Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on risky decision making are mediated by hot and cold decisions, personality, and hemisphere

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on risky decision making are mediated by hot and cold decisions, personality, and hemisphere"

Transcription

1 European Journal of Neuroscience European Journal of Neuroscience, pp. 1 8, 2013 doi: /ejn Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on risky decision making are mediated by hot and cold decisions, personality, and hemisphere J urgen Pripfl, Renate Neumann, Ulla K ohler and Claus Lamm Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience (SCAN) Unit, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, A-1010 Vienna, Austria Keywords: cognitive control, hemispheric specialization, impulsivity, risk-taking, tdcs Abstract Previous results point towards a lateralization of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) function in risky decision making. While the right hemisphere seems involved in inhibitory cognitive control of affective impulses, the left DLPFC is crucial in the deliberative processing of information relevant for the decision. However, a lack of empirical evidence precludes definitive conclusions. The aim of our study was to determine whether anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tdcs) over the right DLPFC with cathodal tdcs over the ldlpfc (anodal right/cathodal left) or vice versa (anodal left/cathodal right) differentially modulates risktaking in a task [the Columbia Card Task (CCT)] specifically engaging affect-charged (Hot CCT) vs. deliberative (Cold CCT) decision making. The facilitating effect of the anodal stimulation on neuronal activity was emphasized by the use of a small anode and a big cathode. To investigate the role of individual differences in risk-taking, participants were either smokers or non-smokers. Anodal left/cathodal right stimulation decreased risk-taking in the cold cognition version of the task, in both groups, probably by modulating deliberative processing. In the hot version, anodal right/cathodal left stimulation led to opposite effects in smokers and non-smokers, which might be explained by the engagement of the same inhibitory control mechanism: in smokers, improved controllability of risk-seeking impulsivity led to more conservative decisions, while inhibition of risk-aversion in non-smokers resulted in riskier choices. These results provide evidence for a hemispheric asymmetry and personality-dependent tdcs effects in risky decision making, and may be important for clinical research on addiction and depression. Introduction Making the right decisions in a complex environment that is full of seductive as well as frightening options requires careful weighing of risks and benefits, and a sound regulation of affective impulses. Recent evidence suggests that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is a pivotal area for deliberative processing (Weber et al., 2004; Krawczyk, 2012; Minati et al., 2012b) as well as cognitive inhibitory control (Cohen, 2005; Ochsner & Gross, 2005; Knoch et al., 2008; Goldstein et al., 2009; Dolcos et al., 2011; Mitchell, 2011; Ridderinkhof et al., 2011). Some neuroimaging (Ernst et al., 2002; Schonberg et al., 2012) and brain-stimulation studies (Knoch et al., 2006; Fecteau et al., 2007a) suggest that the right dorsolateral PFC (rdlpfc) may be particularly critical to exert cognitive control on affective impulses and therefore to limit the influence of affect in risky decisionmaking behavior in healthy participants. Observations in substance abuse disorders that are related to impaired risky decision-making behaviors (for review, see Murphy et al., 2012) further support the idea of a lateralization of DLPFC function in risky decision making Correspondence: Dr J. Pripfl, as above. juergen.pripfl@univie.ac.at Received 21 June 2013, revised 27 August 2013, accepted 3 September 2013 (Cservenka & Nagel, 2012). Smokers, for example, who reliably differ from non-smokers in traits affecting cognitive control like impulsivity and sensation seeking (Baker et al., 2004; Ryan et al., 2013), and who behave highly risky in a variety of real-life situations (Hersch & Viscusi, 1998), show lower rdlpfc activity in cognitive control tasks than non-smoking controls (Nestor et al., 2011). However, the interpretation of these findings in terms of cognitive control is problematic because the tasks commonly used to study risky decision-making behavior did not allow for a clear separation between affect-charged and deliberative processes (Figner et al., 2009). Hence, some authors have interpreted rdlpfc activity during risky decision making in terms of deliberative thinking (Rao et al., 2008; Mohr et al., 2010), although evidence for the involvement of the DLPFC in deliberative processing is most prominent for the left (l)dlpfc in healthy (see Vallar & Bolognini, 2011; for review) as well as clinical populations (see Demirtas-Tatlidede et al., 2013, for review). We assessed hemispheric transcranial direct current stimulation (tdcs) effects on risky decision making by means of placing a small-sized anodal electrode over the rdlpfc with a big-sized cathode over the ldlpfc (anodal right/cathodal left) and vice versa (i.e. small anode left/big cathode right). Different sizes of electrodes were used for two reasons: first to improve focality of anodal tdcs; and second to maximize the facilitating neuronal effect of the anode

2 2 J. Pripfl et al. and to minimize the inhibitory effect of the cathode (see Materials and methods: tdcs). The experimental task we used [Columbia Card Task (CCT); Figner et al., 2009] was specifically designed to assess risky decision making under differential involvement of deliberative vs. affective processes. Furthermore, by including a group of smokers and non-smokers, we aimed to assess if individual differences in risk-taking propensity can be attributed to lateral PFC functions. Materials and methods Participants Forty healthy university students participated in the study. Students received course credits for participation, independent of their performance in the CCT. Four participants had to be excluded for incomplete behavioral data. The remaining sample consisted of 18 smokers (age: mean SD, years; 10 females) and 18 non-smokers ( years; 15 females). Smokers had been smoking at least 10 cigarettes per day for at least 1 year, and had a mean score of in the Fagerstr om Test for Nicotine Dependence (Heatherton et al., 1991). Participants were all righthanded, as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and had been screened for the absence of present or past neurological or psychiatric conditions and use of psychoactive medication. All were naive to the goals of the experiment, and gave informed written consent. The study was conducted in conformity to the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the ethics committee of the University of Vienna. Experimental design In a within-subjects design, each participant joined three tdcs sessions with different stimulation conditions [anodal electrode placed over the ldlpfc with the cathode placed over the rdlpfc (anodal left/cathodal right), anodal electrode placed over the rdlpfc with the cathode placed over the ldlpfc (anodal right/cathodal left), and sham stimulation], with at least 1 week between sessions. The sequence of stimulation conditions was counterbalanced across participants. For sham stimulation, electrode positions of both verum stimulations (anodal left/cathodal right, anodal right/cathodal left) were balanced across participants. The sequence of the tasks, i.e. the Hot and Cold version of the CCT (see below), was counterbalanced across participants, but task order was kept constant within participants for the three sessions to avoid confounding task order effects with stimulation conditions. After electrode application, participants were seated in a comfortable chair in a sound-attenuated, dimmed room equipped with an intercom to the experimenter. Before starting the stimulation, participants were given a training session to familiarize them with the CCT, which was presented on a 19-Zoll-CRT-Monitor placed at a 60-cm viewing distance. Participants were instructed to sit quietly with their eyes open for the first 5 min of stimulation, after which task presentation started. Stimulation of the verum sessions was terminated after 15 min. Processing of the tasks lasted a few (3 5) minutes longer, depending on the individual reaction times. Note that verum post-stimulation effects have been documented to last at least for 1 h (Nitsche & Paulus, 2001). After electrode removal, participants had to answer a questionnaire about the adverse effects of brain stimulation (based on Brunoni et al., 2011). CCT In the CCT, participants are instructed to maximize their game scores during each game round by choosing how many cards they would like to draw out of a total of 32 hidden gain or loss cards. An optimal strategy to maximize the total score needs to take into account three factors, which are orthogonally varied trial by trial: (a) probability of a loss (pl = 1 or 3 loss cards); (b) gain amount [GA = 10 (low) or 30 (high) points per gain card]; and (c) loss amount (LA = 250 or 750 points). In the 24 rounds of the Hot task version of the game, participants make stepwise incremental decisions by turning only one card at a time with immediate feedback provided after each choice. At any point in each round, participants can choose to stop their card selection (thus obtaining the round s payoff), or continue to choose cards until a losing card is selected (which leads to a subtraction of the LA in that round, and ends the round). This dynamic nature in which risk increases over time within a trial is similar to other risk-taking tasks like the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (Lejuez et al., 2002), which has also been used in previous tdcs studies (e.g. Fecteau et al., 2007b; Boggio et al., 2010a). The position of loss cards was randomized for each round and not fixed (as in Figner et al., 2009), in order to avoid the impression of rigged feedback (for details, see Figner et al., 2009). In the Cold task version of the game, decisions are not made stepwise and there is no immediate feedback. Instead, participants are asked to indicate only the number of cards (from 0 to 32) they would like to turn over on a given trial. Players receive feedback on their performance only at the very end of the whole CCT session, by means of a summary score after the last of the 24 rounds. According to Figner et al. (2009), the number of cards chosen, independent of the game factors, represents an index of risk-taking, while the number of cards chosen as a function of the game factors represents an index of information use. It is crucial to note that the two versions of the CCT elicit different decision-making processes. The Hot CCT elicits higher emotional arousal, as indexed by electrodermal activity, is positively correlated with the motivationalaffective construct need-for-arousal, and has been shown to elicit strategies predominantly based on affect-charged decision-making behavior. The Cold CCT, in contrast, elicits strategies predominantly based on mathematical calculations and reasoning, and has been shown to be independent from impulsive personality characteristics (Figner et al., 2009). Hence, risk-taking in the Hot CCT is more strongly influenced by affective processes, while deliberative decision-making processes prevail in the Cold CCT. tdcs Anodal tdcs was delivered over three sintered Ag/AgCl electrodes (sized 5.3 cm² in total), which were connected by an electrode splitter cable (1 to 3) to the anodal output of a battery-driven, constant-current stimulator (DC-STIMULATOR PLUS, neuroconn GmbH, Erlangen, Germany). Small electrodes were used for anodal stimulation to improve the focality of tdcs (Marshall et al., 2004; Nitsche et al., 2007; Datta et al., 2009; Faria et al., 2009; Miranda et al., 2009; Minhas et al., 2010). Positions F1, F3 and AF1 according to the international electrode system (Jasper, 1958) were used for left hemisphere anodal stimulation ( anodal left/cathodal right hitherto). For right hemisphere anodal stimulation ( anodal right/cathodal left ), electrodes were positioned at F2, F4 and AF2. Electrodes were positioned and kept in place by using an elastic and individually fitted electroencephalogram (EEG)-cap (EASYCAP Ges.m.b.H., Herrsching, Germany). The 7 9 5cm² cathodal reference

