Behavioural Brain Research

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Behavioural Brain Research"

Transcription

1 Behavioural Brain Research 209 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: Research report Chronic WIN55,212-2 elicits sustained and conditioned increases in intracranial self-stimulation thresholds in the rat Maria Mavrikaki a, Eirini Markaki a, George G. Nomikos b, George Panagis a, a Laboratory of Behavioral Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, University of Crete, Rethymno, Crete, Greece b Takeda Global Research & Development Center, Inc., Deerfield, IL 60015, USA article info abstract Article history: Received 22 December 2009 Received in revised form 14 January 2010 Accepted 18 January 2010 Available online 25 January 2010 Keywords: Brain stimulation reward Cannabinoid Reinforcement Reward Conditioned anhedonia The present study sought to examine whether repeated administration of the CB 1 receptor agonist WIN55,212-2 affected intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) behavior and induced phenomena of tolerance or sensitization, similar to typical addictive drugs. Rats received intraperitoneal injections of vehicle for 5 days, vehicle or WIN55,212-2 (0.1, 0.3 or 1 mg/kg) for 20 subsequent days, and vehicle for 5 additional days. Thresholds for ICSS were measured before and after each injection. The initial five injections of vehicle did not affect ICSS thresholds. WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg) significantly increased ICSS thresholds from the first day of administration, an effect that remained stable across the subsequent days of administration. During the 5 additional days, where WIN55,212-2 was substituted with vehicle, rats demonstrated a conditioned increase in postinjection thresholds that was significant the first 3 days of this period. These findings indicate that repeated WIN55,212-2 administration elicited a sustained increase in ICSS, i.e., phenomena of tolerance or sensitization were not observed. The present data demonstrate cannabinoidpredictive stimuli that may gain affective salience and play an important role in maintaining cannabinoid administration Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Dependence-producing substances with a high abuse potential that produce euphoric states in humans are believed to affect reinforcement and reward mechanisms by actions on certain areas and pathways of the brain, which are collectively known as brain reward systems. In most cases, these drugs also yield reinforcing and rewarding effects in experimental animals, as assessed by relevant animal models, such as self-administration, conditioned place preference and intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) [29]. Although marijuana is considered to be one of the oldest and most widely used and abused drugs, our knowledge and understanding of the manner it acts in the brain to exert its reinforcing/rewarding effects are far from complete. Animal studies using several behavioral paradigms and under various experimental conditions have shown that 9 -tetrahydrocannabinol ( 9 -THC) and other synthetic cannabinoid agonists can induce both appetitive and aversive effects (for reviews, see [7,9,10,14,15,21,31,33]). Intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) has been suggested to be a valid approach for studying the rewarding/reinforcing proper- Corresponding author. Tel.: ; fax: address: panagis@psy.soc.uoc.gr (G. Panagis). ties of various drugs of abuse [38]. Indeed, drugs with reinforcing properties in animals and addictive potential in humans tend to facilitate the reinforcing effects of brain stimulation in rats, i.e., to lower thresholds for rewarding brain stimulation (ICSS thresholds), shifting to the left the function that relates response strength to stimulation strength. On the other hand, drugs that have a negative impact on reward and reinforcement of behavior typically increase ICSS thresholds shifting the function to the right [4,38]. Interestingly, there are contradictory reports on the effects of cannabinoids on ICSS. Gardner and colleagues found that 9 -THC decreases thresholds for ICSS primarily in Lewis rats [8,13], whereas other studies have found no effect or increased thresholds for ICSS following acute administration of 9 -THC or synthetic cannabinoid agonists in other rat strains [1,2,12,32,34 36]. These discrepancies, which are likely due to methodological differences, remain to be resolved. A possible explanation for the failure to find a clear reward enhancing effect of cannabinoids in the ICSS model is that the first exposure(s) to the cannabinoid may produce severe dysphoric actions, which may mask the rewarding effects of the drug. Interestingly, the effects of chronic cannabinoid administration on ICSS have not been examined. Such a study would allow us to track the cannabinoid effects over time, and this information might provide insight into the mechanisms that contribute to the abuse potential of cannabis preparations /$ see front matter 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi: /j.bbr

