively correlated with the seeds, whereas activity in posterior occipitotemporo-parietal regions was negatively correlated. These findings support the idea that a common neural network underlies the retrieval of declarative memories regardless of memory content. This proposed network consists of increased activity in regions that represent internal processes of memory retrieval and decreased activity in regions that mediate attention to external stimuli Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Introduction Despite extensive research in the past several decades, a consensus has yet to be reached as to the neural organization of declarative memory retrieval. Conceptually, declarative memory retrieval has been traditionally dissociated into three types: (1) semantic retrieval, characterized by the conscious recollection of factual information and general knowledge (Tulving, 1972); (2) episodic retrieval, characterized by the conscious recollection of experienced events, which originally included personally relevant events (Tulving, 1972), but today typically pertains to memory for stimuli encoded in the laboratory (i.e., laboratory memory ; see Cabeza and St. Jacques, 2007); and (3) autobiographical retrieval, characterized by the conscious recollection of personally relevant events (Conway and Pleydell-Pearce, 2000). Much of the data from neuropsychological and neuroimaging experiments (e.g., Cipolotti and Maguire, 2003; Gadian et al., 2000; Hirano and Noguchi, 1998; Manns et al., 2003; Nyberg, McIntosh and Tulving, 1998; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997) support the Corresponding author. Psychology Department, University of Toronto, 100 St. George Street, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5S 3G3. address: hburian@rotman-baycrest.on.ca (H. Burianova). multiple memory systems view of the organization of declarative memory (Tulving, 1987). This view posits that there are separate memory systems, which specialize in the processing of distinct types of information and recruit functionally independent neural networks, each mediating a specific memory function (Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000; Gabrieli, 1998; Nyberg, McIntosh and Tulving, 1998; Nyberg et al., 2002; Tulving and Schacter, 1990; Tulving, 1987). Empirical evidence supporting this view comes primarily from studies showing functional dissociations between episodic, autobiographical, and semantic memory. Neuropsychological studies show that patients with medial temporal lobe lesions are usually impaired on tasks involving autobiographical memory, but not on tasks involving semantic memory (Gadian et al., 2000; Hirano and Noguchi, 1998; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997), suggesting that the medial temporal lobes (the hippocampus, in particular) engage autobiographical memory exclusively (Tulving et al., 1991; Tulving and Markowitsch, 1998; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997). Conversely, patients with semantic dementia whose neural damage often involves frontotemporal lobar degeneration (Hodges and Miller, 2001; Neary et al., 1998) are characterized by severe semantic memory loss, whereas their autobiographical memory is relatively spared (Graham et al., 2003; McKinnon et al., 2006; Snowden, Griffiths and Neary, 1994; Westmacott and Moscovitch, 2003). Further neural dissociations /$ see front matter 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi: /j.neuroimage

2 866 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) have been found in neuroimaging studies that show regional activity in the left inferior prefrontal cortex and left posterior temporal areas related to semantic retrieval (Graham et al., 2003; Vandenberghe et al., 1996; Wiggs, Weisberg and Martin, 1999), left-lateralized activity in the medial temporal and ventromedial frontal regions, temporopolar areas, retrosplenial cingulate cortex, and cerebellum related to autobiographical retrieval (Conway et al., 2002; Gilboa, 2004; Graham et al., 2003; Maguire, 2001), and activity in the right dorsolateral prefrontal areas subserving laboratory episodic retrieval (Duzel et al., 2004; Gilboa, 2004). It is critical to point out, however, that the focus of most of these neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies was largely on differences and functional dissociations of autobiographical, episodic, and semantic memory retrieval. In other words, the experimental paradigms (e.g., using different tasks to test episodic and semantic memory) and statistical methods (e.g., subtracting activity during one memory condition from another, instead of using a neutral baseline) were designed to dissociate neural correlates among the presumably different types of declarative memory. An alternative perspective to the multiple systems view is the unitary system view of the organization of declarative memory, which proposes the idea of a single declarative memory system (Baddley, 1984; Friston, 2002; Kihlstrom, 1984; McIntosh, 1999; Rajah and McIntosh, 2005; Roediger, 1984). Proponents of this view conjecture that a unitary memory system gives rise to all declarative memory retrieval, although memories can vary along a contextual continuum of several dimensions, such as time, space, emotion, or strength of recollection (e.g., Baddley, 1984). This view rests on several theoretical assumptions. Firstly, encoding of to-be-remembered material is almost always contextual (i.e., embedded in already attained knowledge; Baddley, 1984); at retrieval, memories may or may not become decontextualized along one or more of these dimensions (Baddley, 1984; Rajah and McIntosh, 2005; Westmacott and Moscovitch, 2003). Autobiographical and semantic types of memory may thus be conceptualized as the opposite ends of the contextual continuum (Baddley, 1984; Kihlstrom, 1984; Roediger, 1984). Secondly, even if autobiographical memory is at the most detailed end of the contextual continuum, it is not free of factual, semantic information (Gilboa, 2004; Levine et al., 2002); hence, there is considerable overlap among these types of memory, and the boundaries between them are often unclear. Finally, in a similar vein, semantic memory is rarely entirely context-free, but rather contains some contextual and episodic components, although these may be degraded and lack rich detail (Gilboa, 2004; Westmacott and Moscovitch, 2003; Westmacott et al., 2004). Evidence supporting the unitary memory system view consists of both neuroimaging and neuropsychological data that show functional overlap and interdependence of different memory functions (Duncan and Owen, 2000; Kopelman and Kapur, 2001; Manns et al., 2003; Rajah and McIntosh, 2005; Squire and Zola, 1998). These include studies comparing working, episodic, and semantic memory (Nyberg et al., 2002, 2003), working and episodic memory (Braver et al., 2001; Cabeza et al., 2002; Duzel et al., 1999), and semantic and episodic memory (Rajah and McIntosh, 2005; Ryan et al., 2008), all of which reported commonalities in neural activations across the memory tasks. Rajah and McIntosh (2005) provided evidence for the unitary view by modeling separate functional networks for episodic and semantic retrieval tasks and assessing interregional correlations across the two tasks. The results showed no significant differences in the interregional correlations, despite differences in regional activations in the two memory tasks, suggesting the involvement of a single memory system or network. Other data (e.g., Moscovitch, 1992; Seger et al., 2000; Shimamura, 1995) contradict the prefrontal hemispheric differentiation of episodic and semantic retrieval (i.e., activity in the right prefrontal cortex is associated solely with episodic retrieval [Buckner et al., 1998; Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000] and activity in the left prefrontal cortex is solely associated with semantic retrieval [e.g., Goldberg et al., 2007]). In healthy individuals, activity in the right prefrontal cortex was shown to underlie retrieval of novel and creative semantic relations (Dobbins and Wagner, 2005; Seger et al., 2000), whereas activity in the left prefrontal cortex was found to subserve some aspects of episodic remembering (Nolde et al., 1998). This support for the unitary view of declarative retrieval even extends to the hippocampus, traditionally thought to subserve episodic and autobiographical memory only (Tulving et al., 1991; Tulving and Markowitsch, 1998; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997). For example, there is evidence that hippocampal amnesics, when compared to healthy controls, exhibit impairments in semantic retrieval in addition to profound deficits in episodic and autobiographical retrieval (Kopelman and Kapur, 2001; Manns et al., 2003; Squire and Zola, 1998). Similarly, recent imaging studies of healthy individuals have reported hippocampal activation in both episodic and semantic retrieval (e.g., Burianova and Grady, 2007; Ryan et al., 2008). These data suggest that the hippocampus and the medial temporal cortices are involved in all declarative retrieval. In a previous study, we identified regional activations common to autobiographical, laboratory episodic, and semantic retrieval (Burianova and Grady, 2007). While these results reflect the important neural substrates that are involved in declarative retrieval, they do not provide direct evidence about the functional connectivity of these neural regions. It seems reasonable to assume that memory retrieval, a highly complex cognitive process, would not be localized to discrete brain region(s), but rather would be mediated by the interaction among a number of functionally related neural areas. This idea is not new, as many researchers have argued that it is the activity of distributed neural networks and the interactions among anatomically connected brain regions that directly yield cognitive functions, such as memory (e.g., Finger, 1994; Friston, 1997; McIntosh, 1998, 2000; Mesulam, 1990). In other words, cognitive functions are the emergent properties of the neural interactions (i.e., influences that neural constituents have on one another) among numerous brain areas that comprise a neural network (McIntosh, 1999). An essential aspect of the network approach to the neural organization of cognitive function is the examination of the neural context associated with a specific behaviour (McIntosh, 1998, 1999). Neural context is conceptualized as the activity in a selected brain region that arises as a consequence of modulatory influences from other brain regions (McIntosh, 1998, 1999). Thus, what is important in determining the neural underpinning of a cognitive function is the relation of activity in a brain area with activity of those brain areas with which it is connected. In parallel with this argument is the notion that by measuring only how mean neural activity changes with task in a specific brain area or areas, one might fail to observe relevant interregional functional interactions that occur despite no significant change in the mean activity (e.g., Grady et al., 1998; McIntosh et al., 1994). One approach to quantifying neural interactions is to assess the degree of functional connectivity among brain regions, i.e., the degree to which activity in a specific region correlates or covaries with activity in other areas across the whole brain, thus functioning together as a network (Friston et al., 1993; Friston, 1994; Horwitz et al., 1984). A network is thus defined as a pattern of spatially remote brain regions whose activity levels are correlated, or functionally connected across participants, in order to support a particular behaviour, regardless of whether the average level of activity in any single region of the network is different between the experimental conditions (Friston, 1994; Habib et al., 2003). To statistically study complex neural interactions between different brain structures, the analytical methods must provide a means to quantifying the relation between brain regions, rather than focusing on mean activity differences. Multivariate approaches, such as the partial least squares (PLS) approach to image analysis, enable investigation of functional connectivity of neural regions by calculating the covariance between the activity within selected seed voxels and all other brain voxels