3 DLPFC and risk-taking 3 electrode was centered contralateral at positions F3 and F4, respectively, with the short side running in parallel to the imaginary Cz-Fz connecting line. Stimulation electrodes were filled with degassed electrode gel (Electro-Cap International, Eaton, USA), and the skin under the electrodes had been cleaned with alcohol and slightly scratched to decrease skin resistance (Bauer et al., 1989). This method assured electrode impedance values of 3 ko, as individually measured by an impedance meter (Ing. Zickler Ges.m.b.H., Pfaffst atten, Austria). At the side of the surface sponge electrode the skin was only cleaned with alcohol, but not scratched, as this might have created unwanted focal cathodal stimulation effects. A direct current of 0.45 ma intensity, resulting in an anodal current density of ma/cm², was induced for 15 min in the verum sessions, and for 30 s with 30 s fade-in and 30 s fade-out in the sham session. Modeling studies showed that 0.45 ma delivered over an electrode size of about 5 cm² induces a comparable current intensity at the surface of the cortex to 1 ma over a 35 cm² electrode (Miranda et al., 2009). At the cathode a direct current density of ma/cm² was induced. Because a minimum current density of ma/cm² was previously shown to be necessary to modify motor cortex excitability by tdcs (Nitsche & Paulus, 2000), current density at the reference electrode might be considered as functionally irrelevant. Assessment of personality characteristics Participants had to answer questionnaires assessing cognitive control-related personality characteristics [Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS11), Patton et al., 1995; Substance Use Risk Profile Scale (SURPS), Woicik et al., 2009; BIS/BAS scale, Carver & White, 1994] and one additional question concerning risk-aversion: It frightens me when I have to make decisions which involve risk (four-point scale: strongly disagree; disagree; agree; strongly agree). Data analysis The main outcome measures were the number of cards chosen. To account for differences in the procedures and the interpretation of the game parameters, the Hot and Cold versions of the CCT were analysed using separate statistical analyses. Linear mixed models with restricted maximum likelihood estimation were used to control for heteroscedasticity and to include time-dependent covariates, which was necessary because of the within-subject design (Gueorguieva & Krystal, 2004; McCulloch & Searle, 2005; Heck et al., 2011). For the two versions of the CCT we evaluated the withinsubjects fixed factors stimulation condition (sham, anodal left, anodal right), GA (low, high), LA (low, high) and pl (low, high); the between-subjects fixed factor smoking group [smokers (S), non-smokers (NS)] and the random factor subjects in a full-factorial model. For the Hot version the number of cards chosen in the Cold CCT and the number of negative feedbacks were introduced as time-dependent covariates, to control for the influence of deliberative processing and negative feedback, respectively. Schwarz s Bayesian criteria (Schwarz, 1978) were used to determine the bestfitting variance covariance structure, which was determined to be Diagonal. Bonferroni corrected post hoc tests were used to examine interactions and omnibus main effects. Significance was evaluated at P < All post hoc values are reported following correction for multiple comparisons. All data are reported as means SE. Group differences in personality characteristics were assessed by means of independent t-tests. Each dichotomized scale (absent, present) of the adverse effects questionnaire was subjected to a Chi-square test to compare smoking groups and a Cochran-Q test comparing the three stimulation conditions. Statistical analyses were performed using IBM â SPSS â Statistics 20 (IBM, Chicago, IL, USA). Results Adverse effects The frequencies of reported adverse effects did neither differ between the three stimulation conditions nor between smokers and non-smokers, except that more non-smokers reported itching sensations during anodal left stimulation (61% vs. 22%). The three most frequently reported adverse effects were sleepiness (56%), tingling (45%) and itching (32%). Questionnaire data As for the questionnaire data, smokers scored higher in the SURPS subscales Impulsivity (S = ; NS = ; t 34 = 2.525, P = 0.016) and Sensation Seeking (S = ; NS = ; t 34 = 3.947, P < 0.001), and in the BIS11 total score (S = ; NS = ; t 34 = 3.592, P = 0.001). Non-smokers scored higher in the SURPS Anxiety Sensitivity subscale (S = ; NS = ; t 34 = 2.047, P = 0.048), the inhibition scale (BIS) of the BIS/BAS scale (S = ; NS = ; t 34 = 2.319, P = 0.027) and the risk-aversion scale (S = ; NS = ; t 34 = 3.370, P = 0.002). Cold CCT Analysis of the Cold CCT revealed a significant main effect for stimulation condition (F 2,351 = 4.330, P = 0.014). Bonferroni post hoc tests showed that anodal left/cathodal right (P = 0.014) but not anodal right/cathodal left (P = 1.000) stimulation resulted in decreased risk-taking compared with sham stimulation (Fig. 1). Fig. 1. Risk-taking (as indicated by the number of cards chosen in the risky gamble) in the Cold CCT, averaged across smokers and non-smokers. Anodal left/cathodal right stimulation over the DLPFC led to a decrease in risk-taking, in comparison to sham stimulation; whereas anodal right/cathodal left stimulation had no effect. Left: anodal left/cathodal right; right: anodal right/cathodal left.

4 4 J. Pripfl et al. Further significant main effects revealed that the number of chosen cards was higher for trials with high GA (F 1,556 = , P < 0.001), low LA (F 1,556 = , P < 0.001) and lower pl (F 1,556 = , P < 0.001). In addition, there were significant interactions for GA 9 pl (F 1,556 = 9.976, P = 0.002) and LA 9 pl (F 1,556 = , P < 0.001). However, none of the interactions with stimulation condition was significant (all P-values > 0.1). This pattern of findings for the gamble factors reveals that information about risk conditions was appropriately used by participants, but that information use was not modulated by tdcs stimulation. Further significant interactions were found for smoking group 9 GA (F 1,556 = 5.090, P = 0.024) and smoking group 9 pl (F 1,556 = 9.333, P = 0.002). Bonferroni post hoc tests showed that smokers took higher risks than non-smokers under conditions of high GA (P = 0.037), as well as under conditions of low pl (P = 0.027). All other main effects and interactions were non-significant (all P-values > 0.08), including the interaction group 9 stimulation (P = 0.318). Hot CCT The analysis of the Hot condition revealed neither a significant main effect for smoking group nor for stimulation condition (all P-values > 0.195), but a significant interaction of stimulation condition 9 smoking group (F 2,361 = 6.324, P = 0.002). Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed that smokers showed a decrease in risk-taking during anodal right/cathodal left compared with sham stimulation (P = 0.046), whereas non-smokers showed the opposite pattern, i.e. an increase in risk-taking under anodal right/cathodal left compared with sham stimulation (P = 0.031). Furthermore, compared with non-smokers, smokers took significantly higher risk during the sham condition (P = 0.010), but not during anodal left/cathodal right or anodal right/cathodal left stimulation (all P-values > 0.1; Fig. 2). Note that when conducting an analysis without covariates the results were essentially identical (stimulation condition 9 smoking group: F 2, 352 = 7.245, P = 0.001). Fig. 2. Risk-taking in the Hot Columbia Card Task (CCT). Smokers and non-smokers showed the opposite pattern: compared with sham stimulation, anodal right/cathodal left tdcs resulted in lower risk-taking in smokers but higher risk-taking in non-smokers. In addition, the two groups differed in their baseline risk-taking behavior during sham stimulation, where smokers showed higher risk-taking than non-smokers. Left: anodal left/cathodal right; right: anodal right/cathodal left. In the Hot CCT, significances of main effects and interactions of the gambling factors, i.e. GA (F 1,556 = , P < 0.001), LA (F 1,556 = , P < 0.001), pl (F 1,683 = , P < 0.001) and GA 9 pl (F 1,515 = , P < 0.001), were similar to the Cold version except for LA 9 pl, which was very close to the chosen significance threshold though (F 1,519 = 3.162, P = 0.076). Furthermore, significant interactions were found for smoking group 9 LA (F 1,510 = 5.336, P = 0.021), smoking group 9 pl (F 1,515 = 5.811, P = 0.016) and smoking group 9 GA 9 LA (F 1,510 = 4.005, P = 0.046). Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed that in the Hot CCT smokers showed particularly higher risk-taking than non-smokers in conditions that combine high GA with low LA (P = 0.015), and on a trend level in conditions with low pl (P = 0.058). In analogy to the Cold CCT these patterns were not affected by the type of stimulation (P-values > 0.1). Discussion We used two different versions of a risky decision-making game, and investigated whether anodal left/cathodal right or anodal right/ cathodal left tdcs over the DLPFC influences the risky choices made in this game in two different groups (S vs. NS). To improve focality and emphasize the facilitating effect of anodal stimulation, a small-sized anode and a big-sized cathode were used. Our results demonstrate that tdcs over the DLPFC affects risky decision making, and that these effects are modulated by the stimulated hemisphere, the type of task and the group. Briefly, anodal left/cathodal right stimulation decreased risk-taking in the cold cognition version of the task, in both groups, while in the hot version, anodal right/cathodal left stimulation decreased risk-taking in smokers and increased it in non-smokers. In terms of their general behavior, participants acted in line with theoretical predictions and previous observations using similar task setups (Figner et al., 2009). Most importantly, information use was as expected in both the Cold and the Hot CCT, because higher GA, lower LA and a lower pl all resulted in a higher number of cards chosen (i.e. risks taken). However, the difference in cards chosen between the Hot and Cold CCT was reversed and smaller in our study than reported in Figner et al. (2009). This can be explained by the fact that Figner et al. (2009) used a fixed feedback strategy, i.e. the game was programmed in a way that loss cards would only be presented at the very end of a game round (i.e. the last card, or if more than one loss card was included the last possible card). To maintain the impression of playing a game of chance, Figner et al. interspersed a few loss trials in which a fixed loss was incurred, independently of the participant s choice. This procedure implies the possibility that participants detect the rigged nature of the feedback. Therefore, the position of loss cards was fully randomized in our version of the Hot CCT, and this may have resulted in fewer cards chosen than in the Cold CCT. The findings of Huang et al. (2013) further support this interpretation. They used the Warm CCT, which provides feedback at the trial by trial level, rather than the card by card level, to avoid participants detecting the rigged nature of feedback. This manipulation on the feedback strategy of the Hot, respectively, Warm CCT resulted, in agreement with our observations, in more cards turned over on average in the Cold CCT than in the Warm CCT, but with the Warm CCT still arousing stronger emotions (Huang et al., 2013). When comparing information use of smokers and non-smokers, similar patterns could be observed for the two versions of the CCT. For instance, smokers took higher risks than non-smokers when pl was low (Hot and Cold CCT), when there was much to win (Cold CCT), and in trials that