2 M. Mavrikaki et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 209 (2010) Fig. 1. Schematic representation of acquisition, training and experimental procedures for ICSS. Hence, the purpose of the present study was to investigate whether repeated administration of the CB 1 receptor agonist WIN55,212-2 affected ICSS behavior in the rat. Since acute administration of WIN55,212-2 has been shown to increase ICSS thresholds, our study was designed to assess not only the effects of daily WIN55,212-2 injections on brain stimulation reward, but also whether a conditioned stimulus associated with chronic WIN55,212-2 would affect brain reward function. To this end, we used a protocol invented by Markou and co-workers that assessed the effects of repeated cocaine administration on ICSS and potential, associated, conditioned phenomena [11]. 2. Methods 2.1. Animals and surgery this method, plotting the responses of the animals against the various pulse frequencies yields a sigmoidal rate frequency curve as shown in Fig. 2. Shifts in the lateral position of the curve provide a selective measure of stimulation-produced reward, as elegantly demonstrated by Edmonds and Gallistel [6], while vertical shifts provide information on motor/performance capacity. Furthermore, this method offers quantitative scaling of drug-induced changes in reward [3] that is useful when comparing the effects of different drugs. In other words, the rate frequency method appears to have reward selectivity that is required in psychopharmacological research [17,19] Drugs and drug administration WIN55,212-2 mesylate (Tocris, Westwoods Bus. Park Ellisville, U.S.A.) was dissolved in 5% dimethyl sulphoxide (DMSO) and 5% cremophor in 0.9% NaCl and injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) at a volume of 3 ml/kg of body weight. In previous studies, we had found that a single injection of WIN55,212-2 at the 3 mg/kg, but not at the 1 mg/kg, dose raised ICSS thresholds [34]. For this reason, we Male Sprague-Dawley rats (n = 21) weighting g at the time of surgery were used. Before surgery they were housed in groups of three and maintained on a 12 h light 12 h dark cycle with free access to food and water. The animals were anaesthetized with intramuscular (i.m.) injection of ketamine hydrochloride (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg). Atropine sulphate (0.6 mg/kg, i.m.) was injected to reduce bronchial secretion. The animals were implanted with a monopolar stimulation electrode aimed at the medial forebrain bundle (MFB) at the level of lateral hypothalamus (2.56 mm posterior to bregma, 1.8 mm lateral from midsaggital suture, and 8.6 below the outer flat skull), according to Paxinos and Watson [23]. The electrodes were constructed from 0.25 mm stainless-steel wire insulated with Epoxylite except for the conically shaped tip. The anode was an uninsulated stainless-steel wire connected to an amphenol pin. Five miniature skull screws, the electrode and the anode were secured to the skull with acrylic dental cement. Following implantation and for the entire duration of the experiments, the animals were housed individually. Experiments were conducted in accordance with Principles of laboratory animal care (NIH publication No , revised 1985) Apparatus and procedures for self-stimulation After one-week recovery, the rats were tested for self-stimulation in an operant chamber made of transparent Plexiglas (25 cm wide, 25 cm deep and 30 cm high). Each chamber was equipped with a stainless-steel poke device (lever) 4 cm wide and protruded 2 cm from the left side at a height of 4 cm from the bottom. Each bar-press triggered a constant current stimulator (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT) that delivered a 0.4-s train of rectangular cathodal pulses of constant duration (0.1 ms) and intensity (250 (A) and variable frequency ( Hz, i.e., 6 40 number of pulses/0.4 s). The pulse frequency, i.e., the number of pulses within a train, was progressively increased up to 40 per stimulation train until the subject showed vigorous self-stimulation. During the acquisition phase the animals were trained to self-stimulate for at least three consecutive days (1 h daily), using stimulation parameters that maintained near maximal bar-pressing rates. After self-stimulation has been acquired and stabilized for a given pulse frequency, rats were trained to self-stimulate using four alternating series of ascending and descending pulse frequencies. The pulse frequency was varied by steps of approximately 0.1 log units. Each frequency was tested within trials of 60 s in duration, followed by an extinction period of 30 s. For each trial, there was an initial priming phase during which the animals received three trains of stimulation at the frequency which was available for the specific trial. A rate frequency determination session lasted about 45 min. One rate frequency curve was established daily, for days, depending on the period when the self-stimulation indices (i.e., curve-shift and threshold measure) were stable. The stimulation parameters, intracranial self-stimulation sessions and data collection were controlled by a computer. For a schematic representation of acquisition and training schedules for ICSS, see Fig. 1. Since cannabinoids seem to affect performance capacity in a dose-dependent manner, the use of a reward selective measure, like the curve-shift, was requisite. In Fig. 2. (A) Changes in self-stimulation thresholds after chronic administration of vehicle or WIN55,212-2 (0.1, 03 or 1 mg/kg, i.p.) during the testing phase (days 1 20). *** p < WIN55, mg/kg vs vehicle. (B) Changes in asymptotic rate of responding after chronic administration of vehicle or WIN55,212-2 (0.1, 03 or 1 mg/kg, i.p.) during the testing phase (days 1 20).

3 116 M. Mavrikaki et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 209 (2010) conducted a pilot study under the present experimental conditions that includes 5 days of ICSS testing with all the animals receiving vehicle and found that even the 1 mg/kg dose administered on the 6th day raised ICSS thresholds. Adaptation to the experimental conditions, such as that most likely occurring with animals receiving vehicle for 5 days in the present setting, has been shown to affect the behavioral responses to the cannabinoid receptor agonists 9 -THC and WIN55, For example, Pollissidis et al. [24] showed that low doses of these compounds increased locomotion only under habituated conditions, whereas high doses decreased locomotion only under non-habituated conditions. To this end, we speculate that the 5 days of vehicle administration, ICSS testing and further habituation to the experimental procedures rendered the animals more susceptible to the anhedonic effects of WIN55, Based on the present finding, we continued the study, using the 1 mg/kg dose as the highest dose for the chronic regimen, given that we sought to investigate if changes in ICSS thresholds occurred in either direction after chronic administration Experimental procedure Rats were trained in the ICSS procedure until stable thresholds were achieved. The criterion for stability was met when the frequency thresholds did not vary by more than 0.1 log units. Rats were then tested daily for 5 consecutive days in order to evaluate the baseline threshold. Immediately after the first ICSS session (preinjection) the animals were removed from the ICSS chamber and injected with vehicle. Rats were returned to the ICSS chamber and thresholds were assessed 10 min later (postinjection threshold). The mean of the first 4 days of vehicle administration (pre- and post-injection) was considered as baseline, while the response on the 5th day was considered as control (B5). After these 5 days, animals were injected with vehicle [n = 5] or WIN55,212-2 (0.1 [n = 5], 0.3 [n = 5] or 1 mg/kg [n = 6]), returned to the ICSS chamber and thresholds were assessed 10 min later. During this phase, pre- and post-injection thresholds were assessed for 20 consecutive days (testing phase). Thereafter, WIN55,212-2 was substituted with vehicle and thresholds were assessed for 5 more days (washout phase). For a schematic representation of the experimental procedures, see Fig Data analysis and statistics The analysis was performed on two aspects of data obtained from the rate frequency curve: the intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) threshold and the maximum rate of responding or asymptote. [18]. Threshold and asymptote values at B5, testing and washout phases were expressed as percentage of baseline values. Data were initially analyzed using two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures followed whenever appropriate by one-way ANOVAs or one-way ANOVAs with repeated measures and correlated t-test using Bonferroni s adjustment for multiple comparisons Histology At the end of the experiment, the animals were given a lethal dose of sodium pentothal. The location of the terminal stimulation site was then marked according to the following procedure: a direct anodal current of 0.1 ma and 15 s duration was passed through the electrode tip. The animals were perfused intracardially with saline 0.9%, which was followed by a 50 cc solution of potassium ferrocyanide (3%), potassium ferricyanide (3%), and trichloroacetic acid (0.5%) in 10% formalin. The brains were then removed and stored in 10% formalin for 3 days, and 2 days in a 30% sucrose solution. Finally, the brains were sliced in a cryostat microtome and the sections containing the electrode tract were mounted on slides and stained with cresyl violet. Only the rats in which tracks from the electrode were verified to be located in the MFB were included in this study (n = 21). 3. Results In the testing phase (Fig. 2), two-way ANOVA with repeated measures performed on the changes in self-stimulation thresholds showed a statistical significant drug effect [F(3,17) = , p < 0.001], but neither significant interaction of time and drug nor time effect, indicating neither tolerance nor sensitization. Furthermore, paired-samples t-test indicated statistical significant difference between the groups receiving 1 mg/kg and vehicle (p < 0.001) across all 20 days of administration. In contrast, 0.1 and 0.3 mg/kg of WIN55,212-2 did not induce statistically significant effects. To the contrary with the changes in self-stimulation thresholds, two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on the asymptotic rate of responding data showed no statistically significant drug, time or interaction of drug and time effects (p > 0.05). In the washout phase (Fig. 3), two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on the changes in ICSS thresholds showed a statis- Fig. 3. (A) Changes in self-stimulation thresholds after administration of vehicle during the washout phase (days 1 5) in animals that received vehicle or WIN55,212-2 (0.1, 03 or 1 mg/kg) during the testing phase. B5: last day of 5 consecutive days of vehicle administration before the testing phase. * p < 0.05 (B) Changes asymptotic rate of responding after administration of vehicle during the washout phase (days 1 5) in animals that received vehicle or WIN55,212-2 (0.1, 03 or 1 mg/kg) during the testing phase. B5: last day of 5 consecutive days of vehicle administration before the testing phase. tical significant interaction of time and drug [F(15,85) = 1.800, p < 0.01]. One-way ANOVA indicated no differences between groups at B5. One-way ANOVAs on days 1 3 indicated significant differences between groups [F1(3, 17) = 5.658, F2(3, 17) = , F3(3, 17) = , p < 0.01 for each day]. Paired-samples t-tests indicated statistical significant differences only between the groups administered with 1 mg/kg and vehicle on days 1 3 (p < 0.05 for each day). One-way ANOVAs on days 4 and 5 indicated no significant differences. However, two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on the asymptotic rate of responding showed no statistically significant drug, time or interaction of drug and time effects (p > 0.05). Fig. 4 depicts rate frequency functions from representative animals obtained before and after administration of WIN55,212-2 and vehicle from the days 1 and 20 of the testing phase and the day 1 of the washout phase. As indicated in the figure WIN55,212-2 administration produced a parallel curve-shift to the right, indicating a clear decrease in the reinforcing efficacy of the stimulation. 4. Discussion ICSS thresholds remained stable and unaltered when measured before each daily injection of vehicle or WIN55,212-2 over the

