Perimetric Defects and Ganglion Cell Damage: Interpreting Linear Relations Using a Two-Stage Neural Model

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Perimetric Defects and Ganglion Cell Damage: Interpreting Linear Relations Using a Two-Stage Neural Model"

Transcription

1 Perimetric Defects and Ganglion Cell Damage: Interpreting Linear Relations Using a Two-Stage Neural Model William H. Swanson, 1,2 Joost Felius, 2 and Fei Pan 1 From the 1 Glaucoma Institute, SUNY State College of Optometry, New York, New York; and the 2 Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, Texas. Supported by Grant EY07716 from the National Eye Institute (WHS) and by Novartis Ophthalmics (SUNY Glaucoma Institute). Submitted for publication April 12, 2003; revised September 18, 2003; accepted October 13, Disclosure: W.H. Swanson, None; J. Felius, None; F. Pan, None The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by page charge payment. This article must therefore be marked advertisement in accordance with 18 U.S.C solely to indicate this fact. Corresponding author: William H. Swanson, Glaucoma Institute, Department of Clinical Sciences, State University of New York (SUNY) State College of Optometry, 33 West 42nd Street, New York, NY 10036; bswanson@sunyopt.edu. PURPOSE. To better understand the relations between glaucomatous perimetric defects and ganglion cell damage, a neural model was developed to interpret empiric findings on linear relations between perimetric defects and measures of ganglion cell loss. METHODS. A two-stage model computed responses of ganglion cell mosaics (first stage), then computed perimetric sensitivity in terms of processing by spatial filters (second stage) that pool the ganglion cell responses. Cell death and dysfunction were introduced in a local patch of the first-stage ganglion cell mosaic, and perimetric defect depth was computed for the corresponding region of the visual field. Calculations were performed for both sparse and dense ganglion cell mosaics and for spatial filters with peak frequencies from 0.5 to 4.0 cyc/deg. RESULTS. The model yielded nonlinear functions for perimetric defect depth in decibel versus the percentage of ganglion cell damage, but functions for lower spatial frequencies became linear when perimetric defect was expressed as a percentage of normal. The relations between perimetric defects and percentage of ganglion cell loss were determined primarily by spatial tuning of the second-stage spatial filters. For averaging sensitivities across different visual field locations, linear units (arithmetic mean) can more closely approximate mean ganglion cell loss than decibel units (geometric mean). Fits to data from experimental glaucoma required ganglion cell dysfunction in addition to ganglion cell loss. CONCLUSIONS. Pooling by second-stage spatial filters can account for empiric findings of linear relations between perimetric defects and measures of ganglion cell loss. (Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004;45: ) DOI: /iovs Standard automated perimetry is one of the most frequently performed psychophysical tests, because patients being treated for glaucoma routinely undergo perimetric testing one or more times per year as long as they retain functional vision. 1 Perimetry is used clinically to assess functional consequences of retinal ganglion cell damage, yet the relations between perimetric defects and ganglion cell damage are not well understood. This is in part because standard perimetric stimuli were developed in an empiric framework for spatial vision from the first half of the 20th century. The literature on perimetry has not yet fully integrated the linear systems approach for spatial vision that has been widely used over the past half century in both psychophysical and electrophysiological studies of spatial vision. A systematic application of the linear systems approach found that effects of eccentricity on perimetric sensitivity in normal eyes could be accounted for by magnification of spatial filters with eccentricity, 2 but this finding has not yet been applied to analysis of effects of ganglion cell loss. The present study provides a quantitative application of the linear systems approach to estimate the potential contributions of ganglion cell death and dysfunction to glaucomatous perimetric defects. Highly nonlinear correlations between depth of perimetric defect and extent of ganglion cell loss have been obtained by histologic studies of humans 3,4 and monkeys, 5 when perimetric defect is presented in decibels (1.0 db 0.1 log unit) and measures of ganglion cell loss are presented in linear units (e.g., percentage of normal cells). Depth of perimetric defect in decibels shows minimal change until cell density decreases to half of normal, and then increases dramatically as cell loss proceeds. A possible explanation for this was provided by Hood et al., 6,7 who found that the amplitude of the multifocal visual evoked potential (mvep) was linearly related with perimetric sensitivity when perimetric sensitivities were averaged in linear units. They assumed that the mvep signal amplitude reflects a linear sum of ganglion cell responses and hypothesized that, when perimetric sensitivity is expressed in linear units, the relation between local perimetric defect and local ganglion cell loss is nearly linear. Clinical studies have obtained similar results when using structural measures (area of neuroretinal rim, nerve fiber layer thickness) as indices of ganglion cell loss. When mean perimetric sensitivity is computed in decibel units, there are highly nonlinear correlations with indices of structural loss that is, depth of perimetric defect is minimal for mild structural loss and then increases dramatically when structural indices fall below half of mean normal When perimetric sensitivities are averaged in linear units the relation with rim area becomes more linear. 12 The present study presents a theoretical interpretation of the empirically observed relations between clinical measures of structural abnormalities and functional defects. Nerve fiber layer defects and notches in the neuroretinal rim are consistent with localized retinal regions of extensive cell death, but the pattern of cell damage is not clear within these regions. Ganglion cells can die by apoptosis, 13 and it has been argued that cell death could be the sole cause of clinically significant glaucomatous defects. 14 However, clinical reports of improvement in visual function after treatment 15,16 have suggested that dysfunction of ganglion cells, in addition to cell death, also contributes to depth of perimetric defects. The purpose of the present study was to develop a quantitative model for perimetric sensitivity that incorporates key features of psychophysical models of spatial vision as a way to improve understanding of the relation between perimetric defects and ganglion cell damage. The basic parameters of the model are varied to cover the ranges of values found in the literature on ganglion cells and on perimetric spatial summa- Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, February 2004, Vol. 45, No Copyright Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology

2 IOVS, February 2004, Vol. 45, No. 2 Perimetric Defects and Ganglion Cell Damage 467 tion and to establish conditions for which linearity would be expected. METHODS A two-stage model was developed for visual processing of perimetric stimuli: the first stage represents a mosaic of retinal ganglion cells responding to presentation of a perimetric stimulus, and the second stage represents psychophysical spatial filters that pool the responses of the ganglion cell mosaic. Effects of ganglion cell death and cell dysfunction were simulated by altering ganglion cell sensitivities in the first-stage mosaic. The model was implemented on computer (Igor Pro 4.01 software; Wavemetrics, Inc., Lake Oswego, OR). The model used two different types of ganglion cell mosaics and six different peak spatial frequencies for the spatial filters, giving a total of 12 different sets of calculations for each degree of ganglion cell loss. The two types of first-stage ganglion cell mosaics were based on the literature on macaque and human ganglion cells, and the six types of second-stage filters were based on analysis of the literature on spatial summation of perimetry in normal eyes. Robustness of the results was evaluated by comparisons among these 12 sets of calculations, and by secondary calculations (see the Appendix). First Stage: Ganglion Cell Mosaics The first stage of the model computes the responses of an array of ganglion cells to the perimetric stimulus: a circular luminance increment with a diameter of 0.43 on a uniform background, typically referred to as stimulus size III, as defined by Goldmann. 17 This stage of the model requires five parameters to define the mosaic: diameters and relative sensitivities of ganglion cell receptive field centers and surrounds, and ganglion cell spacing. Figure 1 shows diameters of receptive field centers of macaque parvocellular and magnocellular ganglion cells as a function of eccentricity from studies using gratings 18,19 or circular achromatic increments. 20 Across the different ganglion cell types and studies and across eccentricities from 10 to 30, there is a fourfold range of center diameters. To evaluate the robustness of the model in the face of uncertainty about receptive field diameters, we used two sets of parameters reported for ganglion cells from the parvocellular and magnocellular pathways from the only one of these studies that also provided diameter and weight for the inhibitory surround. 19 These parameter sets span much of the range of reported receptive field diameters from 10 to 30 across both ganglion cell types and are shown as large open triangles in Figure 1. Implementation of the first stage of the model also requires estimates of ganglion cell spacing. For eccentricities from 10 to 30, the center center separation between human ganglion cells is estimated to average from to for all cells and from 0.10 to 0.20 if only magnocellular cells are considered. 21 Hexagonal spacing was used to create local patches of a mosaic. The dense mosaic patch had a center center separation of 0.036, and the sparse mosaic patch had a center center separation of A mosaic patch was constructed that spanned an area whose diameter equaled the diameter of the stimulus plus six times the space constant of a single cell s receptive field surround. Cells falling outside this region would have minimal response to presentation of the stimulus. Progressive ganglion cell damage in glaucomatous defects was simulated by generating a series of degraded mosaics, in which cells were successively removed on a random basis to produce mosaics retaining only 75%, 50%, 25%, 12.5%, 6.3%, 3.1%, and 1.6% of normal ganglion cell density. To evaluate effects of cell death, normal responsiveness was retained by the remaining cells. To evaluate the effects of cell dysfunction, responsiveness of surviving cells was decreased using both homogeneous and heterogeneous patterns of dysfunction. For heterogeneous dysfunction, sensitivity of a fraction of ganglion cells was reduced to 10% of normal, whereas the rest of the ganglion cells retained normal sensitivity. For homogeneous dysfunction, sensitivity of all ganglion cells was reduced by the same amount. FIGURE 1. Average receptive field center diameters of macaque parvocellular (PC) and magnocellular (MC) cells reported by studies using gratings 18,19 or circular achromatic increments. 20 Large open triangles: correspond to center diameters for the two sets of parameters used for the dense and sparse mosaics in the primary calculations of the two-stage neural model. Horizontal dashed line: diameter of the standard size III perimetric stimulus. Second Stage: Psychophysical Spatial Filters Key Features of Spatial Filters. Over the past half century, application of linear systems theory has allowed rapid measurement of spatial properties of receptive fields of retinal ganglion cells and visual cortical cells 22 and has enabled psychophysicists 23 to select experimental stimuli that characterize luminance profiles for psychophysical spatial filters. The sensitivity of an individual psychophysical spatial filter to a given stimulus can be estimated by a linear computation (i.e., the convolution of the filter s receptive field by the luminance profile of the stimulus). Just as there can be thousands of cells in the input layer of visual cortex responding to a perimetric stimulus, there can be thousands of linear spatial filters that contribute to perimetric sensitivity. Spatial filters responding to a stimulus can be distinguished in terms of offset from stimulus center, peak spatial frequency, spatial phase, spatial bandwidth, and orientation tuning. The combination of sensitivities of all the spatial filters into a single psychophysical sensitivity requires a nonlinear process referred to as probability summation. The probability of detecting a stimulus is equal to the probability that enough spatial filters are sufficiently responsive to the stimulus. Probability summation can be well-approximated by vector summation of filter sensitivities with an exponent of Most of the effects of eccentricity on sensitivity to perimetric stimuli can be accounted for by magnification of effective stimulus size with eccentricity, 2 which compensates for corresponding decreases in retinal ganglion cell density and area of visual cortex. Magnification of the stimulus to obtain constant sensitivity across eccentricities is consistent with spatial vision models for which the second-stage spatial filters are magnified with eccentricity We have shown 30 that perimetric spatial summation data can be accounted for by a simple two-parameter function for which the primary parameter is the critical area, a measure of magnification; the sensitivity parameter shows little or no variation with eccentricity. 31 For the two-parameter function, sensitivity increases with stimulus area with a slope of 10 db/log unit below the critical diameter and 2.5 db/log unit above the critical diameter.