5 DLPFC and risk-taking 5 combined high wins with low LA (Hot CCT). However, these decision patterns were not affected by the type of stimulation. The results of the Cold CCT, in which more cautious decisions were obtained after anodal left/cathodal right but not anodal right/ cathodal left stimulation in both groups, are in agreement with our hypothesis. The Cold CCT has been shown to elicit strategies predominantly based on mathematical calculations, and does not correlate with the need-for-arousal construct (Figner et al., 2009). Thus, decision making in the Cold CCT is said to be independent from personality characteristics, such as impulsivity or sensation seeking (Figner et al., 2009). This assumption fits well with the lack of baseline differences in risk-taking between smokers and non-smokers in the Cold CCT. We attribute the decrease in risk-taking after anodal left/cathodal right stimulation over the DLPFC in both groups to a modification of deliberative reasoning and stimulus processing. This interpretation is supported by results showing that particularly stimulation with the anode placed over the ldlpfc results in the modification of executive processes that play a major role in deliberative processing, such as working memory (Keeser et al., 2011b), probabilistic reasoning (Hecht et al., 2010) and planning (Dockery et al., 2009). A recent study by Xue et al. (2012) further supports this interpretation. Based on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) data showing the left DLPFC being strongly activated in strategic decision making, subsequently applying anodal tdcs over this region resulted in modification of the decision-making strategy. Our hypothesis concerning the effect of tdcs on affect-charged risky decisions (as assessed by the Hot CCT) could partly be confirmed. As expected, we did not find significant modulation of risktaking by anodal left/cathodal right stimulation over the DLPFC in either group. More importantly, anodal right/cathodal left stimulation decreased risk-taking, but only seen in smokers, whereas non-smokers showed increased risk-taking. When interpreting this opposite effect on risk-taking for smokers and non-smokers, differences in baseline risk behavior need to be taken into account. Comparing the baseline level of risk-taking, i.e. risk-taking during sham stimulation, smokers showed significantly higher risk-taking than non-smokers in the Hot CCT. This is in agreement with previous findings (e.g. Hersch & Viscusi, 1998; Lejuez et al., 2003; Hakes & Viscusi, 2007; Anderson & Mellor, 2008). Furthermore, and as predicted, these differences in risk-taking were accompanied by individual differences in personality characteristics (e.g. Fields et al., 2009; Ryan et al., 2013): while smokers scored higher in impulsivity (SURPS, BIS11) and sensation seeking (SURPS), non-smokers showed higher behavioral inhibition (BIS scale), anxiety sensitivity (SURPS) and risk-aversion. Based on this pattern of findings, the opposing tdcs effects in the two groups might be explained by the engagement of the same inhibitory control mechanism associated with rdlpfc (Knoch et al., 2006). In smokers, with their higher impulsivity and sensation seeking, increased cognitive control of affective impulses leads to a reduction of risky decisions. In the risk-averse and more loss-sensitive non-smokers, on the other hand, cognitive control might act upon and inhibit the influence of bottom-up aversive impulses signaling to be careful in risky choices hence resulting in less conservative and therefore more risky decisions. Two lines of evidence support this interpretation: first, it has been shown that low sensation seekers, as the non-smokers in our study, display enhanced anxious anticipation of negative consequences. For example, indicators of anticipatory aversive affect, such as fear-potentiated startle responses to unpredictable stimuli, are increased in low sensation seekers (Lissek et al., 2005). This suggests a stronger bottom-up engagement of aversive physiological and affective responses in unpredictable situations, such as risky decisions. Second, it has been shown that stimulation by means of anodal right/cathodal left tdcs over the DLPFC can boost response confidence during risky decisions in healthy risk-averse participants (Minati et al., 2012a), and this has been attributed to strengthened cognitive control (Camchong et al., 2007). Concerning the often reported risk-seeking propensity of smokers, our results support the longstanding idea in psychology that resisting temptations reflects competition between impulses and self-control (Hofmann et al., 2009). Strengthening self-control by means of increasing rdlpfc activation helps smokers to stifle risk-seeking impulsivity. However, our data do not allow concluding that increased risk-taking of smokers is due to maladaptive cognitive control (Feil et al., 2010; Goldstein & Volkow, 2011). Our results rather show that different personality characteristics, i.e. smokers being risk-seekers and non-smokers scoring not only lower in sensation seeking and impulsivity but also higher in risk-aversion and anxiety sensitivity, are responsible for the observed differences in risk-taking between groups. It might be argued that general differences in the cognitive processes and task demands engaged by the Hot and Cold CCT, termed learning demands by Figner et al. (2009), might be an alternative explanation for the reported results. For instance, the Hot CCT requires participants to keep track of the increasing probability of losing with each card they have turned over. This might involve higher demands on working memory, which has also been associated with DLPFC function. However, what speaks against this alternative explanation is the fact that smokers and non-smokers showed effects in opposite directions in the Hot CCT contradicting a general effect attributable to modulation of working memory function. Furthermore, neither Figner et al. (2009) nor Huang et al. (2013) found any evidence for differential learning demand effects for the Cold and Hot CCT versions in their studies. As the Cold CCT was designed to trigger mainly deliberative decision making (Figner et al., 2009), but not exclusively, it could also be argued that the Cold CCT is not completely free of affective processes. This could make it difficult to separate deliberative from affective processing in the two versions of the CCT. However, those versions of the CCT that have been designed to engage affective processes (i.e. the Hot CCT and the Warm CCT) indeed seem to arouse stronger emotions during decision making than the Cold CCT, as shown in previous behavioral studies (Figner et al., 2009; Huang et al., 2013). Furthermore, the study by Figner et al. (2009) demonstrated that the Cold CCT elicits strategies predominantly based on mathematical calculations and reasoning, while strategies to solve the Hot CCT are predominantly based on affect-charged decision-making behavior, as shown by self-reported decision strategies. While Huang et al. (2013) could not replicate these differences in strategies when comparing the Warm CCT with the Cold CCT, this might have been due to limitations of the self-report measures used, according to the authors. Thus, although minor affective processing might have been induced during playing the Cold CCT, results of the Cold CCT of our study are more plausibly explained in terms of involvement and modification of deliberative processes. It should further be noted that cognitive inhibitory control or cognitive control is necessary for a variety of impulsive behaviors (for an overview, see Ohmura et al., 2012), like impulsive action (e.g. Go/No Go task or stop-signal-reaction-time task; e.g. Logan et al., 1997; Steinbeis et al., 2012); impulsive choice (e.g. delay discounting; e.g. Cho et al., 2012); risky behavior (for review, see Boyer, 2006); or substance craving (e.g. George & Koob, 2011; Hayashi et al., 2013). Our study refers to cognitive control in risky

