Processes involved in tempo perception: A CNV analysis

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Processes involved in tempo perception: A CNV analysis"

Transcription

1 Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research Processes involved in tempo perception: A CNV analysis MICHA PFEUTY, RICHARDRAGOT, AND VIVIANE POUTHAS Neurosciences Cognitives et Imagerie Ce re brale, LENA-CNRS, Hoˆ pital Salpêtrie re, Paris Cedex 13, France Abstract The purpose of this research was to study the mechanisms underlying tempo perception, by looking at their electrophysiological brain correlates. The subjects task consisted of comparing the tempos of two isochronous tone sequences made up of either three (condition I3) or six (condition I6) 600-ms intervals. Contingent negative variation (CNV), known to be linked to the judgment of a single interval, kept increasing in amplitude for three intervals during tempo encoding, thereby providing evidence of the occurrence of CNVs also for several intervals in succession. This CNV increase could reflect the use of interval-based processes in the building of the interval memory trace. During the comparison phase, a CNV decrease was observed in condition I6, suggesting that subjects did not build a new memory trace, but used beat-based processes to check whether the beats of the new tempo occurred at the times they anticipated. Descriptors: Time perception, Tempo, EEG, CNV, Memory, Audition Many studies have been devoted to the analysis of the processes underlying time perception (for reviews, see Gibbon, Malapani, Dale, & Gallistel, 1997; Harrington & Haaland, 1999; Ivry, 1996; among others). Several questions remain unanswered, and an interesting way to approach this issue is to study the mechanisms that subserve the perception of a series of time intervals, especially in the auditory modality. We all experience such perceptions daily, particularly in language and music. This article will focus on a basic aspect of rhythm perception, tempo, defined as a sequence of tones separated by isochronous intervals. Models of time perception generally assume that the duration of an interval is measured by an internal timer that records the number of clock ticks occurring during the interval. The judgment that a test interval is the same as, shorter than, or longer than a target interval would result from the comparison of the number of ticks recorded during each interval. The accuracy of such a comparison would depend on the variability of the timer. It must be stressed that few studies have taken an interest in the effect of the temporal context (e.g., sequences of intervals, as in speech or music) on judgment of interval duration. However, two types of modelsfinterval-based and beat-based modelsfhave been proposed to account for timing data in these particular contexts. According to interval-based models, the comparison of intervals relies on the timer described above, which is thought to be involved in the estimation of successive intervals. According to beat-based models, such a comparison relies on the synchronization of an internal beat with the external rhythm. Address reprint requests to: Micha Pfeuty, Neurosciences Cognitives et Imagerie Cérébrale, LENA-CNRS, Hoˆ pital Salpêtrière, 47 Bld de l Hoˆ pital, Paris Cedex 13, France. Micha.Pfeuty@chups.- jussieu.fr. 69 Schulze (1978) conducted an experiment in which subjects had to decide whether an auditory sequence was irregular. The author measured the threshold, dt, for the detection of an irregularity in a sequence of type A (T, T, T, T 1 dt, T, T, T, y) or B (T, T, T, T 1 dt, T dt, T, T, y). If performance is subserved by beatbased processes, a lower threshold for detecting irregularity will be observed in sequence A, because the difference between internal and external rhythm would be maintained throughout the sequence. The involvement of interval-based processes would give a lower threshold in sequence B, because the differences T1dT and T dt would be more evident for the timer. The results showed that the threshold for the detection of irregularity was lower in sequence A than in sequence B, as predicted by beatbased models. Oddly, Keele, Nicholetti, Ivry, and Pokorny (1989), who replicated the same experiment, reported the opposite results, and thus proposed that interval-based models better account for performance. However, this last experiment was not sufficient for choosing between the two kinds of models. So in another experiment, in which subjects had to compare the tempos of two isochronous tone sequences, Keele et al. manipulated the duration of the pause between the target sequence and the test sequence. They assumed that, if timing was based on an internal beat, varying the duration of this pause would disturb the comparison. The results did not indicate any significant effect of the pause, thereby refuting beat-based models. Ivry and Hazeltine (1995) examined time discrimination for different interval durations in order to describe the durationdependent variability of the timer. They used either a single interval or a sequence of three intervals. The results showed that the duration-dependent variability of the timer was smaller in the second condition, suggesting that repeating the target interval allows participants to form a more accurate internal representation. In a similar way, Drake and Botte (1993) showed that the threshold of tempo discrimination decreased when the number of

