To compliment the population decoding approach (Figs. 4, 6, main text), we also tested the

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "To compliment the population decoding approach (Figs. 4, 6, main text), we also tested the"

Transcription

1 Supplementary Material, Page 1 of 18 Single-cell neurometric (ROC) analysis To compliment the population decoding approach (Figs. 4, 6, main text), we also tested the ability of individual MSTd neurons to account for cue reweighting behavior. Specifically, we used receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis to quantify the behavior of an ideal observer performing the same task as the animal but using only the firing rate responses of a given neuron. Response distributions for each nonzero heading were compared to the distribution for heading = 0 (taken as a proxy for the internal reference) to compute the proportion of rightward choices made by the ideal observer. As with the psychophysical analysis (Fig. 1b-d), these neurometric choice data were fit with cumulative Gaussian functions, and the neuronal PSE and neuronal threshold were taken as the mean and standard deviation of the fitted function, respectively. For the example neuron shown in Fig. 3 (replotted in Fig. S1a-c), neuronal thresholds in the single-cue trials were 6.7, 13.6, and 2.1 for the vestibular, low-coherence visual, and high-coherence visual conditions, respectively (Fig. S1d). These thresholds quantify the neuron s sensitivity to small variations in heading for each modality, which is primarily dictated by the slopes of the respective tuning functions. Using Eq. 2, single-cue neuronal thresholds were used to generate optimal vestibular weights for the ideal observer (here, 0.80 and 0.08 for low and high coherence, respectively). These were compared to observed weights computed with Eq. 4, using the neurometric PSEs from the combined, cue-conflict trials (Fig. S1e,f). Note that the reference distribution (responses at heading = 0 ) was taken from the Δ = 0 trials, regardless of the value of Δ. The use of non-conflict responses for the reference is a sensible strategy because the monkey cannot predict whether a trial will be a cueconflict trial, and may not even detect the conflict when present 1. Because the firing rates during cueconflict (green and blue traces in Fig. 3b,c and Fig. S1b,c) were compared with the non-conflict trials (orange traces), the shift of the tuning curves led to shifts of the neurometric functions, in a manner 1

2 Supplementary Material, Page 2 of 18 analogous to the psychometric functions (Fig. 1c,d). For this neuron, the observed weights corresponding to these PSE shifts were 0.76 and 0.16 for low and high coherences, respectively. A second example cell is shown in Fig. S1g-l. Because the ROC analysis approach depended on reliable PSE estimates from neurometric functions, it could only be performed on neurons with clear, monotonic tuning around straight ahead. 2

3 Supplementary Material, Page 3 of 18 Otherwise, the neurometric functions were too flat to provide reliable estimates of the neuronal thresholds and PSEs. Thus, we required the slope of the linear regression of firing rate vs. heading during the discrimination task to be significantly greater or less than zero for at least one single-cue modality, as well as the combined modality at both coherence levels. These criteria resulted in a sample of 45 neurons (30 from monkey Y, 15 from monkey W). This sample excluded most neurons with visual and vestibular tuning that was clearly opposite in slope ( opposite cells 2, see Fig. 5), as the combined tuning for these neurons was often too flat or non-monotonic. Of the 23 neurons classified as opposite (congruency index (CI) < -0.4), all but one failed to meet the criteria for ROC analysis, whereas 34 of the 46 congruent cells (CI > 0.4) passed these criteria. Weights for this subpopulation of MSTd neurons are summarized in Fig. S2a, with each neuron represented by two data points (one for each coherence, connected by a thin line) and plotted as observed vs. optimal vestibular weight. The main result to take away from this figure is that most cells showed large changes in observed weights during combined trials (ordinate) as a function of coherence. Median values for low (0.691) and high (0.195) coherence were highly significantly different (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test, p < 10-7 ). The paired difference in observed weights (low minus high coherence) was significantly greater than zero (p < 0.05, two-tailed bootstrap test) for 17 of 45 neurons (38%) and not significantly less than zero for any neurons (Fig. S2c). These results suggest that MSTd neurons respond robustly to trial-by-trial changes in cue reliability. Notably, these effects occurred for changes in reliability that were behaviorally relevant: the two coherence levels were chosen to be effective at driving behavioral reweighting (i.e., Fig. 2a,b), and were not tailored to drive changes in neurometric weights. Psychophysical weights from these 45 sessions are shown in the same formats for comparison (Fig. S2b,d). 3

4 Supplementary Material, Page 4 of 18 4

5 Supplementary Material, Page 5 of 18 The second main result from Fig. S2a involves the comparison between optimal and observed weights. Many of the data points and connecting lines trend along the unity-slope diagonal, indicating weights that were close to optimal predictions. Optimal and observed weights were significantly correlated for both coherences together (Spearman s rank correlation, rho = 0.73, p < ) and for each coherence separately (16%: rho = 0.49, p = ; 60%: rho = 0.77, p < 10-9 ). However, many data points also lie well above the unity-slope diagonal (observed weight > optimal weight; Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test: p < for both monkeys together and monkey W individually; p = 0.21 for monkey Y individually), some with nearly vertical connecting lines. These cells tended to have relatively flat vestibular tuning and therefore low optimal vestibular weights. Yet, the vestibular cue nonetheless had a sizeable influence on the combined tuning, resulting in observed vestibular weights that were greater than the prediction. This apparent deviation from optimality (as defined by Eq. 2) is intriguing because it parallels the over-weighting of the vestibular cue at low coherence, observed in behavior (Fig. 2a,b and Fig. S2b). Statistical comparisons from Fig. S2a also held for Fig. S2b (observed weight > optimal weight; p < for both monkeys together and monkey W individually; p = 0.28 for monkey Y individually), although a notable difference is the vestibular under-weighting (visual over-weighting) for monkey Y at 60% coherence (Fig. S2b, red circles below the diagonal; also evident in Fig. 2a). In summary, individual MSTd neurons reweight visual and vestibular cues on a trial-by-trial basis, supporting a possible role in perceptual reweighting and consistent with the population decoding results (Figs. 4, 6). Many cells perform nearly optimally, while a subset appears to over-weight vestibular cues, similar to the monkeys behavior at low coherence. This unexpected link between neuronal activity and a modality-specific perceptual bias adds support for the hypothesis that MSTd neurons contribute to visual-vestibular cue integration for heading perception. Lastly, neuronal 5