4 M. Mavrikaki et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 209 (2010) Fig. 4. Rate frequency functions taken from representative animals. Each plot represents data from a single animal under pre-drug (filled circles) and drug (open circles) conditions. Rate frequency functions were obtained by logarithmically decreasing the frequency of the stimulation pulses from a value that sustained maximal lever pressing to one that failed to sustain lever pressing. 30 consecutive days of testing. Furthermore, there was no difference between pre- and post-injection ICSS thresholds during the first 5 consecutive days of the baseline phase. However, postinjection ICSS thresholds were increased after each WIN55,212-2 injection during the testing phase. This effect was significant for the highest dose of WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg), and the magnitude of this increase did not vary significantly across the 20 consecutive days of drug administration. These findings indicate that the CB 1 receptor agonist WIN55,212-2 transiently increased ICSS threshold after each daily injection, whereas ICSS thresholds returned to preinjection baseline the next day, and there was no evidence of tolerance or sensitization to this effect over a course of administration. These findings clearly indicate that chronic WIN55,212-2 did not facilitate ICSS, but rather increased the ICSS thresholds, a finding that is in agreement with several previous reports utilizing acute regimens [1,2,12,32,34 36]. Our results differ from those of previous studies by Gardner s group [8,13], whereby acute 9 - THC decreased ICSS threshold. These contrasting results could be attributed to procedural differences. For example, Lepore et al. [13] found the most pronounced action of 9 -THC in Lewis rats, compared to Sprague-Dawley rats, in which the effect was marginal, whereas in the Fisher strain of rats there was no effect. The same group in their previous studies also used Lewis rats, which in general appear to be more sensitive and vulnerable to the effects of addictive drugs [8]. Although one can speculate that the reward facilitating effect of cannabinoids on ICSS may only be obtained in certain strains of rat, suggesting an important genetic component in this action, other studies have shown that 9 -THC produces aversion in the conditioned place preference paradigm in the same strain of rats [22]. Secondly, in the study by Lepore et al. [13], 9 - THC could only shift the rate frequency function to the left by approximately 0.05 log units. Interestingly, these authors adopt a very strict criterion of stable responding (i.e., 0.01 log units for three consecutive days), which increases the likelihood that the observed effect of 9 -THC in their study may reflect normal baseline variation over days. To the contrary, in our study the criterion of stable responding was defined within 0.1 log units over three consecutive days, in agreement with most other studies utilizing the ICSS paradigm (see, e.g., [2]). Although in the present study we did not assess the role of CB 1 receptors in the inhibitory effects of WIN55,212-2 on ICSS, we have previously shown that CB 1 receptors do indeed mediate the observed increases in ICSS thresholds after acute WIN55,212-2 [34]. It appears worthwhile to examine the role of specific CB 1 receptor stimulation in the sustained and conditioned anhedonia in response to repeated administration of a non-selective cannabinoid receptor agonist, such as the WIN55,212-2, that is observed here for the first time. According to our results, vehicle- and WIN55, administered rats did not present any statistical significant differences in the maximum rate of responding, and chronic administration of WIN55,212-2 did not affect the asymptotic rate of responding (see Fig. 2). Thus, the increased postinjection ICSS thresholds observed after each WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg) injection during the testing phase seem to be independent of motor impairment, and the effects of chronic WIN55,212-2 on ICSS thresholds were not confounded by performance effects. This is consistent with previous reports on homologous findings with acute administration of CB 1 receptor agonists [34,35]. Pharmacological studies have shown that chronic administration of cannabinoids, such as 9 -tetrahydrocannabinol and WIN55,212-2, produces cannabinoid receptor desensitization and down-regulation, as well as tolerance to some of the cannabinoidmediated effects, such as hypoactivity, antinociception and hypothermia [20,26 28,30,37]. Our behavioral observations are not congruent with the aforementioned behavioral and biochemical findings. That is, ICSS thresholds increased after the first injection of WIN55,212-2 and remained stable across the 20 days of drug treatment. We speculate that the effects of chronic cannabinoids on brain reward function may be independent from changes in sensitivity, number or function of central cannabinoid receptors. Alternatively, downstream effects elicited by chronic administration of cannabinoids may counteract any ensuing change in cannabinoid receptors resulting in a sustained decrease in ICSS reward. Interestingly, in the washout phase, in which the WIN55,212-2 injection was substituted with a vehicle injection and thresholds were assessed for 5 more days, the postinjection threshold was significantly increased compared with preinjection threshold. This effect was statistically significant for the days 1, 2 and 3 of the group that received 1 mg/kg WIN55,212-2 during the testing phase. This finding demonstrates that after being repeatedly paired with WIN55,212-2 administration, a previously neutral vehicle injection increased ICSS threshold in a manner similar to that induced by WIN55,212-2 administration. A similar phenomenon in the washout (withdrawal) phase, although in the opposite direction considering the testing phase, has been described by Markou and co-workers after repeated cocaine administration [11].