3 468 Swanson et al. IOVS, February 2004, Vol. 45, No. 2 responses of the second-stage filters were combined by probability summation. The range of peak spatial frequencies used for the spatial filters was determined by fitting the two-parameter function 30 to published perimetric spatial summation data from normal eyes 2 tested at a range of eccentricities along four different meridians. Figure 3 shows equivalent peak spatial frequency (one half cycle critical diameter) as a function of eccentricity. Because equivalent peak spatial frequency varied from 0.6 to 4.0 cyc/deg, the model used filters with peak spatial frequencies in the range of 0.5 to 4.0 cyc/deg. The forms used for the spatial filters are described in detail in the Appendix. In areas with deep perimetric defects, dramatic changes in perimetric sensitivity can be caused by minor changes in stimulus location. 35 For perimetric subjects with good fixation, normal microsaccades and drift can cause stimulus location to vary by as much as 1 of the nominal stimulus center. 36 For the model, psychophysical sensitivity for a damaged patch of ganglion cell mosaics was computed for 13 different locations within 1 of nominal stimulus center, using a hexagonal grid with 0.5 separation. The mean of these 13 sensitivities was used as the perimetric sensitivity. FIGURE 2. Number of ganglion cells versus perimetric sensitivity for the size III stimulus from a recent empiric study, 32 fit by the twoparameter spatial summation function 30 discussed in the text. The function is consistent with critical area increasing with eccentricity to compensate for decrease in ganglion cell density. In locations at eccentricities greater than 15 (left side of figure, ganglion cell number 1.5 log unit), perimetric defect is linearly related to the number of ganglion cells. To evaluate the extent to which magnification of the second-stage spatial filters affects the relation between perimetric sensitivity and ganglion cell density in normal eyes, we reanalyzed data from a recent comparison 32 of normative data on human ganglion cell density 33 and perimetric sensitivity. 34 We tested the hypothesis that magnification of the second-stage spatial filters was the only factor affecting perimetric sensitivity and that the magnification factor was related to ganglion cell density. For the standard stimulus size III, perimetric sensitivity would be determined by its size relative to the critical area at each location: When the critical area is smaller than the stimulus, sensitivity decreases at a rate of 10 db per log unit difference in area. When the critical area is larger than the stimulus, sensitivity increases at a rate of 2.5 db per log unit difference in area. Figure 2 shows perimetric sensitivity versus log ganglion cell number. Our two-parameter spatial summation function 30 accounted for 82% of the variance in the data. This confirms that much of the effect of eccentricity on perimetric sensitivity can be explained in terms of magnification of spatial filters. The magnification factor obtained by fitting the data was equivalent to 31 ganglion cells filling the critical area at each retinal location. Based on the anatomic data used for ganglion cell density, 33 at eccentricities greater than 15 the size III stimulus would cover a retinal region that contains fewer than 31 cells. Most of the visual field locations used by the empiric model were for eccentricities greater than 15 (left side of Fig. 2, ganglion cell number 1.5 log unit), and thus, in these locations, perimetric sensitivity would be linearly related to ganglion cell density. In the remaining locations within 15 (right side of Fig. 2, ganglion cell number 1.5 log unit), a nonlinear relation would hold in which sensitivity increases much more slowly with ganglion cell number. This is qualitatively consistent with the empiric model, 32 which used ganglion cell pooling factors that were highly nonlinear in the macula and became more linear as eccentricity increased. Features of the Model. To compute psychophysical sensitivity, the responses of the first-stage ganglion cell mosaics were combined by spatially tuned psychophysical spatial filters, then the RESULTS Figure 4 shows depth of defect versus percentage of ganglion cells remaining. The left graph shows perimetric defects in decibel units, for which all functions are nonlinear. Defects did not reach 3 db until the percentage of ganglion cells remaining dropped to 50%, and they deepened dramatically when the remaining cells declined below 10%. The right graph shows perimetric defects in linear units (percentage of normal sensitivity). The functions are linear when the peak spatial frequency of the second-stage spatial filters is sufficiently low and become nonlinear as spatial frequency increases. At lower spatial frequencies ( cyc/deg for the dense mosaic, cyc/deg for the sparse mosaic), the difference between perimetric defect and ganglion cell loss was less than 6% at all stages of loss. At higher spatial frequencies ( cyc/deg for the sparse mosaic, 4.0 cyc/deg for the dense mosaic), perimetric sensitivity remained within 50% of normal until the FIGURE 3. Equivalent spatial frequency versus eccentricity derived by fitting normative spatial summation data gathered along four different meridians. 2 To span this range of values, the peak spatial frequencies for the second-stage filters varied from 0.5 to 4.0 cyc/deg.

