Adult Interpretations of Communicative Behavior in Learners with Rett Syndrome

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Adult Interpretations of Communicative Behavior in Learners with Rett Syndrome"

Transcription

1 DOI /s z ORIGINAL ARTICLE Adult Interpretations of Communicative Behavior in Learners with Rett Syndrome Hannah M. Julien & Quannah Parker-McGowan & Breanne J. Byiers & Joe Reichle # Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014 Abstract This study examined adults interpretations of the communicative functions of potential communicative acts as defined by Sigafoos, et al. (2009) produced by individuals with Rett Syndrome (RTT). Video recordings of three learners with RTT engaged in daily routines were parsed into 5-second clips. Each clip demonstrated a potential communicative act. The primary caregivers and educational professionals who served each of the learners viewed the video clips and rated the communicative function of each behavior, if any. Analyses examined adults judgments as a function of their familiarity with the learner as well as their relative familiarity and experience in interpreting limited communicative repertoires (i.e., parents compared to special education professionals). The majority of special education professionals and parents interpret potential communicative behavior to represent communication with a clear intention. The three functions most commonly identified by raters were requesting object, commenting and protesting. There was modest variability in these ratings across participants and between professionals and parents. Variability in the interpretation of potential communicative acts between raters highlights the need for future studies with this population. Implications for future applied research along with educational implications are discussed. Keywords Rett syndrome. Communication Introduction Individuals with developmental disabilities may demonstrate delays in developing intentional communication (Johnston, Reichle, Feeley, and Jones 2012). Bates, H. M. Julien (*): Q. Parker-McGowan : B. J. Byiers : J. Reichle University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, 115 Shevlin Hall, 164 Pillsbury Drive SE, Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA julie006@umn.edu

2 Camaioni, and Volterra (1975) described the gradual progression of a typically developing child from unintentional communication to intentional communication. From birth until approximately 9 months of age, a typically developing child demonstrates preintentional communication, also known as the perlocutionary stage, in which the child s vocalizations have an unintended impact on the listener. Around 9 months of age, a child begins the transition to the locutionary stage by purposefully using vocalizations and gestures in order to impact the listener. By 13 months of age, a child communicates intentionally through the use of referential words, vocalizations and gestures (Wetherby, Cain, Yonclas, and Walker 1988). Intentional communication is present when an individual develops awareness a priori of the effect that a signal will have on [the] listener, and [develops persistence] in that behavior until the effect is obtained or failure is clearly indicated (Bates et al. 1979, p. 36). Bates description of intentional communication mentions two important indices: 1) the child sawarenessof the communicative signal s effect and 2) the child s ability to persist in signaling the communicative behavior. For persons with delayed intentional communication skills, having a wide range of social partners who differently interpret (and respond to) pre-intentional communicative behaviors may place them at a disadvantage in receiving consistent reinforcement for their spoken, or gestural behavior (Meadan, Halle, and Kelly 2012). In a previous study, Meadan et al. (2012) examined adults interpretations of the requesting, rejecting and unclear behavior of three preschool age children with limited expressive language and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Adult participants were categorized according to two primary criteria: familiar/unfamiliar and expert/non-expert. The participants were presented with short video clips that had been extracted from longer recordings made during a structured assessment designed to elicit different communicative functions. The participants judged three specific components of each clip: (1) whether or not the child was communicating, (2) whether or not the child s intention was clear, (3) what the child was communicating. The participants also indicated how confident they were in their judgments. Meadan et al. (2012) reported that familiar expert (defined as having formal knowledge about communication and language development, [p. 441]) adults were more accurate in their ratings than non-expert and unfamiliar adults. They also found evidence of greater variability in adults interpretations of rejecting behavior compared to requesting behavior. These findings underscore the importance of collaboration among social partners of individuals with limited expressive communication to provide consistent responses to (potentially) unconventional bids for communication, thus creating a more advantageous communicative interaction for the learner (Meadan et al. 2012). Rett syndrome (RTT) is a neurodevelopmental disorder and is one of the most common causes of profound disability in girls (Ellaway and Christodoulou 2001), as recent estimates place the prevalence between 0.5 and 2 per 10,000 females (Fehr, Bebbington, Nassar, Downs, Ronen, De Klerk, and Leonard 2011; Laurvick et al. 2006). Mutation in the methyl CpG binding protein 2 (MECP2) accounts for % of individuals diagnosed with typical, or classic RTT (Neul et al. 2008). In atypical RTT cases, % have been found to have mutations in MECP2. Necessary criteria for a diagnosis of RTT include regression, loss of purposeful hand motions, and spoken language beginning between 6 and 18 months of age, and the emergence of hand stereotypies and gait abnormalities (Neul et al. 2010).

3 Hagberg (1995; 2002) described the four progressive stages of RTT development. Typically, the first stage, known as the stagnation stage, begins between 6 and 18 months of age and is marked developmental delay and deceleration of head growth. Marschik et al. (2013) found that in terms of communication development, individuals with RTT exhibited atypical speech development as early as cooing and babbling milestones. Stage II, the developmental regression stage, typically begins between 1 and 3 years of age and lasts between a few weeks to a year. This stage is characterized by regression in communication, language, and fine motor skills. Individuals with RTT will, in this stage, lose any spoken language previously acquired. Some individuals may retain some words while others will have never acquired any spoken language. The variability in the communication abilities of these individuals depends upon the specific genetic mutations, use of communication device(s), and the implementation of communication devices (Urbanowicz, Leonard, Girdler, Ciccone, and Downs 2014). The psuedostationary stage, stage III, occurs when some communicative behaviors reappear while the characteristic stereotypical hand movements begin. The final stage, the later motor deterioration period, most often begins around age 10 and is characterized by the further deterioration of mobility. It is in this stage that most individuals become dependent on wheelchairs. The majority of individuals with both typical and atypical RTT demonstrate very limited expressive communication skills as well as extremely limited functional hand use (Hagberg et al. 2002; Hagberg and Witt-Engerstrom 1986). Much of the evidence related to communication skills in this population is based on both naturalistic and structured observations (see Sigafoos, et al. 2011, for a review) given that individuals with RTT are often unable to participate in the standardized protocols used to evaluate communicative comprehension and production (Demeter 2000). Sigafoos et al. (2011) systematically reviewed studies that examined the potential communicative behaviors of individuals with RTT. All of the eight studies that were identified and included in their review utilized direct observations of behavior as a means of assessment; half of these studies also included standardized assessments and structured interviews as a means to determine communicative form and function. The authors cautioned that the validity and reliability of these three forms of assessment (direct observation, standardized assessment, and interviews) has not yet been well established for this population. Given this, Sigafoos et al. (2011) concluded that based on the available evidence (e.g. Baptista et al. 2006; Brady and Halle 1997; Dahlgren Sandberg et al. 2000; Garber and Veydt 1990; Hetzroni and Rubin 2006; Ryan et al. 2004; Sigafoos et al. 2000a; Woodyatt and Ozanne 1997) individuals with RTT demonstrate a range of behaviors (e.g., shifting eye gaze, vocalizations, stereotyped hand movements, facial expressions) that may be either intentionally communicative, may be sensitive to changes in environmental consequences, or may be responses to changes in internal physiological states. Much of the evidence related to productive communication skills in individuals with RTT focuses on establishing the function of specific behaviors and the presence of intentionality. However, there remains very little information related to comprehension skills. In some reports, some parents indicate that their children with RTT can understand words or short sentences (von Tetzchner 1997) and may follow simple commands (Demeter 2000). On the other hand, Woodyatt and Ozanne (1992) described the comprehension skills of a small cohort of girls with RTT as severely affected (p.