5 118 M. Mavrikaki et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 209 (2010) The conditioned increase in ICSS thresholds observed in the present study probably reflects an aversive conditioning phenomenon (i.e., conditioned anhedonia). It has become increasingly apparent that some drug effects can be elicited by environmental cues or neutral stimuli that have been repeatedly paired with drug administration. That is, certain drug effects can be conditioned. The contribution of such drug-predictive environmental cues or neutral stimuli can be explained by the Pavlovian conditioning model. Thus, in our study, cannabinoid-associated conditioned stimuli can inhibit brain reward function, as indicated by the increased ICSS thresholds. Interestingly, exogenous cannabinoid ligands, such as 9 -tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), have previously shown to exert conditioned place avoidance responses (see, e.g., [5,16,25]). The present data demonstrate cannabinoid-predictive stimuli that may gain affective salience and plausibly act as discriminative stimuli and affect/drive subsequent cannabinoid administration or relapse to drug-taking behavior. In conclusion, repeated administration of CB 1 receptor agonist WIN55,212-2 did not increase the reinforcing efficacy of the stimulation. To the contrary, the highest dose increased ICSS threshold, indicating a clear anhedonic effect, which did not undergo tolerance or sensitization but appeared stable with chronic administration. The increase in ICSS thresholds produced by cannabinoid-predictive stimuli may play an important role in maintaining cannabinoid self-administration in a subpopulation of cannabis-dependent individuals, by acting as discriminative stimuli. Acknowledgements This study was supported by a grant from the Research Committee (KA 2303) and the Department of Psychology of the University of Crete. Maria Mavrikaki was supported by a scholarship from Alexander S. Onassis Public Benefit Foundation. References [1] Antoniou K, Galanopoulos A, Vlachou S, Kourouli T, Nahmias V, Thermos K, et al. Behavioral pharmacological properties of a novel cannabinoid 1,1 -dithiolane delta8-thc analog, AMG-3. Behav Pharmacol 2005;16(5 6): [2] Arnold JC, Hunt GE, McGregor IS. Effects of the cannabinoid receptor agonist CP 55,940 and the cannabinoid receptor antagonist SR on intracranial self-stimulation in Lewis rats. Life Sci 2001;70(1): [3] Campbell KA, Evans G, Gallistel CR. A microcomputer-based method for physiologically interpretable measurement of the rewarding efficacy of brain stimulation. Physiol Behav 1985;35(3): [4] Carlezon Jr WA, Chartoff EH. Intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) in rodents to study the neurobiology of motivation. Nat Protoc 2007;2(11): [5] Cheer JF, Kendall DA, Marsden CA. Cannabinoid receptors and reward in the rat: a conditioned place preference study. Psychopharmacology 2000;151(1): [6] Edmonds DE, Gallistel CR. Parametric analysis of brain stimulation reward in the rat: III. Effect of performance variables on the reward summation function. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1974;87(5): [7] Gardner EL. Addictive potential of cannabinoids: the underlying neurobiology. Chem Phys Lipids 2002;121(1 2): [8] Gardner EL, Paredes W, Smith D, Donner A, Milling C, Cohen D, et al. Facilitation of brain stimulation reward by delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol. Psychopharmacology 1988;96(1): [9] Gardner EL, Vorel SR. Cannabinoid transmission and reward-related events. Neurobiol Dis 1998;5(6 Pt B): [10] Justinova Z, Goldberg SR, Heishman SJ, Tanda G. Self-administration of cannabinoids by experimental animals and human marijuana smokers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2005;81(2): [11] Kenny PJ, Koob GF, Markou A. Conditioned facilitation of brain reward function after repeated cocaine administration. Behav Neurosci 2003;117(5): [12] Kucharski LT, Williams JE, Kornetsky C. The effects of levonantradol on rewarding brain stimulation thresholds in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1983;19(1): [13] Lepore M, Liu X, Savage V, Matalon D, Gardner EL. Genetic differences in delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol-induced facilitation of brain stimulation reward as measured by a rate-frequency curve-shift electrical brain stimulation paradigm in three different rat strains. Life Sci 1996;58(25):PL [14] Maldonado R. Study of cannabinoid dependence in animals. Pharmacol Ther 2002;95(2): [15] Maldonado R, Rodriguez de Fonseca F. Cannabinoid addiction: behavioral models and neural correlates. J Neurosci 2002;22(9): [16] Mallet PE, Beninger RJ. Delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol, but not the endogenous cannabinoid receptor ligand anandamide, produces conditioned place avoidance. Life Sci 1998;62(26): [17] Markou A, Koob GF. Construct validity of a self-stimulation threshold paradigm: effects of reward and performance manipulations. Physiol Behav 1992;51(1): [18] Mavrikaki M, Nomikos GG, Panagis G. Effects of mood stabilizers on brain reward processes in rats: studies using the intracranial self-stimulation paradigm. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2009;19(3): [19] Miliaressis E, Rompre PP, Laviolette P, Philippe L, Coulombe D. The curve-shift paradigm in self-stimulation. Physiol Behav 1986;37(1): [20] Oviedo A, Glowa J, Herkenham M. Chronic cannabinoid administration alters cannabinoid receptor binding in rat brain: a quantitative autoradiographic study. Brain Res 1993;616(1 2): [21] Panagis G, Vlachou S, Nomikos GG. Behavioral pharmacology of cannabinoids with a focus on preclinical models for studying reinforcing and dependenceproducing properties. Curr Drug Abuse Rev 2008;1(3): [22] Parker LA, Gillies T. THC-induced place and taste aversions in Lewis and Sprague-Dawley rats. Behav Neurosci 1995;109(1):71 8. [23] Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. sixth ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier; [24] Polissidis A, Chouliara O, Galanopoulos A, Rentesi G, Dosi M, Hyphantis T, et al. Individual differences in the effects of cannabinoids on motor activity, dopaminergic activity and DARPP-32 phosphorylation in distinct regions of the brain. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2009;27(November):1 17. [25] Quinn HR, Matsumoto I, Callaghan PD, Long LE, Arnold JC, Gunasekaran N, et al. Adolescent rats find repeated Delta(9)-THC less aversive than adult rats but display greater residual cognitive deficits and changes in hippocampal protein expression following exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008;33(5): [26] Rodriguez de Fonseca F, Gorriti MA, Fernandez-Ruiz JJ, Palomo T, Ramos JA. Downregulation of rat brain cannabinoid binding sites after chronic delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol treatment. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1994;47(1): [27] Romero J, Berrendero F, Garcia-Gil L, Ramos JA, Fernandez-Ruiz JJ. Cannabinoid receptor and WIN-55,212-2-stimulated [35S]GTP gamma S binding and cannabinoid receptor mrna levels in the basal ganglia and the cerebellum of adult male rats chronically exposed to delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol. J Mol Neurosci 1998;11(2): [28] Romero J, Garcia-Palomero E, Castro JG, Garcia-Gil L, Ramos JA, Fernandez-Ruiz JJ. Effects of chronic exposure to delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol on cannabinoid receptor binding and mrna levels in several rat brain regions. Brain Res 1997;46(1 2): [29] Sanchis-Segura C, Spanagel R. Behavioural assessment of drug reinforcement and addictive features in rodents: an overview. Addict Biol 2006;11(1):2 38. [30] Sim-Selley LJ, Martin BR. Effect of chronic administration of R-(+)- [2,3-Dihydro-5-methyl-3-[(morpholinyl)methyl]pyrrolo[1,2,3-de]-1,4-b enzoxazinyl]-(1-naphthalenyl)methanone mesylate (WIN55,212-2) or delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol on cannabinoid receptor adaptation in mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2002;303(1): [31] Solinas M, Goldberg SR, Piomelli D. The endocannabinoid system in brain reward processes. Br J Pharmacol 2008;154(2): [32] Stark P, Dews PB, Cannabinoids I. Behavioral effects. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1980;214(1): [33] Tanda G, Goldberg SR. Cannabinoids: reward, dependence, and underlying neurochemical mechanisms a review of recent preclinical data. Psychopharmacology 2003;169(2): [34] Vlachou S, Nomikos GG, Panagis G. CB1 cannabinoid receptor agonists increase intracranial self-stimulation thresholds in the rat. Psychopharmacology 2005;179(2): [35] Vlachou S, Nomikos GG, Panagis G. WIN 55,212-2 decreases the reinforcing actions of cocaine through CB1 cannabinoid receptor stimulation. Behav Brain Res 2003;141(2): [36] Vlachou S, Nomikos GG, Stephens DN, Panagis G. Lack of evidence for appetitive effects of Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol in the intracranial self-stimulation and conditioned place preference procedures in rodents. Behav Pharmacol 2007;18(4): [37] Whitlow CT, Freedland CS, Porrino LJ. Functional consequences of the repeated administration of Delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol in the rat. Drug Alcohol Depend 2003;71(2): [38] Wise RA. Addictive drugs and brain stimulation reward. Annu Rev Neurosci 1996;19:

Biphasic effects of Δ 9 -tetrahydrocannabinol on brain stimulation reward and motor activity

Biphasic effects of Δ 9 -tetrahydrocannabinol on brain stimulation reward and motor activity International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology (213), 16, 2273 2284. CINP 213 doi:1.117/s146114571379 Biphasic effects of Δ 9 -tetrahydrocannabinol on brain stimulation reward and motor activity ARTICLE

More information

WIN 55,212-2 decreases the reinforcing actions of cocaine through CB 1 cannabinoid receptor stimulation

WIN 55,212-2 decreases the reinforcing actions of cocaine through CB 1 cannabinoid receptor stimulation Behavioural Brain Research 141 (2003) 215 222 Research report WIN 55,212-2 decreases the reinforcing actions of cocaine through CB 1 cannabinoid receptor stimulation Styliani Vlachou a, George G. Nomikos

More information

Dopamine in Ube3a m-/p+ mice. Online Supplemental Material

Dopamine in Ube3a m-/p+ mice. Online Supplemental Material Online Supplemental Material S1 Supplemental Figure 1. Schematic of rate-dependent intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) (A) Mice implanted with monopolar stimulating electrodes to the medial forebrain

More information

Intravenous self-administration of the cannabinoid CB1 receptor agonist WIN 55,212-2 in rats

Intravenous self-administration of the cannabinoid CB1 receptor agonist WIN 55,212-2 in rats Psychopharmacology (2001) 156:410 416 DOI 10.1007/s002130100734 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Liana Fattore Gregorio Cossu Cristina M. Martellotta Walter Fratta Intravenous self-administration of the cannabinoid

More information

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD in this talk what is marijuana? the brain on marijuana is the teen brain special? current research what is marijuana?