6 Supplementary Material, Page 6 of 18 thresholds for this sample (Fig. S3) decreased under combined stimulation, in accordance with optimal predictions (Eq. 3) and consistent with our previous work 2. 6

7 Supplementary Material, Page 7 of 18 ROC analysis approach: assumptions and caveats The neurometric analysis described above assumes that (a) the ideal observer knows the preferred heading (sign of tuning slope) for each neuron and modality, and (b) the response variability at zero heading is a reasonable approximation of the uncertainty in the internal reference against which the monkey is discriminating. The first assumption is common to all similar approaches and is necessary for the observer to interpret firing rates as signaling a particular stimulus in the world. The second assumption is an alternative to the neuron-antineuron framework used in some studies for measuring neuronal sensitivity with ROC analysis, including our own previous work 2. In that procedure, the response distribution for a particular heading would be compared with the distribution for the corresponding heading of opposite sign, thereby invoking an antineuron with mirror-symmetric tuning. Instead, we found that using the zero-heading responses as the reference distribution was more intuitive when the goal was to use neuronal PSEs during cue-conflict as a measure of cue reweighting. The main effect of changing our analysis to a neuron-antineuron approach would be to increase neuronal sensitivity (decrease neuronal thresholds) across the board; i.e., for monotonic tuning functions, there will generally be greater separation between the firing rate distributions for +θ vs. -θ for a given θ, as opposed to each θ vs. zero heading. However, this change would not alter the relative neuronal thresholds across modalities (i.e., optimal weights), nor the PSEs during cue-conflict (observed weights), provided that the non-conflict (Δ = 0º) responses were still used as the reference. Reweighting index To ensure that the results of Fig. S2a,c were not dependent on any assumptions of ROC analysis or the fitting of neurometric functions, we computed a model-free reweighting index (Fig. S2e) for the same sample of neurons, as follows. First we computed the mean normalized change in firing rate 7

8 Supplementary Material, Page 8 of 18 ( delta-fr ) caused by the cue-conflict (*see note below), separately for each coherence level, and collapsed across positive and negative Δ: delta-fr = (S1) Reweighting Index = delta-fr coh=60% delta-fr coh=16% (S2) In this equation, the index h refers to heading, t refers to each of N trials, r t (h,δ) is the single-trial firing rate in the combined modality for Δ = ±4º, and r-bar is the mean combined firing rate for Δ = 0º. The normalization factor in the denominator is the total firing rate modulation (max-min) for the Δ = 0º tuning curve. Sgn is the sign function (signum), and slope refers to the slope of the combined tuning curve based on linear regression. These sign functions preserve the expected relationship between cue reliability and firing rate as a function of Δ and tuning slope i.e., for the examples in Fig. S1c and Fig. S1i the tuning curve for Δ = +4º (green) shifts downward, but would have shifted upward for that same Δ and coherence if the tuning slope was positive instead of negative, and we wanted the delta-fr metric to have the same sign in both cases. The reweighting index itself was then computed as the difference between the delta-fr values for high versus low coherence, giving positive values for shifts of the tuning curve in the predicted direction. Statistical significance of the reweighting index was determined by bootstrapping the delta-fr metrics 1000 times (resampling across trials and heading values with replacement), recomputing the reweighting index each time, and determining whether more than 95% of the bootstrapped index values were greater or less than zero (percentile method at p < 0.05). The resulting distribution of reweighting index values was strongly shifted toward positive values (t test, p < ), with 26/45 (58%) of neurons having values significantly greater than zero (p < 0.05, bootstrap) and none significantly less than zero (Fig. S2e). 8

9 Supplementary Material, Page 9 of 18 [*Note: the effect of cue-conflict on firing rates may be more aptly described as a lateral tuning shift, since the conflict manipulation itself is a lateral one (change in heading specified by the constituent cues). However, for the monotonic tuning curves in our sample, this was largely indistinguishable from a vertical delta-fr, and we found the latter to be more reliable than common methods for estimating lateral shifts, such as curve fitting or cross-correlation (particularly for cells with relatively shallow tuning).] Likelihood-based decoder: assumptions and caveats Similar to the ROC analysis method for single neurons (see above), the decoder assumes knowledge of the neuronal tuning curves (f i in Eq. 14) at the readout stage. In previous theoretical treatments 3-6, the choice of what to use for f i was clear, mainly because only single-modality likelihood functions (or log-likelihoods) were being computed. In contrast, we are decoding from real neurons in 9 distinct trial types (3 stimulus modalities, 2 coherence levels, and 3 conflict angles), and therefore must make a decision about which tuning curves to substitute for f i when computing the likelihood functions. We chose to allow the decoder full knowledge of the neuronal tuning curves in every condition i.e., f i (θ) was taken as the tuning curve in the particular condition (stimulus modality and coherence level) being simulated on that trial. An important exception to this was the combined, cue-conflict (Δ = ±4º) trials. These trials were decoded with respect to the non-conflict (Δ = 0º) tuning curve, to allow the simulation to produce a shift in the PSEs similar to the monkeys behavior (e.g., see Fig. S4). While we cannot be certain that the brain is capable of such a dynamic readout strategy, it may be feasible in our task because the subject knows the modality (and possibly the coherence level) fairly early in the trial. The presence or absence of platform motion is easily detected from vibration and auditory cues, and the approximate coherence ( low or high, given that only 2 coherence levels are 9

10 Supplementary Material, Page 10 of 18 possible) is apparent from the dynamics of the random dots before they begin to clearly simulate a particular heading trajectory (see Fig. S5 for stimulus temporal profile). The alternative strategy of using a fixed tuning curve for all conditions (e.g., the single-cue vestibular tuning) produced similar results with regard to cue reweighting. However, doing so hampered our attempts to analyze populations with substantial numbers of opposite cells. For example, when 10

11 Supplementary Material, Page 11 of 18 decoding relative to the vestibular tuning curves, the simulation made too many errors in the visual modality, and the choice data could not be fit with a monotonically increasing function such as a cumulative Gaussian. Furthermore, even for congruent or mixed populations, using a tuning curve from one stimulus modality to decode another can produce large choice biases due to offsets between the two tuning curves. Thus, to ensure an unbiased and complete picture of the information contained in our MSTd population, we felt the full knowledge approach was best suited for our purposes. 11