4 165). They went on to elaborate that at least one of the individuals in their cohort did not respond to any environmental or speech sounds. These investigators reported inconsistent responses to phrases like no and the learner s first name being spoken. The skill heterogeneity found across studies could represent the wide range of skills across the population, limitations in assessment valid with this population, or a combination of both of these explanations. In summary, there remain many unanswered empirical questions related to both expressive and receptive communication skills of individuals with RTT. Given the limited spoken and gestural skills, along with the potential production of somewhat idiosyncratic communicative acts among individuals with RTT, it seems plausible to hypothesize that their social partners may differ in their interpretations of whether a child s actions serve as intentional communicative acts. With communicative acts that are intentional, a second area of interpretation is determining the intended purpose (i.e., communicative function) of the communicative act. There is some evidence that parents and caregivers of individuals with RTT are able to consistently identify potential communicative forms but show inter-observer variation in their interpretation of the function (Sigafoos et al. 2000b). This finding is consistent with evidence suggesting that social partners demonstrate inter-observer variation in their interpretations of communicative behavior in both children with (e.g., Yoder 1986) and without disabilities (e.g., Adamson, Bakerman, Smith, and Walters 1987). The extent to which parents attribute intentionality to communicative behavior does not seem to be affected by the severity of their child s disability (Yoder and Feagans 1988). However, parents may identify more communicative behaviors as intentional compared to non-parents (Adamson et al. 1987). Moreover, investigators have observed variation in the interpretations made between members of allied intervention disciplines (e.g., special education professionals versus speech and language pathologists; Wilcox et al. 1990) as well as within each of those groups (Carter and Iacono 2002). This study examined adult interpretations of the communicative intent of potential communicative behaviors of learners with RTT, a population for whom this type of evidence does not presently exist in the literature. A better understanding of how different social partners interpret potential communicative behavior furthers our ability to design and reliably implement communication interventions for individuals with RTT, a task that may be more difficult given their unconventional and limited communication skills (Sigafoos et al. 2009). Meadan et al. (2012) noted the importance of replicating their study with additional children, different communication functions (i.e., commenting) and particularly with samples of communicative behavior drawn from natural communication settings instead of structured elicitation contexts. As such, this study aims to partially replicate Meadan et al. (2012) with a particular focus on potential communicative acts produced by individuals with RTT in natural communication contexts. The two research questions are: (a) How do different adult social partners, specifically, special education professionals and parents, interpret the behavior of learners with RTT? (b) Are the adults interpretations related to their familiarity with learner or level of expertise? Similar to Meadan et al. (2012), we also examined the raters confidence in their interpretations.

5 Method Procedural Overview All procedures were approved by the University s Institutional Review Board prior to the beginning of the study. After obtaining informed consent from the learner s guardian, parents independently completed portions of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (M-CDI) (Fenson et al. 1993), and the researchers administered sections of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales Second Edition (Vineland-II) (Sparrow, Cicchetti, and Balla 2005). Because it was expected that all or most of the participants would score at or near the floor level on these standardized instruments, standard scores were not calculated. Rather, the measures were used to gather descriptive information about each participant s adaptive and communicative behaviors. Finally, parents completed a semi-structured interview regarding the learner s current communication skills and participation in daily routines, based, in part, on the Inventory of Potential Communicative Acts (Sigafoos et al. 2006). During the second visit, the researchers recorded the leaner engaging in several daily routines (details in subsequent section). The recordings were analyzed to identify short clips of potential communicative behavior produced by the learner (detailed below). Then, the recordings were spliced into short (5 8 s) segments such that a naïve observer would be able to see the learner engaged in the potential communicative behavior but would not be able to observe how the learner s social partner responded to her. The adult participants (i.e., raters ) were informed that they would be participating in a study that examined the communication of learners with RTT. After providing informed consent, they met with one of the researchers and participated in a structured interview and rating session in which they interpreted the short video clips. Participants This study included three females with RTT with uncertain receptive language skills and less than five expressive words, as reported by primary caregiver. Fourteen adult participants also participated. Jane Age 8, lived with her mother, father and three typically developing siblings. She had a diagnosis of classic/typical RTT and a confirmed MECP2 mutation (c.391dupa). She was able to walk with assistance. Jane consistently was able to eat solid food but was unable to drink liquids independently, however, she did not use a gastric feeding tube (g-tube). Her mother reported that her medical history was positive for seizure activity but that it was currently controlled with medication. Her mother reported that at the time of the study, Jane responded to her spoken name and understood short phrases such as Are you tired? but did not use any formal communication. Her mother reported that she reacted to loud noises and turned her head towards environmental sounds. Additionally, her mother had observed that Jane would listen to a story for at least 5 min. Jane s mother also reported that she almost always vocalized pleasure and smiled when spoken to by a caregiver. It was unclear whether this behavior was also present with less familiar social partners.

6 Sarah Age 14, lived with her mother and father. She had a diagnosis of classic/typical RTT, although genetic testing did not identify specific type of MECP2 mutation. Her medical history was positive for seizure activity, although during the video recording, seizures were not observed. Sarah consistently ate solid foods but relied on a bottle and g-tube tube to ingest liquids and additional nutrition. Her mother reported that at home, Sarah often reacted to loud noises and sometimes turned her head toward a sound. Additionally, she sometimes smiled when she heard the spoken word yes. She understood simple phrases that include Are you hungry? but did not use any spoken words, sign approximations, or graphic symbols. Judy Age 7, lived with her mother and father and two typically developing siblings. She had a diagnosis of classic/typical RTT and a confirmed MECP2 mutation (R168X). Her mother reported the absence of seizure activity at the time of the study. Judy retained the ability to eat solid food and consistently drink from a straw. She did not use a g-tube. Her mother reported Judy responded when her name was called, produced no, no, and produced some sound effects such as baa-baa for a sheep when shown pictures and asked to make the sound. Her mother also reported that when asked, Judy could point to common objects and body parts. Judy s mother reported that she inconsistently followed instructions involving two contingent steps (e.g., When you are done chewing your food, look at me ). She had access to a speech-generating device (Tobii C12 Communication Device with Eye Control) for approximately 2.5 years prior to the start of the study. The Tobii allowed Judy to directly select buttons using eye gaze from a single home screen that displays 20 symbols. The symbols included line drawings, real photos, and short videos. Adult Participants Fourteen adults ( raters ) also participated in the study. At least one parent of each of each of the learners participated. In addition, parents were asked to identify educators or other professionals who had worked closely with their child in the past year. All of the adult participants completed a 15-min structured interview designed to obtain general demographic information, information about the rater s contact time with the learner, and information about their knowledge of communication. Table 5 in Appendix provides the structured interview questions. Subsequently, the participants viewed video clips of each of the learners and completed forms indicating their interpretations of the potential communicative acts depicted. Table 1 outlines the professional designations of the adult participants and shows their relationship to the individuals with RTT. Inclusion criteria for educators/professionals included at least 2 years of experience working as a special education professional and provision of direct services to one of the learners with RTT for at least 2 months during the preceding school year. All of the special education professionals were female. The majority had worked in their respective fields for more than 12 years (M =17.22, SD =12.01) and displayed a wide range of experience with individuals with significant developmental disabilities. All of the educators (including assistants and therapy disciplines) reported experience with learners who used both high and low-tech augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) systems. The caregivers that participated included: Jane s mother, Sarah s

7 Table 1 Adult participants and relationship to individual with RTT Individuals with RTT Caregiver (n =4) Special Education Professionals (n =10) Jane mother Special Education Teacher (1) Paraprofessionals (2) Speech-Language Pathologist (1) Sarah mother Special Education Teacher (1) Paraprofessional (1) Speech-Language Pathologist (1) Occupational Therapist (1) Physical Therapist (1) Judy mother Paraprofessional (1) father mother and Judy s mother and father. They identified themselves as primary caregivers for their daughters. The raters were categorized by four variables: as familiar if they indicated they knew the learner whose behaviors they were rating, as unfamiliar if they indicated they did not know the learner, as professional if they were a special education professional and as parent if they were a caregiver to an individual with RTT. This created four subgroups: familiar professional, unfamiliar professional, familiar parent, unfamiliar parent. Procedures Preparing video samples of potential communicative acts During the first visit to the participant s home, the researchers identified daily routines in which the learner was reported by the consenting caregiver to engage in potential communicative acts. Jane s mother reported an afterschool mealtime, a transition to a TV activity in another room, and watching a preferred TV program as viable daily routines. Jane s mother was a potential communicative partner during these routines as she was in close proximity to Jane and was engaged in the routines with her. Sarah s mother identified an afterschool mealtime and dialogic book reading while rocking Sarah in a chair as familiar routines in which her daughter may demonstrate communicative behavior. Sarah s mother was the potential communicative partner during these routines. Judy s parents reported that watching a preferred TV program and eating a snack on the couch were daily routines in which she was likely to engage in potential communicative acts. During these routines, Judy s preschool-age brother and mother were potential communicative partners. During a second visit, the researcher observed and videotaped the learner and her communicative partners engaging in the routines, as described above. These sessions lasted approximately 75 min, with approximately 80 % of that time devoted to recording the learner and her communicative partner(s). From the recordings, five instances of the learner producing potential communicative acts were selected. These behaviors represented discrete behaviors that the learner