3 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) across the experimental conditions (McIntosh et al., 1996; McIntosh and Gonzalez-Lima, 1994). For instance, Addis et al. (2004a) identified a number of autobiographical regions that differentiated general and specific autobiographical memories from a control baseline using univariate contrasts, but only a subsequent multivariate PLS analysis (Addis et al., 2004b) revealed a shared functional network with a critical connection to the hippocampus. Furthermore, Addis et al. (2007) expanded on these findings by mapping an effective autobiographical network in healthy individuals and epileptic patients. In addition to Addis' work, other researchers have assessed the autobiographical network (Levine et al., 2004; Maguire et al. 2001), pinpointing the interplay of frontal (medial, middle, inferior, and superior frontal gyri) and temporal areas (temporal pole, hippocampus, and parahippocampal gyrus), temporoparietal junction, retrosplenial cortex, and posterior cingulate gyrus. Episodic retrieval of stimuli learned in the laboratory has been linked to a functional network that overlaps to some degree with the autobiographical network, but that also recruits unique brain regions, for instance, the insula, occipito-temporal cortex, posterior parietal areas, and precuneus (e.g., Nyberg et al., 2002; for a review see Gilboa et al., 2004). Semantic memory processes have been generally reflected in the inter-correlation of activity in a number of temporal regions (inferior, middle, and superior temporal gyrus; Martin and Chao, 2001; Vandenberghe et al., 1996), as well as in an overlap with the autobiographical network, particularly in the lateral temporal gyrus, temporoparietal junction, anterior cingulate gyrus, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (Lee et al., 2002; Mummery et al., 1996). However, despite these explorations of the functional and effective connectivity of the neural correlates that underlie declarative memory, no study to date has attempted to identify a single network common to autobiographical, episodic, and semantic retrieval in one experiment and/or analysis. The purpose of this study was to delineate a functional network of spatially distributed neural regions whose activity covaries across the three memory conditions, i.e., to map a common functional declarative retrieval network. To do so, the data were analyzed with seed voxel PLS analysis (Della-Maggiore et al., 2000; McIntosh, 1999; McIntosh et al., 1997; Schreurs et al., 1997) to determine whole-brain patterns of activity correlated with seed regions that showed similar increases of activity across the memory conditions (Burianova and Grady, 2007). We hypothesized that autobiographical, laboratory episodic, and semantic retrieval would recruit a large-scale functional network that underlies the general processes of memory retrieval, such as top-down attentional control, response monitoring, integration of contextual information, working memory manipulation of to-be-retrieved information, and processing of semantic representations. Methods Participants Twelve right-handed, healthy young participants (mean age=27 years; range=21 37 years; 3 males), with at least 16 years of education, took part in the study. All participants signed an informed consent that was approved by ethics boards at Baycrest and Sunnybrook Health Science Centre. Stimuli Experimental stimuli Fifty colour and black and white photographs depicting general, everyday events (e.g., driving or camping), as well as one-time but highly publicized occurrences (e.g., the 9/11 attack on the World Trade Center) were used as visual cues for the experimental retrieval conditions. Control stimuli Five photographs were selected from the set of 50 described above and scrambled using a Matlab script. This ensured that the visual stimulus was rendered meaningless. Procedure The study consisted of one control and three memory retrieval conditions during fmri scanning. Four 14-min runs of 50 trials each were presented to the participants in a counterbalanced order. Trials were randomized within each run. In each trial, an experimental or control stimulus was shown for 4 s. Each experimental stimulus was shown three times during the experiment, but never in sequence or in the same scanning run. Participants were asked to pay attention to the photograph, so that they could successfully answer a subsequently presented question that pertained to the stimulus. After the 4-s presentation of each picture, a question appeared on the screen with three possible answers. Participants had 10 s to respond by pressing 1, 2, or 3 on a number pad. Note that accuracy of memory retrieval was emphasized over speed, and the participants were instructed not to guess. The response period was chosen to provide sufficient time for autobiographical memory retrieval. According to recent electrophysiological evidence, the range of retrieval times for autobiographical memory is between 3 and 9 s, with an average time of 5 s (Conway et al., 2003). After the 10 second response period there was a 1-s inter-trial interval, followed by the next trial. The three memory conditions were as follows: 1. Autobiographical condition, in which the stimulus was followed by a cue designed to elicit a personal memory (e.g., Think of the last time you went camping ). Participants were asked to relive the memory as vividly as possible and subsequently rate the memory according to its vividness (1= very vivid, 2= somewhat vivid, 3= not vivid at all ). 2. Episodic condition, in which the stimulus was followed by a question about the photograph itself (e.g., In the picture, which you have just seen, what is the colour of the tent? ). Participants chose from three answers presented to them (1 or 2 being correct, 3=I don't know). To ensure that this condition did not engage only working or short-term memory, we varied the degree of difficulty of retrieved contextual information; hence, the participants were unaware of which piece of information about the stimulus they would be asked to retrieve. In addition, the presentation of the three experimental conditions was randomized and the temporal lag between subsequent presentations of the same visual stimulus was at least 14 min; hence, our intent was that participants would need to engage episodic memory retrieval from long-term memory storage about the perceptual details of the stimuli, and not solely working memory processes. 3. Semantic condition, in which the stimulus was followed by a factual type of question (e.g., Are there more than 100 camping grounds in Algonquin Park? ). Responses were made in the same fashion as in the episodic condition. In the control condition, the presentation of a scrambled photograph was followed by an arbitrary instruction that was unrelated to the stimulus itself (e.g., Press a key that corresponds to the letter C ). As in the experimental conditions, responses were made by pressing 1, 2, or 3 on a keypad, and the correct key was either 1 or 2. Reaction times for each button press were recorded across all conditions. These behavioural results have been reported elsewhere (Burianova and Grady, 2007). A post-scan interview was administered immediately after the scan session. Participants viewed the 50 photographs again and were asked to describe the autobiographical memory that had been retrieved during the scan in as much detail as possible. Temporal and spatial information as well as the content of the event and