6 6 J. Pripfl et al. behavior, i.e. limiting the influence of affect in risky decision making. Cognitive control in other impulsive behaviors might be based on partly overlapping (for review, see Cohen & Lieberman, 2010), but probably not identical, brain mechanisms. Comparing, for example, the effects of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rtms) over the ldlpfc (with rdlpfc not studied) on delay discounting and risky decision making [Risky Choices Gambling Task (RCGT)] in the same sample (smokers and non-smokers), significant modification of delay discounting compared with sham stimulation was observed, but there was neither a difference between the active and sham rtms condition for risky decision making nor for cigarette consumption (Sheffer et al., 2013). Using the same risk task, i.e. the RCGT, Knoch et al. (2006) found that inhibiting activity by rtms over the right, but not over the left, DLPFC modulates risky decision making, which is in agreement with our observations in particular because the RCGT measures mainly affect-charged risky decision making with little strategic planning. Together, these findings speak against a domain general cognitive inhibitory control mechanism within the DLPFC and for a prominent role of the rdlpfc for cognitive control of affective processes in risky decision making. It has also been proposed that non-invasive brain-stimulation treatment effects of addiction are linked to shifts in decision making related to the role of the DLPFC in impulsivity control (e.g. Boggio et al., 2010b; for review, see Fecteau et al., 2010). However, as our study did not assess craving and the effects of the specific electrode setting used in our study have never been explored for the potential to modify craving, testing this hypothesis is not within the scope of this study. In general, the spatial resolution of tdcs is known to be limited. To improve spatial specificity and emphasize the facilitating effect of the anodal stimulation, we used small EEG electrodes focused over the DLPFC, and a large sponge electrode was placed contralateral with the intention to make the cathode functionally ineffective (Marshall et al., 2004; Nitsche et al., 2007; Datta et al., 2008; Minhas et al., 2010). Nevertheless, the exact distribution of the electric field generated by tdcs is not known (Datta et al., 2009; Dmochowski et al., 2011; Faria et al., 2011), and cathodal functional ineffectiveness of current densities below ma/cm² as used in this study (0.013 ma/cm²) has only been shown for the motor cortex (Nitsche & Paulus, 2000) but not for other regions. Further studies, in particular ones providing information on the precise neural effects, such as combinations of tdcs with fmri, are therefore necessary to clarify the influence of such electrode settings on DLPFC activity and whether other areas were also stimulated and therefore might contribute to the observed effects (Keeser et al., 2011a). The specific stimulation protocol used, particularly the combination of small-sized EEG electrodes with a big-sized sponge electrode, and the consideration of personality characteristics in our study might also explain some disparity to results of other studies dealing with risky decision making and non-invasive brain stimulation of the DLPFC (Fecteau et al., 2007a,b). We show that anodal right/cathodal left tdcs over the DLPFC predominantly modulates regulation of affective impulses, and the opposite electrode setting, i.e. anodal left/cathodal right stimulation, mainly influences deliberative processing of risky decision-making behavior. Upregulating the activity of the rdlpfc seems to increase cognitive control of affective impulses. However, the direction of effects on risk-taking when strengthening cognitive control depends on the valence of affective impulses: improved controllability of risk-seeking temptations leads to more conservative decisions vs. inhibition of risk-averse apprehensiveness results in riskier choices. These findings are relevant for mechanistic models of decision making, and suggest that individual differences, the type of decisions and hemispheric variability need to be taken into account in such models. Our results also provide further insights into the development of interventions using non-invasive brain stimulation to treat psychiatric disorders associated with decision-making problems, such as addiction or depression. Acknowledgements This study was financially supported by the MMI-CNS research cluster (University of Vienna and Medical University of Vienna), by funds of the Oesterreichische Nationalbank (Anniversary Fund, project number: 14350) and the Austrian Science Fund (P22813). The authors are grateful to Bernd Figner for providing the software of the Columbia Card Task (CCT). The authors declare no competing financial interests. Abbreviations BIS, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; CCT, Columbia Card Task; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; EEG, electroencephalogram; fmri, functional magnetic resonance imaging; GA, gain amount; LA, loss amount; ldlpfc, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; NS, non-smoker; PFC, prefrontal cortex; RCGT, Risky Choices Gambling Task; rdlpfc, right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; rtms, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; S, smoker; SURPS, Substance Use Risk Profile Scale; tdcs, transcranial direct current stimulation. References Anderson, L.R. & Mellor, J.M. (2008) Predicting health behaviors with an experimental measure of risk preference. J. Health Econ., 27, Baker, T.B., Brandon, T.H. & Chassin, L. (2004) Motivational influences on cigarette smoking. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 55, Bauer, H., Korunka, C. & Leodolter, M. (1989) Technical requirements for high-quality scalp DC recordings. Electroen. Clin. Neuro., 72, Boggio, P.S., Campanh~a, C., Valasek, C.A., Fecteau, S., Pascual-Leone, A. & Fregni, F. (2010a) Modulation of decision-making in a gambling task in older adults with transcranial direct current stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci., 31, Boggio, P.S., Zaghi, S., Villani, A.B., Fecteau, S., Pascual-Leone, A. & Fregni, F. (2010b) Modulation of risk-taking in marijuana users by transcranial direct current stimulation (tdcs) of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). Drug Alcohol Depen., 112, Boyer, T.W. (2006) The development of risk-taking: a multi-perspective review. Dev. Rev., 26, Brunoni, A.R., Amadera, J., Berbel, B., Volz, M.S., Rizzerio, B.G. & Fregni, F. (2011) A systematic review on reporting and assessment of adverse effects associated with transcranial direct current stimulation. Int. J. Neuropsychoph., 14, Camchong, J., Goodie, A.S., McDowell, J.E., Gilmore, C.S. & Clementz, B.A. (2007) A cognitive neuroscience approach to studying the role of overconfidence in problem gambling. J. Gambl. Stud., 23, Carver, C.S. & White, T.L. (1994) Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS Scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 67, Cho, S.S., Pellecchia, G., Ko, J.H., Ray, N., Obeso, I., Houle, S. & Strafella, A.P. (2012) Effect of continuous theta burst stimulation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on cerebral blood flow changes during decision making. Brain Stimul., 5, Cohen, J.D. (2005) The vulcanization of the human brain: a neural perspective on interactions between cognition and emotion. J. Econ. Perspect., 19, Cohen, J.R. & Lieberman, M.D. (2010) The common neural basis of exerting self-control in multiple domains. In Hassin, R., Ochsner, K. & Trope, Y. (Eds), Self Control in Cociety, Mind, and Brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp Cservenka, A. & Nagel, B.J. (2012) Risky decision-making: an fmri study of youth at high risk for alcoholism. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res., 36, Datta, A., Elwassif, M., Battaglia, F. & Bikson, M. (2008) Transcranial current stimulation focality using disc and ring electrode configurations: FEM analysis. J. Neural Eng., 5,