12 Supplementary Material, Page 12 of 18 The response of each neuron in the population (r i ) can in principle be drawn from the actual single-trial responses for the desired heading and trial type. However, because population sensitivity (i.e., signal to noise ratio) increases with N, we found that for larger population sizes (N > ~40) the simulated psychometric functions (e.g., Fig. 4c,f; note abscissa scale) became too steep to be reliably fit using only the heading angles used in the experiments (0º, ±1.23º, ±3.5º, and ±10º). Thus, it became necessary to interpolate the tuning curves (linear, 0.1º resolution) and generate simulated firing rate distributions at this finer heading resolution. Responses were typically generated by sampling a Poisson process (poissrnd in Matlab) with the λ parameter (mean and variance) equal to the mean (interpolated) response. The results of the decoding model using this procedure were compared to the method of drawing real single-trial firing rates (when smaller population sizes permitted it), and no significant differences were found. As the Poisson method restricts the variance-to-mean ratio (Fano factor) to be 1, we also tested versions of the model using Gaussian noise and various Fano factors ranging from 1 to 2. We found no substantial differences between the two noise models with regard to the main conclusions of the study. Increasing the Fano factor only had a noticeable effect on the absolute thresholds for a given population size, not on the pattern of relative thresholds across modalities (optimal weights), nor the PSEs during cue-conflict (observed weights). An additional assumption of this version of the decoder is that each neuron is independent, i.e. that noise correlations are zero. Employing the method of Shadlen et al. 7 for generating arbitrary correlation structure in simulated data, we tested other versions that included small positive correlations characteristic of MSTd neuron pairs recorded in the heading discrimination task 8. As expected from the known effects of noise correlations on the fidelity of population codes 9, 10, positive correlations increased the simulated thresholds for a given population size. However, similar to the case of 12

13 Supplementary Material, Page 13 of 18 increasing Fano factor, the relative single-cue thresholds and the combined PSEs were largely unaffected by correlations, and thus the conclusions with regard to cue reweighting did not change. The one case where the basic Poisson decoding method seemed to fail was with artificial combined responses, generated by taking weighted sums of single-cue responses (Fig. 8b-e). Initially, we performed these simulations by generating combined tuning curves (mean firing rates) as weighted sums of single-cue tuning curves, then making draws from a Poisson process at the resulting means. This procedure failed to reproduce appropriate distributions of single-trial firing rates, as evidenced by the fact that even the decoding of optimal responses (i.e., generated with weights according to Eq. 7) did not result in optimal behavior according to Eqs. 2 and 3 (not shown). This problem was remedied by taking weighted sums of single-trial responses (rather than mean responses) to generate the artificial combined data. Since the weights in the summation were not fixed at 1, the resulting response distributions were typically not Poisson, requiring us to use an ad-hoc decoding method in place of Eq. 14. This ad-hoc method began with defining the distribution P(r i θ) empirically, as a normalized histogram of single-trial firing rates for a given heading and cell. We then selected the probability value of the nearest bin corresponding to the particular r being simulated, and repeated this for all neurons and headings to essentially bootstrap the full likelihood function P(r θ). This procedure was validated by the near-perfect match between optimal and observed weights and thresholds in Fig. 8b,c, and was also able to reproduce the basic pattern of results shown in Fig. 6e,f. Variance as a signature of cue combination? The fact that our decoder failed when taking weighted sums of mean single-cue responses and then drawing Poisson random deviates raises an interesting set of questions about the variance of multisensory responses. Assuming, as we have shown (Fig. 7 and ref. 11 ), that mean combined responses 13

14 Supplementary Material, Page 14 of 18 are reasonably well modeled as weighted sums of single-cue responses, we can ask whether the variance of the combined response behaves as predicted from applying these weights. We tested this by applying the weights (derived from fits to the tuning curves; see main text) to the individual trial firing rates in the unimodal vestibular and visual conditions, then comparing the variance of these artificial weighted-sum trials against the measured variance in the combined conditions. This comparison is plotted in Fig. S6a,b for low and high coherence, respectively (one data point per heading per neuron, Δ=0º trials only; sample includes only congruent cells that were significantly well fit by the linear model, as in Fig. 8b-e (N=43)). The predicted (artificial) variance was well correlated with, and not significantly different from, the measured variance across both levels of reliability (Spearman s rho = 0.75 for 16% coh., rho = 0.77 for 60% coh., p << for both; Wilcoxon matched pairs test, p = for 16% coh., p = 0.78 for 60% coh.). This match is not trivial because the weights change rapidly with cue reliability (Fig. 7c,d) in a manner consistent with a network nonlinearity such as divisive normalization 12 (see also Discussion and Fig. S5). With such a nonlinearity at play, one might expect some degree of information loss during cue combination, which would imply greater response variance than predicted by a weighted sum of singlecue trials. The fact that combined variance is as predicted by the weighted sum model lends further support for the role of MSTd in contributing to optimal cue integration, as it suggests the combination can occur without substantial information loss. This apparent summation of variance during cue integration is roughly analogous to a recent result 13 showing an increase in variance in area LIP across time within a trial, due to the accumulation of momentary evidence during perceptual decision-making. Although a detailed examination of this phenomenon is beyond the scope of our study, it may be that variance becomes another useful tool for diagnosing whether and how candidate multisensory neuronal populations carry out the necessary computations for sensory cue integration. 14

15 Supplementary Material, Page 15 of 18 Derivation of optimal neural weights This section describes a derivation of the optimal weight ratio ρ opt (Eq. 7 in main text), which we compared to the measured weight ratio in Fig. 8a. Let f ves (θ), f vis (θ,c), and f comb (θ,c) represent the mean response (tuning function) of a particular cell for the three respective stimulus modalities as a function of heading (θ) and coherence (c). For mathematical convenience, we replace the neural weights A ves and A vis in Eq. 5 with a weight ratio ρ = A ves / A vis and include a scaling constant a. We then have (dropping the θ and c dependence for clarity): 15