4 IOVS, February 2004, Vol. 45, No. 2 Perimetric Defects and Ganglion Cell Damage 469 FIGURE 4. Effects of ganglion cell loss in the first stage of the model, with defect depth shown as a function of the percentage of ganglion cells remaining. Left: depth of defect in db; right: depth of defect as a percentage of normal; thick gray line: perfect linearity. percentage of remaining ganglion cells decreased below 25% of normal, and perimetric sensitivity declined dramatically with further ganglion cell loss. To estimate potential roles for ganglion cell dysfunction, we analyzed published data from a study of perimetric sensitivity versus ganglion cell number in monkeys with experimental glaucoma. 37 These data are plotted as filled circles in Figure 5 (top) with both perimetric defect and ganglion cell loss as percent of normal. Perfect linearity is shown as a thick straight line, and the open circles show an example of highly linear results from the model (dense mosaic, 1.0 cyc/deg). The data from experimental glaucoma deviate substantially from linearity, consistent with dysfunction preceding anatomic cell death. When the second-stage spatial filters are tuned to low enough spatial frequencies to produce highly linear results, homogeneous and heterogeneous dysfunction have identical effects. The lower panel shows the amount of dysfunction necessary to fit the data from experimental glaucoma. When ganglion cell loss was minimal, dysfunction reduced sensitivity (for homogeneous dysfunction) or fraction of normal cells (for heterogeneous dysfunction) to approximately 20% to 30% of normal. When the percentage of ganglion cells remaining decreased below 50%, the effects of dysfunction became even more pronounced. DISCUSSION Our two-stage neural model of perimetric sensitivity found that effects of ganglion cell death yielded nonlinear functions for perimetric defect in decibels versus ganglion cell loss in percentages, for both sparse and dense ganglion cell mosaics and for spatial filters tuned to a wide range of spatial frequencies. The functions are consistent with results of studies comparing perimetric sensitivity to human ganglion cell counts 3,4 and to human neuroretinal rim area. 9 Perimetric defects do not become deeper than 5 db until ganglion cell density is reduced to below half of normal density, and then, at greater amounts of cell death, perimetric defects rapidly become deeper. Perimetric defect expressed in linear units (percentage of normal) was linearly related to ganglion cell loss for spatial filters tuned to relatively low spatial frequencies. This is consistent with the findings of recent studies that compared perimetric sensitivity with mvep signal amplitude, 6,7 neuroretinal rim area and pattern electroretinogram amplitude. 12 The reason for the linear relation in the model is that spatial filters mediating detection linearly sum the weighted responses of the ganglion cells, and therefore their sensitivity tends to decrease linearly as the number of ganglion cells declines. As long as each spatial filter pools responses of a large number of ganglion cells, then cell loss affects all filters similarly. However, if the filters are tuned to a high enough spatial frequency that they sum responses of only a few ganglion cells, ganglion cell loss causes some filters to have greatly reduced sensitivity whereas others retain near-normal sensitivity. At higher spatial frequencies, probability summation across spatial filters results in perimetric sensitivity remaining near normal until the degree of loss becomes so great that all filters under the stimulus are affected. This is a quantitative expression of an idea presented qualitatively in prior studies. 35,37 Cell death alone cannot account for the finding in experimental glaucoma that perimetric defects can occur in the absence of ganglion cell loss. 5,37 For the model to account for this finding, it must include dysfunction in surviving ganglion cells, with the degree of dysfunction increasing as the number of cells remaining declines below 50% (Fig. 5). This type of dysfunction would introduce nonlinearities in the relations between perimetric defect and cell loss for spatial filters tuned to lower spatial frequencies, and could make the relations more linear for spatial filters tuned to higher spatial frequencies. Various combinations of cell death and dysfunction could account for the finding of linear relations between perimetric defect (in percentage) and neuroretinal rim area.

5 470 Swanson et al. IOVS, February 2004, Vol. 45, No. 2 they obtained reflects a combination of effects of ganglion cell death combined with ganglion cell dysfunction. Our analysis of the effects of eccentricity on normal ganglion cell density found that, at eccentricities beyond 15, the relation between perimetric defect and ganglion cell loss should be linear (Fig. 2). To understand better the finding of linear relations between perimetric sensitivity and clinical measures of ganglion cell loss, it would be useful to assess linearity in structure and function studies in retinal regions outside the central 15. CONCLUSIONS A quantitative two-stage neural model for perimetric sensitivity provides a theoretical framework that complements and extends the empiric framework used in the historical development of perimetric methods. The model demonstrates how ganglion cell death and dysfunction can account for empiric findings about relations between perimetric and structural measures of glaucomatous damage, and provides theoretical support for the use of linear units (arithmetic mean) for averaging perimetric sensitivities. The key role of second-stage spatial filters in providing linearity illustrates the potential value of using perimetric stimuli for which sensitivity is mediated by spatial filters that pool the responses of a large number of ganglion cells. 38 Acknowledgments The authors thank Mitchell W. Dul for helpful comments on a draft of the manuscript. APPENDIX FIGURE 5. Use of ganglion cell dysfunction to fit data from experimental glaucoma, with both perimetric defect and ganglion cell loss in percentage of normal. Top: filled circles show perimetric defect versus ganglion cell loss in experimental glaucoma 37 ; Bottom: The amounts of dysfunction used for the fits shown (top). Structural measures are for larger retinal areas than standard perimetric measures; thus, for structure and function studies, it is common to average perimetric sensitivities across a number of locations. The traditional methods for averaging perimetric sensitivities, such as the mean deviation (MD), use a geometric mean (average in decibel units). However, to the extent that perimetric sensitivity is linearly related to ganglion cell loss, geometric means tend to overestimate average loss. For instance, if one location has normal ganglion cell density, and a second location has only 1% of ganglion cells remaining, the average number of ganglion cells would be 50.5%, whereas the geometric mean would be approximately 10 db, or 10%. For averaging sensitivities across regions with inhomogeneous damage, the arithmetic mean (average in linear units) may provide a better estimate of the average amount of loss. 6,7 In a recent study, arithmetic means yielded more linear relations than geometric means for comparisons of perimetric defects with neuroretinal rim area and with pattern ERG amplitude. 12 That study restricted visual field location to a central square ( 10 ), which is the region of the visual field for which critical areas yield the highest equivalent spatial frequencies (Fig. 3). Nonlinearities introduced by higher-spatial frequency filters are more likely for this region than for the rest of the visual field, and therefore it is possible that the linear relation Spatial Filters The primary factor in the model affecting linearity of relations between perimetric defect and ganglion cell defect is the peak spatial frequency of the second-stage spatial filters that pool ganglion cell responses. Secondary calculations were performed to determine the extent to which the conclusions of the model are not affected by varying the form (orientation tuning, spatial phase, and spatial bandwidth) used for the spatial filters. The primary calculations, for which results are shown in Figure 4, used spatial filters with a spatial bandwidth (at halfheight) of one octave, an orientation bandwidth of 54, sine phase, and no DC component (i.e., no change in mean luminance). Each filter was produced by multiplying a one-dimensional fifth-derivative-of-gaussian (D5) function 39 with the desired peak spatial frequency by an orthogonal Gaussian whose space constant was equal to the square root of three divided by times the spatial frequency. 40 There were eight filters centered at each filter location identical except that orientation tuning varied from 0 to 315 in steps of 45. The effects of phase of the spatial filters were explored by comparing results for sine phase and cosine phase. The sine phase filters were the D5 filters used in the primary calculations, and the cosine phase filters were sixth-derivative-of- Gaussian (D6) filters with the same orientation tuning. The effects of orientation tuning of the spatial filters were explored by using nonoriented (circularly-symmetric) filters as well as oriented filters with much narrower orientation bandwidths than the filters used in the primary calculations. The nonoriented circularly symmetric spatial filters were two-dimensional difference-of-gaussians functions (DoGs) with no DC component (the surround had sensitivity one quarter that of the center and space constant twice as large, giving a spatial bandwidth at half-height of two octaves). The strongly oriented spatial filters had a space constant for the orthogonal Gaussian