7 DLPFC and risk-taking 7 Datta, A., Bansal, V., Diaz, J., Patel, J., Reato, D. & Bikson, M. (2009) Gyri-precise head model of transcranial direct current stimulation: improved spatial focality using a ring electrode versus conventional rectangular pad. Brain Stimul., 2, Demirtas-Tatlidede, A., Vahabzadeh-Hagh, A.M. & Pascual-Leone, A. (2013) Can noninvasive brain stimulation enhance cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders? Neuropharmacology, 64, Dmochowski, J.P., Datta, A., Bikson, M., Su, Y. & Parra, L.C. (2011) Optimized multi-electrode stimulation increases focality and intensity at target. J. Neural Eng., 8, Dockery, C.A., Hueckel-Weng, R., Birbaumer, N. & Plewnia, C. (2009) Enhancement of planning ability by transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Neurosci., 29, Dolcos, F., Iordan, A.D. & Dolcos, S. (2011) Neural correlates of emotion - Cognition interactions: a review of evidence from brain imaging investigations. J. Cogn. Psychol., 23, Ernst, M., Bolla, K., Mouratidis, M., Contoreggi, C., Matochik, J.A., Kurian, V., Cadet, J.L., Kimes, A.S. & London, E.D. (2002) Decision-making in a risk-taking task: a PET study. Neuropsychopharmacol., 26, Faria, P., Leal, A. & Miranda, P.C. (2009) Comparing different electrode configurations using the international system in tdcs: a finite element model analysis. Proceedings of the 31st Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society: Engineering the Future of Biomedicine, EMBC 2009, 31st Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society: Engineering the Future of Biomedicine, EMBC Minneapolis, MN, pp Faria, P., Hallett, M. & Miranda, P.C. (2011) A finite element analysis of the effect of electrode area and inter-electrode distance on the spatial distribution of the current density in tdcs. J. Neural Eng., 8, Fecteau, S., Knoch, D., Fregni, F., Sultani, N., Boggio, P. & Pascual-Leone, A. (2007a) Diminishing risk-taking behavior by modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex: a direct current stimulation study. J. Neurosci., 27, Fecteau, S., Pascual-Leone, A., Zald, D.H., Liguori, P., Theoret, H., Boggio, P.S. & Fregni, F. (2007b) Activation of prefrontal cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation reduces appetite for risk during ambiguous decision making. J. Neurosci., 27, Fecteau, S., Fregni, F., Boggio, P.S., Camprodon, J.A. & Pascual-Leone, A. (2010) Neuromodulation of decision-making in the addictive brain. Subst. Use Misuse, 45, Feil, J., Sheppard, D., Fitzgerald, P.B., Y ucel, M., Lubman, D.I. & Bradshaw, J.L. (2010) Addiction, compulsive drug seeking, and the role of frontostriatal mechanisms in regulating inhibitory control. Neurosci. Biobehav. R., 35, Fields, S., Collins, C., Leraas, K. & Reynolds, B. (2009) Dimensions of impulsive behavior in adolescent smokers and nonsmokers. Exp. Clin. Psychopharm., 17, Figner, B., Weber, E.U., Mackinlay, R.J. & Wilkening, F. (2009) Affective and deliberative processes in risky choice: age differences in Risk taking in the Columbia Card Task. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 35, George, O. & Koob, G.F. (2011) Craving, context and the cortex. Nat. Neurosci., 14, Goldstein, R.Z. & Volkow, N.D. (2011) Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 12, Goldstein, R.Z., Craig, A.D., Bechara, A., Garavan, H., Childress, A.R., Paulus, M.P. & Volkow, N.D. (2009) The Neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction. Trends Cogn. Sci., 13, Gueorguieva, R. & Krystal, J.H. (2004) Move over ANOVA: progress in analyzing repeated-measures data and its reflection in papers published in the Archives of General Psychiatry. Arch. Gen. Psychiat., 61, Hakes, J.K. & Viscusi, W.K. (2007) Automobile seatbelt usage and the value of statistical life. South. Econ. J., 73, Hayashi, T., Ko, J.H., Strafella, A.P. & Dagher, A. (2013) Dorsolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex interactions during self-control of cigarette craving. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 110, Heatherton, T.F., Kozlowski, L.T., Frecker, R.C. & Fagerstrom, K.O. (1991) The Fagerstr om test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Brit. J. Addict., 86, Hecht, D., Walsh, V. & Lavidor, M. (2010) Transcranial direct current stimulation facilitates decision making in a probabilistic guessing task. J. Neurosci., 30, Heck, R.H., Thomas, S.L. & Tabata, L.N. (2011) Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling with IBM SPSS. Routledge. Taylor & Francis Group, New York. Hersch, J. & Viscusi, W.K. (1998) Smoking and other risky behaviors. J. Drug Issues, 28, Hofmann, W., Friese, M. & Strack, F. (2009) Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci., 4, Huang, Y., Wood, S., Berger, D. & Hanoch, Y. (2013) Risky choice in younger versus older adults: affective context matters. Judgm. Decis. Mak., 8, Jasper, H.H. (1958) The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroen. Clin. Neuro., 10, Keeser, D., Meindl, T., Bor, J., Palm, U., Pogarell, O., Mulert, C., Brunelin, J., M oller, H.J., Reiser, M. & Padberg, F. (2011a) Prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation changes connectivity of resting-state networks during fmri. J. Neurosci., 31, Keeser, D., Padberg, F., Reisinger, E., Pogarell, O., Kirsch, V., Palm, U., Karch, S., M oller, H.J., Nitsche, M.A. & Mulert, C. (2011b) Prefrontal direct current stimulation modulates resting EEG and event-related potentials in healthy subjects: a standardized low resolution tomography (slo- RETA) study. NeuroImage, 55, Knoch, D., Gianotti, L.R.R., Pascual-Leone, A., Treyer, V., Regard, M., Hohmann, M. & Brugger, P. (2006) Disruption of right prefrontal cortex by low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation induces risktaking behavior. J. Neurosci., 26, Knoch, D., Nitsche, M.A., Fischbacher, U., Eisenegger, C., Pascual-Leone, A. & Fehr, E. (2008) Studying the neurobiology of social interaction with transcranial direct current stimulation - The example of punishing unfairness. Cereb. Cortex, 18, Krawczyk, D.C. (2012) The cognition and neuroscience of relational reasoning. Brain Res., 1428, Lejuez, C.W., Richards, J.B., Read, J.P., Kahler, C.W., Ramsey, S.E., Stuart, G.L., Strong, D.R. & Brown, R.A. (2002) Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the balloon analogue risk task (BART). J. Exp. Psychol. Appl., 8, Lejuez, C.W., Aklin, W.M., Jones, H.A., Strong, D.R., Richards, J.B., Kahler, C.W. & Read, J.P. (2003) The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. Exp. Clin. Psychopharm., 11, Lissek, S., Baas, J.M.P., Pine, D.S., Orme, K., Dvir, S., Rosenberger, E. & Grillon, C. (2005) Sensation seeking and the aversive motivational system. Emotion, 5, Logan, G.D., Schachar, R.J. & Tannock, R. (1997) Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychol. Sci., 8, Marshall, L., M olle, M., Hallschmid, M. & Born, J. (2004) Transcranial direct current stimulation during sleep improves declarative memory. J. Neurosci., 24, McCulloch, C.E. & Searle, S.R. (2005) Linear Mixed Models (LMMs) Generalized, Linear, and Mixed Models. John Wiley & Sons Inc., Hoboken, NJ, USA, pp Minati, L., Campanh~a, C., Critchley, H.D. & Boggio, P.S. (2012a) Effects of transcranial direct-current stimulation (tdcs) of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) during a mixed-gambling risky decision-making task. Cogn. Neurosci., 3, Minati, L., Grisoli, M., Seth, A.K. & Critchley, H.D. (2012b) Decisionmaking under risk: a graph-based network analysis using functional MRI. NeuroImage, 60, Minhas, P., Bansal, V., Patel, J., Ho, J.S., Diaz, J., Datta, A. & Bikson, M. (2010) Electrodes for high-definition transcutaneous DC stimulation for applications in drug delivery and electrotherapy, including tdcs. J. Neurosci. Meth., 190, Miranda, P.C., Faria, P. & Hallett, M. (2009) What does the ratio of injected current to electrode area tell us about current density in the brain during tdcs? Clin. Neurophysiol., 120, Mitchell, D.G.V. (2011) The nexus between decision making and emotion regulation: a review of convergent neurocognitive substrates. Behav. Brain Res., 217, Mohr, P.N.C., Biele, G. & Heekeren, H.R. (2010) Neural processing of risk. J. Neurosci., 30, Murphy, A., Taylor, E. & Elliott, R. (2012) The detrimental effects of emotional process dysregulation on decision making in substance dependence. Front. Integr. Neurosci., 6, 101. Nestor, L., McCabe, E., Jones, J., Clancy, L. & Garavan, H. (2011) Differences in bottom-up and top-down neural activity in current and former cigarette smokers: evidence for neural substrates which may promote

8 8 J. Pripfl et al. nicotine abstinence through increased cognitive control. NeuroImage, 56, Nitsche, M.A. & Paulus, W. (2000) Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol., 527, Nitsche, M.A. & Paulus, W. (2001) Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology, 57, Nitsche, M.A., Doemkes, S., Karak ose, T., Antal, A., Liebetanz, D., Lang, N., Tergau, F. & Paulus, W. (2007) Shaping the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol., 97, Ochsner, K.N. & Gross, J.J. (2005) The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci., 9, Ohmura, Y., Tsutsui-Kimura, I. & Yoshioka, M. (2012) Impulsive behavior and nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. J. Pharm. Sci., 118, Oldfield, R.C. (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, Patton, J.H., Stanford, M.S. & Barratt, E.S. (1995) Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J. Clin. Psychol., 51, Rao, H., Korczykowski, M., Pluta, J., Hoang, A. & Detre, J.A. (2008) Neural correlates of voluntary and involuntary risk taking in the human brain: an fmri Study of the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART). NeuroImage, 42, Ridderinkhof, R.K., Forstmann, B.U., Wylie, S.A., Burle, B. & van den Wildenberg, W.P.M. (2011) Neurocognitive mechanisms of action control: resisting the call of the Sirens. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci., 2, Ryan, K.K., MacKillop, J. & Carpenter, M.J. (2013) The relationship between impulsivity, risk-taking propensity and nicotine dependence among older adolescent smokers. Addict. Behav., 38, Schonberg, T., Fox, C.R., Mumford, J.A., Congdon, E., Trepel, C. & Poldrack, R.A. (2012) Decreasing ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity during sequential risk-taking: an FMRI investigation of the balloon analog risk task. Front. Neurosci., 6, 80. Schwarz, G. (1978) Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat., 6, Sheffer, C.E., Mennemeier, M., Landes, R.D., Bickel, W.K., Brackman, S., Dornhoffer, J., Kimbrell, T. & Brown, G. (2013) Neuromodulation of delay discounting, the reflection effect, and cigarette consumption. J. Subst. Abuse Treat., 45, Steinbeis, N., Bernhardt, B.C. & Singer, T. (2012) Impulse control and underlying functions of the left DLPFC mediate age-related and age-independent individual differences in strategic social behavior. Neuron, 73, Vallar, G. & Bolognini, N. (2011) Behavioural facilitation following brain stimulation: implications for neurorehabilitation. Neuropsychol. Rehabil., 21, Weber, E.U., Shafir, S. & Blais, A.R. (2004) Predicting risk sensitivity in humans and lower animals: risk as variance or coefficient of variation. Psychol. Rev., 111, Woicik, P.A., Stewart, S.H., Pihl, R.O. & Conrod, P.J. (2009) The substance use risk profile scale: a scale measuring traits linked to reinforcementspecific substance use profiles. Addict. Behav., 34, Xue, G., Juan, C.H., Chang, C.F., Lu, Z.L. & Dong, Q. (2012) Lateral prefrontal cortex contributes to maladaptive decisions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 109,

Chapter 2 Knowledge Production in Cognitive Neuroscience: Tests of Association, Necessity, and Sufficiency

Chapter 2 Knowledge Production in Cognitive Neuroscience: Tests of Association, Necessity, and Sufficiency Chapter 2 Knowledge Production in Cognitive Neuroscience: Tests of Association, Necessity, and Sufficiency While all domains in neuroscience might be relevant for NeuroIS research to some degree, the field

More information

Activation of Prefrontal Cortex by Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Reduces Appetite for Risk during Ambiguous Decision Making

Activation of Prefrontal Cortex by Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Reduces Appetite for Risk during Ambiguous Decision Making 6212 The Journal of Neuroscience, June 6, 2007 27(23):6212 6218 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Activation of Prefrontal Cortex by Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Reduces Appetite for Risk during

More information

Pump up the volume! Can transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) enhance Cognitive Bias Modification in Addiction?