4 868 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) participant's emotion at the time of its occurrence were recorded by the experimenter, to ensure that each autobiographical memory was accompanied by a vivid recollection of a particular episode. fmri data acquisition Anatomical and functional images were collected using a 3T GE scanner with a standard head coil. A standard, high resolution, T1- weighted volumetric anatomical MRI (124 axial slices, 1.4 mm thick, FOV=22 cm, acquisition matrix= , TR=35 ms, TE=6 ms, flip angle=35 ) was acquired for each participant. Brain activation was assessed using the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) effect (Ogawa et al., 1990) with optimal contrast. For functional imaging, 26 axial slices of 5 mm thickness were obtained, utilizing a T2 -weighted pulse sequence with spiral in-out readout (TR = 2000 ms, TE=30 ms, FOV=20, acquisition matrix= , flip angle=80 ). Visual stimuli were presented using fmri-compatible goggles (Avotec, Inc.) mounted on the head coil. Responses were collected with the Rowland USB Response Box (RURB). fmri data preprocessing Images were reconstructed and motion-corrected utilizing the Analysis of Functional Neuroimages (AFNI; Cox, 1996). The images were spatially co-registered to correct for head motion of the participants by using a 3D Fourier transform interpolation. The peak range of head motion did not exceed 1.2 mm across all participants. To enable group comparisons, each brain scan was spatially normalized, i.e., scaled and warped to match a standard template (the Montreal Neurological Institute [MNI] spiral template) utilizing Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM99) software. The warping of the brain surface was achieved via a linear transformation with sinc interpolation (i.e., a signal resampling method designed to minimize aliasing in the signal). Lastly, the images were smoothed with a 6 mm Gaussian filter (in SPM), which, acting as a low pass filter, makes the data less noisy by reducing the images' high-frequency components. The voxel size, after preprocessing, was mm. Seed voxel PLS In the seed PLS analysis, we included those trials for the semantic and episodic conditions for which participants made a correct response, and all very vivid and somewhat vivid trials for the autobiographical condition. Seed PLS is a multivariate statistical method utilized in the investigation of the relation of activity in a selected brain region or regions (i.e., a seed voxel) and activity in the rest of the brain across the task conditions (Della-Maggiore et al., 2000; Schreurs et al., 1997; McIntosh, 1999; McIntosh et al., 1997). In other words, seed PLS analysis examines task-related functional connectivity. The selection of the seed voxel(s) can be either datadriven (i.e., determined by previous analyses of the data) or hypothesis-driven (i.e., determined by theoretical assumptions), or both. In our study, the selection of the seed voxels used in the seed PLS analysis was data-driven. In a previous study (Burianova and Grady, 2007), we identified eight brain regions whose activity was significantly increased across all three memory conditions, thus showing activity common to the conditions. To determine whether these regions are also a part of the same functional network, we entered all eight regions into a seed PLS analysis. The analysis revealed a connectivity pattern across the three memory conditions, but not all regions were reliably correlated with this pattern. Therefore, a subsequent analysis was carried out, using the three seed regions that covaried most strongly with the rest of the brain across the three memory conditions the left hippocampus, right caudate nucleus, and left lingual gyrus (see Table 1 for the coordinates). Table 1 Seed voxel regions. Region Hem BA Talairach coordinates Ratio x y z Caudate nucleus R n/a Lingual gyrus L Hippocampus L n/a Abbreviations: Hem=hemisphere; BA=Brodmann's area; R=right; L =left; Ratio=salience/SE ratio from the bootstrap analysis; x coordinate =right/left; y coordinate =anterior/posterior; z coordinate=superior/inferior. This analytical procedure for seed PLS was threefold: firstly, the BOLD values from the selected seed(s) were extracted (i.e., from the peak voxels identified in our previous study), across 8 timepoints after each presentation of the question cue to capture activity during the retrieval phase of the trial. The activity for each seed was averaged across the peak and adjacent timepoints, and then this average measure of seed activity was correlated with activity in all other brain voxels, across all participants, within each condition. Secondly, these correlations were combined into a matrix and decomposed with singular value decomposition (SVD), resulting in a set of latent variables (LVs; mutually orthogonal variables). Each LV consists of a singular image (i.e., brain LV, or the pattern of brain regions that covary in activity with the seed voxel), a singular profile (i.e., seed LV, or the pattern of covariance of the seed voxel and the rest of the brain across the experimental conditions), and a singular value (i.e., the amount of covariance accounted for by each LV). Finally, the significance for each LV is determined by using a permutation test (McIntosh et al., 1996), which involves a random reordering of the data matrix and calculation of a new set of LVs for each reordering. The singular value of each newly permuted LV is compared to the singular value of the original LV, yielding a probability of the number of occurrences that the permuted values exceed the original value. Five hundred permutations were conducted. Because this study used an event-related design, the PLS analysis provided a set of correlated regions (i.e., a map of areas correlated with the seeds) for each of the eight TRs in the analysis. For each TR, a brain score was calculated for each participant that is an index of how strongly that participant shows the particular pattern of brain activity identified for that TR. The brain scores can be used to examine differences in brain activity across conditions, because greater activity in brain areas with positive (or negative) weights on a latent variable will yield positive (or negative) mean scores for a given condition. We calculated the correlation between the brain scores from each significant LV and the seed BOLD values to assess the relation between the whole-brain pattern and activity in the three reference regions. In addition to the permutation test, a second and independent step is to determine the reliability of the saliences (or weights) for the brain voxels characterizing each pattern identified by the LVs. To do this, all saliences for each TR were submitted to a bootstrap estimation of the standard errors (Efron and Tibshirani, 1985), randomly resampling participants, with replacement, and computing the standard error of the saliences after 100 bootstrap samples. Peak voxels with a salience/se ratio N3.0 were considered to be reliable, as this approximates pb0.005 (Sampson et al., 1989). Because PLS uses images in the format developed by the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI), all coordinates resulting from the PLS analyses were converted from MNI space to Talairach coordinates using the algorithm developed by Brett and colleagues ( Correlation analysis The seed PLS, using the three seeds, resulted in a set of 18 regions whose activity was reliably and strongly correlated with all three, and this set of regions constituted the putative common network. To