16 Supplementary Material, Page 16 of 18 (S3) where f' refers to the derivative of the tuning functions. If we assume that the spike count variability on single-cue trials is Poisson-like (i.e., can be well approximated by a broad family of functions known as the exponential family with linear sufficient statistics 14-16, a type of variability believed to be common across cortical areas 15, 17-19, including in our MSTd recordings (Y. Gu, CRF, DEA and GCD, unpublished observations)), the variance σ 2 will be proportional to the mean, where the proportionality constant is the Fano factor (F = variance/mean). Because the variance of a sum of independent random variables is the sum of their respective variances, and applying the identity Var(aX) = a 2 Var(X), we have: (S4) What we seek is the value of ρ that maximizes linear Fisher information (the information that can be recovered by an optimal linear decoder) from the combined responses (I comb ), which is defined as 20 : (S5) To find the weight that maximizes information we need to solve: (S6) Solving this equation (note that the constants a and F drop out) gives the following expression for the optimal neural weight ratio ρ opt : (Eq. 7 in text) 16

17 Supplementary Material, Page 17 of 18 (with θ and c dependence restored, to illustrate that this ratio is evaluated at θ = 0 and computed separately for the two coherence levels (Fig. 8a)). Note: although it appears that we have derived the weights maximizing linear Fisher information only, Eq. 7 in fact maximizes the full Fisher information. Indeed, it is easy to show that for the weight ratio above, the combined responses also belong to the Poisson-like family, and for the Poisson-like family it turns out that Fisher information is equal to linear Fisher information 20. As mentioned in the main text, Eq. 7 describes the neural operation one would expect to see in an area performing optimal multisensory integration within a probabilistic population code (PPC) framework 16. By taking into account the observed tuning parameters (which define the mean of the likelihood as a function of heading), this optimal weight ratio allows a population to implicitly encode the posterior P(θ r), even if tuning is not simply scaled multiplicatively by coherence (see Eq. 6). Thus, the fact that measured weights vary with coherence (Fig. 7c,d) in a manner roughly similar to optimal weights (Fig. 8a) constitutes indirect evidence for a probabilistic coding scheme such as PPC. To estimate the terms on the right side of Eq. 7 (i.e., for Fig. 8a-c), responses in each single-cue condition were fit separately via linear regression, and the slope and intercept were taken as f' and f, respectively. Because the derivation above assumes linear tuning, these linear fits were used in place of real single-cue data for the decoding simulations depicted in Fig. 8b,c; this accounts for the slight difference in single-cue thresholds (and thus optimal weights) in Fig. 8b,c versus d,e. 17

18 Supplementary Material, Page 18 of 18 References 1. Fetsch, C.R., Turner, A.H., DeAngelis, G.C. & Angelaki, D.E. Dynamic reweighting of visual and vestibular cues during self-motion perception. J Neurosci 29, (2009). 2. Gu, Y., Angelaki, D.E. & Deangelis, G.C. Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd. Nat Neurosci 11, (2008). 3. Jazayeri, M. & Movshon, J.A. Optimal representation of sensory information by neural populations. Nat Neurosci 9, (2006). 4. Sanger, T.D. Probability density estimation for the interpretation of neural population codes. J Neurophysiol 76, (1996). 5. Dayan, P. & Abbott, L.F. Theoretical Neuroscience (MIT press, Cambridge, MA, 2001). 6. Foldiak, P. The 'ideal homunculus': statistical inference from neural population responses. in Computation and Neural Systems (ed. F.H. Eeckman & J.M. Bower) (Kluwer Academic Publishers, Norwell, MA, 1993). 7. Shadlen, M.N., Britten, K.H., Newsome, W.T. & Movshon, J.A. A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motion. J Neurosci 16, (1996). 8. Gu, Y., et al. Perceptual learning reduces interneuronal correlations in macaque visual cortex. Neuron 71, (2011). 9. Zohary, E., Shadlen, M.N. & Newsome, W.T. Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance. Nature 370, (1994). 10. Averbeck, B.B., Latham, P.E. & Pouget, A. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat Rev Neurosci 7, (2006). 11. Morgan, M.L., Deangelis, G.C. & Angelaki, D.E. Multisensory integration in macaque visual cortex depends on cue reliability. Neuron 59, (2008). 12. Ohshiro, T., Angelaki, D.E. & DeAngelis, G.C. A normalization model of multi-sensory integration. Nat Neurosci In Press. (2010). 13. Churchland, A.K., et al. Variance as a signature of neural computations during decision making. Neuron 69, (2011). 14. Beck, J., Ma, W.J., Latham, P.E. & Pouget, A. Probabilistic population codes and the exponential family of distributions. Prog Brain Res 165, (2007). 15. Beck, J.M., et al. Probabilistic population codes for Bayesian decision making. Neuron 60, (2008). 16. Ma, W.J., Beck, J.M., Latham, P.E. & Pouget, A. Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes. Nat Neurosci 9, (2006). 17. Shadlen, M.N. & Newsome, W.T. The variable discharge of cortical neurons: implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J Neurosci 18, (1998). 18. Carandini, M. Amplification of trial-to-trial response variability by neurons in visual cortex. PLoS Biol 2, E264 (2004). 19. Tolhurst, D.J., Movshon, J.A. & Dean, A.F. The statistical reliability of signals in single neurons in cat and monkey visual cortex. Vision Res 23, (1983). 20. Beck, J., Bejjanki, V.R. & Pouget, A. Insights from a simple expression for linear fisher information in a recurrently connected population of spiking neurons. Neural Comput 23, (2011). 18

Neural correlates of reliability-based cue weighting during multisensory integration

Neural correlates of reliability-based cue weighting during multisensory integration a r t i c l e s Neural correlates of reliability-based cue weighting during multisensory integration Christopher R Fetsch 1, Alexandre Pouget 2,3, Gregory C DeAngelis 1,2,5 & Dora E Angelaki 1,4,5 211

More information

Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd

Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd Yong Gu, Dora E Angelaki,3 & Gregory C DeAngelis 3 Human observers combine multiple sensory cues synergistically to achieve greater perceptual

More information

Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks. Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology

Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks. Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology Applying the coding scheme to dynamic cue combination (Experiment, Kording&Wolpert,2004) Dynamic sensorymotor

More information

Choice-related activity and correlated noise in subcortical vestibular neurons

Choice-related activity and correlated noise in subcortical vestibular neurons Choice-related activity and correlated noise in subcortical vestibular neurons Sheng Liu,2, Yong Gu, Gregory C DeAngelis 3 & Dora E Angelaki,2 npg 23 Nature America, Inc. All rights reserved. Functional

More information

Spontaneous Cortical Activity Reveals Hallmarks of an Optimal Internal Model of the Environment. Berkes, Orban, Lengyel, Fiser.