Pump up the volume! Can transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) enhance Cognitive Bias Modification in Addiction? Pump up the volume! Can transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) enhance Cognitive Bias Modification in Addiction? Reinout W. Wiers Thomas E. Gladwin Tess den Uijl Addiction Development and Psychopathology

More information

Brain Stimulation. Comparing Cortical Plasticity Induced by Conventional and High-Definition 4 1 Ring tdcs: A Neurophysiological Study

Brain Stimulation. Comparing Cortical Plasticity Induced by Conventional and High-Definition 4 1 Ring tdcs: A Neurophysiological Study Brain Stimulation 6 (2013) 644e648 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Brain Stimulation journal homepage: www.brainstimjrnl.com Original Articles Comparing Cortical Plasticity Induced by

More information

Modulation of decision-making in a gambling task in older adults with transcranial direct current stimulation

Modulation of decision-making in a gambling task in older adults with transcranial direct current stimulation European Journal of Neuroscience European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 31, pp. 593 597, 2010 doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07080.x COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE Modulation of decision-making in a gambling task

More information

Statement on Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Depression. Position statement CERT03/17

Statement on Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Depression. Position statement CERT03/17 Statement on Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Depression Position statement CERT03/17 Approved by the Royal College of Psychiatrists, Committee on ECT and Related Treatments: February 2017

More information

FRONTAL TDCS MODULATES ORBITOFRONTAL REALITY FILTERING

FRONTAL TDCS MODULATES ORBITOFRONTAL REALITY FILTERING Neuroscience 265 (2014) 21 27 FRONTAL TDCS MODULATES ORBITOFRONTAL REALITY FILTERING A. L. MANUEL, a * A. W. DAVID, b M. BIKSON b AND A. SCHNIDER a,c a Laboratory of Cognitive Neurorehabilitation, Department

More information

The basics of tdcs: Marom Bikson. The City College of New York of CUNY

The basics of tdcs: Marom Bikson. The City College of New York of CUNY The basics of tdcs: Technology and Mechanism Marom Bikson Lucas Parra, Jacek Dmochowski,Asif Rahman, Niranjan Khadka, Mark Jackson, Dennis Truong, Belen Lafon, Gregory Kronberg, Devin Adair, Nigel Gebodh

More information

"Hot" cognition and dual systems: Introduction, criticisms, and ways forward

Hot cognition and dual systems: Introduction, criticisms, and ways forward Title: "Hot" cognition and dual systems: Introduction, criticisms, and ways forward Authors: Thomas E. Gladwin *, a, b Bernd Figner *, a, c a Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands. b University

More information

Cognitive enhancement. using tdcs. Does tdcs. enhance cognition? Does tdcs enhance cognition? Why combine tdcs with other treatments?

Cognitive enhancement. using tdcs. Does tdcs. enhance cognition? Does tdcs enhance cognition? Why combine tdcs with other treatments? Cognitive enhancement Does tdcs enhance cognition? using tdcs Why combine tdcs with other treatments? Anna-Katharine Brem, PhD, MASNP Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford Berenson-Allen

More information

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Session 4 Virtual Lesion Approach I Alexandra Reichenbach MPI for Biological Cybernetics Tübingen, Germany Today s Schedule Virtual Lesion Approach : Study Design Rationale

More information

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab This report details my summer research project for the REU Theoretical and Computational Neuroscience program as

More information

Sophie C. Andrews, a Kate E. Hoy, a Peter G. Enticott, a Zafiris J. Daskalakis, b Paul B. Fitzgerald a

Sophie C. Andrews, a Kate E. Hoy, a Peter G. Enticott, a Zafiris J. Daskalakis, b Paul B. Fitzgerald a Brain Stimulation (2011) 4, 84 9 www.brainstimjrnl.com Improving working memory: the effect of combining cognitive activity and anodal transcranial direct current stimulation to the left dorsolateral prefrontal

More information

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver NRSC January 28, 2010 Costs: Health, Crime, Productivity Costs in billions of dollars (2002) $400 $350 $400B legal

More information

Evidence for a role of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in. controlling stimulus-response integration: A transcranial direct

Evidence for a role of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in. controlling stimulus-response integration: A transcranial direct 1 tdcs S-R Evidence for a role of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in controlling stimulus-response integration: A transcranial direct current stimulation (tdcs) study Sharon Zmigrod Lorenza S.

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Moriguchi and Hiraki 10.1073/pnas.0809747106 SI Text Differences in Brain Activation Between Preswitch and Postswitch Phases. The paired t test was used to compare the brain activation

More information

The Impact of Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation on Self-Control Dissertation submitted by Ozgur Yalcin November 2013

The Impact of Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation on Self-Control Dissertation submitted by Ozgur Yalcin November 2013 Running head: TRANSCRANIAL DIRECT CURRENT SELF CONTROL i The Impact of Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation on Self-Control Dissertation submitted by Ozgur Yalcin November 2013 as partial fulfilment

More information

tdcs in Clinical Disorders

tdcs in Clinical Disorders HBM Educational course Brain Stimulation: Past, Present and Future Hamburg, June 8th, 2014 tdcs in Clinical Disorders Agnes Flöel NeuroCure Clinical Research Center, Neurology, & Center for Stroke Research

More information

TREATMENT-SPECIFIC ABNORMAL SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN EARLY PARKINSON S DISEASE

TREATMENT-SPECIFIC ABNORMAL SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN EARLY PARKINSON S DISEASE TREATMENT-SPECIFIC ABNORMAL SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN EARLY PARKINSON S DISEASE Angel Lago-Rodriguez 1, Binith Cheeran 2 and Miguel Fernández-Del-Olmo 3 1. Prism Lab, Behavioural Brain Sciences, School of

More information

Altruistic Behavior: Lessons from Neuroeconomics. Kei Yoshida Postdoctoral Research Fellow University of Tokyo Center for Philosophy (UTCP)

Altruistic Behavior: Lessons from Neuroeconomics. Kei Yoshida Postdoctoral Research Fellow University of Tokyo Center for Philosophy (UTCP) Altruistic Behavior: Lessons from Neuroeconomics Kei Yoshida Postdoctoral Research Fellow University of Tokyo Center for Philosophy (UTCP) Table of Contents 1. The Emergence of Neuroeconomics, or the Decline

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way Mortality and morbidity attributable to use of addictive substances in the United States. The Association of American Physicians from 1999 60 million tobacco smokers in the U.S. 14 million dependent on

More information

Recovery mechanisms from aphasia

Recovery mechanisms from aphasia Recovery mechanisms from aphasia Dr. Michal Ben-Shachar 977 Acquired language and reading impairments 1 Research questions Which brain systems can support recovery from aphasia? Which compensatory route

More information

Can brain stimulation help with relearning movement after stroke?

Can brain stimulation help with relearning movement after stroke? stroke.org.uk Final report summary Can brain stimulation help with relearning movement after stroke? The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning after stroke PROJECT CODE: TSA

More information

In the context of decision-making research, people s regulatory orientation mode (i.e., assessment and locomotion

In the context of decision-making research, people s regulatory orientation mode (i.e., assessment and locomotion International Journal of Psychology, 2014 Vol. 49, No. 3, 211 215, DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12026 Self-regulation predicts risk-taking through people s time horizon Angelo Panno, Antonio Pierro, and Marco Lauriola

More information

Your support makes a vast difference to the success of our research and the health of our community.

Your support makes a vast difference to the success of our research and the health of our community. STEWARDSHIP REPORT THE 256-CHANNEL ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAM SYSTEM FOR SCHIZOPHRENIA RESEARCH 2015-2016 PROGRESS REPORT Prepared for the BC Schizophrenia Society Foundation By the UBC Faculty of Medicine May

More information

Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP

Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP Disclosures This speaker has no conflicts of interest to disclose Objectives Define drug abuse and addiction Identify the

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Statistics and Results This file contains supplementary statistical information and a discussion of the interpretation of the belief effect on the basis of additional data. We also present

More information

The Neural Basis of Economic Decision- Making in The Ultimatum Game

The Neural Basis of Economic Decision- Making in The Ultimatum Game The Neural Basis of Economic Decision- Making in The Ultimatum Game Sanfey, Rilling, Aronson, Nystrom, & Cohen (2003), The neural basis of economic decisionmaking in the Ultimatum game, Science 300, 1755-1758

More information

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) A Promising Treatment for Depression?