5 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) Table 2 Correlations with seed activity. Region Hem BA Talairach coordinates Ratio Cluster x y z Positive correlations Inferior FG L R Medial FG L Anterior CG L 24/ Superior TG L Middle TG L R Temporal pole R Inferior PL (TPJ) L Posterior CG 23/ Negative correlations Middle OG L Precuneus L R Thalamus R n/a Inferior TG L Fusiform gyrus R Inferior PL (SMG) L R Abbreviations: Hem=hemisphere; BA=Brodmann's area; R=right; L =left; Ratio=salience/SE ratio from the bootstrap analysis; Cluster=size of each cluster in number of voxels; FG=frontal gyrus; CG=cingulate gyrus; TG=temporal gyrus; OG=occipital gyrus; PL=parietal lobule; TPJ=temporoparietal junction; SMG=supramarginal gyrus; x coordinate=right/left; y coordinate=anterior/ posterior; z coordinate =superior/inferior. specifically examine the intercorrelations among these regions, we extracted the BOLD signal from them, i.e., from the peak voxel in each cluster (see Table 2 for peak coordinates), and averaged the activity at the timepoint corresponding to the strongest correlation with the seeds and adjacent timepoints. We then calculated interregional correlations within conditions (creating three correlation matrices) and compared these correlation patterns across conditions. We compared the correlation patterns across the conditions by calculating the squared differences in correlations (for each cell of the matrix) between conditions and summing them to obtain a single value for the overall difference in correlations between conditions. The statistical significance of this difference was assessed using a permutation test on the sum of squared differences (McIntosh et al., 1996), which involved a random reordering of the data matrix and calculation of a new set of values for each reordering. Results Behavioural performance Behavioural performance was assessed by comparing the means of the response times across the four conditions (correct trials only), using a repeated-measures ANOVA. The effect of condition was significant, F(3,33) =73.1, pb Pairwise t-tests with Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons showed that the response times for autobiographical retrieval (M=6989 ms, SD=1478) differed significantly from that for the control task (M=1925 ms, SD=728) and episodic retrieval (M=4640 ms, SD=1150, both at pb0.01). The difference in reaction time (RT) for autobiographical retrieval and semantic retrieval (M=5858 ms, SD=1366) approached significance (p=.06). Seed PLS The first latent variable yielded by the three-seed PLS analysis accounted for the largest amount of covariance in the data (32%; p b0.002), delineating a group of brain regions whose activity correlated with all three seed regions (the left hippocampus, left lingual gyrus, and right caudate nucleus) during the retrieval of autobiographical, episodic, and semantic memories (see Fig. 1). Positive correlations with the three seed regions were found in temporal cortex (bilaterally in the middle temporal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus), frontal cortex (bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, medial frontal gyrus, and left anterior cingulate gyrus), as well as in the left temporoparietal junction and posterior cingulate gyrus. Negative correlations with the seed regions were found in the left middle occipital gyrus, right thalamus, bilateral precuneus, bilateral fusiform gyrus, and bilateral supramarginal gyrus (see Table 2 for a summary). These patterns of correlation were found across all three memory conditions. Correlation analysis The seed PLS analysis identified a set of regions correlated with the seeds across all three memory conditions. To assess further whether this set of regions represents a common memory retrieval network, we determined if the correlations among all the nodes of this putative network were statistically equivalent across the tasks, as one would expect if the network operates in a similar fashion regardless of the task demands. Fig. 2 shows three correlation matrices representing the 3 seeds and the 18 regions that showed correlated activity with the seeds, both positive and negative. Each matrix shows the visual representation of interregional correlations of activity among the 21 regions, within each memory condition. No significant differences (pn0.05) were found in the overall interregional correlation strengths across the three memory conditions. The correlation matrices indicate that during all memory conditions, most of the regions that positively correlated with the seeds were also correlated positively with one another (denoted in lighter shades of gray), but negatively with regions that show negative correlations with the seeds (denoted in darker shades). Similarly, most of the regions with negative seed correlations correlated positively with each other, but negatively with the regions with positive seed correlations. Despite some apparent interregional differences, there are obvious clusters of strong positive and negative correlations among the voxels that are consistent across the three memory conditions. As a whole, the strength of the overall interregional correlations did not differ across the conditions, consistent with the idea of a common functional memory network. Discussion The purpose of this study was to identify a functional network that is common to autobiographical, episodic, and semantic retrieval. The results of the seed PLS analysis yielded a large-scale network of brain regions that were functionally connected to the left hippocampus, right caudate nucleus, and left lingual gyrus in all memory retrieval conditions. The inferior frontal gyri, medial frontal gyrus, anterior and posterior cingulate gyri, left temporoparietal junction, and a number of temporal areas (bilateral middle temporal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, and right temporal pole) positively correlated with the seed regions, whereas the left middle occipital gyrus, bilateral precuneus, right thalamus, left inferior temporal gyrus, right fusiform gyrus, and bilateral inferior parietal lobule negatively correlated with the seed regions. These results suggest that these areas, as a whole, comprise a common functional network that subserves the general processes of declarative memory retrieval. We propose that these processes include a multitude of higher-cognitive functions, for instance, working memory, selective attention, error monitoring, response verification, and comprehension of concept representations. We conclude from these results that, together, these cognitive processes and their integration are essential to what is defined as declarative memory retrieval. The three seed regions selected for the functional connectivity analysis have been previously identified as important for memory

6 870 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) Fig. 1. Seed PLS results: common functional network. (a) A pattern of correlated activity at 6 8 s after retrieval onset is shown. (b) A pattern of correlated activity at s after retrieval onset is shown. (c) Correlations of activity in the three seeds with the brain scores (summary measures of whole-brain activity in each subject per condition) are plotted to show how activity in the seeds correlates with activity in the entire network. The error bars represent confidence intervals based on the bootstrap. (d) Activity in the caudate nucleus at 2 3 s after retrieval onset is shown. Yellow/red = positive correlations with the seeds; blue = negative correlations with the seeds. processing (e.g., Burianova and Grady, 2007). The hippocampus has been implicated as a critical component of declarative retrieval processes (Cabeza et al., 2004; Eldridge et al., 2000; Prince et al., 2005) and relational memory (e.g., memory for the relations between items and context [e.g., Davachi and Wagner, 2002], or between objects and features of a scene [e.g., Ryan and Cohen, 2004]). The hippocampus also may play a role in non-declarative processes, such as working memory and object organization (Seeck et al., 1995) and nonmnemonic processes, such as local feature extraction in form perception (Barense et al., 2005; Beason-Held et al., 1998) and visual discrimination of scenes (Davies et al., 2004). The left lingual gyrus has been linked to working memory maintenance of visuospatial information (Ragland et al., 2002), imagery (Kosslyn et al., 1993; Mazard et al., 2005), and crossmodal attention (Ferber et al., 2007). The caudate nucleus also was found to be involved in learning and memory, particularly in regard to feedback processing and the ability to shift a cognitive set in response to the environment (Packard and Knowlton, 2002). The three seed regions were found to positively covary with a number of frontal, temporal, and parietal areas. These areas of functional connectivity broadly overlap with the common memoryrelated brain regions, which were found in our previous analysis (Burianova and Grady, 2007), although the exact locations are not identical. The inferior frontal gyrus has been implicated in declarative and working memory retrieval (Nyberg et al., 2002), as well as in response inhibition and selection control (Aron et al., 2004; Brass et al., 2005; Liddle et al., 2001), and top-down attentional control (Banich et al., 2000; Dove et al., 2008). The medial prefrontal areas are involved in social functions and self-referential processes in both semantic and episodic memory retrieval (Addis et al., 2004a,b; Gilboa et al., 2004; Levine et al., 2004). The functional connectivity of these frontal areas with the three seed regions suggests involvement of the frontostriatal and hippocampo-frontal pathways. The frontrostriatal pathway has been implicated in abstract reasoning (Melrose et al., 2007), working memory (Ashby et al., 2005; Gazzaley et al., 2004), and internal object representations (Kraut et al., 2002), as well as in attentional control and cognitive planning and execution of a set-shift (Monchi et al., 2006), due to its predominant dopamine dependency (Chudasama and Robbins, 2006) and dopamine glutamate interactions (Smith et al., 2004). The hippocampo-frontal pathway, involving the left hippocampus, has been linked to the retrieval of contextual details (Svoboda et al., 2006), despite some researchers' arguing its sole involvement in autobiographical memory retrieval (e.g., Addis et al., 2007). The common prefrontal link of the frontostriatal and hippocampo-frontal pathways allows for an indirect effect of the caudate nucleus on the hippocampus and vice versa. Some researchers speculate that the hippocampus and caudate nucleus functionally interact to jointly mediate behavioural output during performance (Dagher et al., 2001; Packard and Knowlton, 2002). We propose that the frontal nodes of the common retrieval network subserve topdown attentional, inhibitory, and monitoring functions, as well as working memory manipulation and maintenance of information necessary for a successful declarative recollection of memories that relate to the self and others. The middle and superior temporal gyri were involved in our common network, and have been linked to the processing of personal