6 IOVS, February 2004, Vol. 45, No. 2 Perimetric Defects and Ganglion Cell Damage 471 that was four times larger than for the primary orientations, yielding an orientation bandwidth of 14 (orientation of different filters was varied in steps of 15 ). The only effect of varying the form of the spatial filters was for the cosine phase D6 filters when the percentage of ganglion cells remaining was moderate (25% 75%). At higher spatial frequencies (4.0 cyc/deg for the dense mosaic, 2.0 and 4.0 cyc/deg for the sparse mosaic) sensitivity became as much as 4 db better than normal. This effect was minimal or absent with the other types of filters. Because the D6 filters are in cosine phase, there are two inhibitory flanks with similar magnitude, and for the most sensitive filters the stimulus falls on only one of the two inhibitory flanks. The increased sensitivity to moderate amounts of ganglion cell loss reflects the effects of loss of ganglion cells providing input to the inhibitory flanks of spatial filters near the inside edge of the stimulus. For the D5 filters used in the primary calculations, there was only one major inhibitory flank, and therefore the most sensitive filters were those with the inhibitory flank outside the stimulus. For these filters, there was little inhibition produced by the stimulus, and loss of ganglion cell input to the flank therefore had little or no effect on sensitivity. The circular DoG filters have a circular inhibitory surround, so the stimulus must fall on at least a portion of the inhibitory region of filters near the edge of the stimulus and therefore must produce some inhibition. However, the surround of the circular DoG filters is weaker than the flanks for the D6 filters, and thus loss of cells providing input to the surround produces a smaller increase in sensitivity. The prediction of increased sensitivity does not affect the conclusions that for higher spatial frequencies there is relatively little loss in perimetric sensitivity until the percentage of ganglion cells remaining decreases below 25% and that perimetric sensitivity decreases dramatically when the percentage of ganglion cells remaining declines below 10%. In summary, varying the form used for the spatial filters did not alter the conclusions of the model. Spacing of Ganglion Cells and Spatial Filters In the primary calculations, the spacing between the ganglion cells in the mosaics varied with the size of the receptive field centers. The primary difference between the sparse and dense mosaics was that sensitivity-versus-cell-loss functions became nonlinear at higher spatial frequencies for the dense mosaic than for the sparse mosaic (Fig. 4). In secondary calculations that explored the effects of ganglion cell spacing, the small receptive field size of the dense mosaic was used with the ganglion cell spacing of the sparse mosaic. The results were similar to results for the sparse mosaic with large receptive fields: functions were linear for 1.0 cyc/deg and became nonlinear for 2.0 and 4.0 cyc/deg. The main effect of smaller ganglion cell receptive fields was to make the nonlinearity at 2.0 cyc/deg slightly less severe for the smaller receptive fields than for the larger receptive fields (i.e., defects were as much as 3 db deeper). Density of ganglion cells, rather than receptive field size, accounted for the primary differences between the two mosaics. For the primary calculations the spacing between spatial filters was set equal to the spacing between the ganglion cells. 41 The diameter of the array of locations used for the filters equaled the sum of the stimulus diameter plus the width of four cycles of the filter s peak spatial frequency; filters more distant than this would have minimal response to the stimulus. The number of filter locations ranged from a minimum of 120 for the sparse mosaic sampled by filters with a peak spatial frequency of 4.0 cyc/deg (cyc/deg) to more than 60,000 for the dense mosaic sampled by filters with a peak spatial frequency of 0.5 cyc/deg. For the dense mosaic with spatial filters tuned to 0.5 cyc/deg, calculation on a computer (466 MHz Macintosh G4; Apple Computer, Cupertino, CA) of perimetric sensitivity for the eight different patterns of loss required over 30 days. In secondary calculations, the spacing of the spatial filters was varied independently of the spacing of the ganglion cells. Results were similar to those for the primary calculations, except that the nonlinearity at 4.0 cyc/deg became more pronounced for the sparse mosaic sampled with spatial filters at the density of the dense mosaic (i.e., defect depth was not as deep as in the primary calculations). Variation in spacing of the spatial filters had no effect on the basic result that for higher spatial frequencies there is relatively little loss in perimetric sensitivity until the percentage of ganglion cells remaining decreases to below 25% and then increases dramatically as the number of ganglion cells declines below 10%. Dysfunction of Ganglion Cells Effects of heterogeneous ganglion cell dysfunction were simulated in secondary calculations for the sparse mosaic with second-stage filters tuned to 1.0 cyc/deg. A binary distribution was used in which half of the cells (randomly selected) retained normal sensitivity and half had sensitivity reduced to 10% of normal. This gave the same result as an equivalent loss of ganglion cells. In general, for low-frequency filters, heterogeneous dysfunction has results similar to the effects of cell loss, in that dysfunctional cells make little contribution to perimetric sensitivity. Effects of homogeneous dysfunction were simulated by reducing responsiveness of all ganglion cells in a mosaic by a fixed amount. For all degrees of ganglion cell loss, homogeneous dysfunction decreased perimetric sensitivity by the same amount. For low-spatial-frequency filters, perimetric sensitivity is linearly related to percentage of ganglion cells remaining, and thus homogeneous and heterogeneous dysfunction are equivalent. Cortical Reorganization in Response to Ganglion Cell Loss The model assumes that the relative weights assigned by the second-stage spatial filters to the ganglion cell inputs remains unchanged as the ganglion cells die or become dysfunctional. This assumption may not be valid, because a lesion study found that, when local regions of retinal cells are destroyed, the corresponding cortical cells can undergo reorganization of their receptive fields. 42 The lesion study, in which all cells from a given region are damaged, is not directly comparable to the heterogeneous damage used in the current model. However, it is possible that there is a reorganization of the weighting functions of the spatial filters. If the filters lost orientation tuning or expanded in size, there would be little impact on the predictions of the model for filters with peak frequencies of 1.0 cyc/deg or lower. If filters tuned to higher spatial frequencies expanded in size, then the perimetric defect would be more linearly related to ganglion cell loss (as seen with lower spatial frequencies). If the filters shifted the location in visual space where they have peak sensitivity, then they would be similar to filters in the model already at that location and would have little effect on the predictions of the model. Reorganization of second-stage filters may affect the precise shape of the functions in Figure 4, but would have little impact on the general conclusions drawn from these functions, other than possibly to extend linearity to higher spatial frequencies. References 1. The American Academy of Ophthalmology Preferred Practice Patterns Committee Glaucoma Panel. Primary Open-Angle Glaucoma: Preferred Practice Pattern. San Francisco: American Academy of Ophthalmology; 2000;1 40.

7 472 Swanson et al. IOVS, February 2004, Vol. 45, No Latham K, Whitaker D, Wild JM, Elliott DB. Magnification perimetry. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993;34: Quigley HA, Dunkelberger GR, Green WR. Retinal ganglion cell atrophy correlated with automated perimetry in human eyes with glaucoma. Am J Ophthalmol. 1989;107: Kerrigan-Baumrind LA, Quigley HA, Pease ME, Kerrigan DF, Mitchell RS. Number of ganglion cells in glaucoma eyes compared with threshold visual field tests in the same persons. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41: Harwerth RS, Carter-Dawson L, Shen F, Smith EL III, Crawford ML. Ganglion cell losses underlying visual field defects from experimental glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999;40: Hood DC, Greenstein VC. Multifocal VEP and ganglion cell damage: applications and limitations for the study of glaucoma. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2003;22: Hood DC, Greenstein VC, Odel JG, et al. Visual field defects and multifocal visual evoked potentials: evidence of a linear relationship. Arch Ophthalmol. 2002;120: Jonas JB, Grundler AE. Correlation between mean visual field loss and morphometric optic disk variables in the open-angle glaucomas. Am J Ophthalmol. 1997;124: Bartz-Schmidt KU, Thumann G, Jonescu-Cuypers CP, Krieglstein GK. Quantitative morphologic and functional evaluation of the optic nerve head in chronic open-angle glaucoma. Surv Ophthalmol. 1999;44(suppl 1):S41 S Garway-Heath DF, Viswanathan A, Westcott M, Kamal D, Fitzke FW, Hitchings RA. Relationship between perimetric light sensitivity and optic disc neuroretinal rim area. In: Wall M, Wild JM, eds. Perimetry Update 1998/1999. The Hague: Kugler Publications; 1999: Kwon YH, Hong S, Honkanen RA, Alward WL. Correlation of automated visual field parameters and peripapillary nerve fiber layer thickness as measured by scanning laser polarimetry. J Glaucoma. 2000;9: Garway-Heath DF, Holder GE, Fitzke FW, Hitchings RA. Relationship between electrophysiological, psychophysical, and anatomical measurements in glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2002; 43: Quigley HA. Neuronal death in glaucoma. Prog Retin Eye Res. 1999;18: Quigley HA. Open-angle glaucoma. N Engl J Med. 1993;328: Katz LJ, Spaeth GL, Cantor LB, Poryzees EM, Steinmann WC. Reversibility of optic disc cupping and visual field improvement in adults with glaucoma. Am J Ophthalmol. 1989;107: Quaranta L, Bettelli S, Uva MG, Semeraro F, Turano R, Gandolfo E. Effect of Ginkgo biloba extract on preexisting visual field damage in normal tension glaucoma. Ophthalmology. 2003;110: Goldmann H. Fundamentals of exact perimetry Optom Vis Sci. 1999;76: Derrington AM, Lennie P. Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivities of neurones in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. J Physiol (Lond). 1984;357: Croner LJ, Kaplan E. Receptive fields of P and M ganglion cells across the primate retina. Vision Res. 1995;35: Crook JM, Lange-Malecki B, Lee BB, Valberg A. Visual resolution of macaque retinal ganglion cells. J Physiol (Lond). 1988;396: Sjostrand J, Olsson V, Popovic Z, Conradi N. Quantitative estimations of foveal and extra-foveal retinal circuitry in humans. Vision Res. 1999;39: DeValois R, DeValois KK. Spatial Vision. New York: Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; Graham N. Visual Pattern Analyzers. New York: Oxford University Press; Quick RF. A vector-magnitude model of contrast detection. Kybernetik. 1974;16: Robson JG, Graham N. Probability summation and regional variation in contrast sensitivity across the visual field. Vision Res. 1981;21: Meese TS, Williams CB. Probability summation for multiple patches of luminance modulation. Vision Res. 2000;40: Virsu V, Hari R. Cortical magnification, scale invariance and visual ecology. Vision Res. 1996;36: Watson AB. Estimation of local spatial scale. J Opt Soc Am A. 1987;4: Swanson WH, Wilson HR. Eccentricity effects on contrast matching and oblique masking. Vision Res. 1985;25: Felius J, Swanson WH, Fellman RL, Lynn JR, Starita RJ. Spatial summation for selected ganglion cell mosaics in patients with glaucoma. In: Wall M, Heijl A, eds. Perimetry Update 1996/1997. The Hague, The Netherlands: Kugler; 1997: Wilson ME. Invariant features of spatial summation with changing locus in the visual field. J Physiol (Lond). 1970;207: Garway-Heath DF, Caprioli J, Fitzke FW, Hitchings RA. Scaling the hill of vision: the physiological relationship between light sensitivity and ganglion cell numbers. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000; 41: Curcio CA, Allen KA. Topography of ganglion cells in human retina. J Comp Neurol. 1990;300: Heijl A, Lindgren G, Olsson J. Normal variability of static perimetric threshold values across the central visual field. Arch Ophthalmol. 1987;105: Fellman RL, Lynn JR, Starita RJ, Swanson WH. Clinical importance of spatial summation in glaucoma. In: Heijl A, ed. Perimetry Update 1988/1989. Berkeley, CA: Kugler & Ghedini; 1989; Henson DB, Evans J, Chauhan BC, Lane C. Influence of fixation accuracy on threshold variability in patients with open angle glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1996;37: Harwerth RS, Crawford ML, Frishman LJ, Viswanathan S, Smith EL III, Carter-Dawson L. Visual field defects and neural losses from experimental glaucoma. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2002;21: Pearson P, Swanson WH, Fellman RL. Chromatic and achromatic defects in patients with progressing glaucoma. Vision Res. 2001; 41: Swanson WH, Wilson HR, Giese SC. Contrast matching data predicted from contrast increment thresholds. Vision Res. 1984;24: Swanson WH, Birch EE. Infant spatiotemporal vision: dependence of spatial contrast sensitivity on temporal frequency. Vision Res. 1990;30: Alonso JM, Usrey WM, Reid RC. Rules of connectivity between geniculate cells and simple cells in cat primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2001;21: Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Receptive field dynamics in adult primary visual cortex. Nature. 1992;356:

TUMBLING E RESOLUTION PERIMETRY IN GLAUCOMA

TUMBLING E RESOLUTION PERIMETRY IN GLAUCOMA Tumbling E resolution perimetry in glaucoma 179 TUMBLING E RESOLUTION PERIMETRY IN GLAUCOMA FERGAL A. ENNIS 1, ROGER S. ANDERSON 1, WINSTON S. McCLEAN 1 and SIMON J.A. RANKIN 2 1 Vision Science Research

More information

The Relationship between Standard Automated Perimetry and GDx VCC Measurements METHODS

The Relationship between Standard Automated Perimetry and GDx VCC Measurements METHODS The Relationship between Standard Automated Perimetry and GDx VCC Measurements Nicolaas J. Reus and Hans G. Lemij PURPOSE. To investigate the relationship between retinal light sensitivity measured with

More information

Perimetric testing is used clinically to detect visual field

Perimetric testing is used clinically to detect visual field Quantifying Effects of Retinal Illuminance on Frequency Doubling Perimetry William H. Swanson, Mitchell W. Dul, and Susan E. Fischer PURPOSE. To measure and quantify effects of variation in retinal illuminance

More information

Ganglion cell complex scan in the early prediction of glaucoma

Ganglion cell complex scan in the early prediction of glaucoma Original article in the early prediction of glaucoma Ganekal S Nayana Super Specialty Eye Hospital and Research Center, Davangere, Karnataka, India Abstract Objective: To compare the macular ganglion cell

More information

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3 eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3 retinal specialization fovea: highest density of photoreceptors, aimed at where you are looking -> highest acuity optic disk: cell-free area, where

More information

T he retinal ganglion cells of different sizes have distinct

T he retinal ganglion cells of different sizes have distinct 604 CLINICAL SCIENCE Agreement between frequency doubling perimetry and static perimetry in eyes with high tension glaucoma and normal tension glaucoma S Kogure, Y Toda, D Crabb, K Kashiwagi, F W Fitzke,

More information

Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons

Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons Vincent Bonin *, Valerio Mante and Matteo Carandini Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute 2318 Fillmore Street San Francisco, CA 94115, USA Institute of Neuroinformatics

More information

Responses of Primate Retinal Ganglion Cells to Perimetric Stimuli METHODS

Responses of Primate Retinal Ganglion Cells to Perimetric Stimuli METHODS Glaucoma Responses of Primate Retinal Ganglion Cells to Perimetric Stimuli William H. Swanson, 1 Hao Sun, 2 Barry B. Lee, 3,4 and Dingcai Cao 5 PURPOSE. Perimetry is used clinically to assess glaucomatous

More information

THE STRUCTURE-FUNCTION JUNCTION

THE STRUCTURE-FUNCTION JUNCTION THE STRUCTURE-FUNCTION JUNCTION Craig Thomas, O.D. 3900 West Wheatland Road Dallas, Texas 75237 972-780-7199 thpckc@yahoo.com Paul M. Karpecki, O.D., FAAO 120 N Eagle Creek Drive # 431 Lexington, KY 40509

More information

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1 Announcements exam 1 this Thursday! review session: Wednesday, 5:00-6:30pm, Meliora 203 Bryce s office hours: Wednesday, 3:30-5:30pm, Gleason https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zdw7pvgz0um M Cells M cells

More information

Study of Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer Thickness Within Normal Hemivisual Field in Primary Open-Angle Glaucoma and Normal-Tension Glaucoma

Study of Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer Thickness Within Normal Hemivisual Field in Primary Open-Angle Glaucoma and Normal-Tension Glaucoma Study of Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer Thickness Within Normal Hemivisual Field in Primary Open-Angle Glaucoma and Normal-Tension Glaucoma Chiharu Matsumoto, Shiroaki Shirato, Mai Haneda, Hiroko Yamashiro

More information

Visual field defects and neural losses from experimental glaucoma

Visual field defects and neural losses from experimental glaucoma Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 21 (2002) 91 125 Visual field defects and neural losses from experimental glaucoma Ronald S. Harwerth a, *, M.L.J. Crawford b, Laura J. Frishman a, Suresh Viswanathan

More information

IUSO Bill Swanson PubMed Papers June 28, 2012

IUSO Bill Swanson PubMed Papers June 28, 2012 1. Malik R, Swanson WH, Garway-Heath DF. 'Structure-function relationship' in glaucoma: past thinking and current concepts. Clin Experiment Ophthalmol. 2012 May;40(4):369-80. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-9071.2012.02770.x.

More information

Monocular and Binocular Mechanisms of Contrast Gain Control. Izumi Ohzawa and Ralph D. Freeman

Monocular and Binocular Mechanisms of Contrast Gain Control. Izumi Ohzawa and Ralph D. Freeman Monocular and Binocular Mechanisms of Contrast Gain Control Izumi Ohzawa and alph D. Freeman University of California, School of Optometry Berkeley, California 9472 E-mail: izumi@pinoko.berkeley.edu ABSTACT

More information

Electronic Letters to: Electronic letters published: Multiple Mechanisms in the VEP 23 July von :24

Electronic Letters to: Electronic letters published: Multiple Mechanisms in the VEP 23 July von :24 QUICK SEARCH: Author: Go [advanced] Keyword(s): HOME HELP FEEDBACK SUBSCRIPTIONS ARCHIVE SEARCH Year: Vol: Page: Electronic Letters to: Visual Neurophysiology: Givago S. Souza, Bruno D. Gomes, Cézar A.