8 had independently produced. The behaviors were initially identified according to examples of communicative potential communicative acts outlined by Sigafoos et al. (2000a). The researcher operating the video recorder identified potential communicative functions of learner s behaviors (five for each learner) that had been selected. For each learner, the instances of potential communicative behavior represented at least one potential comment, one potential request, and one potential protest. For example, a potential request was captured as Jane s mother is cutting pieces of a preferred food item and feeding them to her. Jane s mother pauses to say something to the researcher who is also in the room. Jane directs her gaze toward her mother, then toward the food, then back toward her mother. An instance of a potential request was captured when Sarah s mother is feeding her. Sarah sits across from her mother who is giving her a spoonful of food. Her mother pauses, shifting her attention away from Sarah. Sarah turns her head and shifts her gaze toward the plate of food. A potential comment was captured when Judy is watching television. Judy is sitting on the couch, an audible change occurs on the television (outside of the camera shot). Judy smiles, starts to rock quickly, move her arms, and looks toward the camera (and the researcher off-camera). Reliability The behaviors that were identified were partitioned into 5 8 s clips, which limited the contextual information that could bias the raters, most importantly, the verbal and nonverbal behavior of the adult following the child s potential communicative behavior. The context that was shown provided information related to the daily routine in which the child was engaged (e.g., audible television noise, visible food and utensils, visible book), but did not show the communicative partner s response to any of the potential communicative behaviors. To establish reliability, a second researcher (a doctoral student and speech-language pathologist) viewed the video clips and identified potential functions of the learners behaviors. Agreement on the potential function, calculated by (number of instances of agreement total opportunities 100) was 100 %. Obtaining video ratings from social partners One of three researchers met individually with each rater at a convenient location (e.g., school, home) and time. The meeting locations provided privacy in order to protect the learners and raters identities. After the structured interview (see above for more detail), the researcher administered the video clip rating instructions (adapted from Meadan et al. 2012): You will be viewing short video clips of an individual with RTT. These clips were extracted from longer recordings of the learner engaging in routine activities in her home setting. You may be familiar with one of the learners. I will play each video clip once. After each clip, I will ask you a series of questions. The questions are related to the child s behavior shown in the clip. The rater sat facing a MacBook Pro 13 laptop computer. The five potential communicative behaviors for each learner were presented as a set; the sets were presented in a counterbalanced order across raters. The researcher reminded the rater that he/she would watch each video clip and then respond to the series of questions. The researcher

9 recorded their responses and verified with the rater regarding his/her response. The raters responded to these questions: (a) Was the child communicating? (b) Was the child s intention clear? (c) What was the child communicating? (choices available were: Request object, Request action, Protest or Reject, Request Comfort, Greeting, Comment, Other) (d) For each of the preceding questions informants were asked, How confident are you with your response? This was indexed on a scale from 1 to 6 with 0 anchored with the words, not confident, 3 4 anchored with confident and 6 representing very confident. All 14 raters responded to five video clips for each of the three learners (yielding 210 total ratings). That is, they rated the communicative behavior of both learners with whom they had contact time and learners with whom they had no contact time. For the purposes of data analysis, some of the options regarding communicative function were collapsed into broader categories. This resulted in four general categories: rejections/ protests, requests (for action, object, or comfort), comments (comment or greeting), and unclear (not communicating or communication but with unclear intent). Results There were no clear, qualitative differences with respect to special education professionals and parents definitions of communication. Generally, raters defined communication as a critical ability that involves an exchange of verbal and/or nonverbal symbols and occurs between at least two people. When judging the video clips, the raters indicated that the learners were communicating in 89 % (187/210) of the clips. For these potential communicative behaviors, the raters indicated that the learners intent was clear in 85 % (159/187) of the opportunities. A variety of communicative functions were reported for each participant, with requesting object, commenting and protesting most frequently observed. Table 2 represents the percent of total rating opportunities that were assigned to each of the general communicative function categories across the different adult rater groups. On average, familiar parents rated more of the video clips as unclear - meaning that there was no communicative act, or that the function was unclear - compared to the other rater groups. Both familiar and unfamiliar professionals rated more clips as representing comments than either parent group. Familiar raters (parents and professionals) rated more clips as representing requests compared to unfamiliar raters. The percentage of clips identified as representing protests did not differ much across rater groups. Table 3 shows the frequency of agreement regarding the identified function within and across adult rater groups for each of the children. On average, agreement was relatively poor, ranging from 5 to 87 %. With the exception of unfamiliar parents rating Sarah s behaviors, agreement within groups of similar raters was, overall, no better than between groups. These results cannot be extended to ratings within familiar parents, however, as only one pair of familiar parents participated in the study. Comparisons between the functions identified by the adult rater groups and the research team suggest similarly limited agreement, for the functions of commenting and protesting. Table 4 shows the percent of agreement between adult raters and the research team. Interestingly, familiar professionals and unfamiliar parents showed strong agreement with the research team regarding request behaviors, but familiar

10 Table 2 Percent of total ratings assigned to communicative functions across adult raters subgroups Communicative function Familiar Unfamiliar Total Professional Parent Professional Parent Comment Jane 40 % (8/20) 20 % (1/5) 27 % (8/30) 20 % (3/15) 29 % (20/70) Sarah 12 % (3/25) 0 % (0/5) 16 % (4/25) 0 % (0/15) 10 % (7/70) Judy 20 % (1/5) 10 % (1/10) 33 % (15/45) 10 % (1/10) 26 % (18/70) Total 24 % (12/50) 10 % (2/20) 17 % (17/100) 10 % (4/40) 17 % (35/210) Reject/protest Jane 15 % (3/20) 0 % (0/5) 30 % (6/30) 13 % (2/15) 16 % (11/70) Sarah 16 % (4/25) 20 % (1/5) 12 % (3/25) 13 % (2/15) 14 % (10/70) Judy 20 %(1/5) 20 % (2/10) 29 % (13/45) 0 % (0/10) 23 % (16/70) Total 16 % (8/50) 15 % (3/20) 22 % (22/100) 10 % (4/40) 18 % (37/210) Request Jane 40 % (8/20) 40 % (2/5) 33 % (10/30) 33 % (5/15) 36 % (25/70) Sarah 32 % (8/25) 40 % (2/5) 36 % (9/25) 40 % (6/15) 36 % (25/70) Judy 60 % (3/5) 40 % (4/10) 20 % (9/45) 10 % (1/10) 24 % (17/70) Total 38 % (19/50) 40 % (8/20) 28 % (28/100) 30 % (12/40) 32 % (67/210) Unclear Jane 15 % (3/20) 40 % (2/5) 10 % (3/30) 20 % (3/15) 16 % (11/70) Sarah 28 % (7/25) 40 % (2/5) 32 % (8/25) 20 % (3/15) 29 % (20/70) Judy 0 % (0/5) 30 % (3/10) 13 % (6/45) 60 % (6/10) 21 % (15/70) Total 20 % (10/50) 35 % (7/20) 17 % (17/100) 30 % (12/40) 21 % (46/210) Functions identified as other not counted towards in any of the categories listed parents did not. Familiar professionals also showed the highest agreement for commenting behaviors, with familiar parents again showing the lowest agreement. Finally, familiar and unfamiliar professionals showed slightly higher agreement regarding rejecting/protesting behavior compared to familiar and unfamiliar parents. Figure 1 provides average confidence ratings across each of the adult rater groups for all three learners. Overall, average confidence was relatively high and there were no consistent differences between rater groups. Discussion This study examined adults interpretations of potential communicative acts produced by three individuals with RTT. The findings indicate that despite limited behavioral repertoires and often, vague communicative signals, special education professionals and parents interpret potential communicative acts as having clear intentions and representing a range of communicative functions. There was modest variability in how familiar and unfamiliar professionals and parents judged the communicative functions of potential communicative acts when viewing short clips of learners