2 70 M. Pfeuty, R. Ragot, and V. Pouthas intervals in the sequence increased. They proposed a multiplelook model of tempo perception. According to these authors, when a standard interval is repeated, multiple looks at the same interval would allow listeners to improve the statistical estimate of it. Thus, interval-based models account for context effects in terms of the statistics of the intervals constituting a pattern. Tempo encoding would rest on the elaboration of a memory trace of the reference interval equal to the statistical mean of the successive intervals. This trace is thought to become more precise as the number of intervals included in the tempo increases. Nevertheless, Panissal (1998) showed that the threshold of tempo discrimination ceases to decrease after a critical number of intervals, depending on the reference interval duration. This suggests the existence of a temporal window, which would be equal to the minimal number of intervals needed to establish the most accurate internal interval reference. This temporal window is then said to be filled after that number of intervals. In short, there have been behavioral studies on tempo perception guided by either interval-based or beat-based models (Drake & Botte, 1993; Ivry & Hazeltine, 1995; Keele et al., 1989; Schulze, 1978; among others), but to our knowledge, there have been no EEG studies like those conducted on the perception and the production of a single interval. A slow negative wave, called the contingent negative variation (CNV), has been shown to develop over frontal areas during the temporal interval between two events, S1 corresponding to a warning stimulus, and S2 to an imperative stimulus (Walter, Cooper, Aldridge, McCallum, & Winter, 1964). This wave would reflect anticipatory processing of S2 and motor preparation when a response is temporally related to S2. Several lines of evidence support the hypothesis that the CNV is the main ERP correlate of the estimation or production of a time interval. McAdam s (1966) results showed that CNV amplitude and accuracy increased in parallel during the learning of a temporal interval. Ruchkin, McCalley, and Glaser (1977) observed that the latency of CNV termination was correlated with temporal judgments and concluded that this ERP partly reflected cognitive activity leading up to the formation of a temporal judgment. In Macar, Vidal, and Casini (1999), larger negativities were found either when longer intervals than the target were produced or when intervals were judged longer than a memorized standard interval. They proposed that these negativities would reflect the accumulation of pulses during the interval to be estimated. The aim of the present study was to examine the ERP correlates of the estimation of intervals that form an isochronous sequence, and to find out if and how successive CNVs are related to tempo perception. Based on behavioral results on a tempo comparison task (Drake & Botte, 1993; Panissal, 1998) and on the assumption that CNV reflects the operation of a timer, we hypothesized the following: If tempo encoding relies on the building of a memory trace issued from the multiple estimation of successive intervals, EEG recordings will reveal a systematic increase in CNV amplitude reflecting the temporal judgment for those intervals. We also assumed that intervals going beyond a definite temporal window would no longer be taken into account, and that CNV amplitude would therefore stop increasing. For a tempo period of 600 ms, behavioral studies have suggested that this temporal window is filled after three or four intervals (Panissal, 1998). In the present study, subjects were presented with a tempo made up of either three (condition I3) or six (condition I6) intervals. We expected to observe a systematic increase in CNV amplitude throughout the encoding phase in condition I3, and no further increase after three or four intervals in condition I6. A complementary goal of the present study was to examine ERPs during the comparison phase. Drake and Botte (1993) proposed that subjects would directly compare the successive intervals with the average memory trace previously elaborated. According to this assumption, no sustained negativity should then be observed during the comparison phase. Method Participants Fifteen volunteer students (8 men, 7 women) ages participated in this experiment. They reported being righthanded, having normal hearing, and having never attended music school or played a musical instrument. Procedure Tempos were made of regular sequences of auditory tones presented at a regular interval. Each tone was made of a 440-Hz sine wave, 50 ms in duration, presented binaurally at 70 db (SPL). The tempo was defined by the interonset interval (IOI) between two successive tones. Each trial consisted of two sequences (standard and test sequences) separated by a random delay (between 1,100 and 1,500 ms long). The IOI defining the tempo of the first sequence was always 600 ms long (standard IOI). The test IOI defining the tempo of the second sequence was either the same (600 ms 5 standard), shorter (576 ms ms 4% of 600 ms), or longer (624 ms ms þ 4% of 600 ms; Figure 1). The participants task consisted in comparing the tempo of the second test sequence with that of the first standard sequence. They were given a response device with three keys, and had to respond using the three inner fingers of their right hand. The sequences consisted of either three intervals (condition I3) or six intervals (condition I6). The order of conditions I3 and I6 was counterbalanced among the participants. For each condition, there were 96 trials, each divided into four sections of 24. The total number of trials was the same (1/3 of ) for each test IOI category (standard, short, or long). Participants used one of the three keys to state whether they judged the second tempo to be similar (standard IOI, middle finger), faster (shorter IOI, index finger), or slower (longer IOI, ring finger) than the first tempo. They were asked to respond as accurately as possible, without worrying about speed. They had to wait for the end of the second tempo sample before responding, even if they had already decided which button to press. ERP Recording Participants sat comfortably in a dimly lit room. During the task, they were asked to focus their gaze on a red point located 1 m in front of them. EEG activity was recorded through 62 electrodes placed at standard electrode sites and referred to the earlobes. The sampling rate per channel was 500 Hz (bandwidth Hz). Horizontal and vertical eye movements and blinks (EOG) were recorded by two bipolar electrodes. EEG signals were automatically corrected for ocular artifacts using an off-line digital method (Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1983). Eventrelated potentials were obtained by averaging trials in each condition (I3, I6), phase (encoding, comparison), and test IOI length (short, standard, long). There was a 200-ms baseline

3 Processes involved in tempo perception 71 Figure 1. Diagram of the tempo encoding and comparison paradigm. At the end of the comparison phase, subjects had to decide whether the presented tempo was slower than, equal to, or longer than the standard tempo presented before in the encoding phase. In condition I3, because of the small number of intervals (three) in the encoding and comparison phases, the temporal window (see text) can be considered as hardly filled. In condition I6, because of the large number of intervals (six) in the encoding and comparison phases, the temporal window can be considered as completely filled. epoch prior to the first tone of each sequence. This was used as a reference for amplitude evaluation. Behavioral Analysis Two performance indexes were used during this experiment: (1) The d 0 measure of detection sensitivity from signal detection theory, known to be an indicator of judgment difficulty independent of the strategy adopted by the subject (bias to respond different or identical ), was measured for each subject in conditions I3 and I6. The correct detection rate was obtained by averaging the percentages of correct responses for short and long test IOIs, and the false alarm rate was equal to the percentage of false responses for the standard IOI. The d 0 measure was calculated using the following formula: d 0 ¼ z fa z cd ; where z fa is the false alarm rate and z cd is the correct detection rate, both converted into z units. (2) The mean response latency, measured from the beginning of the last tone of the sequence, was calculated for each test IOI length and for each participant. The response latencies for short and long test IOIs were averaged and gave the correct detection latency, and the latency on the standard test IOI was the correct rejection latency. The effect of the condition (I3, I6) on d 0 was tested using a one-way ANOVA. The effect of the condition (I3, I6) and the type of response (detection, rejection) on the response latency was tested using a two-way ANOVA (Condition Type of Response). ERP Analysis CNVamplitude: Grand mean analysis. Mean CNVamplitude was measured over frontal electrodes (F5, F3, F1, FZ, F2, F4, F6, FC5, FC3, FC1, FCZ, FC2, FC4, FC6) for each interval, from 250 ms after each tone until the next tone. It was then averaged over all frontal sites in conditions I3 and I6 during the encoding and comparison phases. ANOVAS were carried out on these measures, to test whether or not there was an increase in CNV amplitude during these two phases. It was subsequently averaged for three groups of frontal sites, left (F5, F3, FC5, FC3), medial (F1, FZ, F2, FC1, FCZ, FC2), and right (F4, F6, FC4, FC6), to determine whether or not a specific hemispheric distribution of CNV was observed. Greenhouse Geisser corrections were made to account for the fact that the measurements of CNV amplitude on successive intervals were not independent. CNVamplitude: Individual analysis. In addition to the grand mean analysis, individual analyses were performed on CNV amplitude during the encoding phase, at the FCZ electrode where the CNV peaked. Given that we were interested in the time-course analysis of CNV amplitude (i.e., increase or decrease), individual differences in the overall CNV amplitude were eliminated. To do this, the amplitude at FCZ was replaced by a normalized z value for each subject: z ¼ ða FCS m ½b:eŠ Þ=s ½b:eŠ ; where A FCZ is the amplitude at FCZ, and m [b:e] and s [b:e] are the average and standard deviation, respectively, of the successive amplitudes measured at FCZ between the beginning and the end of the encoding sequence (from 0 to 1,800 ms in condition I3 and from 0 to 3,600 ms in condition I6). (Note: the minus sign in front of the formula allows an increase in the z value to correspond to an increase in CNV amplitude.) To characterize the time course of the z value along the encoding sequence, a mean z value was calculated during each interval, from 250 ms after each tone until the next tone. Then a z-slope value was obtained by linear regression of the different mean z values measured over the successive intervals (between the first and the last). This z-slope value was expressed in z units per interval. A positive and a negative z-slope value meant an increase and a decrease in CNV amplitude, respectively. Results Behavioral Data The d 0 measure of detection sensitivity was significantly greater in condition I6 than in condition I3, F(1,14) , po.05. Tempo discrimination accuracy improved between condition I3, d , and condition I6, d