7 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) Fig. 2. Correlations among peak voxels of the common functional network. The three correlation matrices show similarities in regional interrelations across the three memory conditions. Correlation values are indicated by shades of gray (positive correlations are denoted in lighter shades; negative correlations are denoted in darker shades). Values on the vertical and horizontal axes correspond to the region numbers in the attached legend. The matrix is symmetrical about the main diagonal, which corresponds to perfect correlation (+1) of each voxel with itself. STG=superior temporal gyrus; CN=caudate nucleus; IPL=inferior parietal lobule; IFG=inferior frontal gyrus; pcg=posterior cingulate gyrus; acg=anterior cingulate gyrus; LG=lingual gyrus; MTG =middle temporal gyrus; med FG=medial frontal gyrus; HIPP=hippocampus; MOG=middle occipital gyrus; PCU=precuneus; TH=thalamus; ITG=inferior temporal gyrus. and general semantic representations (e.g., Martin and Chao, 2001), which are essential in the retrieval of all declarative memory. Martin and Chao (2001) further note a temporo-frontal pathway, connecting the middle and superior temporal areas with the inferior prefrontal cortex, enabling the retrieval, monitoring, and manipulation of semantic representations. Levine et al. (1999) found impaired autonoetic processing (due, hypothetically, to deficits in self-regulation) in patient ML, whose traumatic brain injury led to a disconnection of the temporal and frontal lobes, in the right hemisphere. The functional relevance of the temporopolar area is much less clear but its functional connectivity with the hippocampus, middle and superior temporal gyri, and the temporoparietal junction suggests its involvement in multimodal perceptual analysis (Olson et al., 2007), as the temporal pole is speculated to be a convergence area, which integrates diverse streams of information into a unified whole (Markowitsch et al., 1985). Other researchers have suggested that both the temporal pole and the left temporoparietal junction are critical in the encoding of personal memories (Nakamura and Kubota, 1996), multisensory processing of body and self (Arzy et al., 2006), retrieval of theory-of-mind information (i.e., when comprehending someone else's state of mind; Arzy et al., 2006; Moriguchi et al., 2006), and conceptual knowledge (Graham et al., 2003). Our finding that these temporal and parietal regions are nodes of the common retrieval network suggests that these areas participate in the processing of necessary semantic representations, as well as the convergence of relevant semantic, perceptual, and others' state of mind information of which declarative memories are composed. The anterior and posterior cingulate gyri are known to be bidirectionally connected to one another via the cingulum bundle (Goldman-Rakic, 1988; Vogt et al., 1979) and indirectly via the medial temporal lobes (Morris et al., 1999), with functional connections to the temporal, frontal, and parietal lobes. Thus, they are speculated to be involved in a variety of cognitive and emotional processes (e.g., Bush et al., 1998; Critchley et al., 2003), including self-referential

8 872 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) (Gusnard, 2005; Gusnard et al., 2001) and visuospatial processing (e.g., Rosenbaum et al., 2004). Although the anterior part of the cingulate gyrus is often linked to uniquely autobiographical processes (Svoboda et al., 2006), other researchers found it active in both episodic and semantic processes (e.g., Mummery et al., 1996). Johnson et al. (2006) linked both the anterior and posterior cingulate gyri to self-reflection, albeit linking the anterior cingulate cortex to instrumental self-reflection (i.e., directed towards the inner self) and the posterior cingulate cortex to experiential self-reflection (i.e., directed towards the self as it relates, externally, to others). The involvement of the anterior and posterior cingulate gyri in the common retrieval network emphasizes the critical role of intrinsically driven and selfrelated processes in declarative memory search and retrieval. Thus, it should come as no surprise that these regions overlap with the default-mode regions whose activity decreases during demanding external tasks, but increases during self-referential processing and monitoring of the internal environment (e.g., Gusnard, 2005). The anterior and posterior cingulate gyri are two frequently delineated nodes of this self-referential default-mode network (e.g., Fox et al., 2005; Greicius and Menon, 2004; Gusnard et al., 2001; Toro et al., 2008). Finally, the connectivity analysis yielded a number of functionally connected brain areas that negatively correlated with the left hippocampus, lingual gyrus, and right caudate nucleus, yet positively correlated with one another. These areas included the left middle occipital gyrus, bilateral precuneus, right thalamus, left inferior temporal gyrus, right fusiform gyrus, and bilateral inferior parietal lobule. Given the similarity between these regions and attentional networks proposed by Corbetta et al. (2000, 2008), these negatively correlating regions are likely involved in attention to external stimuli. In a recent study, Fu et al. (2005) found a network of posterior brain areas, including the middle occipital gyrus, precuneus, fusiform gyrus, and inferior parietal lobes, that was involved in externally driven early visuospatial attentional modulation, as well as in later perceptual feedback to the primary visual areas. In the paradigm used here, the processing of memory search is internally driven and our analysis focused only on the period of retrieval, not on the period of cue presentation. Thus, when the common network is recruited for this internal process (i.e., an increase in activity), activity in the externally driven occipito-temporo-parietal network is reduced, or maybe even suppressed (i.e., a decrease in activity). It is likely that the externally driven network would be more active if memory were more directly cued by an external stimulus, consistent with the idea of Ciaramelli et al. (2008) that different parts of the parietal cortex mediate attention to retrieval cues in memory search. In conclusion, the results of this study yielded a large-scale functional network of frontal, temporal, and parietal areas, as well as the anterior and posterior cingulate gyri, in line with the unitary systems view that suggests a single declarative memory system (Baddley, 1984; Friston, 2002; Kihlstrom, 1984; McIntosh, 1999; Rajah and McIntosh, 2005; Roediger, 1984). The functional connections within this network did not significantly differ across the memory conditions, indicating its importance in all types of declarative retrieval. This neural overlap of autobiographical, laboratory episodic, and semantic retrieval suggests that all declarative memory retrieval engages a set of functional processes, mediated by the use of this common network, and provides further support for the notion of a unitary memory system. One implication of this unitary system is that no declarative memory retrieval is ever purely semantic or autobiographical, for example, but necessarily involves overlapping processes and multiple types of content, both contextual and contextfree. The regions that we found to be part of the common network suggest that some of these overlapping cognitive processes that are involved in declarative memory are likely to include top-down attentional, inhibitory, and monitoring processes, working memory manipulation, maintenance of information, and convergence of semantic and self-referential information. Activity in this network during memory retrieval may be facilitated by down-regulation of an occipito-temporo-parietal network of posterior brain areas, which supports attentional modulation in response to external stimuli. Acknowledgments We thank the staff at Sunnybrook Health Science Centre for their assistance in this experiment. This work was funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (grant MOP 14036). C.L. Grady is also supported by the Canada Research Chairs program, the Ontario Research Fund, and the Canadian Foundation for Innovation. Appendix A. Supplementary data Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at doi: /j.neuroimage References Addis, D.R., Moscovitch, M., Crawley, A.P., McAndrews, M.P., 2004a. Recollective qualities modulate hippocampal activation during autobiographical memory retrieval. Hippocampus 14, Addis, D.R., McIntosh, A.R., Moscovitch, M., Crawley, A.P., McAndrews, M.P., 2004b. Characterizing spatial and temporal features of autobiographical memory retrieval networks: a partial least squares approach. NeuroImage 23, Addis, D.R., Moscovitch, M., McAndrews, M.P., Consequences of hippocampal damage across the autobiographical memory network in left temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 130, Aron, A.R., Robbins, T.W., Poldrack, R.A., Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, Arzy, S., Overney, L.S., Landis, T., Blanke, O., Neural mechanisms of embodiment: asomatognosia due to premotor cortex damage. Arch. Neurol. 63, Ashby, F.G., Ell, S.W., Valentin, V.V., Casale, M.B., FROST: a distributed neurocomputational model of working memory maintenance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, Baddley, A.D., Neuropsychological evidence and the semantic/episodic distinction [commentary]. Behav. Brain Sci. 7, Banich, M.T., Milham, M.P., Atchley, R.A., Cohen, N.J., Webb, A., Wszalek, T., Kramer, A.F., Liang, Z., Barad, V., Gullett, D., Shah, C., Brown, C., Prefrontal regions play a predominant role in imposing an attentional set : evidence from fmri. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 10, 1 9. Barense, M.D., Bussey, T.J., Lee, A.C.H., Rogers, T.T., Davies, R.R., Saksida, L.M., Murray, E.A., Graham, K.S., Functional specialization in the human medial temporal lobe. J. Neurosci. 25, Beason-Held, L.L., Purpura, K.P., van Meter, J.W., Azari, N.P., Mangot, D.J., Optican, L.M., Mentis, M.J., et al., PET reveals occipitotemporal pathway activation during elementary form perception in humans. Vis. Neurosci. 15, Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., Forstmann, B., von Cramon, D.Y., The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Neurosciences 9, Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M., Kelley, W.M., Buckner, R.L., Cohen, N.J., Miezin, F.M., Snyder, A.Z., Ollinger, J.M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T.E., Petersen, S.E., Direct comparison of prefrontal cortex regions engaged by working and long-term memory tasks. NeuroImage 14, Buckner, R.L., Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D.L., Wagner, A.D., Rosen, B.R., Functional anatomic study of episodic retrieval using fmri. Retrieval effort versus retrieval success. NeuroImage 7, Burianova, H., Grady, C.L., Common and unique activations in autobiographical, episodic, and semantic retrieval. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, Bush, G., Whalen, P.J., Rosen, B.R., Jenike, M.A., McInerney, S.C., Rauch, S.L., The counting Stroop: an interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging validation study with functional MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 6, Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fmri studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, Cabeza, R., St. Jacques, P., Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, Cabeza, R., Dolcos, F., Graham, R., Nyberg, L., Similarities and differences in the neural correlates of episodic memory retrieval and working memory. NeuroImage 16, Cabeza, R., Prince, S.E., Daselaar, S.M., Greenberg, D.L., Budde, M., Dolcos, F., LaBar, K.S., Rubin, D.C., Brain activity during episodic retrieval of autobiographical and laboratory events: an fmri study using a novel photo paradigm. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, Chudasama, Y., Robbins, T.W., Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys, and humans. Biol. Psychol. 73, Ciaramelli, E., Grady, C., Moscovitch, M., Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: a hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia 46,