Spontaneous Cortical Activity Reveals Hallmarks of an Optimal Internal Model of the Environment. Berkes, Orban, Lengyel, Fiser. Statistically optimal perception and learning: from behavior to neural representations. Fiser, Berkes, Orban & Lengyel Trends in Cognitive Sciences (2010) Spontaneous Cortical Activity Reveals Hallmarks

More information

Single cell tuning curves vs population response. Encoding: Summary. Overview of the visual cortex. Overview of the visual cortex

Single cell tuning curves vs population response. Encoding: Summary. Overview of the visual cortex. Overview of the visual cortex Encoding: Summary Spikes are the important signals in the brain. What is still debated is the code: number of spikes, exact spike timing, temporal relationship between neurons activities? Single cell tuning

More information

A Neural Signature of Divisive Normalization at the Level of Multisensory Integration in Primate Cortex

A Neural Signature of Divisive Normalization at the Level of Multisensory Integration in Primate Cortex Article A Neural Signature of Divisive Normalization at the Level of Multisensory Integration in Primate Cortex Highlights d Many neurons in macaque area MSTd show a specific form of cross-modal suppression

More information

Tuning Curves, Neuronal Variability, and Sensory Coding

Tuning Curves, Neuronal Variability, and Sensory Coding Tuning Curves, Neuronal Variability, and Sensory Coding Daniel A. Butts 1*[, Mark S. Goldman 2[ 1 Division of Engineering and Applied Sciences, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States

More information

To Accompany: Thalamic Synchrony and the Adaptive Gating of Information Flow to Cortex Wang, Webber, & Stanley

To Accompany: Thalamic Synchrony and the Adaptive Gating of Information Flow to Cortex Wang, Webber, & Stanley SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL To Accompany: Thalamic Synchrony and the Adaptive Gating of Information Flow to Cortex Wang, Webber, & Stanley Supplementary Note 1: Parametric fits of spike count distributions

More information

Neural response time integration subserves. perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s. reduced model

Neural response time integration subserves. perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s. reduced model Neural response time integration subserves perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s reduced model Chris Ayers and Narayanan Krishnamurthy December 15, 2008 Abstract A neural network describing the

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

Sensory Cue Integration

Sensory Cue Integration Sensory Cue Integration Summary by Byoung-Hee Kim Computer Science and Engineering (CSE) http://bi.snu.ac.kr/ Presentation Guideline Quiz on the gist of the chapter (5 min) Presenters: prepare one main

More information

Three methods for measuring perception. Magnitude estimation. Steven s power law. P = k S n

Three methods for measuring perception. Magnitude estimation. Steven s power law. P = k S n Three methods for measuring perception 1. Magnitude estimation 2. Matching 3. Detection/discrimination Magnitude estimation Have subject rate (e.g., 1-10) some aspect of a stimulus (e.g., how bright it

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Neuron. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 December 18.

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Neuron. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 December 18. How can single sensory neurons predict behavior? Xaq Pitkow 1,2,*, Sheng Liu 1, Dora E. Angelaki 1,2, Gregory C. DeAngelis 3, and Alex Pouget 3,4 1 Baylor College of Medicine, Department of Neuroscience,

More information

Overview of the visual cortex. Ventral pathway. Overview of the visual cortex

Overview of the visual cortex. Ventral pathway. Overview of the visual cortex Overview of the visual cortex Two streams: Ventral What : V1,V2, V4, IT, form recognition and object representation Dorsal Where : V1,V2, MT, MST, LIP, VIP, 7a: motion, location, control of eyes and arms

More information

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) 1 Outline Method of Adjustment Method of Limits Method of Constant Stimuli Probit Analysis 2 Method of Constant Stimuli A set of equally spaced levels of the stimulus

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 5: Data analysis II Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis II 6 Single

More information

Probing Sensory Readout via Combined Choice- Correlation Measures and Microstimulation Perturbation

Probing Sensory Readout via Combined Choice- Correlation Measures and Microstimulation Perturbation Article Probing Sensory Readout via Combined Choice- Correlation Measures and Microstimulation Perturbation Highlights d Sensory-choice congruent and opposite cells in the MST and MT contribute to motion

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Current Biology, in press Supplemental Information: Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Grigorios Sotiropoulos, Aaron R. Seitz, and Peggy Seriès Supplemental Data Detailed

More information

A Probabilistic Network Model of Population Responses

A Probabilistic Network Model of Population Responses A Probabilistic Network Model of Population Responses Richard S. Zemel Jonathan Pillow Department of Computer Science Center for Neural Science University of Toronto New York University Toronto, ON MS

More information

Behavioral generalization

Behavioral generalization Supplementary Figure 1 Behavioral generalization. a. Behavioral generalization curves in four Individual sessions. Shown is the conditioned response (CR, mean ± SEM), as a function of absolute (main) or

More information

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4.

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4. Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4. Jude F. Mitchell, Kristy A. Sundberg, and John H. Reynolds Systems Neurobiology Lab, The Salk Institute,

More information

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo Business Statistics The following was provided by Dr. Suzanne Delaney, and is a comprehensive review of Business Statistics. The workshop instructor will provide relevant examples during the Skills Assessment

More information

2 INSERM U280, Lyon, France

2 INSERM U280, Lyon, France Natural movies evoke responses in the primary visual cortex of anesthetized cat that are not well modeled by Poisson processes Jonathan L. Baker 1, Shih-Cheng Yen 1, Jean-Philippe Lachaux 2, Charles M.