More information

Glaucoma THE STRUCTURE-FUNCTION JUNCTION. Measuring Structure and Function. Structure-Function Relationship. Structure-Function Relationship

Glaucoma THE STRUCTURE-FUNCTION JUNCTION. Measuring Structure and Function. Structure-Function Relationship. Structure-Function Relationship THE STRUCTURE-FUNCTION JUNCTION Craig Thomas, O.D. 3900 West Wheatland Road Dallas, Texas 75237 972-780-7199 thpckc@yahoo.com Glaucoma Glaucoma is a family of chronic, progressive optic neuropathies characterized

More information

Characteristics of Frequency-of-Seeing Curves in Normal Subjects, Patients With Suspected Glaucoma, and Patients With Glaucoma

Characteristics of Frequency-of-Seeing Curves in Normal Subjects, Patients With Suspected Glaucoma, and Patients With Glaucoma Characteristics of Frequency-of-Seeing Curves in Normal Subjects, Patients With Suspected Glaucoma, and Patients With Glaucoma Balwantray C. Chauhan* James D. Tompkins,-\ Raymond P. LeBlanc* and Terry

More information

The overall rate of decrease in visual field sensitivity in

The overall rate of decrease in visual field sensitivity in Glaucoma Estimating the True Distribution of Visual Field Progression Rates in Glaucoma Andrew J. Anderson Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Australia

More information

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the Some transformations of color information from lateral geniculate nucleus to striate cortex Russell L. De Valois*, Nicolas P. Cottaris, Sylvia D. Elfar*, Luke E. Mahon, and J. Anthony Wilson* *Psychology

More information

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I Monocular Sensory Processes of Vision: Color Vision Mechanisms of Color Processing . Neural Mechanisms of Color Processing A. Parallel processing - M- & P- pathways B. Second

More information

Visual Fields: Back to the Future

Visual Fields: Back to the Future Optometry in Practice 2011 Volume 12 Issue 1 11 20 Visual Fields: Back to the Future Tony Redmond 1 BSc PhD MCOptom, and Roger S Anderson 1, 2 BSc MPhil PhD MCOptom 1 NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for

More information

Retinal DOG filters: high-pass or high-frequency enhancing filters?

Retinal DOG filters: high-pass or high-frequency enhancing filters? Retinal DOG filters: high-pass or high-frequency enhancing filters? Adrián Arias 1, Eduardo Sánchez 1, and Luis Martínez 2 1 Grupo de Sistemas Inteligentes (GSI) Centro Singular de Investigación en Tecnologías

More information

Optic neuritis (ON) is a clinical syndrome characterized

Optic neuritis (ON) is a clinical syndrome characterized Tracking the Recovery of Local Optic Nerve Function after Optic Neuritis: A Multifocal VEP Study Donald C. Hood, 1 Jeffrey G. Odel, 2 and Xian Zhang 1 PURPOSE. To explore the multifocal visual evoked potential

More information

Advances in OCT Murray Fingeret, OD

Advances in OCT Murray Fingeret, OD Disclosures Advances in OCT Murray Fingeret, OD Consultant Alcon, Allergan, Bausch & Lomb, Carl Zeiss Meditec, Diopsys, Heidelberg Engineering, Reichert, Topcon Currently Approved OCT Devices OCT Devices

More information

CHAPTER 11 KINETIC PERIMETRY WHAT IS KINETIC PERIMETRY? LIMITATIONS OF STATIC PERIMETRY LOW SPATIAL RESOLUTION

CHAPTER 11 KINETIC PERIMETRY WHAT IS KINETIC PERIMETRY? LIMITATIONS OF STATIC PERIMETRY LOW SPATIAL RESOLUTION 205 CHAPTER 11 KINETIC PERIMETRY WHAT IS KINETIC PERIMETRY? LIMITATIONS OF STATIC PERIMETRY LOW SPATIAL RESOLUTION Static perimetry is currently the most commonly used type of perimetry. With static perimetry,

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

Contrast sensitivity and retinal ganglion cell responses in the primate

Contrast sensitivity and retinal ganglion cell responses in the primate PSYCHOLOGY NEUROSCIENCE Psychology & Neuroscience, 2011, 4, 1, 11-18 DOI: 10.3922/j.psns.2011.1.003 Contrast sensitivity and retinal ganglion cell responses in the primate Barry B. Lee 1,2 and Hao Sun

More information

Correlating Structure With Function in End-Stage Glaucoma

Correlating Structure With Function in End-Stage Glaucoma C L I N I C A L S C I E N C E Correlating Structure With Function in End-Stage Glaucoma Eytan Z. Blumenthal, MD; Amjad Horani, MD; Rajesh Sasikumar, MD; Chandrasekhar Garudadri, MD; Addepalli Udaykumar,

More information

Science & Technologies

Science & Technologies STANDARD COMPUTERIZED PERIMETRY IN FUNCTION OF DIAGNOSTIC GLAUCOMA Iljaz Ismaili, 1 Gazepov Strahil, 2, Goshevska Dashtevska Emilija 1 1 University Eye Clinic,Skopje 2 Clinical Hospital, Shtip Abstract

More information

The Nottingham eprints service makes this work by researchers of the University of Nottingham available open access under the following conditions.

The Nottingham eprints service makes this work by researchers of the University of Nottingham available open access under the following conditions. Denniss, Jonathan and Turpin, Andrew and McKendrick, Allison M. (2014) Visual contrast detection cannot be predicted from surrogate measures of retinal ganglion cell number and sampling density in healthy

More information

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System Lecturer: Jitendra Malik Scribe: Ryan White (Slide: layout of the brain) Facts about the brain:

More information

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye: Problems confronting the visual system: Visual Physiology image contains a huge amount of information which must be processed quickly. image is dim, blurry and distorted. Light levels vary enormously.

More information

Method for comparing visual field defects to local RNFL and RGC damage seen on frequency domain OCT in patients with glaucoma.

Method for comparing visual field defects to local RNFL and RGC damage seen on frequency domain OCT in patients with glaucoma. Method for comparing visual field defects to local RNFL and RGC damage seen on frequency domain OCT in patients with glaucoma. Donald C. Hood 1,2,* and Ali S. Raza 1 1 Department of Psychology, Columbia

More information

New Concepts in Glaucoma Ben Gaddie, OD Moderator Murray Fingeret, OD Louis Pasquale, MD

New Concepts in Glaucoma Ben Gaddie, OD Moderator Murray Fingeret, OD Louis Pasquale, MD New Concepts in Glaucoma Ben Gaddie, OD Moderator Murray Fingeret, OD Louis Pasquale, MD New Concepts in Glaucoma Optical Coherence Tomography: Is it necessary and needed to diagnose and monitor glaucoma?

More information

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity S. B. Stevenson *, L. K. Cormack Vision Research 40, 2881-2884. (2000) * University of Houston College of Optometry, Houston TX 77204

More information

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) What happens beyond the retina? What happens in Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)- 90% flow Visual cortex Information Flow Superior colliculus 10% flow Slide 2 Information

More information

Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning

Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning Maximilian Riesenhuber Tomaso Poggio Center for Biological and Computational Learning and Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences Massachusetts

More information

Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation

Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation articles Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation Paul V. McGraw 1, Dennis M. Levi 2 and David Whitaker 1 1 Department of Optometry, University of Bradford,

More information

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function VISION Vol. 20, No. 2, 67 71, 2008 Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function Keizo SHINOMORI Department of Information Systems Engineering, Kochi University of Technology

More information

Glaucoma is defined as a progressive optic neuropathy that. A Comparison of Functional and Structural Measures for Identifying Progression of Glaucoma

Glaucoma is defined as a progressive optic neuropathy that. A Comparison of Functional and Structural Measures for Identifying Progression of Glaucoma Glaucoma A Comparison of Functional and Structural Measures for Identifying Progression of Glaucoma Daiyan Xin, 1 Vivienne C. Greenstein, 2 Robert Ritch, 3,4 Jeffrey M. Liebmann, 3 Carlos Gustavo De Moraes,

More information

Financial Disclosure. Visual Field Interpretation RELIABILITY VISUAL FIELD INTERPRETATION IN GLAUCOMA METHODS OF DATA PRESENTATION

Financial Disclosure. Visual Field Interpretation RELIABILITY VISUAL FIELD INTERPRETATION IN GLAUCOMA METHODS OF DATA PRESENTATION VISUAL FIELD INTERPRETATION IN GLAUCOMA Danica J. Marrelli, OD, FAAO University of Houston College of Optometry Financial Disclosure I have received speaking and/or consulting fees from: Aerie Pharmaceutical

More information

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Javier R. Movellan Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0515 movellan@inc.ucsd.edu Thomas

More information

A Single Mechanism Can Explain the Speed Tuning Properties of MT and V1 Complex Neurons

A Single Mechanism Can Explain the Speed Tuning Properties of MT and V1 Complex Neurons The Journal of Neuroscience, November 15, 2006 26(46):11987 11991 11987 Brief Communications A Single Mechanism Can Explain the Speed Tuning Properties of MT and V1 Complex Neurons John A. Perrone Department

More information

As methods become available to measure the structure of

As methods become available to measure the structure of Combining Ganglion Cell Topology and Data of Patients with Glaucoma to Determine a Structure Function Map Andrew Turpin, 1 Geoff P. Sampson, 2 and Allison M. McKendrick 2 PURPOSE. To introduce techniques

More information

The Effect of Stimulus Size on the Reliable Stimulus Range of Perimetry

The Effect of Stimulus Size on the Reliable Stimulus Range of Perimetry Article DOI: 10.1167/tvst.4.2.10 The Effect of Stimulus Size on the Reliable Stimulus Range of Perimetry Stuart K. Gardiner 1, Shaban Demirel 1, Deborah Goren 1, Steven L. Mansberger 1, and William H.