Characterizing spatial and temporal features of autobiographical memory retrieval networks: a partial least squares approach

Characterizing spatial and temporal features of autobiographical memory retrieval networks: a partial least squares approach www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 23 (2004) 1460 1471 Characterizing spatial and temporal features of autobiographical memory retrieval networks: a partial least squares approach Donna Rose Addis,

More information

NeuroImage 59 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 59 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 59 (2012) 2908 2922 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Routes to the past: Neural substrates of direct and generative

More information

Neural correlates of recognition memory for emotional faces and scenes

Neural correlates of recognition memory for emotional faces and scenes doi:10.1093/scan/nsq003 Neural correlates of recognition memory for emotional faces and scenes SCAN (2011) 6, 24 ^37 Michelle L. Keightley, 1,2,3,4,5 Kimberly S. Chiew, 6 John A. E. Anderson, 5,6 and Cheryl

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration

Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration Neuropsychologia 45 (2007) 1363 1377 Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration Donna Rose Addis a,b,, Alana T. Wong

More information

Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory

Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.5 Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory Roberto Cabeza 1,2 and Peggy St Jacques 1,2 1 Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Box

More information

Supplemental information online for

Supplemental information online for Supplemental information online for Sleep contributes to the strengthening of some memories over others, depending on hippocampal activity at learning. Géraldine Rauchs (1,2), Dorothée Feyers (1), Brigitte

More information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis (OA). All subjects provided informed consent to procedures

More information

A Partial Least Squares Analysis of the self reference effect in Alzheimer's disease: A reply to Irish

A Partial Least Squares Analysis of the self reference effect in Alzheimer's disease: A reply to Irish A Partial Least Squares Analysis of the self reference effect in Alzheimer's disease: A reply to Irish Sarah Genon 1,2,3, Christine Bastin 1, Lucie Angel 5, Fabienne Collette 1, Mohamed Ali Bahri 1* and

More information

Overlap in the Functional Neural Systems Involved in Semantic and Episodic Memory Retrieval

Overlap in the Functional Neural Systems Involved in Semantic and Episodic Memory Retrieval Overlap in the Functional Neural Systems Involved in Semantic and Episodic Memory Retrieval M. N. Rajah 1 and A. R. McIntosh 2 Abstract & Neuroimaging and neuropsychological data suggest that episodic

More information

Attentional selection and the processing of task-irrelevant information: insights from fmri examinations of the Stroop task

Attentional selection and the processing of task-irrelevant information: insights from fmri examinations of the Stroop task C. Casanova and M. Ptito (Eds.) Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 134 2001 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved CHAPTER 29 Attentional selection and the processing of task-irrelevant information: insights

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Differential Neural Activity during Retrieval of Specific and General. Autobiographical Memories derived from Musical Cues.

Differential Neural Activity during Retrieval of Specific and General. Autobiographical Memories derived from Musical Cues. Differential Neural Activity during Retrieval of Specific and General Autobiographical Memories derived from Musical Cues Jaclyn Hennessey A thesis submitted to the faculty of the University of North Carolina

More information

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective MILITARY PSYCHOLOGY, 21:(Suppl. 1)S108 S112, 2009 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0899-5605 print / 1532-7876 online DOI: 10.1080/08995600802554748 Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future:

More information

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 1428 1442 Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Alessandra Schiavetto a, Stefan Köhler a, Cheryl L. Grady a, Gordon Winocur a,c,

More information

The Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Autobiographical Memory: Neural Correlates of Recall, Emotional Intensity, and Reliving

The Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Autobiographical Memory: Neural Correlates of Recall, Emotional Intensity, and Reliving Cerebral Cortex January 2008;18:217-229 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm048 Advance Access publication June 4, 2007 The Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Autobiographical Memory: Neural Correlates of Recall, Emotional

More information

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 341 356 Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Joseph B. Sala a,, Pia Rämä a,c,d, Susan M.

More information

Brain regions associated with successful and unsuccessful retrieval of verbal episodic memory as revealed by divided attention

Brain regions associated with successful and unsuccessful retrieval of verbal episodic memory as revealed by divided attention Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 1115 1127 Brain regions associated with successful and unsuccessful retrieval of verbal episodic memory as revealed by divided attention Myra A. Fernandes a,, Morris Moscovitch

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism Justin H G Williams Morven McWhirr Gordon D Waiter Cambridge Sept 10 th 2010 Joint attention in autism Declarative and receptive aspects initiating

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/324/5927/646/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves Modulation of the vmpfc Valuation System Todd A. Hare,* Colin F. Camerer, Antonio

More information

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes Brain (1998), 121, 1985 2002 Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes Anthony D. Wagner, 1 John E. Desmond, 1,2 Gary H. Glover 2 and John

More information

Importance of Deficits

Importance of Deficits Importance of Deficits In complex systems the parts are often so integrated that they cannot be detected in normal operation Need to break the system to discover the components not just physical components

More information

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention Goldsmiths Research Online. The Authors. Originally published: Science vol.291 2 March 2001 1803-1806. http://www.sciencemag.org. 11 October 2000; accepted 17 January 2001 The Role of Working Memory in

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Subjects The experiment included twelve subjects: ten sighted subjects and two blind. Five of the ten sighted subjects were expert users of a visual-to-auditory

More information

Cognitive Subtractions May Not Add Up: The Interaction between Semantic Processing and Response Mode