More information

A Neural Model of Context Dependent Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex

A Neural Model of Context Dependent Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex A Neural Model of Context Dependent Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex Sugandha Sharma (s72sharm@uwaterloo.ca) Brent J. Komer (bjkomer@uwaterloo.ca) Terrence C. Stewart (tcstewar@uwaterloo.ca) Chris

More information

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08 Auditory Perception - Detection versus Discrimination - Localization versus Discrimination - - Electrophysiological Measurements Psychophysical Measurements Three Approaches to Researching Audition physiology

More information

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08 Auditory Perception - Detection versus Discrimination - Localization versus Discrimination - Electrophysiological Measurements - Psychophysical Measurements 1 Three Approaches to Researching Audition physiology

More information

Perceptual learning as improved probabilistic inference in early sensory areas

Perceptual learning as improved probabilistic inference in early sensory areas Perceptual learning as improved probabilistic inference in early sensory areas Vikranth R Bejjanki,, Jeffrey M Beck,,, Zhong-Lin Lu 3 & Alexandre Pouget Nature America, Inc. All rights reserved. Extensive

More information

Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlations

Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlations Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlations Marlene R Cohen & John H R Maunsell 29 Nature America, Inc. All rights reserved. Visual attention can improve behavioral

More information

Models of Attention. Models of Attention

Models of Attention. Models of Attention Models of Models of predictive: can we predict eye movements (bottom up attention)? [L. Itti and coll] pop out and saliency? [Z. Li] Readings: Maunsell & Cook, the role of attention in visual processing,

More information

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions Tsung-Ren Huang Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University trhuang@ntu.edu.tw Abstract Shibata et al. reported that humans could learn to

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training.

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training. Supplementary Figure 1 Behavioral training. a, Mazes used for behavioral training. Asterisks indicate reward location. Only some example mazes are shown (for example, right choice and not left choice maze

More information

Sensory Adaptation within a Bayesian Framework for Perception

Sensory Adaptation within a Bayesian Framework for Perception presented at: NIPS-05, Vancouver BC Canada, Dec 2005. published in: Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems eds. Y. Weiss, B. Schölkopf, and J. Platt volume 18, pages 1291-1298, May 2006 MIT

More information

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material Supplemental Material Recording technique Multi-unit activity (MUA) was recorded from electrodes that were chronically implanted (Teflon-coated platinum-iridium wires) in the primary visual cortex representing

More information

Still important ideas

Still important ideas Readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 & Appendix D & E (online) Plous Chapters 17 & 18 - Chapter 17: Social Influences - Chapter 18: Group Judgments and Decisions Still important ideas Contrast the measurement

More information

Multisensory Integration in Macaque Visual Cortex Depends on Cue Reliability

Multisensory Integration in Macaque Visual Cortex Depends on Cue Reliability Article Multisensory Integration in Macaque Visual Cortex Depends on Cue Reliability Michael L. Morgan, 1 Gregory C. DeAngelis, 2,3 and Dora E. Angelaki 1,3, * 1 Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology,

More information

Report. A Causal Role for V5/MT Neurons Coding Motion-Disparity Conjunctions in Resolving Perceptual Ambiguity

Report. A Causal Role for V5/MT Neurons Coding Motion-Disparity Conjunctions in Resolving Perceptual Ambiguity Current Biology 23, 1454 1459, August 5, 2013 ª2013 The Authors. Open access under CC BY license. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.023 A Causal Role for V5/MT Neurons Coding Motion-Disparity Conjunctions

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Is the homunculus aware of sensory adaptation?

Is the homunculus aware of sensory adaptation? Is the homunculus aware of sensory adaptation? Peggy Seriès 1, Alan A. Stocker 2 and Eero P. Simoncelli 2. March 2, 29 (1) IANC, University of Edinburgh, 1 Crighton Street, UK EH8 9AB. (2) CNS and CIMS,

More information

PSYCH-GA.2211/NEURL-GA.2201 Fall 2016 Mathematical Tools for Cognitive and Neural Science. Homework 5

PSYCH-GA.2211/NEURL-GA.2201 Fall 2016 Mathematical Tools for Cognitive and Neural Science. Homework 5 PSYCH-GA.2211/NEURL-GA.2201 Fall 2016 Mathematical Tools for Cognitive and Neural Science Homework 5 Due: 21 Dec 2016 (late homeworks penalized 10% per day) See the course web site for submission details.

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 Supporting Information Variances and biases of absolute distributions were larger in the 2-line

More information

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 (emphasis on Chapter 12) Online readings: Appendix D, E & F

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 (emphasis on Chapter 12) Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 (emphasis on Chapter 12) Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Plous Chapters 17 & 18 Chapter 17: Social Influences Chapter 18: Group Judgments and Decisions

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:1.138/nature1216 Supplementary Methods M.1 Definition and properties of dimensionality Consider an experiment with a discrete set of c different experimental conditions, corresponding to all distinct

More information

Context-Dependent Changes in Functional Circuitry in Visual Area MT

Context-Dependent Changes in Functional Circuitry in Visual Area MT Article Context-Dependent Changes in Functional Circuitry in Visual Area MT Marlene R. Cohen 1, * and William T. Newsome 1 1 Howard Hughes Medical Institute and Department of Neurobiology, Stanford University

More information

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Yingxue Wang, Sandro Romani, Brian Lustig, Anthony Leonardo, Eva Pastalkova Supplementary Materials Supplementary Modeling

More information

Efficient coding provides a direct link between prior and likelihood in perceptual Bayesian inference

Efficient coding provides a direct link between prior and likelihood in perceptual Bayesian inference Efficient coding provides a direct link between prior and likelihood in perceptual Bayesian inference Xue-Xin Wei and Alan A. Stocker Departments of Psychology and Electrical and Systems Engineering University

More information

A model of encoding and decoding in V1 and MT accounts for motion perception anisotropies in the human visual system

A model of encoding and decoding in V1 and MT accounts for motion perception anisotropies in the human visual system B R A I N R E S E A R C H 1 2 9 9 ( 2 0 0 9 ) 3 1 6 a v a i l a b l e a t w w w. s c i e n c e d i r e c t. c o m w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / b r a i n r e s Research Report A model of

More information

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Christopher J. Rozell, Don. H. Johnson and Raymon M. Glantz Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering Department of

More information

Chapter 1: Introduction to Statistics

Chapter 1: Introduction to Statistics Chapter 1: Introduction to Statistics Variables A variable is a characteristic or condition that can change or take on different values. Most research begins with a general question about the relationship

More information

STATISTICS AND RESEARCH DESIGN

STATISTICS AND RESEARCH DESIGN Statistics 1 STATISTICS AND RESEARCH DESIGN These are subjects that are frequently confused. Both subjects often evoke student anxiety and avoidance. To further complicate matters, both areas appear have