More information

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION INTRODUCTION Epidemiologic reports indicate that mood disorders in children and adolescents are quite common, with up to 70% of depressed children and adolescents experiencing a recurrence within 5 years

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT ACTA NEUROBIOL: EXP. 1988, 48: 117-121 Short communication LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT W. Jeffrey WILSON and Jennifer C. HALL Department of Psychological

More information

Dissociable Effects of the Cannabinoid Receptor Agonists

Dissociable Effects of the Cannabinoid Receptor Agonists 1521-0103/12/3432-389 400$25.00 THE JOURNAL OF PHARMACOLOGY AND EXPERIMENTAL THERAPEUTICS Vol. 343, No. 2 Copyright 2012 by The American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 197780/3801449

More information

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD in this talk what is marijuana? the brain on marijuana is the teen brain special? current research what is marijuana?

More information

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD in this talk what is marijuana? the brain on marijuana is the teen brain special? current research what is marijuana?

More information

Recent Advances in Energy, Environment, Biology and Ecology

Recent Advances in Energy, Environment, Biology and Ecology Acute and long-term effects elicited by psychoactive drugs on 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats: development of a new experimental tool for the study of drug-mediated reward NICOLA SIMOLA Department

More information

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus Behavioral Neuroscience 1995. Vol. 109, No. 2, 366-370 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/$3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With

More information

Supplementary Methods

Supplementary Methods 1 Supplementary Methods Social Preference Test Subjects Seventy-four Long-Evans, male rats served as subjects (S-rats) in the foodpreference test, with 40 assigned to the CXT-Same (CXT-S) Condition and

More information

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University Undergraduate Honors Theses 5-2014 Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? Tarra L. Baker Follow

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D.

DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D. DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D. Psychology Department American University Washington, DC 20016 Phone: 202-885-1725 Email: kearns@american.edu EDUCATION 2005 American University Washington, DC Ph.D., Psychology

More information

The Effect of Electrical Priming Stimulation on the Reward-Summation Function. Esthelle Ewusi-Boisvert

The Effect of Electrical Priming Stimulation on the Reward-Summation Function. Esthelle Ewusi-Boisvert The Effect of Electrical Priming Stimulation on the Reward-Summation Function Esthelle Ewusi-Boisvert Presented in Partial Fulfillment for the Requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of Arts Honours Psychology

More information

Food restriction: enhancing effects on drug reward and striatal cell signaling

Food restriction: enhancing effects on drug reward and striatal cell signaling Food restriction: enhancing effects on drug reward and striatal cell signaling K.D. Carr Departments of Psychiatry & Pharmacology NYU School of Medicine Common Neural Substrates for Incentive-Motivating

More information

Repeated stress exposure causes strain-dependent shifts in the behavioral economics of cocaine in rats

Repeated stress exposure causes strain-dependent shifts in the behavioral economics of cocaine in rats bs_bs_banneraddiction Biology PRECLINICAL STUDY doi:10.1111/adb.12123 Repeated stress exposure causes strain-dependent shifts in the behavioral economics of cocaine in rats Peter A. Groblewski 1, Chad

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

INTRODUCTION. M Solinas 1, LV Panlilio 1 and SR Goldberg*,1

INTRODUCTION. M Solinas 1, LV Panlilio 1 and SR Goldberg*,1 (2004) 29, 1301 1311 & 2004 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0893-133X/04 $30.00 www.neuropsychopharmacology.org Exposure to D-9-Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) Increases Subsequent Heroin Taking

More information

Cannabidiol inhibits the reward-facilitating effect of morphine: involvement of 5-HT 1A receptors in the dorsal raphe nucleus

Cannabidiol inhibits the reward-facilitating effect of morphine: involvement of 5-HT 1A receptors in the dorsal raphe nucleus bs_bs_banneraddiction Biology PRECLINICAL STUDY doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00483.x Cannabidiol inhibits the reward-facilitating effect of morphine: involvement of 5-HT 1A receptors in the dorsal raphe

More information

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson, H Djafari Marbini, AJ Toner, TW Robbins, BJ Everitt Departments of

More information

VENTRAL TEGMENTAL AREA LESIONS DIFFERENTIALLY AFFECT RESPONSES CONTROLLED BY CS-US CONTIGUITY AND RESPONSE-REINFORCER CONTINGENCY IN THE RAT

VENTRAL TEGMENTAL AREA LESIONS DIFFERENTIALLY AFFECT RESPONSES CONTROLLED BY CS-US CONTIGUITY AND RESPONSE-REINFORCER CONTINGENCY IN THE RAT ACTA NEUROBIOL. EXP. 1987, 47: 83-91 VENTRAL TEGMENTAL AREA LESIONS DIFFERENTIALLY AFFECT RESPONSES CONTROLLED BY CS-US CONTIGUITY AND RESPONSE-REINFORCER CONTINGENCY IN THE RAT W. Jeffrey WILSON and Ericka

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5983/1288/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Induction of Fear Extinction with Hippocampal-Infralimbic BDNF Jamie Peters, Laura M. Dieppa-Perea, Loyda M. Melendez,

More information

Assessment of anti-seizure properties of two proprietary compounds in the electrical kindling model of epilepsy. Date

Assessment of anti-seizure properties of two proprietary compounds in the electrical kindling model of epilepsy. Date Assessment of anti-seizure properties of two proprietary compounds in the electrical kindling model of epilepsy Date This study was conducted under the terms of a Laboratory Services Agreement between

More information

Behavioral Pharmacology

Behavioral Pharmacology Psych 181: Dr. Anagnostaras Lecture 4 Behavioral Pharmacology Behavioral Pharmacology Behavioral Pharmacology The study of the relationship between the physiological actions of drugs and their effects

More information

Marijuana and cannabinoids

Marijuana and cannabinoids Psych 181: Dr. Anagnostaras Lec 10: Marijuana Marijuana and cannabinoids Cannabis sativa, hemp One of earliest non-food plants cultivated fiber for rope, seeds for oil and birdseed 1st archaeological evidence

More information

AM281, Cannabinoid Antagonist/Inverse agonist, Ameliorates Scopolamine- Induced Cognitive Deficit

AM281, Cannabinoid Antagonist/Inverse agonist, Ameliorates Scopolamine- Induced Cognitive Deficit Iranian Journal of Basic Medical Sciences Vol. 15, No. 5, Sep-Oct 2012, 1106-1110 Received: May 28, 2011; Accepted: Dec 24, 2011 www.mums.ac.ir Short Communication AM281, Cannabinoid Antagonist/Inverse

More information

NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 2002 VOL. 27, NO American College of Neuropsychopharmacology

NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 2002 VOL. 27, NO American College of Neuropsychopharmacology Conditioned Locomotion Is Not Correlated with Behavioral Sensitization to Cocaine: An Intra-Laboratory Multi-Sample Analysis Gregory Hotsenpiller, Ph.D., and Marina E. Wolf, Ph.D. Pre-clinical and clinical

More information

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience.