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) A Promising Treatment for Depression? Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) A Promising Treatment for Depression? transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) Alternating current (AC) 1-3 ma, 9V Direct Current (DC) Direct current:

More information

Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Lisa Koski, PhD, Clin Psy TMS Neurorehabilitation Lab Royal Victoria Hospital 2009-12-14 BIC Seminar, MNI Overview History, basic principles, instrumentation

More information

Assessing the Validity of Three Tasks of Risk-Taking Propensity: Behavioral Measure and Computational Modeling

Assessing the Validity of Three Tasks of Risk-Taking Propensity: Behavioral Measure and Computational Modeling Assessing the Validity of Three Tasks of Risk-Taking Propensity: Behavioral Measure and Computational Modeling Ran Zhou 1 (zhou.1500@osu.edu), Jay I. Myung 1 (myung.1@osu.edu), Carol A. Mathews 2 (carolmathews@ufl.edu),

More information

The Adolescent Developmental Stage

The Adolescent Developmental Stage The Adolescent Developmental Stage o Physical maturation o Drive for independence o Increased salience of social and peer interactions o Brain development o Inflection in risky behaviors including experimentation

More information

MagPro by MagVenture. Versatility in Magnetic Stimulation

MagPro by MagVenture. Versatility in Magnetic Stimulation MagPro by MagVenture Versatility in Magnetic Stimulation MagPro A proven Record of Innovation With 7 different magnetic stimulators and 27 different coils, the MagPro line from MagVenture provides the

More information

The Contribution of Neuroscience to Understanding Human Behaviour

The Contribution of Neuroscience to Understanding Human Behaviour The Contribution of Neuroscience to Understanding Human Behaviour Gail Tripp Human Developmental Neurobiology Unit Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Promotion Corporation Okinawa Japan Today

More information

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy Methods Participants We examined a group of twelve male pathological gamblers (ten strictly right handed, all smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy males,

More information

Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation

Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation Introduction (tdcs) is a non-invasive form of brain stimulation similar to transcranial magnetic stimulation, but instead of using magnets, it uses a lowintensity, constant current applied through scalp

More information

Patients with disorders of consciousness: how to treat them?

Patients with disorders of consciousness: how to treat them? Patients with disorders of consciousness: how to treat them? Aurore THIBAUT PhD Student Coma Science Group LUCA meeting February 25 th 2015 Pharmacological treatments Amantadine Giacino (2012) 184 TBI

More information

MagPro by MagVenture. Versatility in Magnetic Stimulation

MagPro by MagVenture. Versatility in Magnetic Stimulation MagPro by MagVenture Versatility in Magnetic Stimulation MagPro A proven record of innovation With 7 different magnetic stimulators and 33 different coils, the MagPro line from MagVenture provides the

More information

Clinical Applications of tdcs: past, present and future

Clinical Applications of tdcs: past, present and future Clinical Applications of tdcs: past, present and future Felipe Fregni, MD, PhD, MPH, MMSc, MEd Spaulding Neuromodulation Center Spaulding Rehabilitation Hospital Massachusetts General Hospital Harvard

More information

Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging

Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging Angelica Morales, PhD Postdoctoral Fellow Developmental Brain Imaging Lab Oregon Health & Science University No Financial Disclosures The

More information

Short duration transcranial direct current stimulation (tdcs) modulates verbal memory

Short duration transcranial direct current stimulation (tdcs) modulates verbal memory Brain Stimulation (2012) 5, 468 74 www.brainstimjrnl.com Short duration transcranial direct current stimulation (tdcs) modulates verbal memory Amir Homayoun Javadi, a,b Paul Cheng, c Vincent Walsh a,c

More information

TMS: Full Board or Expedited?

TMS: Full Board or Expedited? TMS: Full Board or Expedited? Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation: - Neurostimulation or neuromodulation technique based on the principle of electro-magnetic induction of an electric field in the brain.

More information

Veronika Grimm, Friederike Mengel. Let me sleep on it: Delay reduces rejection rates in Ultimatum Games RM/10/017

Veronika Grimm, Friederike Mengel. Let me sleep on it: Delay reduces rejection rates in Ultimatum Games RM/10/017 Veronika Grimm, Friederike Mengel Let me sleep on it: Delay reduces rejection rates in Ultimatum Games RM/10/017 Let me sleep on it: Delay reduces rejection rates in Ultimatum Games Veronika Grimm Friederike

More information

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Outline Neuroanatomy of emotion Critical conceptual, experimental design, and interpretation issues in neuroimaging research Fear and anxiety Neuroimaging research

More information

Summary of my talk. Cerebellum means little brain but a huge neural resource. Studying the cerebellum in. Chris Miall

Summary of my talk. Cerebellum means little brain but a huge neural resource. Studying the cerebellum in. Chris Miall Studying the cerebellum in sensory motor control Chris Miall Behavioural Brain Sciences School of Psychology University of Birmingham Summary of my talk Cerebellum means little brain but a huge neural

More information

Neurophysiological Markers of Multiple Facets of Impulsivity. Lauren B. Neal & Philip A. Gable. The University of Alabama

Neurophysiological Markers of Multiple Facets of Impulsivity. Lauren B. Neal & Philip A. Gable. The University of Alabama Running head: EMOTION-BASED IMPULSIVITY Neurophysiological Markers of Multiple Facets of Impulsivity Lauren B. Neal & Philip A. Gable The University of Alabama Department of Psychology The University of

More information

Nafisa Cassimjee University of Pretoria, South Africa. Raegan Murphy University College Cork, Ireland

Nafisa Cassimjee University of Pretoria, South Africa. Raegan Murphy University College Cork, Ireland Impulsive and rigid temperament subtypes and executive functioning an exploratory study of temperament configurations and neuropsychological performance Raegan Murphy University College Cork, Ireland Nafisa

More information

Proactive Motor Control Reduces Monetary Risk Taking in Gambling

Proactive Motor Control Reduces Monetary Risk Taking in Gambling Research Article Proactive Motor Control Reduces Monetary Risk Taking in Gambling Psychological Science 23(7) 805 815 The Author(s) 2012 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav DOI:

More information

Neural Basis of Decision Making. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Neural Basis of Decision Making. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Neural Basis of Decision Making Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Phineas Gage: Sept. 13, 1848 Working on the rail road Rod impaled his head. 3.5 x 1.25 13 pounds What happened

More information

Computational Cognitive Neuroscience (CCN)

Computational Cognitive Neuroscience (CCN) introduction people!s background? motivation for taking this course? Computational Cognitive Neuroscience (CCN) Peggy Seriès, Institute for Adaptive and Neural Computation, University of Edinburgh, UK

More information

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT Why delaying the onset of alcohol and other drug use during adolescence is so important Aaron White, PhD Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research National Institute

More information

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Outline Neuroanatomy of emotion Fear and anxiety Brain imaging research on anxiety Brain functional activation fmri Brain functional connectivity fmri Brain structural

More information

NeuroReport 2014, 25: a US Army Natick Soldier Research, Development, and Engineering Center, Natick

NeuroReport 2014, 25: a US Army Natick Soldier Research, Development, and Engineering Center, Natick Integrative systems 1175 Direct current brain stimulation enhances navigation efficiency in individuals with low spatial sense of direction Tad T. Brunyé a,b, Amanda Holmes a,b, Julie Cantelon a,b, Marianna

More information

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M.

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M. Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making Johanna M. Jarcho 1,2 Elliot T. Berkman 3 Matthew D. Lieberman 3 1 Department of Psychiatry

More information

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source

More information

CONSUMER NEUROSCIENCE

CONSUMER NEUROSCIENCE CONSUMER NEUROSCIENCE Spring 2014 New York University Stern School of Business Professor Manuel Garcia-Garcia Wednesday, 6:00pm 9:00pm Phone: (917) 769 9839 E Mail: mgarcia@stern.nyu.edu Office Hours:

More information

Consumer Neuroscience Research beyond fmri: The Importance of Multi-Method Approaches for understanding Goal Value Computations

Consumer Neuroscience Research beyond fmri: The Importance of Multi-Method Approaches for understanding Goal Value Computations Consumer Neuroscience Research beyond fmri: The Importance of Multi-Method Approaches for understanding Goal Value Computations Hilke Plassmann INSEAD, France Decision Neuroscience Workshop, August 23,

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

EFFECTS OF TRANSCRANIAL ULTRASOUND ( TUS ) ON WORKING MEMORY

EFFECTS OF TRANSCRANIAL ULTRASOUND ( TUS ) ON WORKING MEMORY EFFECTS OF TRANSCRANIAL ULTRASOUND ( TUS ) ON WORKING MEMORY Item Type text; Electronic Thesis Authors Lazar, Michael Phillip Publisher The University of Arizona. Rights Copyright is held by the author.

More information

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 1 The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning Anila D Mello Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 2 Abstract Learning and changing

More information

Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions

Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions Anneke. Goudriaan 1,2, Ph.D. Ruth van Holst 1,2, M.Sc. Dick

More information

Commentary 1 Toward New Therapeutics in Depression: tdcs s Secrets

Commentary 1 Toward New Therapeutics in Depression: tdcs s Secrets Commentary 1 Toward New Therapeutics in Depression: tdcs s Secrets Adeline Etiévant 1,2* and Julie Monnin 1,2,3 1 Laboratoire de Neurosciences intégratives et cliniques, EA 481, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté,

More information

Understanding Addiction: Why Can t Those Affected Just Say No?