4 72 M. Pfeuty, R. Ragot, and V. Pouthas The ANOVA on response latency indicated an effect of the condition, F(1,14) , po.01, and of the type of response, F(1,14) , po.005. There was no interaction between these two factors, F(1,14) , p4.10. The response latency was shorter in condition I6 (837 ms) than in condition I3 (956 ms), and shorter for correct detection (796 ms) than for correct rejection (1,097 ms). ERP Data CNV amplitude: Grand mean analysis. Figure 2 presents the grand mean ERPs over frontal electrodes in conditions I3 and I6 for the encoding and comparison phases. Mean CNVamplitudes measured for each interval, from 250 ms after each tone until the next tone, were averaged over frontal electrodes to obtain the mean frontal amplitude. To find out whether there was an increase in the mean frontal amplitude during the encoding and comparison phases, the effect of the interval rank (R1 to R3 in condition I3 and R1 to R6 in condition I6) was tested using a one-way ANOVA. For the encoding phase, the ANOVA yielded an effect of the interval rank in condition I3, F(2,28) , po.05, e 5.52, and in condition I6, F(5,70) , p o.05, e Table 1, part A, shows the mean CNV amplitude for successive intervals in both conditions. To test the hypothesis that a systematic increase in CNV amplitude would be observed throughout the encoding phase in condition I3, and no further increase after three or four intervals in condition I6, planned comparisons were carried out between successive interval ranks. In condition I3, the negativity increased between R1 and R2, F(1,14) , po.05, and tended to increase between R2 and R3, F(1,14) , po.10. In condition I6, the negativity increased between R1 and R2, F(1,14) , po.05, and between R2 and R3, F(1,14) , po.001, but not between R3 and R4, F(1,14) , p4.10, between R4 and R5, F(1,14) , p4.10, or between R5 and R6, F(1,14) , p4.10. Thus, during the encoding phase, an increase in CNV amplitude was only observed up to the third interval. For the comparison phase, there was no effect of the test IOI length factor in condition I3, F(2,28)o1, n.s., or I6, F(2,28) , p4.10, so this factor was removed from subsequent analyses. The interval rank effect was tested using a one-way ANOVA. No effect was observed on CNVamplitude in condition I3, F(2,28) , p4.10, whereas an effect was observed in condition I6, F(5,70) , po.0005, e Table 1, Part B, shows the mean CNV amplitude for successive intervals in both conditions. As no a priori assumptions have Figure 2. Grand mean event-related potentials (ERPs) over frontal (F5, F3, F1, FZ, F2, F4, F6) and frontal-central electrodes (FC5, FC3, FC1, FCZ, FC2, FC4, FC6), elicited during tempo encoding (thick lines) and tempo comparison (thin lines) in the six-interval and three-interval conditions. been made, post hoc comparisons using Tukey s HSDmeasures were carried out. These showed that in condition I6, the negative potential did not vary between R1 and R2, then decreased and changed into a positive potential between R2 and R4, po.001. Thus, CNVamplitude was sustained in condition I3, whereas in condition I6, it decreased after the second interval. To determine whether or not a hemispheric-specific distribution of the CNV was observed over the scalp, the left-frontal amplitude resulting from the averaging of electrodes F5, F3, Table 1. Mean CNV Amplitude Measured over Frontal Electrodes for Successive Intervals in Both Conditions I3 and I6 for Both Encoding (A) and Comparison (B) Conditions Interval rank Condition I3 Condition I6 R1 R2 R3 R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 Part A: Encoding phase CNV amplitude (mv) Part B: Comparison phase CNV amplitude (mv)