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience. Chapter 6: Learning Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience. Conditioning = a process in which environmental stimuli and behavioral processes become connected Two types of

More information

Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling

Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling Research Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling Kate M. Wassum, 1,2,4 Sean B. Ostlund, 1,2 Bernard W. Balleine, 3

More information

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys PII S0091-3057(99)00058-1 Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, Vol. 64, No. 2, pp. 367 371, 1999 1999 Elsevier Science Inc. Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0091-3057/99/$ see front matter Effects

More information

Zoektocht naar innovatieve geneesmiddelen voor de toekomst - Verslaving en ander gedrag in moleculair perspectief

Zoektocht naar innovatieve geneesmiddelen voor de toekomst - Verslaving en ander gedrag in moleculair perspectief Zoektocht naar innovatieve geneesmiddelen voor de toekomst - Verslaving en ander gedrag in moleculair perspectief Prof. dr. Chris Kruse Swammerdam Institute, UvA Solvay Pharmaceuticals Drug Discovery Assets

More information

Food Intake After Diazepam, Morphine or Muscimol: Microinjections in the Nucleus Accumbens Shell

Food Intake After Diazepam, Morphine or Muscimol: Microinjections in the Nucleus Accumbens Shell PII S0091-3057(00)00220-3 Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, Vol. 66, No. 2, pp. 429 434, 2000 2000 Elsevier Science Inc. Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0091-3057/00/$ see front matter Food

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation.

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation. Unit 06 - Overview How We Learn and Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Operant Conditioning s Applications, and Comparison to Classical Conditioning Biology, Cognition, and Learning Learning By

More information

Effects of adolescent THC exposure on the behavioral effects of cocaine in adult Sprague-Dawley rats

Effects of adolescent THC exposure on the behavioral effects of cocaine in adult Sprague-Dawley rats Effects of adolescent THC exposure on the behavioral effects of cocaine in adult Sprague-Dawley rats Amy L. Friedman, and Dr. Emily M. Jutkiewicz, Ph.D. Department of Pharmacology, University of Michigan

More information

REWARDING AND PUNISHING EFFECTS FROM STIMULATING THE SAME PLACE IN THE RAT'S BRAIN 1

REWARDING AND PUNISHING EFFECTS FROM STIMULATING THE SAME PLACE IN THE RAT'S BRAIN 1 REWARDING AND PUNISHING EFFECTS FROM STIMULATING THE SAME PLACE IN THE RAT'S BRAIN 1 Delgado, Roberts, and Miller (1) found that electrical stimulation in hypothalamic structures of the cat brain would

More information

<student name> Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal

<student name> Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal A. Project Description Objective of research: The objective of this study is to determine if hippocampal dopamine D1 receptors facilitate novel object

More information

Cannabinoids Reveal the Necessity of Hippocampal Neural Encoding for Short-Term Memory in Rats

Cannabinoids Reveal the Necessity of Hippocampal Neural Encoding for Short-Term Memory in Rats The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1, 2000, 20(23):8932 8942 Cannabinoids Reveal the Necessity of Hippocampal Neural Encoding for Short-Term Memory in Rats Robert E. Hampson and Sam A. Deadwyler Department

More information

Book 3: Lab Procedures Book 3: Ch. 1: The Hypothesis and Overview

Book 3: Lab Procedures Book 3: Ch. 1: The Hypothesis and Overview Book 3: Lab Procedures Book 3: Ch. 1: The Hypothesis and Overview 13 Introduction This experiment will investigate how cocaine acts on dopamine neurons in the brain. Cocaine is a drug of abuse that increases

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

REINFORCEMENT1 UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN. this procedure required that an animal emit a. pause of at least 15 min, was found to be sensitive

REINFORCEMENT1 UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN. this procedure required that an animal emit a. pause of at least 15 min, was found to be sensitive JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR USE OF PROGRESSIVE FIXED-RATIO PROCEDURES IN THE ASSESSMENT OF INTRACRANIAL REINFORCEMENT1 RICHARD E. KEESEY AND MICHAEL D. GOLDSTEIN UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN

More information

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review Jim Morrill, MD, PhD MGH Charlestown HealthCare Center Massachusetts General Hospital Disclosures Neither I nor my spouse/partner has a relevant financial relationship

More information

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions Behavioral Neuroscience 1996, Vol. 110, No. 5, 933-945 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/96/S3.00 Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal

More information

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning Learn Behav (2011) 39:79 86 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0022-x Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning Gabriel Rodríguez & Gumersinda Alonso Published

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

Cannabis for Medical Use: Body and Mind

Cannabis for Medical Use: Body and Mind Cannabis for Medical Use: Body and Mind Shaul Schreiber, MD Division of Psychiatry, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Tel Aviv University Sackler Faculty of Medicine; Sagol School of Neuroscience, Israel

More information

Serotonergic mechanisms of MDMA self-administration. Susan Schenk Victoria University of Wellington School of Psychology

Serotonergic mechanisms of MDMA self-administration. Susan Schenk Victoria University of Wellington School of Psychology Serotonergic mechanisms of MDMA self-administration Susan Schenk Victoria University of Wellington School of Psychology My lab works in the broad area of Behavioural Pharmacology We test the effects of

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Xue Y-X, Deng J-H, Chen Y-Y, et al. Effect of selective inhibition of reactivated nicotineassociated memories with propranolol on nicotine craving. JAMA Psychiatry. Published

More information

Representation of negative motivational value in the primate

Representation of negative motivational value in the primate Representation of negative motivational value in the primate lateral habenula Masayuki Matsumoto & Okihide Hikosaka Supplementary Figure 1 Anticipatory licking and blinking. (a, b) The average normalized