11 Table 3 Matrix of inter-rater agreement (agreements /[agreements+disagreements]) of communicative functions within and across adult rater groups Participant/Rater group Familiar parent Familiar professional Unfamiliar parent Jane Familiar parent N/A Familiar professional 35 % (7/20) 47 % (14/30) Unfamiliar parent 40 % (6/15) 47 % (28/60) 47 % (7/15) Unfamiliar professional 30 % (9/30) 51 % (69/120) 59 % (53/90) Sarah Familiar parent N/A Familiar professional 32 % (8/25) 30 % (15/50) Unfamiliar parent 53 % (8/15) 44 % (23/75) 87 % (13/15) Unfamiliar professional 48 % (12/25) 33 % (41/125) 63 % (47/75) Judy Familiar parent 60 % (3/5) Familiar professional 20 % (2/10) N/A Unfamiliar parent 5 % (1/20) 20 % (2/10) 40 % (2/5) Unfamiliar professional 24 % (22/90) 49 % (22/45) 20 % (18/90) Total across participants Familiar parent N/A Familiar professional 31 % (17/55) 36 % (29/80) Unfamiliar parent 30 % (15/50) 37 % (53/145) 49 % (22/35) Unfamiliar professional 30 % (43/145) 46 % (132/290) 46 % (118/225) Numbers in parentheses indicate agreements/total opportunities. N/A denotes agreement was not calculated due to only 1 rater in the group behaviors during daily routines, despite overall high levels of confidence reported by most groups. Overall, these results support the hypothesis that different communicative partners are likely to interpret and respond to the potential communicative behaviors exhibited individuals with RTT in different ways. This present study partially supports the findings of Meadan et al. (2012). First, Meadan et al. (2012) found no qualitative differences across the four groups of adult raters, either in how they defined communication or how they described how children communicate. Similarly, raters in our Table 4 Percent of agreement in identified communication functions between adult raters and research team Function Familiar Unfamiliar Parent Professional Parent Professional Comments 25 % (2/8) 42 % (8/19) 31 % (5/16) 32 % (13/41) Protests 40 % (2/5) 47 % (7/15) 36 % (4/11) 48 % (12/25) Requests 43 % (3/7) 75 % (12/16) 86 % (11/13) 56 % (19/34) Numbers in parentheses indicate agreements/total opportunities

12 Parent Familiar Expert Familiar Expert Unfamiliar Parent Unfamiliar 1 0 Jane Sarah Judy Fig. 1 Average confidence levels across potential communicative acts for each learner; not confident =0 1, confident =3 4, very confident =5 6 study did not show qualitative differences in their definition of communication and description of how children communicate. Second, Meadan et al. (2012) demonstrated that raters who were both familiar and expert were more confident and accurate in their ratings than those who were unfamiliar and non-expert. In the present study, there were no clear differences in confidence between groups. Findings reported by Meadan et al. (2012) suggested greater variability in accuracy across raters in their judgments of rejecting behavior compared to requesting behavior. The present study was less clear regarding accuracy of judgments of the function of behaviors, but found that familiar professionals and unfamiliar parents showed strong agreement with the research team regarding request behaviors. Familiar professionals also showed the highest agreement with the research team for commenting behaviors. Finally, familiar and unfamiliar professionals showed slightly higher agreement regarding rejecting/protesting behavior compared to familiar and unfamiliar parents. Implications There are important implications from the findings reported in this study. First, a better understanding of how adults understand and interpret potential communicative acts could lead to the development of more appropriate assessments and interventions for individuals with RTT. The variability in interpretation of communicative function across participants combined with our limited evidence base for assessing communicative intent in individuals with RTT (and other individuals with atypical communication development) highlights the critical need for systematic coordination between home and school environments to ensure accurate assessment of communication status. Second, if there are differences in how special education professionals and parents understand and interpret potential communicative acts, then feedback to the learner could vary, which could compromise the acquisition of new communicative skills (Meadan et al. 2012). For example, if special education professionals and parents

13 reinforce a learner inconsistently for a communicative act, she may be less likely to acquire and maintain that emerging skill/behavior. It seems important for social partners, across settings, to establish reliable responses for specific (and perhaps unclear or unconventional) topographies of potential communicative behavior. Responses that are delivered by various social partners with high fidelity would potentially increase the learner s propensity to use the forms that are reinforced consistently, thus increasing her overall communicative competence. Limitations There are limitations to our findings. This study reports the interpretations of a small number of raters who interpreted a small number of behaviors from individuals with RTT. Given the relatively small number of participants in the current investigation, a replication with another group of individuals with RTT and their social partners, including special education professionals is warranted. This would provide more evidence related to how adults interpret the potential communicative behavior of individuals with RTT and the inter-observer reliability of their interpretations. The small sample also precluded the use of statistical analyses to facilitate interpretation of the findings. Moreover, it is possible that the communicative behaviors that were selected and shown to raters served multiple functions. As the function of the behaviors shown in the video clips was not elicited in a structured protocol, or confirmed via functional analysis, these findings do not provide evidence regarding the accuracy with which special education professionals and parents interpret the function of the potential communicative acts produced by individuals with RTT, only their reliability with each other and with the research team. A structured protocol would provide evidence related to how different individuals with RTT perform during tasks designed to elicit specific communicative behaviors. A functional analysis would provide evidence to guide professionals and parents as they plan how to best respond to potential communicative behavior and increase consistency in their responses across contexts. Finally, the protocol was not designed to establish an exhaustive list of the learners potential communicative behaviors, but rather to identify those that were most salient to the primary caregivers. The methods we used were designed to answer a question related to reliable consensus among social partners, including the interpretations made by the research team. Future Directions Future research may incorporate functional analyses to verify the functions of potential communicative acts produced by individuals with RTT and compare these functions with the interpretations made by social partners. Knowledge about discrepancies between form, function and response, could aid interventionists as they seek to establish consistent patterns of reinforcement to bids for communication and thus increase communicative behaviors in individuals with RTT. As we continue to understand how potential communicative acts may be interpreted by the social partners of individuals with RTT, as well as describe the forms and functions of communicative behavior within this population, we will be better able to provide continuity of intervention across settings.

14 Appendix Table 5 Structured interview questions 1. Do you know this child? (Examiner will circle yes/no) 2. How do you know this child? (Examiner will note the relationship between rater and learner.) 3. How many months have you worked with his learner? 4. How many minutes each day are you in close proximity with the child? This means you are likely able to observe and/or interact with the child during these minutes. 5. Approximately how many days per week do you spend with this child? 6. From your knowledge and experience, how would you define communication? 7. Do you observe this learner to: a. make vocalizations? b. shake or turn her head? c. use gestures? d. use signs? References Adamson, L. B., Bakerman, R., Smith, C. B., & Walters, A. S. (1987). Adults interpretation of infants acts. Developmental Psychology, 23(3), Baptista, P. M., Mercadante, M. T., Macedo, E. C., & Schwartzman, J. S. (2006). Cognitive performance in Rett syndrome: a pilot study using eyetracking technology. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50, Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill- Palmer Quarterly, 21(3), Bates, E., Benigni, T., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V., (Eds.). (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic. Brady, N. C., & Halle, J. W. (1997). Functional analysis of communicative behaviors. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disorders, 12, Carter, M., & Iacono, T. (2002). Professional judgments of the intentionality of communicative acts. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 18, Dahlgren Sandberg, A., Ehlers, S., Hagberg, B., & Gillberg, C. (2000). The Rett syndrome complex: communication functions in relation to developmental level and autistic features. Autism, 4, Demeter, K. (2000). Assessing the developmental level in Rett syndrome: an alternative approach. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 9, Ellaway, C., & Christodoulou, J. (2001). Rett syndrome: clinical characteristics and recent genetic advances. Disability and Rehabilitation, 23, Fehr, S., Bebbington, A., Nassar, N., Downs, J., Ronen, G. M., De Klerk, N., & Leonard, H. (2011). Trends in the diagnosis of Rett syndrome in Austrailia. Pediatric Research, 70, Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. S. (1993). The MacArthur communicative development inventories: User s guide and technical manual. SanDiego: Singular Publishing Group. Garber, N., & Veydt, N. (1990). Rett syndrome: a longitudinal developmental case report. Journal of Communication Disorders, 23, Hagberg, B. (1995). Rett syndrome: clinical peculiarities and biological mysteries. Acta Paediatrica, 84, Hagberg, B. (2002). Clinical manifestations and states of Rett syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 8,