5 Processes involved in tempo perception 73 FC5, and FC3 was compared to the right-frontal amplitude resulting from the averaging of electrodes F4, F6, FC4, and FC6. The laterality effect was tested using a two-way ANOVA (Interval Rank Laterality) in both the encoding and comparison phases. No main effect of laterality was observed on the encoding and comparison phases in either condition I3 or I6. An interaction was observed for the encoding phase in condition I3 only, F(2,28) , po.05. Post hoc comparisons showed that during the first interval, left ( 4.17 mv) and right ( 3.82 mv), p4.10, frontal negativity did not differ significantly whereas during the third interval, frontal negativity was significantly larger on the left ( 7.51 mv) than on the right ( 6.53 mv), p o.001. CNV amplitude: Individual analysis. Figure 3 presents the time course of the individual ERPs at electrode FCZ, first transformed into a normalized z value, in both conditions (I3 and I6). Individual z-slope values (in z units per interval) are displayed below. The individual data showed that for 11 out of 15 subjects, the z-slope value, which is equivalent to the CNV amplitude slope at the FCZ electrode, was positive in at least one of the two conditions. By contrast, for 4 subjects out of 15, the slope was negative in both conditions. On the basis of these results, subjects were separated into two groups: group G1 consisting of 11 subjects, whose overall slope was positive, and group G2 consisting of 4 subjects whose overall slope was negative. For G1 subjects, CNVamplitude increased throughout the encoding phase, whereas for G2 subjects, CNV amplitude was high during the first interval but decreased after that. This is illustrated in Figure 4, which presents the time course of CNV amplitude over frontal electrodes, averaged separately for groups G1 and G2. To find out whether this group difference occurred over all frontal electrodes, frontal activity was averaged separately for three groups of frontal sites: left (F5, F3, FC5, FC3), medial (F1, FZ, F2, FC1, FCZ, FC2), and right (F4, F6, FC4, FC6). A three-way ANOVA was performed (Group Interval Rank Location). The results indicated a Group Interval Rank interaction effect in conditions I3, F(2,26) , po.05, e 5.53, and I6, F(5,65) , po.01, e 5.29, showing that in both conditions, CNV amplitude increased throughout the sequence for G1 subjects while decreasing after the end of the first interval for G2 subjects. Moreover, no interaction effect was observed with the location factor. Thus, the difference in the CNV amplitude slope between the two groups did not vary significantly across the different frontal sites. We therefore checked whether these different CNV patterns corresponded to different behaviors. The results showed that the behavioral performance of G2 subjects was significantly better than that of G1 subjects (mean d vs. 1.04, po.01, Mann Whitney U test). This finding is an additional argument for separating subjects into two groups. Discussion Behavioral Data The behavioral results showed that the tempo comparison task was easier with six than with three intervals. The d 0 measure of detection sensitivity rose, and response latency fell between conditions I3 and I6. This suggests (a) that three intervals are not sufficient for achieving optimal performance in the comparison of two tempos with a 600-ms interval, and (b) that temporal information after the third interval is still useful in enhancing performance. Panissal s (1998) data indicated that three or four intervals might be sufficient to encode a tempo of 600 ms. In our experiment, though, subjects did not undergo any special training, unlike most psychophysical studies. This may be why they needed more intervals to optimize their performance. However, it is not possible to determine whether the increase in sensitivity observed in condition I6 was due to supplementary intervals during the encoding or the comparison phase. Response latency was greater for correct rejection (equal tempo) than for correct detection (slower or longer tempo). This result suggests that the relative judgment of two tempos is not based on recognition processes, but on the detection of differences. ERP Data CNV Amplitude: Grand Mean Analysis Encoding phase. The grand mean ERPs over frontal electrodes showed that in conditions I3 and I6 alike, CNV amplitude increased during the encoding phase over the frontal part of the scalp (Figure 2). Such an increase did not occur during the comparison phase, and a decrease was even observed in condition I6. Thus, the increase in CNV amplitude seems to reflect processes strongly related to tempo encoding. Several studies have shown the CNVto be linked to time processing for a single interval (Macar et al., 1999; McAdam, 1966; Ruchkin et al., 1977). Our data provide evidence of CNVs also occurring for several intervals in succession. This suggests that during tempo encoding, intervals are estimated in succession, in accordance with the predictions of interval-based models of rhythm perception (Drake & Botte, 1993; Ivry & Hazeltine, 1995; Keele et al., 1989). We therefore propose that such an increase in CNV amplitude would reflect the building of a memory trace of the reference interval. Moreover, in condition I6, the results showed that the negativity stopped increasing after the third interval. Thus, beyond a critical number of intervals (defined as the temporal window), the memory trace would be optimal and time intervals would no longer be estimated. Two alternative hypotheses can be put forward to explain how the increase in CNVamplitude might be linked to the building of the interval memory trace. Regarding the first, McAdam (1966) suggested that CNV amplitude depends on the amount of attention allocated to time estimation. His study showed that CNV amplitude increased when the subjects learned a duration, and decreased after the learning phase (when performance became maximal). The author proposed that subcortical structures may take over for cortical structures when the task becomes automatic. Thus, in the present study, an increase in CNV amplitude could reflect attention allocation for successive time intervals. Once the memory trace became strong enough after three intervals, the role of attentional processes devoted to time would decrease and tempo would be perceived in a more automatic and efficient way until the end of the sequence. Regarding the second hypothesis, Ruchkin et al. (1997) showed that when subjects had to retain the presentation order of a series of syllables in working memory, a bilateral frontal slow negativity developed for syllables presented in the auditory modality but not for those presented in the visual modality. These authors proposed that this bilateral frontal slow negativity reflects the automatic storage of auditory information. Accordingly, because our temporal intervals were defined by tones, the

6 74 M. Pfeuty, R. Ragot, and V. Pouthas Figure 3. Time course of the individual ERPs at electrode FCZ (transformed into a normalized z value) elicited by tempo encoding with six (upper part) and three intervals (lower part), and grouped separately for G1 subjects (blue-green colors on left) and for G2 subjects (red colors on right). The corresponding individual z-slope values (in z units per time interval) are displayed below the curves. increase in CNVamplitude found in the present study may reflect the automatic storage of the successive tones in working memory. This type of storage could thus have been used to build the memory trace of the interval. Comparison phase. During the comparison phase, a decrease was observed in condition I6 over the frontal part of the scalp (Figure 2), after the second interval. This suggests that subjects did not build a new memory trace during this comparison phase. They may have directly compared the trace elaborated during the encoding phase with the current intervals. Decisional processes should play a more important role in order to make such a comparison. Numerous studies have shown that positive waves (P300-like) can reflect decisional processes taking place after the CNV (for a review, see Timsit-Berthier, 1984). Thus, the decrease in CNV amplitude observed on the comparison phase might