More information

Correlation of Blue Chromatic Macular Sensitivity with Optic Disc Change in Early Glaucoma Patients

Correlation of Blue Chromatic Macular Sensitivity with Optic Disc Change in Early Glaucoma Patients Correlation of Blue Chromatic Macular Sensitivity with Optic Disc Change in Early Glaucoma Patients Yoshio Yamazaki, Kenji Mizuki, Fukuko Hayamizu and Chizuru Tanaka Department of Ophthalmology, Nihon

More information

Variability in Patients With Glaucomatous Visual Field Damage Is Reduced Using Size V Stimuli

Variability in Patients With Glaucomatous Visual Field Damage Is Reduced Using Size V Stimuli ariability in Patients With Glaucomatous isual Field Damage s Reduced Using Size Stimuli Michael Wall* Kim E. Kutzko,* and Balwantray C. Chauhanf Purpose. To test the hypothesis that variability of conventional

More information

Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color tuning properties

Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color tuning properties Visual Neuroscience (2004), 21, 275 282. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2004 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238004 $16.00 DOI: 10.10170S0952523804213426 Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color

More information

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli Vision Research 39 (1999) 3849 3854 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli A. Johnston a, *, C.P.

More information

Contrast detection thresholds are regularly measured in

Contrast detection thresholds are regularly measured in Glaucoma Impairments of Contrast Discrimination and Contrast Adaptation in Glaucoma Allison M. McKendrick, 1 Geoff P. Sampson, 1,2 Mark J. Walland, 3 and David R. Badcock 4 PURPOSE. Contrast detection

More information

STANDARD AUTOMATED PERIMETRY IS A GENERALLY

STANDARD AUTOMATED PERIMETRY IS A GENERALLY Comparison of Long-term Variability for Standard and Short-wavelength Automated Perimetry in Stable Glaucoma Patients EYTAN Z. BLUMENTHAL, MD, PAMELA A. SAMPLE, PHD, LINDA ZANGWILL, PHD, ALEXANDER C. LEE,

More information

T he lamina cribrosa is a sieve-like membrane in the

T he lamina cribrosa is a sieve-like membrane in the 1299 EXTENDED REPORT Three dimensional analysis of the lamina cribrosa in glaucoma J Morgan-Davies, N Taylor, A R Hill, P Aspinall, CJ O Brien, A Azuara-Blanco... See end of article for authors affiliations...

More information

STRUCTURE & FUNCTION An Integrated Approach for the Detection and Follow-up of Glaucoma. Module 3a GDx

STRUCTURE & FUNCTION An Integrated Approach for the Detection and Follow-up of Glaucoma. Module 3a GDx STRUCTURE & FUNCTION An Integrated Approach for the Detection and Follow-up of Glaucoma Module 3a GDx Educational Slide Deck Carl Zeiss Meditec, Inc. November 2005 1 Structure & Function Modules Module

More information

Multifocal Electroretinograms in Patients with Branch Retinal Artery Occlusion

Multifocal Electroretinograms in Patients with Branch Retinal Artery Occlusion Multifocal Electroretinograms in Patients with Branch Retinal Artery Occlusion Shigeru Hasegawa, Akira Ohshima, Yuuki Hayakawa, Mineo Takagi, and Haruki Abe PURPOSE. To investigate the usefulness of second-order

More information

NERVE FIBER LAYER THICKNESS IN NORMALS AND GLAUCOMA PATIENTS

NERVE FIBER LAYER THICKNESS IN NORMALS AND GLAUCOMA PATIENTS Nerve fiber layer thickness in normals and glaucoma patients 403 NERVE FIBER LAYER THICKNESS IN NORMALS AND GLAUCOMA PATIENTS HIROTAKA SUZUMURA, KAYOKO HARASAWA, AKIKO KOBAYASHI and NARIYOSHI ENDO Department

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Normal and amblyopic contrast sensitivity functions in central and peripheral retinas

Normal and amblyopic contrast sensitivity functions in central and peripheral retinas Normal and amblyopic contrast sensitivity functions in central and peripheral retinas Joseph Thomas Contrast sensitivity functions (CSF's)for temporally modulated sine wave gratings were established at

More information

Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension

Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension Christopher W. Tyler Temporal modulation sensitivity for a 5 flickering field was measured in central and peripheral retina

More information

G laucoma is a progressive optic neuropathy in which

G laucoma is a progressive optic neuropathy in which 1135 EXTENDED REPORT The correlation between optic nerve head topographic measurements, peripapillary nerve fibre layer thickness, and visual field indices in glaucoma Y-W Lan, D B Henson, A J Kwartz...

More information

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Baran Çürüklü Department of Computer Science and Engineering Mälardalen University Västerås S-721 23, Sweden Abstract. In this paper

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences Marc Pomplun 1, Yueju Liu 2, Julio Martinez-Trujillo 2, Evgueni Simine 2, and John K. Tsotsos 2 1 Department

More information

Balance between pattern and flicker sensitivities in

Balance between pattern and flicker sensitivities in British Journal of Ophthalmology, 1984, 68, 31-315 Balance between pattern and flicker sensitivities in the visual fields of ophthalmological patients D. REGAN AND D. NEIMA From the Department of Ophthalmology,

More information

Fluctuations on the Humphrey and Octopus Perimeters

Fluctuations on the Humphrey and Octopus Perimeters May 987 Vol. 28/ Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science A Journal of Dosic and Clinical Research Articles Fluctuations on the and Perimeters Randall S. Drenton and William A. Argus Fluctuation of

More information

Scotopic contrast sensitivity in infants evaluated by evoked potentials

Scotopic contrast sensitivity in infants evaluated by evoked potentials Scotopic contrast sensitivity in infants evaluated by evoked potentials Adriana Fiorentini, M. Pirchio, and Donatella Spinelli* The contrast sensitivity function of infants 2V% to 6 months old has been

More information

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 05 Why do you need bright light to read? Rods and cones Photoreceptors are not evenly distributed across the retina 1 Rods and cones Cones are most dense in

More information

Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey

Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey Visual Neuroscience (2001), 18, 501 516. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2001 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238001 $12.50 DOI: 10.1017.S0952523801184014 Motion direction signals in the primary visual

More information

Early Treatment of Congenital Unilateral Cataract Minimizes Unequal Competition

Early Treatment of Congenital Unilateral Cataract Minimizes Unequal Competition Early Treatment of Congenital Unilateral Cataract Minimizes Unequal Competition 12 Eileen E. Birch, 12 David Stager? Joel Leffler? and David Weakley 23 PURPOSE. Dense congenital unilateral cataracts may

More information

Retinal nerve fiber layer thickness in Indian eyes with optical coherence tomography

Retinal nerve fiber layer thickness in Indian eyes with optical coherence tomography Original articles in Indian eyes with optical coherence tomography Malik A, Singh M, Arya SK, Sood S, Ichhpujani P Department of Ophthalmology Government Medical College and Hospital, Sector 32, Chandigarh,

More information

Spectrograms (revisited)

Spectrograms (revisited) Spectrograms (revisited) We begin the lecture by reviewing the units of spectrograms, which I had only glossed over when I covered spectrograms at the end of lecture 19. We then relate the blocks of a

More information

Vision Research 48 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 48 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 48 (2008) 2657 2662 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Chromatic temporal integration and retinal eccentricity: Psychophysics,

More information

The integration of visual information over space is a critical

The integration of visual information over space is a critical Linkage between retinal ganglion cell density and the nonuniform spatial integration across the visual field MiYoung Kwon a,1, and Rong Liu a,1 a Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University

More information

The frequency-doubling illusion occurs when a low-spatialfrequency

The frequency-doubling illusion occurs when a low-spatialfrequency Sensitivity and Specificity of Frequency Doubling Perimetry in Neuro-ophthalmic Disorders: A Comparison with Conventional Automated Perimetry Michael Wall, 1,2 Richard K. Neahring, 1 and Kimberly R. Woodward

More information

Visual Search: A Novel Psychophysics for Preattentive Vision

Visual Search: A Novel Psychophysics for Preattentive Vision Visual Search: A Novel Psychophysics for Preattentive Vision Elizabeth Williams under the direction of Dr. Jeremy M. Wolfe and Ms. Serena Butcher Brigham and Women s Hospital Research Science Institute

More information

Macular Ganglion Cell Complex Measurement Using Spectral Domain Optical Coherence Tomography in Glaucoma