15 Hagberg, B., & Witt-Engerstrom, I. (1986). Rett syndrome: a suggested staging system for describing impairment profile with increasing age towards adolescence. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 24, Hagberg, B., Hanefeld, F., Percy, A., & Skjeldal, O. (2002). An update on clinically applicable diagnostic criteria in Rett syndrome: comments to the Rett syndrome clinical criteria consensus panel satellite to european paediatric neurology society meeting Baden baden, German, 11 september European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 6, Hetzroni, O. E., & Rubin, C. (2006). Identifying patterns of communicative behaviors in girls with Rett syndrome. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 22, Johnston, S., Reichle, J., Feeley, K., & Jones, E. (2012). Augmentative and alternative communication strategies for individuals with severe disabilities. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Laurvick, C. L., De Klerk, N., Bower, C., Christodoulou, J., Ravine, D., Ellaway, C., & Leonard, H. (2006). Rett syndrome in Australia: a review of the epidemiology. The Journal of Pediatrics, 148(3), Marschik,P.B.,Kaufmann,W.E.,Sigafoos,J.,Wolin,T.,Zhang,D.,Bartl-Pokorny,K.D.,&Johnston,M.V. (2013). Changing the perspective on early development of Rett syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(4), Meadan, H., Halle, J. W., & Kelly, S. M. (2012). Intentional communication of young children with autism spectrum disorder: judgments of different communication partners. Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 24, Neul, J. L., Fang, P., Barrish, J., Lane, J., Caeg, E. B., Smith, E. O., & Glaze, D. G. (2008). Specific mutations in methyl-cpg-binding protein 2 confer different severity in Rett syndrome. Neurology, 70(16), Neul, J. L., Kaufmann, W. E., Glaze, D. G., Christodoulou, J., Clarke, A. J., Bahi-Buisson, N., & Percy, A. K. (2010). Rett Syndrome: revised diagnostic criteria and nomenclature. Annals of Neurology, 68(6), Ryan, D., McGregor, F., Akermanis, M., Southwell, K., Ramke, M., & Woodyatt, G. (2004). Facilitating communication in children with multiple disabilities: three cases of girls with Rett syndrome. Disability and Rehabilitation, 26, Sigafoos, J., Woodyatt, G., Keen, D., Tait, K., Tucker, M., Roberts-Pennell, D., & Pittendreigh, N. (2000a). Identifying potential communicative acts in children with developmental and physical disabilities. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 21(2), Sigafoos, J., Woodyatt, G., Tucker, M., Roberts-Pennel, D., & Pittendreigh, N. (2000b). Assessment of potential communicative acts in three individuals with Rett syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 12, Sigafoos, J., Woodyatt, G., Keen, D., Tait, K., Tucker, M., & Roberts-Pennel, D. (2006). The inventory of potential communicative acts. In J. Sigafoos, M. Arthur-Kelly, & N. Butterfield (Eds.), Enhancing everyday communication for children with disabilities (pp ). Baltimore: P. H. Brookes. Sigafoos, J., Green, V. A., Scholosser, R., O Reilly, M. F., Lancioni, G. E., Rispoli, M., & Lang, R. (2009). Communicative intervention in Rett syndrome: a systematic review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, Sigafoos, J., Kagohara, D., van der Meer, L., Green, V. A., O Reilly, M. F., & Lancioni, G. E. (2011). Communication assessment for individuals with Rett syndrome: a systematic review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland-II adaptive behavior scales: Survey forms manual. Circle Pines: AGS Publishing. Urbanowicz, A., Leonard, H., Girdler, S., Ciccone, N., & Downs, J. (2014). Parental perspectives on the communication abilities of their daughters with Rett syndrome. Developmental neurorehabilitation, (0), 1 9. von Tetzchner, S. (1997). Communication skills among females with Rett syndrome. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 6, Wetherby, A. M., Cain, D. H., Yonclas, D. G., & Walker, V. G. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31(2), Wilcox, M. J., Kouri, T. A., & Caswell, S. (1990). Partner sensitivity to communication behavior of young children with developmental disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, Woodyatt, G., & Ozanne, A. (1992). Communication abilities and Rett syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22, Woodyatt, G., & Ozanne, A. (1997). Rett syndrome (RS) and profound intellectual disability: cognitive and communication similarities and differences. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 6,

16 Yoder, P. (1986). Clarifying the relationship between degree of infant handicap and mental responsivity to infant communicative cues: measurement issues. Infant Mental Health Journal, 7(4), Yoder, P., & Feagans, L. (1988). Mothers attributions of communication to prelinguistic behavior of developmentally delayed and mentally retarded infants. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 93(1),

An exploration of the use of eye gaze & gestures in females with Rett syndrome. Anna Urbanowicz

An exploration of the use of eye gaze & gestures in females with Rett syndrome. Anna Urbanowicz An exploration of the use of eye gaze & gestures in females with Rett syndrome Anna Urbanowicz a.urbanowicz@uq.edu.au Background Communication Critical to participation in everyday life Challenging for

More information

Functional Communication Training in Rett Syndrome: A Preliminary Study

Functional Communication Training in Rett Syndrome: A Preliminary Study Functional Communication Training in Rett Syndrome: A Preliminary Study Breanne J. Byiers, Adele Dimian, and Frank J. Symons Abstract Rett syndrome (RTT) is associated with a range of serious neurodevelopmental

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[johns Hopkins University] On: 30 July 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 731635996] Publisher: Informa Healthcare Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Communication and Swallowing in Rett Syndrome: An Update for Clinicians

Communication and Swallowing in Rett Syndrome: An Update for Clinicians Communication and Swallowing in Rett Syndrome: An Update for Clinicians Theresa E. Bartolotta, PhD, CCC-SLP Patricia A. Remshifski, PhD, CCC-SLP Seton Hall University, South Orange, NJ Disclosure Statement

More information

Title: Symbol-Infused Play for Young Children with Complex Communication Needs

Title: Symbol-Infused Play for Young Children with Complex Communication Needs Title: Symbol-Infused Play for Young Children with Complex Communication Needs This presentation will discuss the importance of symbol-infused play to the development of communication and language in young

More information

Choice making in Rett syndrome: A descriptive study using video data. Anna Urbanowicz

Choice making in Rett syndrome: A descriptive study using video data. Anna Urbanowicz Choice making in Rett syndrome: A descriptive study using video data Anna Urbanowicz a.urbanowicz@uq.edu.au Background Diagnostic criteria for Rett syndrome Main criteria A period of regression: Loss of

More information

Title: Choice making in Rett syndrome: A descriptive study using video data

Title: Choice making in Rett syndrome: A descriptive study using video data This is an Author's Original Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in Disability and Rehabilitation on 23/01/2017 available online at http://www.tandfonline.com/10.1080/09638288.2016.1277392

More information

Emily A. Jones, PhD, BCBA 1

Emily A. Jones, PhD, BCBA 1 Joint Attention Intervention for Children with Autism Emily A. Jones, Ph.D., BCBA CW Post, Long Island University Joint Attention an early social-communicative behavior in which two people share al focus

More information

Getting Started with AAC

Getting Started with AAC Getting Started with AAC P A R E N T G U I D E Many children have medical conditions that impact their ability to speak and learn language. But thanks to augmentative and alternative communication (AAC),

More information

DATA Model Skills Checklist: Curriculum Crosswalk

DATA Model Skills Checklist: Curriculum Crosswalk APPENDIX B DATA Model Skills Checklist: Curriculum Crosswalk This document is a crosswalk of the DATA Model Skills Checklist with the Assessment, Evaluation, Programming System (AEPS). The crosswalk with

More information

In Young Children with ASD Kristy Benefield Speech-Language Pathologist St. Tammany Parish School System

In Young Children with ASD Kristy Benefield Speech-Language Pathologist St. Tammany Parish School System DEVELOPING COMMUNICATION SKILLS In Young Children with ASD By Kristy Benefield Speech-Language Pathologist St. Tammany Parish School System Today we will learn.. OVERVIEW The definition of communication

More information

Autism Research Update

Autism Research Update . Autism Research Update Issue 10: Challenging behaviours Autism Research Network Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth For more information contact: Dr Beatriz López, Department of Psychology

More information

Communication What does the research say?

Communication What does the research say? Communication Communication is often perceived as referring merely to the act of verbalising. However, verbalisations (and actions) can and do occur devoid of communicative intent. Communication is comprised

More information

When neurotypical children look at peoples faces, regions in the limbic system light up with endorphins and reward that child.