7 Processes involved in tempo perception 75 come from a decrease in attentional or working memory processes (generating less negativity), together with a possible increase in decisional processes (generating more positivity). Our hypothesis is that the subjects first mentally rehearsed the memorized tempo (internal), and then checked whether the beats of the new tempo (external) occurred at the times they anticipated. This is consistent with beat-based models (Schulze, 1978). In condition I3, the grand mean ERPs did not exhibit a decrease in CNVamplitude (Figure 2). This could result from less accurate encoding of the tempo, as suggested by the poorer d 0 obtained in this condition. The explanatory hypothesis we can propose is that some subjects encoded a new memory trace during the comparison phase in order to make their judgment. Laterality analysis. For the encoding phase in condition I3, the results showed that, over time, the left frontal hemisphere became more active than the right frontal hemisphere. Given that the laterality effect did not occur in condition I6, this result suggests that left frontal activity was enhanced when the task was more difficult (i.e., when the number of intervals was smaller). Ruchkin et al. (1997) showed, using a working memory task, that a left frontal slow wave occurred for auditory rehearsal. Because in condition I3 the reference interval was apparently not repeated enough to be thoroughly encoded, subjects may have used auditory rehearsal as a compensatory strategy in a way that reinforced their interval memory trace. Such a strategy may not have been necessary in condition I6 due to the larger number of intervals. CNV Amplitude: Individual Analysis Individual analyses of CNV slope calculated at electrode FCZ (where the grand mean CNV peaked) showed that subjects could be separated into two groups (Figure 3). For 11 subjects out of 15 (group G1), the CNV slope was globally positive (increase in CNV amplitude) in both conditions (I3 and I6) whereas for 4 subjects out of 15 (group G2), the CNV slope was negative (decrease in CNV amplitude) in both conditions. This group difference in the CNV pattern occurred over all frontal electrodes (Figure 4). The behavioral performance of G2 subjects was significantly better than that of G1 subjects, further supporting this subject division. Thus, subjects who failed to show an increase in CNVamplitude during the encoding phase performed better. This result corroborates numerous studies using temporal tasks that have found a higher CNVamplitude for poor than for good performers (Ladanyi & Dubrovsky, 1985) or greater prefrontal activity for incorrect than for correct responses (Casini & Macar, 1996). The same result was also found in a mental imagery task, where the authors showed that the left frontal slow wave amplitude was larger for poor than for good performers (Ro sler, Heil, & Ro der, 1997). How can one explain why a decrease in CNV amplitude was observed for G2 subjects during the encoding phase? G2 subjects may have an accurate estimation of the very first interval, which directly provided them with a memory trace of the reference interval. They would then only have to check if this trace was correct during subsequent intervals, that is, if it allowed for a good prediction about the temporal occurrence of subsequent tones. Thus, they would behave in the same way as during the comparison phase (Figure 4). Their memory trace could be maintained by a checking process. This interpretation is consistent with beat-based models. Such an encoding strategy would enhance performance, while being more economical by not requiring reliance on high-level cognitive functions such as working memory or attention. General Discussion The main goal of our study was to determine how the CNV develops during tempo encoding and how it is related to memory trace building and temporal window length (I3 versus I6). An increase in CNV amplitude was observed in both conditions, I3 and I6. This increase seems to reflect the building of a memory trace. Moreover, in condition I6, CNV amplitude stopped increasing after the third interval, suggesting that three intervals were clearly a critical limit to the building of this memory trace. This explanation is apparently discrepant with behavioral results, in which there was an enhancement of performance in condition I6 as compared to condition I3. The question is to find out how these additional intervals help subjects improve their performance, as they have already built their memory trace. During the encoding phase, G2 subjects did not exhibit any increase in CNV amplitude after the first interval. Moreover, their behavioral performance was better than that of the other subjects. Apparently, in these G2 subjects, the first time interval was sufficient for creating a memory trace. The other intervals would Figure 4. ERPs over frontal (F5, F3, F1, FZ, F2, F4, F6) and frontalcentral electrodes (FC5, FC3, FC1, FCZ, FC2, FC4, FC6), elicited during tempo encoding in the six-interval and three-interval conditions, averaged separately for G1 subjects (thick lines) and G2 subjects (thin lines).

8 76 M. Pfeuty, R. Ragot, and V. Pouthas then serve merely to check this memory. In other subjects, three intervals seemed to be necessary for performing the same operation, that is, building the memory trace (the next three intervals being used to check it). Thus, the reinforcement of the memory could be due, not only to its building, but also to further checking operations. Interval-based processes relying on attention and working memory appear to be used in the building phase, whereas beat-based processes, certainly more automatic, appear to be used in the checking phase. Conclusion This study suggests that, in tempo perception, two different processes can be differentiated on the basis of the time-course analysis of the CNV. An increase in CNV amplitude reflecting memory trace building would mainly occur during tempo encoding, and a decrease in CNV amplitude reflecting memory trace checking would mainly occur during tempo comparison. REFERENCES Casini, L., & Macar, F. (1996). Can the level of prefrontal activity provide an index of performance in humans? Neuroscience Letters, 219, Drake, C., & Botte, M. C. (1993). Tempo sensitivity in auditory sequences: Evidence for a multiple-look model. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, Gibbon, J., Malapani, C., Dale, C. L., & Gallistel, C. R. (1997). Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: Advances and challenges. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artefacts. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, Harrington, D. L., & Haaland, K. Y. (1999). Neural underpinnings of temporal processing: A review of focal lesion, pharmacological, and functional imaging research. Review Neuroscience, 10, Ivry, R. B. (1996). The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6, Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, R. E. (1995). Perception and production in temporal intervals across a range of durations: Evidence for a common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, Keele, S., Nicholetti, R., Ivry, R., & Pokorny, R. (1989). Mechanisms of perceptual timing: Beat-based or interval-based judgments? Psychological Research, 50, Ladanyi, M., & Dubrovsky, B. (1985). CNV and time estimation. International Journal of Neuroscience, 26, Macar, F., Vidal, F., & Casini, L. (1999). The supplementary motor area in motor and sensory timing: Evidence from slow brain potential changes. Experimental Brain Research, 125, McAdam, D. W. (1966). Slow potentials changes recorded from human brain during learning of a temporal interval. Psychonomic Science, 6, Panissal, N. (1998). Le traitement temporel de la duree des intervalles: Mecanisme supramodal et role de la memoire [Time processing of interval durations: Supra-modal mechanisms and the function of memory]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Paris University René Descartes. Ro sler, F., Heil, M., & Röder, B. (1997). Slow negative brain potentials as reflections of specific modular resources of cognition. Biological Psychology, 45, Ruchkin, D. S., Berndt, R. S., Johnson, R., Jr., Ritter, W., Grafman, J., & Canoune, H. L. (1997). Modality-specific streams in verbal working memory: Evidence from spatio-temporal patterns of brain activity. Cognitive Brain Research, 6, Ruchkin, D. S., McCalley, M. G., & Glaser, E. M. (1977). Event-related potentials and time estimation. Psychophysiology, 14, Schulze, H. H. (1978). The detectability of local and global displacement in regular rhythmic patterns. Psychological Research, 40, Timsit-Berthier, M. (1984). Variation contingente negative et composantes endogenes du potentiel evoque. Revue d electroencephalographie et de neurophysiologie clinique, 14, Walter, W. G., Cooper, R., Aldridge, V. J., McCallum, W. C., & Winter, A. L. (1964). Contingent negative variation: An electric sign of sensori-motor association and expectancy in the human brain. Nature, 203, (Received March 19, 2001; Accepted July 1, 2002)

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals Takako Mitsudo *1, Yoshitaka Nakajima 2, Gerard B. Remijn 3, Hiroshige Takeichi 4, Yoshinobu Goto

More information

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential Title of Thesis Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential 2014 September Yang Weiping The Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology (Doctor s Course)

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE Isabelle Guay and Simon Grondin Université Laval, Québec, Canada Email: simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca ABSTRACT

More information

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 827 831. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research BRIEF REPT Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the