Understanding Addiction: Why Can t Those Affected Just Say No? Understanding Addiction: Why Can t Those Affected Just Say No? 1 The Stigma of Addiction There continues to be a stigma surrounding addiction even among health care workers. Consider the negative opinions

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 March 06.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Subst Use Misuse. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 March 06. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Subst Use Misuse. 2010 September ; 45(11): 1766 1786. doi:10.3109/10826084.2010.482434. Neuromodulation of Decision-Making in the

More information

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) and the Face Inversion Effect: Anodal stimulation at Fp3 reduces recognition for upright faces

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) and the Face Inversion Effect: Anodal stimulation at Fp3 reduces recognition for upright faces Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) and the Face Inversion Effect: Anodal stimulation at Fp3 reduces recognition for upright faces Ciro Civile (ciroc@mcmaster.ca) Department of Psychology, Neuroscience,

More information

The Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (TDCS) on Reducing. the Desire for Methadone among Addicts under Treatment in Taleghani

The Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (TDCS) on Reducing. the Desire for Methadone among Addicts under Treatment in Taleghani Available online at www.scholarsresearchlibrary.com Scholars Research Library Der Pharmacia Lettre, 2017, 9 [4]:179-185 [http://scholarsresearchlibrary.com/archive.html] ISSN 0975-5071 USA CODEN: DPLEB4

More information

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation:

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation: Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation: What has experience told us? Jonathan Downar MD PhD FRCPC MRI-Guided rtms Clinic University Health Network www.rtmsclinic.ca Disclosures Research funding: Canadian Institutes

More information

Making Assessments While Taking Sequential Risks

Making Assessments While Taking Sequential Risks Making Assessments While Taking Sequential Risks Avishai Wershbale (wershbal@msu.edu) & Timothy J. Pleskac (pleskact@msu.edu) Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824

More information

Chapter 37 Effects of Acupuncture on Anxiety Levels and Prefrontal Cortex Activity Measured by Near-Infrared Spectroscopy: A Pilot Study

Chapter 37 Effects of Acupuncture on Anxiety Levels and Prefrontal Cortex Activity Measured by Near-Infrared Spectroscopy: A Pilot Study Chapter 37 Effects of Acupuncture on Anxiety Levels and Prefrontal Cortex Activity Measured by Near-Infrared Spectroscopy: A Pilot Study K. Sakatani, M. Fujii, N. Takemura, and T. Hirayama Abstract There

More information

Neuroimaging. BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT. Human Brain Mapping

Neuroimaging. BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT. Human Brain Mapping 11/8/2013 Neuroimaging N i i BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT 2 Human Brain Mapping H Human m n brain br in m mapping ppin can nb

More information

Frontal EEG Asymmetry and Regulation during Childhood

Frontal EEG Asymmetry and Regulation during Childhood Frontal EEG Asymmetry and Regulation during Childhood KEE JEONG KIM a AND MARTHA ANN BELL b a Department of Human Development, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, Virginia,

More information

Analysing the inability to focus on the driving task in young males, inexperienced and offender drivers. Castro, C., Gugliotta, A. & Padilla, J.L.

Analysing the inability to focus on the driving task in young males, inexperienced and offender drivers. Castro, C., Gugliotta, A. & Padilla, J.L. Analysing the inability to focus on the driving task in young males, inexperienced and offender drivers Castro, C., Gugliotta, A. & Padilla, J.L. Example suggested by Tom Vanderbilt, reading (2008). Traffic:

More information

Toward a Mechanistic Understanding of Human Decision Making Contributions of Functional Neuroimaging

Toward a Mechanistic Understanding of Human Decision Making Contributions of Functional Neuroimaging CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Toward a Mechanistic Understanding of Human Decision Making Contributions of Functional Neuroimaging John P. O Doherty and Peter Bossaerts Computation and Neural

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making Matthew A J Apps 1,2, Laura Grima 2, Sanjay Manohar 2, Masud Husain 1,2 1 Nuffield Department of Clinical Neuroscience,

More information

Effects of slow rtms at the right. dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on EEG. asymmetry and mood

Effects of slow rtms at the right. dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on EEG. asymmetry and mood Effects of slow rtms at the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on EEG asymmetry and mood Dennis J.L.G. Schutter, Jack van Honk CA, Alfredo A.L. d Alfonso, Albert Postma, Edward H.F. de Haan Psychological

More information

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): 359-364 ISSN 0970-938X http://www.biomedres.info Investigating relative strengths and positions of electrical activity in the left and right hemispheres of the human brain

More information

Adolescent risk-taking

Adolescent risk-taking Adolescent risk-taking OVERVIEW This document provides an overview of the scientific community s current understanding of why adolescents are more likely than children or adults to engage in risk-taking

More information

The Effect of Regular Naltrexone Dosing on Disordered Gamblers: An Examination of Neural Activation, Gambling Urges, and Gambling Behaviour

The Effect of Regular Naltrexone Dosing on Disordered Gamblers: An Examination of Neural Activation, Gambling Urges, and Gambling Behaviour The Effect of Regular Naltrexone Dosing on Disordered Gamblers: An Examination of Neural Activation, Gambling Urges, and Gambling Behaviour Darren R. Christensen, PhD Chair in Gambling, Alberta Gambling

More information

P1: OTA/XYZ P2: ABC JWST JWST066-Adinoff July 16, :36 Printer Name: Yet to Come. Introduction COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL

P1: OTA/XYZ P2: ABC JWST JWST066-Adinoff July 16, :36 Printer Name: Yet to Come. Introduction COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL Introduction COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL Chapter 1 3 P1: OTA/XYZ P2: ABC Chapter 1 Introduction Bryon Adinoff 1,2 and Elliot Stein 3 1 Department of Psychiatry, UT Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX 2 VA

More information

Gender specific attitudes towards risk and ambiguity an experimental investigation

Gender specific attitudes towards risk and ambiguity an experimental investigation Research Collection Working Paper Gender specific attitudes towards risk and ambiguity an experimental investigation Author(s): Schubert, Renate; Gysler, Matthias; Brown, Martin; Brachinger, Hans Wolfgang

More information

Poor impulse control and heightened attraction to alcohol-related imagery in repeat DUI offenders

Poor impulse control and heightened attraction to alcohol-related imagery in repeat DUI offenders Poor impulse control and heightened attraction to alcohol-related imagery in repeat DUI offenders Abstract Melissa A. Miller, M.S. and Mark T. Fillmore, Ph.D. Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky

More information

Assistive and adaptive technology

Assistive and adaptive technology Chapter 8: Assistive and adaptive technology Introduction Assistive and adaptive technology refers to products, devices or equipment that are used to maintain, increase or improve the capabilities of individuals

More information

A Feasibility Study of Bilateral Anodal Stimulation of the Prefrontal Cortex Using High-Definition Electrodes in Healthy Participants

A Feasibility Study of Bilateral Anodal Stimulation of the Prefrontal Cortex Using High-Definition Electrodes in Healthy Participants YALE JOURNAL OF BIOLOGY AND MEDICINE 88 (2015), pp.219-225. ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION A Feasibility Study of Bilateral Anodal Stimulation of the Prefrontal Cortex Using High-Definition Electrodes in Healthy

More information

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman BIOMED 509 Executive Control Cambridge University Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan UNM SOM JL Brigman 4-7-17 Symptoms and Assays of Cognitive Disorders Symptoms of Cognitive Disorders Learning & Memory

More information

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience School of Psychological Sciences Faculty of Medical & Human Sciences Module Information *Please note that this is a sample guide to modules. The exact

More information

Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision

Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision Andy McEwen, PhD University College London & National Centre for Smoking Cessation and Training (NCSCT) Tuesday 29th January 2013 Cardiff Conflict

More information

Research on Brand Trust and Financing Risk Preference of E-commerce Based on Neuroeconomic Experiment

Research on Brand Trust and Financing Risk Preference of E-commerce Based on Neuroeconomic Experiment Research on Brand Trust and Financing Risk Preference of E-commerce Based on Neuroeconomic Experiment Ronggang Zhang ABSTRACT Brand trust and financing of e-commerce are two key factors for the development

More information

Neural Basis of Decision Making. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Neural Basis of Decision Making. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Neural Basis of Decision Making Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Phineas Gage: Sept. 13, 1848 Working on the rail road Rod impaled his head. 3.5 x 1.25 13 pounds What happened

More information

Centre for Analysis of Youth Transitions. There are 2 group sessions spread across 2 weeks, with each session lasting 90 minutes.

Centre for Analysis of Youth Transitions. There are 2 group sessions spread across 2 weeks, with each session lasting 90 minutes. Centre for Analysis of Youth Transitions AYT STUDY REFERENCE: REP07 Programme name: PREVENTURE: a group of brief, selective, personality-targeted interventions aimed at reducing substance (alcohol and

More information

Programme Name: Climate Schools: Alcohol and drug education courses

Programme Name: Climate Schools: Alcohol and drug education courses STUDY REFERENCE: C/ADEPIS01 Programme Name: Climate Schools: Alcohol and drug education courses Contact Details: Nicola Newton, University of New South Wales, email: n.newton@unsw.edu.au Natasha Nair,

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

Time Experiencing by Robotic Agents

Time Experiencing by Robotic Agents Time Experiencing by Robotic Agents Michail Maniadakis 1 and Marc Wittmann 2 and Panos Trahanias 1 1- Foundation for Research and Technology - Hellas, ICS, Greece 2- Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology

More information

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related?

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related? Shackman, Afterword Q2 1 Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related? Alexander J. Shackman Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, and Maryland Neuroimaging

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Burton-Chellew and West 10.1073/pnas.1210960110 SI Results Fig. S4 A and B shows the percentage of free riders and cooperators over time for each treatment. Although Fig. S4A shows

More information

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H

More information

Myers Psychology for AP*

Myers Psychology for AP* Myers Psychology for AP* David G. Myers PowerPoint Presentation Slides by Kent Korek Germantown High School Worth Publishers, 2010 *AP is a trademark registered and/or owned by the College Board, which

More information

BEHAVIOR CHANGE THEORY

BEHAVIOR CHANGE THEORY BEHAVIOR CHANGE THEORY An introduction to a behavior change theory framework by the LIVE INCITE team This document is not a formal part of the LIVE INCITE Request for Tender and PCP. It may thus be used

More information