When neurotypical children look at peoples faces, regions in the limbic system light up with endorphins and reward that child. Addressing Social Competence in Children and Adolescents with Ausm Spectrum Disorder at Pre- symbolic and Emerging Language Stages Presented by Emily Rubin, MS, CCC- SLP Contemporary research in the neurodevelopment

More information

Program. PECS (cont.) Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) SGDs (Cont.) 11/19/2010. Wendt, Boesch, & Subramanian ASHA Convention 2010

Program. PECS (cont.) Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) SGDs (Cont.) 11/19/2010. Wendt, Boesch, & Subramanian ASHA Convention 2010 Annual Convention of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Moving from PECS to SGDs in AAC Intervention for Autism Oliver Wendt Miriam Boesch Anu Subramanian Ning Hsu Katie Johnstone Program

More information

5. Diagnostic Criteria

5. Diagnostic Criteria 5. Diagnostic Criteria The questions that are going to be answered in this chapter are: What are the diagnostic criteria of ASD? Are the diagnostic criteria laid down in the DSM-IV-TR or ICD-10 manuals

More information

Critical Review: Do Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) Increase Natural Speech Production in Children with Developmental Disabilities?

Critical Review: Do Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) Increase Natural Speech Production in Children with Developmental Disabilities? Critical Review: Do Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) Increase Natural Speech Production in Children with Developmental Disabilities? Sonya Chan M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School

More information

Evidence-Based Communication Interventions for Persons with Severe Disabilities

Evidence-Based Communication Interventions for Persons with Severe Disabilities Evidence-Based Communication Interventions for Persons with Severe Disabilities National Joint Committee for the Communication Needs of Persons with Severe Disabilities (NJC) What is the evidence base

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs DSM-IV to DSM-5: Primary Changes Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs Laurie Swineford, PhD CCC-SLP Washington State University DSM-IV Previously we used the

More information

Julie Bolton M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Julie Bolton M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: In non-verbal children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), is verbal output increased for those who engage in the picture exchange communication System (PECS)? Julie Bolton M.Cl.Sc (SLP)

More information

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Alex Rice M.Cl.Sc SLP Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication

More information

Autism.

Autism. Autism http://aut.sagepub.com A pilot study of the effects of a social-pragmatic intervention on the communication and symbolic play of children with autism Deb Keen, Sylvia Rodger, Kim Doussin and Michelle

More information

Motor Hand Movements in Prelinguistic Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders

Motor Hand Movements in Prelinguistic Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Motor Hand Movements in Prelinguistic Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Barbara Braddock, PhD, CCC-SLP Saint Louis University Jena McDaniel, BSEd Vanderbilt University Jess Poyer and Jane Hilton,

More information

Bringing Your A Game: Strategies to Support Students with Autism Communication Strategies. Ann N. Garfinkle, PhD Benjamin Chu, Doctoral Candidate

Bringing Your A Game: Strategies to Support Students with Autism Communication Strategies. Ann N. Garfinkle, PhD Benjamin Chu, Doctoral Candidate Bringing Your A Game: Strategies to Support Students with Autism Communication Strategies Ann N. Garfinkle, PhD Benjamin Chu, Doctoral Candidate Outcomes for this Session Have a basic understanding of

More information

WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One

WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One Autism is a life-long developmental disability that prevents people from understanding what they see, hear, and otherwise sense. This results in severe problems with social

More information

Fostering Communication Skills in Preschool Children with Pivotal Response Training

Fostering Communication Skills in Preschool Children with Pivotal Response Training Fostering Communication Skills in Preschool Children with Pivotal Response Training Mary Mandeville-Chase, MS, CCC-SLP 1 Training Objectives 1. Participants will name two pivotal behaviors associated with

More information

Fact Sheet. Receptive Communication By Rebecca M. Wilson

Fact Sheet. Receptive Communication By Rebecca M. Wilson Fact Sheet Receptive Communication By Rebecca M. Wilson This information is from an original article found in the Mississippi Services for Individuals Who Are Deafblind February 1994, Issue Number 3, Page

More information

Tempt-Trigger Intervention to Promote Intentional Communication in Three Young Children with ASD

Tempt-Trigger Intervention to Promote Intentional Communication in Three Young Children with ASD Tempt-Trigger Intervention to Promote Intentional Communication in Three Young Children with ASD Susan Lien, Cynthia J. Cress, Heather Kampschnieder, HopeEllen Petro, Laura Thayer, Peggy Merrill, Josie

More information

Teaching Communication to Individuals with Autism. Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA

Teaching Communication to Individuals with Autism. Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA Teaching Communication to Individuals with Autism Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA Participant Outcomes Overview of Communication strategies Ideas on ways to teach communication Qualitative impairment in COMMUNICATION,

More information

Teaching Communication Across the Day. Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA

Teaching Communication Across the Day. Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA Teaching Communication Across the Day Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA Participant Outcomes Overview of Communication strategies Ideas on ways to teach communication Qualitative impairment in COMMUNICATION,

More information

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi We ll cover Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) ASD in young adults Social Communication (definition, components, importance,

More information

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism [Slide 1]: Welcome Welcome to the New Mexico TEAM technical assistance module on making eligibility determinations under the category of autism. This module will review the guidance of the NM TEAM section

More information

Fact Sheet. Receptive Communication By Rebecca M. Wilson. (303) or (303)

Fact Sheet. Receptive Communication By Rebecca M. Wilson. (303) or (303) (303) 866-6681 or (303) 866-6605 COLORADO Assistance for those SERVICES TO with both vision CHILDREN WITH and hearing loss DEAFBLINDNESS Fact Sheet Receptive Communication By Rebecca M. Wilson This information

More information

Device Modeling as Prompting Strategy for Users of AAC Devices. Meher Banajee, Ph.D., CCC-SLP Nino Acuna, M.A. Hannah Deshotels, B.A.

Device Modeling as Prompting Strategy for Users of AAC Devices. Meher Banajee, Ph.D., CCC-SLP Nino Acuna, M.A. Hannah Deshotels, B.A. Device Modeling as Prompting Strategy for Users of AAC Devices Meher Banajee, Ph.D., CCC-SLP Nino Acuna, M.A. Hannah Deshotels, B.A. Introduction What is dual device modeling? Review of literature Anecdotal

More information

Section 5: Communication. Part 1: Early Warning Signs. Theresa Golem. December 5, 2012

Section 5: Communication. Part 1: Early Warning Signs. Theresa Golem. December 5, 2012 Section 5: Communication Part 1: Early Warning Signs Theresa Golem December 5, 2012 Deficits in the area of communication are one of the key characteristics of autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Early warning

More information

Kayla Dickie M.Cl.Sc. SLP Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Kayla Dickie M.Cl.Sc. SLP Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: Is peer mediated intervention effective in increasing social communication in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder who use Alternative and Augmentative Communication? Kayla Dickie M.Cl.Sc.

More information

Page24. Indexed in: Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Sunny Agarwal Founder / Digital Marketing Specialist at Code- LIT. September 2018; 5(3):24-28

Page24. Indexed in: Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Sunny Agarwal Founder / Digital Marketing Specialist at Code- LIT. September 2018; 5(3):24-28 Page24 Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Sunny Agarwal Founder / Digital Marketing Specialist at Code- LIT Email: agarwalsunny68@gmail.com KEY WORDS: Autism, Sign/symptoms of ASD (Autism Spectrum Disorder),

More information

Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- NOS)

Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- NOS) Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- NOS) What is Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS)? (*Please note that the criteria according to the DSM-V changed

More information

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Toddler Module

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Toddler Module The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Toddler Module A new module of a standardized diagnostic measure for Autism spectrum disorders in toddlers Romina Moavero About the ADOS The Autism Diagnostic

More information

10/18/2016. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition 1. Meet Dr. Saulnier. Bio. Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Vineland-3 Author

10/18/2016. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition 1. Meet Dr. Saulnier. Bio. Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Vineland-3 Author Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Vineland-3 Author Director of Research Operations at the Marcus Autism Center & Associate Professor in the Department of Pediatrics

More information

What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA.

What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA. What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA. What is Autism? ) Autism is a complex developmental disability that has a neurological basis that causes impairments in social interactions, communication,

More information

How Does the ProxTalker Speech- Generating Device Compare to PECS?