Cognitive Subtractions May Not Add Up: The Interaction between Semantic Processing and Response Mode NEUROIMAGE 5, 229 239 (1997) ARTICLE NO. NI970257 Cognitive Subtractions May Not Add Up: The Interaction between Semantic Processing and Response Mode Janine M. Jennings,* Anthony R. McIntosh,* Shitij

More information

BRAIN RESEARCH 1429 (2012) Available online at

BRAIN RESEARCH 1429 (2012) Available online at Available online at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Incidental encoding of goal irrelevant information is associated with insufficient engagement of the dorsal frontal

More information

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Supplemental Information Table of Contents 2 Behavioral Data 2 Table S1. Participant demographics

More information

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus FRONTAL LOBE Central Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Cingulate Lateral Lateral View Medial View Motor execution and higher cognitive functions (e.g., language production, impulse inhibition, reasoning

More information

SHARED COMPONENT PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND LONG-TERM MEMORY: INSIGHTS FROM FUNCTIONAL BRAIN IMAGING. Petter Marklund

SHARED COMPONENT PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND LONG-TERM MEMORY: INSIGHTS FROM FUNCTIONAL BRAIN IMAGING. Petter Marklund SHARED COMPONENT PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND LONG-TERM MEMORY: INSIGHTS FROM FUNCTIONAL BRAIN IMAGING Petter Marklund UMEÅ PSYCHOLOGY SUPPLEMENT REPORTS Supplement No. 2 2004 Umeå Psychology Supplement

More information

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Figure I. Changes in connectivity with the right anterior insular cortex. (A) wake > mild sedation, showing a reduction in connectivity between the anterior

More information

Stimulus content and the neural correlates of source memory

Stimulus content and the neural correlates of source memory available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Stimulus content and the neural correlates of source memory Audrey Duarte a,b,, Richard N. Henson a, Kim S. Graham a,c

More information

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information The Journal of Neuroscience, 2000, Vol. 20 RC108 1of5 Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information Charan Ranganath, 1 Marcia K. Johnson, 2 and Mark

More information

Supplementary Digital Content

Supplementary Digital Content Supplementary Digital Content Contextual modulation of pain in masochists: involvement of the parietal operculum and insula Sandra Kamping a, Jamila Andoh a, Isabelle C. Bomba a, Martin Diers a,b, Eugen

More information

Memory Processes in Perceptual Decision Making

Memory Processes in Perceptual Decision Making Memory Processes in Perceptual Decision Making Manish Saggar (mishu@cs.utexas.edu), Risto Miikkulainen (risto@cs.utexas.edu), Department of Computer Science, University of Texas at Austin, TX, 78712 USA

More information

Neural Bases of Autobiographical Support for Episodic Recollection of Faces

Neural Bases of Autobiographical Support for Episodic Recollection of Faces HIPPOCAMPUS 00:000 000 (2009) Neural Bases of Autobiographical Support for Episodic Recollection of Faces Iris Trinkler, 1 John A. King, 1 Christian F. Doeller, 1,2 Michael D. Rugg, 3 and Neil Burgess

More information

A Basic-Systems Approach to Autobiographical Memory David C. Rubin

A Basic-Systems Approach to Autobiographical Memory David C. Rubin CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE A Basic-Systems Approach to Autobiographical Memory David C. Rubin Duke University ABSTRACT Memory for complex everyday events involving vision, hearing, smell,

More information

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Overview of the SIIPS1 development. The development of the SIIPS1 consisted of individual- and group-level analysis steps. 1) Individual-person

More information

NeuroImage 53 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 53 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 53 (2010) 303 317 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network,

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex in dual-task performance Fabienne Collette a,b, Laurence 01ivier b,c, Martial Van der Linden a,d, Steven Laureys b, Guy Delfiore b, André Luxen

More information

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Online supplementary information for: Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Joel L. Voss, Brian D. Gonsalves, Kara D. Federmeier,

More information

The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition

The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition Rachel J. Garoff-Eaton, Elizabeth A. Kensinger and Daniel L. Schacter Learn. Mem. 2007 14: 684-692 Access the most recent version at

More information

The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load

The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load Vogan et al. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 2014, 6:19 RESEARCH Open Access The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Redlich R, Opel N, Grotegerd D, et al. Prediction of individual response to electroconvulsive therapy via machine learning on structural magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA

More information

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and 77 5 Discussion The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and implicit learning systems. Chapter 2 described the effects of performing a working memory task

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of Placebo effects in fmri Supporting online material 1 Supporting online material Materials and Methods Study 1 Procedure and behavioral data We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was

More information

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Supplemental informations Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Stéphanie Lefebvre, PhD^1,2, Guillaume Baille, MD^4, Renaud Jardri MD, PhD 1,2 Lucie Plomhause, PhD

More information

Neuroscience of Consciousness II

Neuroscience of Consciousness II 1 C83MAB: Mind and Brain Neuroscience of Consciousness II Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Consciousness State of consciousness - Being awake/alert/attentive/responsive Contents

More information

As Time Goes By: Hippocampal Connectivity Changes With Remoteness of Autobiographical Memory Retrieval

As Time Goes By: Hippocampal Connectivity Changes With Remoteness of Autobiographical Memory Retrieval HIPPOCAMPUS 00:000 000 (2011) As Time Goes By: Hippocampal Connectivity Changes With Remoteness of Autobiographical Memory Retrieval Hedvig Söderlund, 1 * Morris Moscovitch, 2,3 Namita Kumar, 2 Marina

More information

Over-recruitment in the aging brain as a function of task demands: evidence for a compensatory view

Over-recruitment in the aging brain as a function of task demands: evidence for a compensatory view Over-recruitment in the aging brain 1 THIS IS THE UNEDITED AUTHORS VERSION OF A PAPER ACCEPTED FOR PUBLICATION IN THE JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE. Please cite this work as follows: Vallesi A., McIntosh

More information

Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 2222 2238 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Constructive episodic simulation of the future

More information

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Background Maintenance: Modality specificity and buffers Interference resolution: Distraction and

More information

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis International Journal of Innovative Research in Computer Science & Technology (IJIRCST) ISSN: 2347-5552, Volume-2, Issue-6, November-2014 Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using

More information

Mathematical models of visual category learning enhance fmri data analysis

Mathematical models of visual category learning enhance fmri data analysis Mathematical models of visual category learning enhance fmri data analysis Emi M Nomura (e-nomura@northwestern.edu) Department of Psychology, 2029 Sheridan Road Evanston, IL 60201 USA W Todd Maddox (maddox@psy.utexas.edu)

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition Gloria Wong 1,2, Sanda Dolcos 1,3, Ekaterina Denkova 1, Rajendra A. Morey 4,5, Lihong Wang 4,5, Nicholas Coupland 1, Gregory

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

Activity in Both Hippocampus and Perirhinal Cortex Predicts the Memory Strength of Subsequently Remembered Information

Activity in Both Hippocampus and Perirhinal Cortex Predicts the Memory Strength of Subsequently Remembered Information Report Activity in Both Hippocampus and Perirhinal Cortex Predicts the Memory Strength of Subsequently Remembered Information Yael Shrager, 1 C. Brock Kirwan, 4 and Larry R. Squire 1,2,3,5, * 1 Department

More information

The Hippocampus Remains Activated over the Long Term for the Retrieval of Truly Episodic Memories

The Hippocampus Remains Activated over the Long Term for the Retrieval of Truly Episodic Memories The Hippocampus Remains Activated over the Long Term for the Retrieval of Truly Episodic Memories Caroline Harand 1,2,3,4, Françoise Bertran 1,2,3,5, Renaud La Joie 1,2,3,4, Brigitte Landeau 1,2,3,4, Florence

More information

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018)

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) 1 / 22 Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) Jérôme Dockès, ussel Poldrack, Demian Wassermann, Fabian Suchanek, Bertrand