More information

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Biological Psychology 66 (2004) 1 20 The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Bettina Forster, Martin Eimer School of

More information

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task Psychophysiology, 44 (2007), 913 917. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2007 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00561.x BRIEF REPORT Reward prediction

More information

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction Proceedings of the First International Conference on Complex Medical Engineering (CME2005) May 15-18, 2005, Takamatsu, Japan (Organized Session No. 20). Paper No. 150, pp. 715-719. Event-Related Potentials

More information

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS Simon Grondin, Vincent Laflamme, Nicolas Bisson, Emi Hasuo, Tsuyoshi Kuroda Université Laval, Québec, Canada simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca Abstract

More information

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University Extraversion and the LRP 1 Running head: EXTRAVERSION AND THE LRP Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized Readiness Potential Dianna Monteith Saint Thomas

More information

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition RESEARCH ARTICLE A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition Sameer Sethi 1 *, Dr. Simon Rigoulot 2, Dr. Marc D. Pell 3 1 Faculty of Science, McGill University,

More information

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE Åke Hellström Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, S-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden Email: hellst@psychology.su.se Abstract Participants compared successive

More information

EFFECTS OF RHYTHM ON THE PERCEPTION OF URGENCY AND IRRITATION IN 1 AUDITORY SIGNALS. Utrecht University

EFFECTS OF RHYTHM ON THE PERCEPTION OF URGENCY AND IRRITATION IN 1 AUDITORY SIGNALS. Utrecht University EFFECTS OF RHYTHM ON THE PERCEPTION OF URGENCY AND IRRITATION IN 1 Utrecht University Psychology Master: Applied Cognitive Psychology Effects of Rhythm on the Perception of Urgency and Irritation in Auditory

More information

DCNV RESEARCH TOOL: INVESTIGATION OF ANTICIPATORY BRAIN POTENTIALS

DCNV RESEARCH TOOL: INVESTIGATION OF ANTICIPATORY BRAIN POTENTIALS DCNV RESEARCH TOOL: INVESTIGATION OF ANTICIPATORY BRAIN POTENTIALS Roman Golubovski, Dipl-Eng J.Sandanski 116-3/24 91000 Skopje, Macedonia Phone: + 389 91 165 367, Fax: + 389 91 165 304 email: roman.golubovski@iname.com

More information

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source

More information

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures Psychophysiology, 42 (25), 361 368. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 25 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 1.1111/j.1469-8986.25.295.x BRIEF REPORT Event-related potentials

More information

EVOKED RESPONSE CORRELATES OF LETTER RECOGNITION

EVOKED RESPONSE CORRELATES OF LETTER RECOGNITION ACTA NEUROBIOL. EXIP. 1980, 40 : 1009-1015 Short communication EVOKED RESPONSE CORRELATES OF LETTER RECOGNITION I. RADILOVA, A. GRABOWSKA, T. RADIL-WEISS, L. MARAS 'and W. BUDOHOSKA Institute of Physiology,

More information

Provided by the author(s) and NUI Galway in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite the published version when available. Title Prefrontal cortex and the generation of oscillatory visual persistence

More information

What is common to brain activity evoked by the perception of visual and auditory filled durations? A study with MEG and EEG co-recordings

What is common to brain activity evoked by the perception of visual and auditory filled durations? A study with MEG and EEG co-recordings Cognitive Brain Research 21 (2004) 250 268 Research report What is common to brain activity evoked by the perception of visual and auditory filled durations? A study with MEG and EEG co-recordings Karim

More information

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change Elena Yago, MarõÂa Jose Corral and Carles Escera CA Neurodynamics Laboratory, Department

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2004 Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude Tim Skinner University of South Florida Follow this and

More information

Temporally selective attention modulates early perceptual processing: Event-related potential evidence

Temporally selective attention modulates early perceptual processing: Event-related potential evidence Perception & Psychophysics 2008, 70 (4), 732-742 doi: 10.3758/PP.70.4.732 Temporally selective attention modulates early perceptual processing: Event-related potential evidence LISA D. SANDERS AND LORI

More information

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear Pitch & Binaural listening Review 25 20 15 10 5 0-5 100 1000 10000 25 20 15 10 5 0-5 100 1000 10000 Part I: Auditory frequency selectivity Tuning

More information

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:296 301 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0353-4 BRIEF REPORT Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Risa Sawaki & Steven J. Luck Published online: 20 December 2012 #

More information

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln 1 DATA MANAGEMENT Backups Storage Identification Analyses 2 Data Analysis Pre-processing Statistical Analysis

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

Timing & Schizophrenia. Deana Davalos Colorado State University

Timing & Schizophrenia. Deana Davalos Colorado State University Timing & Schizophrenia Deana Davalos Colorado State University What is Temporal Processing? (Thank you to all for background) In everyday terms Can I cross the street without being hit by a car? Do I have

More information

Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception

Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception Sundeep Teki Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception Auditory Cognition Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging SENSORY CORTEX SMA/PRE-SMA HIPPOCAMPUS BASAL

More information

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study Armita Faghani Jadidi, Raheleh Davoodi, Mohammad Hassan Moradi Department of Biomedical Engineering Amirkabir University

More information

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Submitted to Current Biology Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Anna Plodowski, Rachel Swainson, Georgina M. Jackson, Chris Rorden and Stephen R. Jackson School of Psychology

More information

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Cognitive Brain Research 18 (2004) 255 272 Research report Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Audrey Duarte a, *, Charan Ranganath

More information

On the nature of Rhythm, Time & Memory. Sundeep Teki Auditory Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London

On the nature of Rhythm, Time & Memory. Sundeep Teki Auditory Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London On the nature of Rhythm, Time & Memory Sundeep Teki Auditory Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London Timing substrates Timing mechanisms Rhythm and Timing Unified timing

More information

Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns

Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns Perception & Psychophysics 1986, 40 (2), 69-73 Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns PETER J. ESSENS TNO Institute for Perception, Soesterberg, The Netherlands In two reproduction experiments,

More information

An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials

An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials Psychophysiology, 36 ~1999!, 22 34. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1999 Society for Psychophysiological Research An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related

More information

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing Cognitive Neuroscience 0 0 0 0 0 p Website publication November NeuroReport, () ONE of the basic properties of the auditory system is the ability to analyse complex temporal patterns. Here, we investigated

More information

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm Research paper Acta Neurobiol Exp 2008, 68: 362 372 The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm Eligiusz Wronka 1,2 *, Jan Kaiser 1, and Anton M.L. Coenen 2 1 Institute of Psychology,

More information

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search Steven Phillips (steve@ni.aist.go.jp) Neuroscience Research Institute (AIST), Tsukuba Central 2, 1-1-1 Umezono, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8568

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

Language Speech. Speech is the preferred modality for language.