How Does the ProxTalker Speech- Generating Device Compare to PECS? How Does the ProxTalker Speech- Generating Device Compare to PECS? Implications for selecting Augmentative & Alternative Communication (AAC) strategies Miriam C. Boesch, Ph.D. Assistant Professor in Special

More information

There are two types of activities: Think about it! And apply it! Each activity will be marked by an icon and a specified color as you can see below:

There are two types of activities: Think about it! And apply it! Each activity will be marked by an icon and a specified color as you can see below: Module 1 Introduction VCUAutism Center for Excellence There are two types of activities: Think about it! And apply it! Each activity will be marked by an icon and a specified color as you can see below:

More information

Copyright: Bopp & Mirenda.ASHA (2008) 1

Copyright: Bopp & Mirenda.ASHA (2008) 1 Heterogeneity of Language Development in Young Children with Autism Over 4-5 Years Karen D. Bopp, Ph.D., S-LP(C) Pat Mirenda, Ph.D., BCBA The University of British Columbia, Canada November 20, 2008 American

More information

USING FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS TO TEST WHETHER IDIOSYNCRATIC AND CHALLENGING BEHAVIOR IN RETT SYNDROME IS SENSITIVE TO SOCIAL REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES

USING FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS TO TEST WHETHER IDIOSYNCRATIC AND CHALLENGING BEHAVIOR IN RETT SYNDROME IS SENSITIVE TO SOCIAL REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES USING FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS TO TEST WHETHER IDIOSYNCRATIC AND CHALLENGING BEHAVIOR IN RETT SYNDROME IS SENSITIVE TO SOCIAL REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL

More information

Valarie Kerschen M.D.

Valarie Kerschen M.D. Valarie Kerschen M.D. Greek word meaning self 1940 s Dr Leo Kanner describes classic autism 1940 s Dr Hans Asperger describes Aspergers Syndrome 1960 s Autism theorized to be due to refrigerator mothers

More information

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Overview Diagnostic Criteria Current: Diagnostic & Statistical

More information

Overview. Clinical Features

Overview. Clinical Features Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Clinical Features Overview Diagnostic & Statistical Manual IV (DSM IV) Prevalence Course of Onset Etiology Early Recognition Early Recognition

More information

LSU Health Sciences Center

LSU Health Sciences Center LSU Health Sciences Center Speech-Language-Hearing Clinic, Department of Communication Disorders, School of Allied Health Professions, 1900 Gravier Street, 9 th Floor, New Orleans, La 70112 Date: Identification

More information

2017 Gatlinburg Conference Symposium Submission SS-1

2017 Gatlinburg Conference Symposium Submission SS-1 Symposium Title: Outcomes for Young Children with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities: A Discussion of Behavioral Phenotypes, Differential Responses and Outcome Measures Chair: Jena McDaniel 1

More information

Neurodevelopmental Disorders

Neurodevelopmental Disorders Neurodevelopmental Disorders Intellectual Disability Disorder Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADD/ADHD) Motor Disorders/Tourette s Disorder Intellectual Disability

More information

1/30/2018. Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Disclosures. Learning Objectives

1/30/2018. Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Disclosures. Learning Objectives Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Associate Professor Emory University School of Medicine Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition 1 Disclosures As

More information

Sign Language and Early Childhood Development

Sign Language and Early Childhood Development University of Arkansas, Fayetteville ScholarWorks@UARK Rehabilitation, Human Resources and Communication Disorders Undergraduate Honors Theses Rehabilitation, Human Resources and Communication Disorders

More information

Autism or Something Else? Knowing the Difference

Autism or Something Else? Knowing the Difference Autism or Something Else? Knowing the Difference SUSAN BUTTROSS, M.D., FAAP PROFESSOR OF PEDIATRICS CENTER FOR THE ADVANCEMENT OF YOUTH UNIVERSITY OF MISSISSIPPI MEDICAL CENTER JACKSON, MISSISSIPPI Disclosure

More information

SPEECH THERAPY: Supports for the Newly Diagnosed EARLY CHILDHOOD AND ELEMENTARY YEARS

SPEECH THERAPY: Supports for the Newly Diagnosed EARLY CHILDHOOD AND ELEMENTARY YEARS SPEECH THERAPY: Supports for the Newly Diagnosed EARLY CHILDHOOD AND ELEMENTARY YEARS Speech/language/feeding for ASD: Receptive language Expressive language Social language/pragmatics Feeding skills Receptive

More information

7/31/12. Background. It is estimated that anywhere between 14-20% (Lord, Risi & Pickles, 2004) to 50% (National Research Council,

7/31/12. Background. It is estimated that anywhere between 14-20% (Lord, Risi & Pickles, 2004) to 50% (National Research Council, Background Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is one of the fast growing disability categories in the United States (Tincani, Crozier & Alazetta, 2006). Effects of an Aided Modeling Intervention on Expressive

More information

CLINICAL BOTTOM LINE Early Intervention for Children With Autism Implications for Occupational Therapy

CLINICAL BOTTOM LINE Early Intervention for Children With Autism Implications for Occupational Therapy Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, J.,... Varley, J. (2010). Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: The Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics,

More information

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: Can joint attention, imitation, and/or play skills predict future language abilities of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)? Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology)

More information

AAC Child Case History Form

AAC Child Case History Form Client Name: Date-of-Birth: School: Native Language: Mother s Name: Address: Phone: AAC Child Case History Form Date: Age: Grade: Primary Language: Father s Name: Address (if different): Phone: Home Work

More information

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP About the Author The research presented today was conducted while the author was a Speech- Language

More information

AAC Child Case History Form

AAC Child Case History Form AAC Child Case History Form Name Date Date-of-Birth School Age Grade Native Language Primary Language Mother s Name Alernate Email Home Work Cell (CIRCLE ONE) Home Work Cell (CIRCLE ONE) Father s Name

More information

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER What is the DSM-5? The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (the DSM) is developed by the

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorders: Interventions and supports to promote independence

Autism Spectrum Disorders: Interventions and supports to promote independence Autism Spectrum Disorders: Interventions and supports to promote independence Presented by: Greg Valcante, Ph.D. University of Florida www.card.ufl.edu How to Get a Copy of These Handouts www.card.ufl.edu/training/asd-powerpoint

More information

07/11/2016. Agenda. Role of ALL early providers. AAP Guidelines, Cont d. Early Communication Assessment

07/11/2016. Agenda. Role of ALL early providers. AAP Guidelines, Cont d. Early Communication Assessment Early Communication Assessment Early Social Communication Assessment: Models for Infant Siblings at Risk for ASD How early? 9-12 months we attempt to make critical observations of younger sibs, capturing

More information

8/1/2018 ADDING THE EARLY START DENVER MODEL INTO PART C SERVICE DELIVERY: THE EXPERIENCE OF TWO EI PROVIDERS DISCLOSURE

8/1/2018 ADDING THE EARLY START DENVER MODEL INTO PART C SERVICE DELIVERY: THE EXPERIENCE OF TWO EI PROVIDERS DISCLOSURE ADDING THE EARLY START DENVER MODEL INTO PART C SERVICE DELIVERY: THE EXPERIENCE OF TWO EI PROVIDERS August 8, 2018 Barbara Weber, M.S, CCC-SLP, BCBA Merle Crawford, M.S., OTR/L, BCBA DISCLOSURE Barb and

More information

Assistive Technology, AAC, and ASD

Assistive Technology, AAC, and ASD Evidence-Based Practice: part of the decision making process of selecting assessment and interventions approach in which current, high-quality research evidence is integrated with professional knowledge

More information

From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM IV

From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM IV From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM IV (I) A total of six (or more) items from (A), (B), and (C), with at least two from (A), and one each from (B) and (C) (A) qualitative impairment

More information

Paraprofessional Training Module

Paraprofessional Training Module Paraprofessional Training Module Module 8 One and one half hour module What is the Para Educator s Role in Education of Students with Autism? References: National Professional Development Center on Autism

More information

The Effects of A Parent-Mediated Social-Communication Intervention on Joint Attention Outcomes in Young Children with Autism

The Effects of A Parent-Mediated Social-Communication Intervention on Joint Attention Outcomes in Young Children with Autism 1 The Effects of A Parent-Mediated Social-Communication Intervention on Joint Attention Outcomes in Young Children with Autism Introduction and Purpose Recent evidence suggests that human social-communicative

More information

Child AAC Intake Form

Child AAC Intake Form Inspiring Talkers Brandi-Lynn Greig, M.S., CCC-SLP Firestone, CO 80504 www.inspiringtalkers.com Child AAC Intake Form General Information: Full name of child: Social Security Number: Parents/Guardians

More information

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Marci Laurel, MA, CCC-SLP mlaurel@salud.unm.edu and Services (UCEDD) 1 Training Objectives Participants will: 1. Name at least three communication

More information

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby A Fully Integrated Platform for Observation, Daily Reporting, Communications and Assessment For Early Childhood Professionals and the Families that they Serve Alignment