More information

University of California- Los Angeles. Affiliations: University of California- Los Angeles

University of California- Los Angeles. Affiliations: University of California- Los Angeles (1) Overview Title A random dot kinematogram for online visual psychophysics Paper Authors 1. Rajananda, Sivananda; 2. Lau, Hakwan; 3. Odegaard, Brian Paper Author Roles and Affiliations 1. Designed software;

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

A normalization model suggests that attention changes the weighting of inputs between visual areas

A normalization model suggests that attention changes the weighting of inputs between visual areas A normalization model suggests that attention changes the weighting of inputs between visual areas Douglas A. Ruff a,1 and Marlene R. Cohen a a Department of Neuroscience and Center for the Neural Basis

More information

Supervised Calibration Relies on the Multisensory Percept

Supervised Calibration Relies on the Multisensory Percept Article Supervised Calibration Relies on the Multisensory Percept Adam Zaidel, 1, * Wei Ji Ma, 1,2 and Dora E. Angelaki 1 1 Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX 77030, USA

More information

A Bayesian Model of Conditioned Perception

A Bayesian Model of Conditioned Perception presented at: NIPS 21, Vancouver BC Canada, December 5th, 27. to appear in: Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems vol 2, pp XXX-XXX, May 28 MIT Press, Cambridge MA. A Bayesian Model of Conditioned

More information

2012 Course : The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Science

2012 Course : The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Science 2012 Course : The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Science Stanislas Dehaene Chair in Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture No. 4 Constraints combination and selection of a

More information

Signal detection theory. Direct comparison of perception and neural activity. ? Choice probability. Visual stimulus. Behavioral judgement

Signal detection theory. Direct comparison of perception and neural activity. ? Choice probability. Visual stimulus. Behavioral judgement Signal detection theory yes no Visual stimulus Neurometric function Psychometric function Neuronal response? Choice probability Behavioral judgement Direct comparison oerception and neural activity Record

More information

Multisensory Convergence of Visual and Vestibular Heading Cues in the Pursuit Area of the Frontal Eye Field

Multisensory Convergence of Visual and Vestibular Heading Cues in the Pursuit Area of the Frontal Eye Field Cerebral Cortex, November 216;26: 3785 381 doi:1.193/cercor/bhv183 Advance Access Publication Date: 18 August 215 Original Article ORIGINAL ARTICLE Multisensory Convergence of Visual and Vestibular Heading

More information

Neurons in Dorsal Visual Area V5/MT Signal Relative

Neurons in Dorsal Visual Area V5/MT Signal Relative 17892 The Journal of Neuroscience, December 7, 211 31(49):17892 1794 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Neurons in Dorsal Visual Area V5/MT Signal Relative Disparity Kristine Krug and Andrew J. Parker Oxford

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Task timeline for Solo and Info trials.

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Task timeline for Solo and Info trials. Supplementary Figure 1 Task timeline for Solo and Info trials. Each trial started with a New Round screen. Participants made a series of choices between two gambles, one of which was objectively riskier

More information

Optimal information decoding from neuronal populations with specific stimulus selectivity

Optimal information decoding from neuronal populations with specific stimulus selectivity Optimal information decoding from neuronal populations with specific stimulus selectivity Marcelo A. Montemurro The University of Manchester Faculty of Life Sciences Moffat Building PO Box 88, Manchester

More information

Decoding a Perceptual Decision Process across Cortex

Decoding a Perceptual Decision Process across Cortex Article Decoding a Perceptual Decision Process across Cortex Adrián Hernández, 1 Verónica Nácher, 1 Rogelio Luna, 1 Antonio Zainos, 1 Luis Lemus, 1 Manuel Alvarez, 1 Yuriria Vázquez, 1 Liliana Camarillo,

More information

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Goals for the lecture Be able to: Conceptually understand some of the analysis and jargon encountered in a typical (sensory)

More information

Neuroscience 2016 SHORT COURSE 2. Data Science and Data Skills for Neuroscientists Organizers: Konrad P. Kording, PhD, and Alyson Fletcher, PhD

Neuroscience 2016 SHORT COURSE 2. Data Science and Data Skills for Neuroscientists Organizers: Konrad P. Kording, PhD, and Alyson Fletcher, PhD Neuroscience 2016 SHORT COURSE 2 Data Science and Data Skills for Neuroscientists Organizers: Konrad P. Kording, PhD, and Alyson Fletcher, PhD Short Course 2 Data Science and Data Skills for Neuroscientists

More information

Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning

Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning Introduction Learning new motor skills Variability in sensors and task Tennis: Velocity of ball Not all are equally probable over time Increased uncertainty:

More information

SI Appendix: Full Description of Data Analysis Methods. Results from data analyses were expressed as mean ± standard error of the mean.

SI Appendix: Full Description of Data Analysis Methods. Results from data analyses were expressed as mean ± standard error of the mean. SI Appendix: Full Description of Data Analysis Methods Behavioral Data Analysis Results from data analyses were expressed as mean ± standard error of the mean. Analyses of behavioral data were performed

More information

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences 2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences Stanislas Dehaene Chair of Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture n 5 Bayesian Decision-Making Lecture material translated

More information

Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot

Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot Rajani Raman* and Sandip Sarkar Saha Institute of Nuclear Physics, HBNI, 1/AF, Bidhannagar,

More information

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination Timothy N. Rubin (trubin@uci.edu) Michael D. Lee (mdlee@uci.edu) Charles F. Chubb (cchubb@uci.edu) Department of Cognitive

More information

Business Statistics Probability

Business Statistics Probability Business Statistics The following was provided by Dr. Suzanne Delaney, and is a comprehensive review of Business Statistics. The workshop instructor will provide relevant examples during the Skills Assessment

More information

Adapting to Remapped Auditory Localization Cues: A Decision-Theory Model

Adapting to Remapped Auditory Localization Cues: A Decision-Theory Model Shinn-Cunningham, BG (2000). Adapting to remapped auditory localization cues: A decisiontheory model, Perception and Psychophysics, 62(), 33-47. Adapting to Remapped Auditory Localization Cues: A Decision-Theory