Language Speech. Speech is the preferred modality for language. Language Speech Speech is the preferred modality for language. Outer ear Collects sound waves. The configuration of the outer ear serves to amplify sound, particularly at 2000-5000 Hz, a frequency range

More information

Perception and Production of Brief Durations: Beat-Based Versus Interval-Based Timing

Perception and Production of Brief Durations: Beat-Based Versus Interval-Based Timing Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2001, Vol. 27, No. 2, 485-493 Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/01/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.2.485

More information

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation Rongjun Yu a and Xiaolin Zhou a,b,c a Department of Psychology, Peking University, b State

More information

ERP Correlates of Identity Negative Priming

ERP Correlates of Identity Negative Priming ERP Correlates of Identity Negative Priming Jörg Behrendt 1,3 Henning Gibbons 4 Hecke Schrobsdorff 1,2 Matthias Ihrke 1,3 J. Michael Herrmann 1,2 Marcus Hasselhorn 1,3 1 Bernstein Center for Computational

More information

What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010

What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010 What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010 You are trying to determine if a small amplitude signal is a consistent marker of a neural process. How might you design an experiment to

More information

The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel

The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel Perception & Psychophysics 1983, 34, 331-337 The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel DIANA DEUTSCH University of California, La Jolla, California Subjects were presented with two parallel

More information

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion Perception & Psychophysics 1980.28 (5). 465-470 Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion GREGORY M. CORSO Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia 30332 A pair of pure-tone sine

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Nick Berggren and Martin Eimer Abstract During the retention of

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University; Graduate School of Economics, Osaka University syamane@kindai.ac.jp Abstract We examined

More information

The observation of contingent negative variation with reaction times in non-human. primates. Nam Soo Kim. Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty of the

The observation of contingent negative variation with reaction times in non-human. primates. Nam Soo Kim. Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty of the The observation of contingent negative variation with reaction times in non-human primates By Nam Soo Kim Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Vanderbilt University in Partial fulfillment

More information

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task? Bachelor s Thesis Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a sequential movement task? Nils Hatger Prof. Dr. Ing. Willem Verwey Jonathan Barnhoorn, PhD student BMS Faculty/Cognitive

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shikakeology: Designing Triggers for Behavior Change: Papers from the 2013 AAAI Spring Symposium The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University;

More information

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Hearing-aids Induce Plasticity in the Auditory System: Perspectives From Three Research Designs and Personal Speculations About the

More information

Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials

Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials Ángel Correa,

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Perceptual Organization of Complex Auditory Sequences: Effect of Number of Simultaneous Subsequences and Frequency Separation

Perceptual Organization of Complex Auditory Sequences: Effect of Number of Simultaneous Subsequences and Frequency Separation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 999, Vol. 5, No. 6,7-759 Copyright 999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 96-5/99/S. Perceptual Organization of Complex

More information

The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time

The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School January 2013 The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time Glen Robert Forester University of South

More information

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific?

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Temporal Binding and Internal Clocks: Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Richard Fereday (FeredayR@cardiff.ac.uk), Marc J. Buehner (BuehnerM@cardiff.ac.uk) Cardiff University, School of Psychology,

More information

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry International Journal of Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities Volume 5 Article 3 June 2013 Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry Whitney R. Arthur Central Washington

More information

Observations of the CNV during a simple auditory task

Observations of the CNV during a simple auditory task Physiological Psychology 1976, Vol. 4 (4),451-456 Observations of the CNV during a simple auditory task HENRY J. MICHALEWSKI and HAROLD WEINBERG Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society Event-Related Potentials Reveal Perceptual Simulation of Color in Language Comprehension Aitao Lu (Center for Studies

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 5: Data analysis II Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis II 6 Single

More information

Auditory Scene Analysis

Auditory Scene Analysis 1 Auditory Scene Analysis Albert S. Bregman Department of Psychology McGill University 1205 Docteur Penfield Avenue Montreal, QC Canada H3A 1B1 E-mail: bregman@hebb.psych.mcgill.ca To appear in N.J. Smelzer

More information

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Running Head: Visual Working Memory and the CDA Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Nick Berggren and

More information

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics Scott Makeig Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla CA sccn.ucsd.edu Talk given at the EEG/MEG course

More information

9/29/14. Amanda M. Lauer, Dept. of Otolaryngology- HNS. From Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics, Green & Swets (1966)

9/29/14. Amanda M. Lauer, Dept. of Otolaryngology- HNS. From Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics, Green & Swets (1966) Amanda M. Lauer, Dept. of Otolaryngology- HNS From Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics, Green & Swets (1966) SIGNAL D sensitivity index d =Z hit - Z fa Present Absent RESPONSE Yes HIT FALSE ALARM

More information

Beat perception in 3D: A comparative analysis between sight, sound, and touch

Beat perception in 3D: A comparative analysis between sight, sound, and touch Beat perception in 3D: A comparative analysis between sight, sound, and touch Heather Khey Beldman (Dr. Jessica A. Grahn) The Brain and Mind Institute, Western University Physiology 4980E Honors Thesis

More information

Combined effects of alcohol and caffeine on the late components of the event-related potential and on reaction time. Introduction.

Combined effects of alcohol and caffeine on the late components of the event-related potential and on reaction time. Introduction. P100 P200 P300 Combined effects of alcohol and caffeine on the late components of the event-related potential and on reaction time N100 N200 Late components Frances Heritage Martin Joshua Garfield 1 Biological

More information

Dual Mechanisms for the Cross-Sensory Spread of Attention: How Much Do Learned Associations Matter?

Dual Mechanisms for the Cross-Sensory Spread of Attention: How Much Do Learned Associations Matter? Cerebral Cortex January 2010;20:109--120 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp083 Advance Access publication April 24, 2009 Dual Mechanisms for the Cross-Sensory Spread of Attention: How Much Do Learned Associations

More information

False Memory: P300 Amplitude, Topography, and Latency. Antoinette R. Miller, Christopher Baratta, Christine Wynveen, and J.