More information

Evergreen Speech & Hearing Clinic, Inc. Transforming Lives Through Improved Communication Since 1979

Evergreen Speech & Hearing Clinic, Inc. Transforming Lives Through Improved Communication Since 1979 Audiology Hearing Testing VRA VNG/VEMP OAE BAER/ECochG Hearing Aids Cochlear/Bone Implants Tinnitus CAPD EHDDI Speech-Language Pathology Language Voice Accent Modification Autism Evaluation & Treatment

More information

Floortime - Affectively rich play to match child s individual differences and stimulate developmental growth

Floortime - Affectively rich play to match child s individual differences and stimulate developmental growth What is DIR /Floortime? Developmental - children progress through stages toward ability to think and communicate flexibly in the world Individual Difference - each child has an unique set of biologically

More information

OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION

OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS IN EMERGING ADULTS Douglas J. Scambler, Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist Presented at the University of Wyoming November 6, 2012 OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION What are the autism spectrum

More information

Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Associate Professor Emory University School of Medicine Director of Research Operations Marcus Autism Center Vineland Adaptive

More information

Fact Sheet 8. DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder

Fact Sheet 8. DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder Fact Sheet 8 DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder A diagnosis of autism is made on the basis of observed behaviour. There are no blood tests, no single defining symptom and no physical characteristics that

More information

Municipal Employee Guide to Autism Awareness

Municipal Employee Guide to Autism Awareness RISK Winter 2014 Municipal Employee Guide to Autism Awareness A D M M R M A V I S O A D M R Y C I N O I S T R A T M M I T T I V E E Michigan Municipal Risk Management Authority Administrative Advisory

More information

Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders

Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders Background Autistic is a broad term coined in the twentieth century by Bleuler that

More information

AUTISM PARENT HANDBOOK. Answers to common questions. Artwork: Hey Diddle Diddle, by Eytan Nisinzweig, an artist with autism.

AUTISM PARENT HANDBOOK. Answers to common questions. Artwork: Hey Diddle Diddle, by Eytan Nisinzweig, an artist with autism. AUTISM PARENT HANDBOOK Answers to common questions Artwork: Hey Diddle Diddle, by Eytan Nisinzweig, an artist with autism. 2nd Edition Dear Parents: We are very pleased to publish the 2nd edition of the

More information

UCC EI Underlying Characteristics Checklist Early Intervention 11/19/09. Starting Points. Prevalence of ASD. Starting Points

UCC EI Underlying Characteristics Checklist Early Intervention 11/19/09. Starting Points. Prevalence of ASD. Starting Points Starting Points Underlying Characteristics Checklist EI for ages 0 to 5: Development and Uses Ruth Aspy, Ph.D., Barry G. Grossman, Ph.D., Kathleen Quill, Ed.D., and Nicole Brin, M.A., CCC-SLP Autism spectrum

More information

Early Autism Detection Screening and Referral. What is Autism? ASD Epidemiology. ASD Basic Facts 10/10/2010. Early Autism Detection and Referral

Early Autism Detection Screening and Referral. What is Autism? ASD Epidemiology. ASD Basic Facts 10/10/2010. Early Autism Detection and Referral Early Autism Detection and Referral Early Autism Detection Screening and Referral Learning Objectives: Define autistic spectrum disorders, their epidemiology and etiology; Recognize the earliest signs

More information

Greetings: Video Social Stories via ipad for Children with Autism. Kathryn Young, B.S. Pamela Smith Ph.D, CCC-SLP

Greetings: Video Social Stories via ipad for Children with Autism. Kathryn Young, B.S. Pamela Smith Ph.D, CCC-SLP Greetings: Video Social Stories via ipad for Children with Autism Kathryn Young, B.S. Pamela Smith Ph.D, CCC-SLP Disclosure Statement Yes, I have financial or nonfinancial relationships relevant to the

More information

SURVEY OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER CONCERNS

SURVEY OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER CONCERNS Survey of Autism Spectrum Disorder Concerns Presented by Curtis L. Timmons, Ph.D., LSSP GOALS OF THE WORKSHOP 1. Understand why there were changes between the DSM-IV and the DSM-5 2. Understand the current

More information

AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS. Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician

AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS. Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician Disclosure I do not have any financial relationships to disclose I do not plan to discuss

More information

Early Interventions for ASD: State of the Science

Early Interventions for ASD: State of the Science Early Interventions for ASD: State of the Science Connie Kasari, Ph.D. University of California, Los Angeles Malaysian Regional Conference April 22-23 Today What is the evidence for current early interventions?

More information

Matrix of NI by Outcome and Age (years)

Matrix of NI by Outcome and Age (years) Brief Introduction Naturalistic intervention (NI) is a collection of practices designed to encourage specific target behaviors based on learners interests. It occurs within the typical settings, activities,

More information

PRINCIPLES OF CAREGIVING DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES MODULE

PRINCIPLES OF CAREGIVING DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES MODULE PRINCIPLES OF CAREGIVING DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES MODULE CHAPTER 1: KNOWLEDGE OF DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES CONTENT: A. Developmental Disabilities B. Introduction to Human Development C. The Four Developmental

More information

Starting Strong 2015 Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders and An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis

Starting Strong 2015 Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders and An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis Starting Strong 2015 Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders and An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis Robin Talley, M.Ed., BCBA UW Autism Center Presentation Overview Overview of Autism Spectrum

More information

2 Autistic disorder symptoms in Rett

2 Autistic disorder symptoms in Rett 2 Autistic disorder symptoms in Rett syndrome Josette Wulffaert Ina A. van Berckelaer-Onnes Evert M. Scholte Autism, 2009, 13, 567-581 29 Chapter 2 ABSTRACT According to the major classification systems

More information

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not.

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not. Autism Summary Autism What is Autism? The Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a developmental disability that can have significant implications on a child's ability to function and interface with the world

More information

Chapter 1. A Review of the Efficacy of Using Picture Exchange to Improve Spoken and Nonspoken. Communicative Behaviors:

Chapter 1. A Review of the Efficacy of Using Picture Exchange to Improve Spoken and Nonspoken. Communicative Behaviors: Chapter 1 A Review of the Efficacy of Using Picture Exchange to Improve Spoken and Nonspoken Communicative Behaviors: Introduction and Literature Review Picture Exchange Systems Several studies are available

More information

Research in Developmental Disabilities

Research in Developmental Disabilities Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 1749 1756 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Research in Developmental Disabilities Profiling early socio-communicative development in five

More information

Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update. Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011

Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update. Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011 Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011 Overview Diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorders Screening/referral

More information

Rating purpose (choose one): (Entry) Periodic # Exit

Rating purpose (choose one): (Entry) Periodic # Exit CHILD OUTCOMES SUMMARY FORM Program Name: Preschool Rating purpose (choose one): (Entry) Periodic # Exit Date: 10 / 7 / 09 Mon Day Yr Child Information Name: Tony Date of birth: 6 / 28 / 06 Mon Day Yr

More information

Potential Outcomes for Children Who Are Deaf-Blind with Cochlear Implants

Potential Outcomes for Children Who Are Deaf-Blind with Cochlear Implants Facilitating Communication & Language for Children with Cochlear Implants and Vision Impairments EARLY HEARING DETECTION & INTERVENTION Addison, Texas March 10 th, 2009 Kathleen Stremel stremelk@wou.edu

More information

Autism/Autism Spectrum Disorders

Autism/Autism Spectrum Disorders Autism/Autism Spectrum Disorders Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are a group of developmental disabilities that can cause significant social, communication and behavioral challenges. People with ASDs

More information

COMBINING SINGLE CASE DESIGN AND GROUP EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN RESEARCH

COMBINING SINGLE CASE DESIGN AND GROUP EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN RESEARCH COMBINING SINGLE CASE DESIGN AND GROUP EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN RESEARCH Ann P. Kaiser, PhD Vanderbilt University IES SCD June 2018 1 Today s Talk Overview of research on Enhanced Milieu Teaching (EMT) Illustrate

More information

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45, 431 435 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 2012) COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO MARCUS

More information

Evidence-Based Practices Comparison Chart. National Autism Center (NAC) 1

Evidence-Based Practices Comparison Chart. National Autism Center (NAC) 1 Evidence-Based Practices Comparison Chart This chart compares the lists of evidence-based practices developed by the Centers for Medicare & Medicaid Services, the National Autism Center, and the National

More information