More information

Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems

Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Advance Access published August 16, 2013 Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems Shamay-Tsoory, Simone G. Department of Psychology,

More information

Reasoning and working memory: common and distinct neuronal processes

Reasoning and working memory: common and distinct neuronal processes Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 1241 1253 Reasoning and working memory: common and distinct neuronal processes Christian C. Ruff a,b,, Markus Knauff a,c, Thomas Fangmeier a, Joachim Spreer d a Centre for Cognitive

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 197 (2009) 186 197 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report Top-down attentional

More information

Age-Related Differences in Face Processing: A Meta- Analysis of Three Functional Neuroimaging Experiments

Age-Related Differences in Face Processing: A Meta- Analysis of Three Functional Neuroimaging Experiments Age-Related Differences in Face Processing: A Meta- Analysis of Three Functional Neuroimaging Experiments CHERYL L. GRADY, Rotman Research Institute, Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

fmri activity in the medial temporal lobe during famous face processing

fmri activity in the medial temporal lobe during famous face processing fmri activity in the medial temporal lobe during famous face processing Elfgren, Christina; van Westen, Danielle; Passant, Ulla; Larsson, Elna-Marie; Mannfolk, Peter; Fransson, Peter Published in: NeuroImage

More information

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Neuron, Volume 70 Supplemental Information Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Luke J. Chang, Alec Smith, Martin Dufwenberg, and Alan G. Sanfey Supplemental Information

More information

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Hillyard(1973) Dichotic Listening Task N1 component enhanced for attended stimuli Supports early selection Effects of Voluntary

More information

Face encoding and recognition in the human brain

Face encoding and recognition in the human brain Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 922-927, January 1996 Neurobiology Face encoding and recognition in the human brain (positron emission tomography/cerebral blood flow) JAMES V. HAXBY*t, LESLIE G.

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Types and Structure of Memory Types of Memory Type of Memory Time Course Capacity Conscious Awareness Mechanism of Loss Sensory Short-Term and Working Long-Term Nondeclarative

More information

Supplementary Materials for

Supplementary Materials for Supplementary Materials for Folk Explanations of Behavior: A Specialized Use of a Domain-General Mechanism Robert P. Spunt & Ralph Adolphs California Institute of Technology Correspondence may be addressed

More information

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate Supplementary Results Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate behavioral experiment was conducted (n = 16) to verify (a) that retrieval-induced forgetting is observed

More information

Self-involvement modulates the effective connectivity of the autobiographical memory network

Self-involvement modulates the effective connectivity of the autobiographical memory network doi:10.1093/scan/nsp043 Self-involvement modulates the effective connectivity of the autobiographical memory network SCAN (2010) 5, 68 ^76 Keely A. Muscatell, 1,2,3 Donna Rose Addis, 4 and Elizabeth A.

More information

positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval processes

positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval processes Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 9212-9217, August 1996 Neurobiology Memory for object features versus memory for object location: A positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Braver et al. 10.1073/pnas.0808187106 SI Methods Participants. Participants were neurologically normal, righthanded younger or older adults. The groups did not differ in gender breakdown

More information

Two Distinct Functional Networks for Successful Resolution of Proactive Interference

Two Distinct Functional Networks for Successful Resolution of Proactive Interference Cerebral Cortex July 2007;17:1650--1663 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl076 Advance Access publication September 12, 2006 Two Distinct Functional Networks for Successful Resolution of Proactive Interference Jeremy

More information

Age-Related Changes in Right Middle Frontal Gyrus Volume Correlate with Altered Episodic Retrieval Activity

Age-Related Changes in Right Middle Frontal Gyrus Volume Correlate with Altered Episodic Retrieval Activity The Journal of Neuroscience, December 7, 2011 31(49):17941 17954 17941 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Age-Related Changes in Right Middle Frontal Gyrus Volume Correlate with Altered Episodic Retrieval Activity

More information

Two cortical systems for memoryguided

Two cortical systems for memoryguided Two cortical systems for memoryguided behaviour Charan Ranganath 1,2 and Maureen Ritchey 2 Abstract Although the perirhinal cortex (PRC), parahippocampal cortex (PHC) and retrosplenial cortex (RSC) have

More information

HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR

HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR Applicants Principal Investigator Student ID 4039921 Collaborators Name(s) Institution(s) Title of project: Neural basis of verbal and non-verbal false

More information

Material-specific lateralization of prefrontal activation during episodic encoding and retrieval

Material-specific lateralization of prefrontal activation during episodic encoding and retrieval Brain Imaging 0 0 0 0 0 p Website publication November NeuroRepor t, () ALTHOUGH numerous neuroimaging studies have examined the functional neuroanatomy supporting episodic memory for verbal material,

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information

Not All False Memories Are Created Equal: The Neural Basis of False Recognition

Not All False Memories Are Created Equal: The Neural Basis of False Recognition Cerebral Cortex November 2006;16:1645--1652 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj101 Advance Access publication December 28, 2005 Not All False Memories Are Created Equal: The Neural Basis of False Recognition Rachel

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Theories of Sensory, short-term & long-term memories Memory & brain Cellular bases

More information

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion Kevin N. Ochsner, a, * Rebecca D. Ray, b Jeffrey C. Cooper, b Elaine R. Robertson, b Sita Chopra,

More information

Attentional Control in the Aging Brain: Insights from an fmri Study of the Stroop Task

Attentional Control in the Aging Brain: Insights from an fmri Study of the Stroop Task Brain and Cognition 49, 277 296 (2002) doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1501 Attentional Control in the Aging Brain: Insights from an fmri Study of the Stroop Task Michael P. Milham,* Kirk I. Erickson,* Marie T.

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 224 (2011) 387 396 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research j ourna l ho mepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report Cross-modal versus within-modal

More information

Dissociating prefrontal contributions during a recency memory task

Dissociating prefrontal contributions during a recency memory task Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 350 364 Dissociating prefrontal contributions during a recency memory task M.N. Rajah a,, A.R. McIntosh b a Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, 132

More information

Neural Correlates of Temporal Context Retrieval. Fang Wang. Thesis submitted to the faculty of the

Neural Correlates of Temporal Context Retrieval. Fang Wang. Thesis submitted to the faculty of the Running Head: TEMPORAL CONTEXT RETRIEVAL MECHANISMS Neural Correlates of Temporal Context Retrieval Fang Wang Thesis submitted to the faculty of the Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University

More information

Brain Network Activity During Face Perception: The Impact of Perceptual Familiarity and Individual Differences in Childhood Experience

Brain Network Activity During Face Perception: The Impact of Perceptual Familiarity and Individual Differences in Childhood Experience Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published August 13, 2016 Cerebral Cortex, 2016; 1 13 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw232 Original Article ORIGINAL ARTICLE Brain Network Activity During Face Perception: The Impact

More information

Anterior prefrontal involvement in episodic retrieval reflects contextual interference

Anterior prefrontal involvement in episodic retrieval reflects contextual interference www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 28 (2005) 256 267 Anterior prefrontal involvement in episodic retrieval reflects contextual interference J.A. King, a, * T. Hartley, a H.J. Spiers, b E.A. Maguire,

More information

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References Supplementary Information Supplementary Figure 1. The mean parameter

More information

Changes in Default Mode Network as Automaticity Develops in a Categorization Task

Changes in Default Mode Network as Automaticity Develops in a Categorization Task Purdue University Purdue e-pubs Open Access Theses Theses and Dissertations January 2015 Changes in Default Mode Network as Automaticity Develops in a Categorization Task Farzin Shamloo Purdue University

More information

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Available online at www.sciencedirect.com R NeuroImage 18 (2003) 806 810 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Technical Note Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Andrea Mechelli,

More information

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain 1 Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain Emily Falk, Elliot Berkman, Traci Mann, Brittany Harrison, Matthew Lieberman This document contains: Example

More information