False Memory: P300 Amplitude, Topography, and Latency. Antoinette R. Miller, Christopher Baratta, Christine Wynveen, and J. 1 False Memory: P300 Amplitude, Topography, and Latency Antoinette R. Miller, Christopher Baratta, Christine Wynveen, and J. Peter Rosenfeld Northwestern University Address correspondence to: J. Peter

More information

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction Alexandra Muller-Gass, Erich

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Introduction

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Introduction YNIMG-04114; No. of pages: 14; 4C: 9 model 5 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage xx (2006) xxx xxx Investigating the functional interaction between semantic and episodic memory: Convergent behavioral

More information

The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex

The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex Nelli H. Salminen and Hannu Tiitinen Department of Biomedical Engineering and Computational Science, Helsinki University of Technology,

More information

Readiness Potentials Related to Self-Initiated Movement and to Movement Preceded by Time Estimation: A Comparison

Readiness Potentials Related to Self-Initiated Movement and to Movement Preceded by Time Estimation: A Comparison Physiol. Res. 45:235-239, 1996 Readiness Potentials Related to Self-Initiated Movement and to Movement Preceded by Time Estimation: A Comparison M. KUKLETA, P. BUSER1,1. REKTOR, M. LAMARCHE1 M edical Faculty,

More information

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition Chair: Jason Samaha, University of Wisconsin-Madison Co-Chair: Ali Mazaheri, University of Birmingham

More information

Rhythm Categorization in Context. Edward W. Large

Rhythm Categorization in Context. Edward W. Large Rhythm Categorization in Context Edward W. Large Center for Complex Systems Florida Atlantic University 777 Glades Road, P.O. Box 39 Boca Raton, FL 3343-99 USA large@walt.ccs.fau.edu Keywords: Rhythm,

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Teaching brain-machine interfaces as an alternative paradigm to neuroprosthetics control

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Teaching brain-machine interfaces as an alternative paradigm to neuroprosthetics control SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Teaching brain-machine interfaces as an alternative paradigm to neuroprosthetics control Authors: Iñaki Iturrate 1,2, Ricardo Chavarriaga 2, Luis Montesano 1, Javier Minguez 1,

More information

Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli

Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli Neuroscience Letters 424 (2007) 55 60 Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli Udo Will, Eric Berg School of Music, Cognitive Ethnomusicology, Ohio State University, 110

More information

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials Chinese Science Bulletin 2007 Science in China Press Springer-Verlag Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials SONG Yan 1, PENG

More information

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

HCS 7367 Speech Perception Long-term spectrum of speech HCS 7367 Speech Perception Connected speech Absolute threshold Males Dr. Peter Assmann Fall 212 Females Long-term spectrum of speech Vowels Males Females 2) Absolute threshold

More information

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2008, 8 (2), 195-210 doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.2.195 The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies MIKA

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Forsyth et al. 10.1073/pnas.1509262112 SI Methods Inclusion Criteria. Participants were eligible for the study if they were between 18 and 30 y of age; were comfortable reading in

More information

The Mechanism of Valence-Space Metaphors: ERP Evidence for Affective Word Processing

The Mechanism of Valence-Space Metaphors: ERP Evidence for Affective Word Processing : ERP Evidence for Affective Word Processing Jiushu Xie, Ruiming Wang*, Song Chang Center for Studies of Psychological Application, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, Guangdong

More information

ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: Direct versus indirect memory tasks

ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: Direct versus indirect memory tasks available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: Direct versus indirect memory tasks Michael Hornberger a, Michael D. Rugg b,

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Top-down control of audiovisual search by bimodal search templates

Top-down control of audiovisual search by bimodal search templates Psychophysiology, 50 (013), 996 1009. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright 013 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.1086 Top-down control of audiovisual search by

More information

Working with EEG/ERP data. Sara Bögels Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics

Working with EEG/ERP data. Sara Bögels Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics Working with EEG/ERP data Sara Bögels Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics Overview Methods Stimuli (cross-splicing) Task Electrode configuration Artifacts avoidance Pre-processing Filtering Time-locking

More information

Psychology Perception

Psychology Perception Psychology 343 - Perception James R. Sawusch, 360 Park Hall jsawusch@buffalo.edu 645-0238 TA is Timothy Pruitt, 312 Park tapruitt@buffalo.edu Text is Sensation & Perception by Goldstein (8th edition) PSY

More information

Categorical Perception

Categorical Perception Categorical Perception Discrimination for some speech contrasts is poor within phonetic categories and good between categories. Unusual, not found for most perceptual contrasts. Influenced by task, expectations,

More information

Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach

Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach PQJE178995 TECHSET COMPOSITION LTD, SALISBURY, U.K. 6/16/2006 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 0000, 00 (0), 1 17 Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection

More information

ERP Feature of Vigilance. Zhendong Mu

ERP Feature of Vigilance. Zhendong Mu 5th International Conference on Education, Management, Information and Medicine (EMIM 205) ERP Feature of Vigilance Zhendong Mu Institute of Information Technology, Jiangxi University of Technology, Nanchang,

More information

The error-related potential and BCIs

The error-related potential and BCIs The error-related potential and BCIs Sandra Rousseau 1, Christian Jutten 2 and Marco Congedo 2 Gipsa-lab - DIS 11 rue des Mathématiques BP 46 38402 Saint Martin d Héres Cedex - France Abstract. The error-related

More information

Loudness Processing of Time-Varying Sounds: Recent advances in psychophysics and challenges for future research

Loudness Processing of Time-Varying Sounds: Recent advances in psychophysics and challenges for future research Loudness Processing of Time-Varying Sounds: Recent advances in psychophysics and challenges for future research Emmanuel PONSOT 1 ; Patrick SUSINI 1 ; Sabine MEUNIER 2 1 STMS lab (Ircam, CNRS, UPMC), 1

More information

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Rui Cao (caorui.beilia@gmail.com) Thomas A. Busey (busey@indiana.edu) Robert M. Nosofsky (nosofsky@indiana.edu) Richard

More information

Relative contribution of expectancy and immediate arousal to the facilitatory effect of an auditory accessory stimulus

Relative contribution of expectancy and immediate arousal to the facilitatory effect of an auditory accessory stimulus Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research (2004) 37: 1161-1174 Priming by an auditory stimulus ISSN 0100-879X 1161 Relative contribution of expectancy and immediate arousal to the facilitatory

More information

In what way does the parietal ERP old new effect index recollection?

In what way does the parietal ERP old new effect index recollection? Ž. International Journal of Psychophysiology 35 2000 81 87 In what way does the parietal ERP old new effect index recollection? Edward L. Wilding School of Psychology, Cardiff Uni ersity, Cardiff, CF10

More information