Macular Ganglion Cell Complex Measurement Using Spectral Domain Optical Coherence Tomography in Glaucoma Med. J. Cairo Univ., Vol. 83, No. 2, September: 67-72, 2015 www.medicaljournalofcairouniversity.net Macular Ganglion Cell Complex Measurement Using Spectral Domain Optical Coherence Tomography in Glaucoma

More information

Transducer model produces facilitation from opposite-sign flanks

Transducer model produces facilitation from opposite-sign flanks Vision Research 39 (1999) 987 992 Transducer model produces facilitation from opposite-sign flanks Joshua A. Solomon a, *, Andrew B. Watson b, Michael J. Morgan a a Institute of Ophthalmology, Bath Street,

More information

Supplementary Appendix

Supplementary Appendix This appendix has been provided by the authors to give readers additional information about their work. Supplement to: Edwards TL, Jolly JK, MacLaren RE, et al.. N Engl J Med 206;374:996-8. DOI: 0.056/NEJMc50950

More information

Parafoveal Scanning Laser Polarimetry for Early Glaucoma Detection

Parafoveal Scanning Laser Polarimetry for Early Glaucoma Detection Yamanashi Med. J. 18(1), 15~ 20, 2003 Original Article Parafoveal Scanning Laser Polarimetry for Early Glaucoma Detection Satoshi KOGURE, Yoshiki TODA, Hiroyuki IIJIMA and Shigeo TSUKAHARA Department of

More information

OCT in the Diagnosis and Follow-up of Glaucoma

OCT in the Diagnosis and Follow-up of Glaucoma OCT in the Diagnosis and Follow-up of Glaucoma Karim A Raafat MD. Professor Of Ophthalmology Cairo University Hmmmm! Do I have Glaucoma or not?! 1 Visual Function 100% - N Gl Structure : - 5000 axon /

More information

Scanning Laser Tomography to Evaluate Optic Discs of Normal Eyes

Scanning Laser Tomography to Evaluate Optic Discs of Normal Eyes Scanning Laser Tomography to Evaluate Optic Discs of Normal Eyes Hiroshi Nakamura,* Toshine Maeda,* Yasuyuki Suzuki and Yoichi Inoue* *Eye Division of Olympia Medical Clinic, Tokyo, Japan; Department of

More information

Test-retest variability in visual field testing using frequency doubling technology

Test-retest variability in visual field testing using frequency doubling technology European Journal of Ophthalmology / Vol. 17 no. 2, 2007 / pp. 203-207 Test-retest variability in visual field testing using frequency doubling technology A. HORANI, S. FRENKEL, E.Z. BLUMENTHAL Department

More information

Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 8 November 1972)

Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 8 November 1972) J. Physiol. (1973), 232, pp. 149-162 149 With 8 text-figures Printed in Great Britain PSYCHOPHYSICAL EVIDENCE FOR SUSTAINED AND TRANSIENT DETECTORS IN HUMAN VISION BY J. J. KULIKOWSKI AND D. J. TOLHURST*

More information

How is a grating detected on a narrowband noise masker?

How is a grating detected on a narrowband noise masker? Vision Research 39 (1999) 1133 1142 How is a grating detected on a narrowband noise masker? Jacob Nachmias * Department of Psychology, Uni ersity of Pennsyl ania, 3815 Walnut Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6228,

More information

Electrodiagnostics Alphabet Soup

Electrodiagnostics Alphabet Soup Nathan Lighthizer, O.D., F.A.A.O Assistant Professor, NSUOCO Chief of Specialty Care Clinics Chief of Electrodiagnostics Clinic What is electrodiagnostics testing? Visual Pathway Basic Understanding VEP

More information

The Eye. Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. Today s goals. 5 From eye to brain. Today s reading

The Eye. Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. Today s goals. 5 From eye to brain. Today s reading Cognitive Neuroscience of Language 5 From eye to brain Today s goals Look at the pathways that conduct the visual information from the eye to the visual cortex Marielle Lange http://homepages.inf.ed.ac.uk/mlange/teaching/cnl/

More information

Most aspects of spatial vision are quite immature in the

Most aspects of spatial vision are quite immature in the Development of VEP Vernier Acuity and Grating Acuity in Human Infants Ann M. Skoczenski and Anthony M. Norcia PURPOSE. To compare the developmental sequences of two basic measures of pattern vision, Vernier

More information

CHAPTER 10 NON-CONVENTIONAL PERIMETRY

CHAPTER 10 NON-CONVENTIONAL PERIMETRY 193 CHAPTER 10 NON-CONVENTIONAL PERIMETRY INTRODUCTION Static Standard Automated Perimetry (SAP, alternatively called white-on-white perimetry), which uses a white Goldmann size III stimulus presented

More information

Neuronal responses to plaids

Neuronal responses to plaids Vision Research 39 (1999) 2151 2156 Neuronal responses to plaids Bernt Christian Skottun * Skottun Research, 273 Mather Street, Piedmont, CA 94611-5154, USA Received 30 June 1998; received in revised form

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 October 22.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 October 22. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006 February ; 47(2): 753 767. Effect of Experimental Glaucoma in Primates on Oscillatory Potentials of

More information

LABORATORY SCIENCES. Spatial and Temporal Processing of Threshold Data for Detection of Progressive Glaucomatous Visual Field Loss

LABORATORY SCIENCES. Spatial and Temporal Processing of Threshold Data for Detection of Progressive Glaucomatous Visual Field Loss LABORATORY SCIENCES Spatial and Temporal Processing of Threshold Data for Detection of Progressive Glaucomatous Visual Field Loss Paul G. D. Spry, PhD; Chris A. Johnson, PhD; Alex B. Bates; Andrew Turpin,

More information

Early Detection Of Glaucoma Clinical Clues. Points To Live By. Glaucoma Risk Factors 10/3/2014

Early Detection Of Glaucoma Clinical Clues. Points To Live By. Glaucoma Risk Factors 10/3/2014 Early Detection Of Glaucoma Clinical Clues Eric E. Schmidt, O.D. Omni Eye Specialists Wilmington, NC schmidtyvision@msn.com Points To Live By 25% of G pxs NEVER have IOP >21mm 50% of G pxs have trough

More information

Optic nerve head morphometry in healthy adults using confocal laser scanning tomography

Optic nerve head morphometry in healthy adults using confocal laser scanning tomography 761 SCIENTIFIC REPORT Optic nerve head morphometry in healthy adults using confocal laser scanning tomography M M Hermann, I Theofylaktopoulos, N Bangard, C Jonescu-Cuypers, S Coburger, M Diestelhorst...

More information

VISUAL FIELD TESTING OCCUpies

VISUAL FIELD TESTING OCCUpies CLINICAL SCIENCES Prediction of Glaucomatous Visual Field Loss by Extrapolation of Linear Trends Boel Bengtsson, PhD; Vincent Michael Patella, OD; Anders Heijl, MD, PhD Objective: To investigate how well

More information

Visual improvement following glaucoma surgery: a case report

Visual improvement following glaucoma surgery: a case report Foulsham et al. BMC Ophthalmology 2014, 14:162 CASE REPORT Open Access Visual improvement following glaucoma surgery: a case report William S Foulsham 1, Lanxing Fu 2 and Andrew J Tatham 2* Abstract Background:

More information

RETINAL NERVE FIBER LAYER

RETINAL NERVE FIBER LAYER CLINICAL SCIENCES The Effect of Scan Diameter on Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer Thickness Measurement Using Stratus Optic Coherence Tomography Giacomo Savini, MD; Piero Barboni, MD; Michele Carbonelli, MD;

More information

Dual-directional optokinetic nystagmus elicited by the intermittent display of gratings in primary open-angle glaucoma and normal eyes

Dual-directional optokinetic nystagmus elicited by the intermittent display of gratings in primary open-angle glaucoma and normal eyes Current Eye Research 0271-3683/02/2506-355$16.00 2002, Vol. 25, No. 6, pp. 355 362 Swets & Zeitlinger Dual-directional optokinetic nystagmus elicited by the intermittent display of gratings in primary

More information

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus Central Visual Pathways V1/2 NEUR 3001 dvanced Visual Neuroscience The Lateral Geniculate Nucleus () is more than a relay station LP SC Professor Tom Salt UCL Institute of Ophthalmology Retina t.salt@ucl.ac.uk

More information

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA) Comments Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA) Is phase locking to transposed stimuli as good as phase locking to low-frequency

More information

The psychophysics of glaucoma:improving the structure/function relationship

The psychophysics of glaucoma:improving the structure/function relationship Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 25 (2006) 79 97 www.elsevier.com/locate/prer The psychophysics of glaucoma:improving the structure/function relationship Roger S. Anderson Vision Science Research Group,

More information