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:10.1038/nature10776 Supplementary Information 1: Influence of inhibition among blns on STDP of KC-bLN synapses (simulations and schematics). Unconstrained STDP drives network activity to saturation

More information

Complexity and specificity of experimentally induced expectations in motion perception

Complexity and specificity of experimentally induced expectations in motion perception Journal of Vision (2012)?, 1-? http://journalofvision.org/?/?/?/ 1 Complexity and specificity of experimentally induced expectations in motion perception Nikos Gekas Matthew Chalk Aaron R. Seitz Peggy

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information ten Oever and Sack 10.1073/pnas.1517519112 SI Materials and Methods Experiment 1. Participants. A total of 20 participants (9 male; age range 18 32 y; mean age 25 y) participated

More information

Neural Variability and Sampling-Based Probabilistic Representations in the Visual Cortex

Neural Variability and Sampling-Based Probabilistic Representations in the Visual Cortex Article Neural Variability and Sampling-Based Probabilistic Representations in the Visual Cortex Highlights d Stochastic sampling links perceptual uncertainty to neural response variability Authors Gerg}o

More information

Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making

Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making Leo P. Surgre, Gres S. Corrado and William T. Newsome Presenter: He Crane Huang 04/20/2010 Outline Studies on neural

More information

A simple approach to ignoring irrelevant variables by population decoding based on multisensory neurons

A simple approach to ignoring irrelevant variables by population decoding based on multisensory neurons J Neurophysiol 116: 1449 1467, 216. First published June 22, 216; doi:1.1152/jn.5.216. simple approach to ignoring irrelevant variables by population decoding based on multisensory neurons HyungGoo R.

More information

Information in the Neuronal Representation of Individual Stimuli in the Primate Temporal Visual Cortex

Information in the Neuronal Representation of Individual Stimuli in the Primate Temporal Visual Cortex Journal of Computational Neuroscience 4, 309 333 (1997) c 1997 Kluwer Academic Publishers, Boston. Manufactured in The Netherlands. Information in the Neuronal Representation of Individual Stimuli in the

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Recording sites.

Supplementary Figure 1. Recording sites. Supplementary Figure 1 Recording sites. (a, b) Schematic of recording locations for mice used in the variable-reward task (a, n = 5) and the variable-expectation task (b, n = 5). RN, red nucleus. SNc,

More information

Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory

Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory 1 Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory Imagine that you are watching a game of tennis on television and someone accidentally mutes the sound. You will probably notice that following the game becomes

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Trial structure for go/no-go behavior

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Trial structure for go/no-go behavior Supplementary Figure 1 Trial structure for go/no-go behavior a, Overall timeline of experiments. Day 1: A1 mapping, injection of AAV1-SYN-GCAMP6s, cranial window and headpost implantation. Water restriction

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

Internal Representations of Temporal Statistics and Feedback Calibrate Motor-Sensory Interval Timing

Internal Representations of Temporal Statistics and Feedback Calibrate Motor-Sensory Interval Timing Internal Representations of Temporal Statistics and Feedback Calibrate Motor-Sensory Interval Timing Luigi Acerbi 1,2 *, Daniel M. Wolpert 3, Sethu Vijayakumar 1 1 Institute of Perception, Action and Behaviour,

More information

Neural variability in premotor cortex provides a signature of motor preparation

Neural variability in premotor cortex provides a signature of motor preparation Neural variability in premotor cortex provides a signature of motor preparation Mark M Churchland, Byron M Yu, Stephen I Ryu, Gopal Santhanam, Afsheen Afshar, Krishna V Shenoy How does the brain generate

More information

A Computational Analysis of the Relationship between Neuronal and Behavioral Responses to Visual Motion

A Computational Analysis of the Relationship between Neuronal and Behavioral Responses to Visual Motion The Journal of Neuroscience, February 15, 1996, 76(4):1486-l 510 A Computational Analysis of the Relationship between Neuronal and Behavioral Responses to Visual Motion Michael N. Shadlen, Kenneth H. Britten,

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Quantifying the signals contained in heterogeneous neural responses and determining their relationships with task performance

Quantifying the signals contained in heterogeneous neural responses and determining their relationships with task performance Quantifying the signals contained in heterogeneous neural responses and determining their relationships with task performance Marino Pagan and Nicole C. Rust J Neurophysiol :58-598,. First published June

More information

Technical Specifications

Technical Specifications Technical Specifications In order to provide summary information across a set of exercises, all tests must employ some form of scoring models. The most familiar of these scoring models is the one typically

More information

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14 CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines Assignment 5: Neural mechanisms of object-based attention Due: Friday, April 14 This Assignment is a guided reading of the 2014 paper, Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Dynamic Integration of Reward and Stimulus Information in Perceptual Decision-Making

Dynamic Integration of Reward and Stimulus Information in Perceptual Decision-Making Integration of reward and stimulus information Dynamic Integration of Reward and Stimulus Information in Perceptual Decision-Making Juan Gao,2, Rebecca Tortell James L. McClelland,2,, Department of Psychology,

More information

Group Redundancy Measures Reveal Redundancy Reduction in the Auditory Pathway

Group Redundancy Measures Reveal Redundancy Reduction in the Auditory Pathway Group Redundancy Measures Reveal Redundancy Reduction in the Auditory Pathway Gal Chechik Amir Globerson Naftali Tishby Institute of Computer Science and Engineering and The Interdisciplinary Center for

More information

Time [ms]

Time [ms] Magazine R1051 Brief motion stimuli preferentially activate surroundsuppressed neurons in macaque visual area MT Jan Churan, Farhan A. Khawaja, James M.G. Tsui and Christopher C. Pack Intuitively one might

More information

Supplemental Information: Adaptation can explain evidence for encoding of probabilistic. information in macaque inferior temporal cortex

Supplemental Information: Adaptation can explain evidence for encoding of probabilistic. information in macaque inferior temporal cortex Supplemental Information: Adaptation can explain evidence for encoding of probabilistic information in macaque inferior temporal cortex Kasper Vinken and Rufin Vogels Supplemental Experimental Procedures

More information

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES Correlational Research Correlational Designs Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are

More information

Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning

Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning Maximilian Riesenhuber Tomaso Poggio Center for Biological and Computational Learning and Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences Massachusetts

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psych.colorado.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information