More information

brain valuation & behavior

brain valuation & behavior brain valuation & behavior 9 Rangel, A, et al. (2008) Nature Neuroscience Reviews Vol 9 Stages in decision making process Problem is represented in the brain Brain evaluates the options Action is selected

More information

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING Pergamon Neuroscience Vol. 83, No. 3, pp. 837 841, 1998 Copyright 1998 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved PII: S0306-4522(97)00457-0 0306 4522/98 $19.00+0.00

More information

Push-in Head Restraining Apparatus for Intracranial Self Stimulation Tasks in Rats

Push-in Head Restraining Apparatus for Intracranial Self Stimulation Tasks in Rats Korean J Physiol Pharmacol Vol 18: 103-108, April, 2014 http://dx.doi.org/10.4196/kjpp.2014.18.2.103 Push-in Head Restraining Apparatus for Intracranial Self Stimulation Tasks in Rats Mootaek Roh 1, Il-Sung

More information

Ultra-low-dose naltrexone suppresses rewarding effects of opiates and aversive effects of opiate withdrawal in rats

Ultra-low-dose naltrexone suppresses rewarding effects of opiates and aversive effects of opiate withdrawal in rats Psychopharmacology (2005) 181: 576 581 DOI 10.1007/s00213-005-0022-7 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Mary C. Olmstead. Lindsay H. Burns Ultra-low-dose naltrexone suppresses rewarding effects of opiates and aversive

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival? Threats are much better reinforcers? Fear is a prime

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

Development of a Touch-Screen-Based Paradigm for Assessing Working Memory in the Mouse

Development of a Touch-Screen-Based Paradigm for Assessing Working Memory in the Mouse http://dx.doi.org/10.5607/en.2015.24.1.84 Exp Neurobiol. 2015 Mar;24(1):84-89. pissn 1226-2560 eissn 2093-8144 Original Article Development of a Touch-Screen-Based Paradigm for Assessing Working Memory

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Pharmacol Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 November 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Pharmacol Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 November 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Pharmacol Res. 2007 November ; 56(5): 406 417. Modulation Of The Endo-Cannabinoid System: Therapeutic Potential Against Cocaine Dependence

More information

A comparison of MDMA and d-amphetamine in the drug discrimination paradigm.

A comparison of MDMA and d-amphetamine in the drug discrimination paradigm. A comparison of MDMA and d-amphetamine in the drug discrimination paradigm. D.N. Harper, A. Crowther, S. Schenk School of Psychology Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand AIM To compare the subjective

More information

MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION. Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre

MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION. Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre 1 MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre Reward 2 Humans, as well as other organisms engage in behaviours that are rewarding The pleasurable feelings provide positive reinforcement

More information

Cannabinoid Agonists but not Inhibitors of Endogenous Cannabinoid Transport or Metabolism Enhance the Reinforcing Efficacy of Heroin in Rats

Cannabinoid Agonists but not Inhibitors of Endogenous Cannabinoid Transport or Metabolism Enhance the Reinforcing Efficacy of Heroin in Rats (2005) 30, 2046 2057 & 2005 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0893-133X/05 $30.00 www.neuropsychopharmacology.org Cannabinoid Agonists but not Inhibitors of Endogenous Cannabinoid Transport or

More information

Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia. Hai-Yan He, Baisali Ray, Katie Dennis and Elizabeth M.

Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia. Hai-Yan He, Baisali Ray, Katie Dennis and Elizabeth M. Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia Hai-Yan He, Baisali Ray, Katie Dennis and Elizabeth M. Quinlan a 3. 2.5 2. 1.5.5 Deprived eye Non-deprived VCtx * * b 8.

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 247 (2013) 125 131 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research j ourna l ho mepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report Role of intra-accumbal

More information

Nucleus accumbens lesions impair context, but not cue, conditioning in rats

Nucleus accumbens lesions impair context, but not cue, conditioning in rats Learning and Memory PREVIOUS work has provided evidence of a role for the hippocampal formation in contextual as opposed to cue conditioning. Similar deficits have been observed after transection of the

More information

No! No! No! No! With the possible exception of humans Public Health Question Does the compound have the potential to be abused? Public Health Question Does the compound have the potential to be abused?

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more strongly when an image was negative than when the same

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:10.1038/nature12024 entary Figure 1. Distribution of the number of earned cocaine Supplementary Figure 1. Distribution of the number of earned cocaine infusions in Shock-sensitive

More information

Neurobiology of Addiction

Neurobiology of Addiction Neurobiology of Addiction Tiffany Love, Ph.D. Department of Psychiatry The University of Utah What is Addiction? Addiction is a chronic, relapsing, and treatable brain disorder. Compulsive drug seeking

More information

12µl) was implanted in the jugular vein (S1) under ketamine (100 mg/kg) /xylacine (1 mg/kg)

12µl) was implanted in the jugular vein (S1) under ketamine (100 mg/kg) /xylacine (1 mg/kg) Supporting Online Material Material and Methods Subjects. Male Sprague-Dawley rats (n=102) weighing 280-300 g at the beginning of the experiments were used. A 12 hr dark/light cycle (on 20h00, off 8h00)

More information

Kappa Opioid Receptor Activation Potentiates the Cocaine-Induced Increase in Evoked Dopamine Release Recorded In Vivo in the Mouse Nucleus Accumbens

Kappa Opioid Receptor Activation Potentiates the Cocaine-Induced Increase in Evoked Dopamine Release Recorded In Vivo in the Mouse Nucleus Accumbens (214) 39, 336 348 & 214 American College of. All rights reserved 893-133X/14 www.neuropsychopharmacology.org Kappa Opioid Receptor Activation Potentiates the aine-induced Increase in Evoked Dopamine Release

More information

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories Liz Miller Molly McGinnis Lindsey Richardson A research thesis submitted in partial completion of PSY451 senior research thesis, at Wofford

More information

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Psicológica (2004), 25, 45-65. Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Matías López*, Raúl Cantora*, and Luis Aguado** 1 * Universidad de

More information

The Ventral Tegmental Area Is Required for the Behavioral and Nucleus Accumbens Neuronal Firing Responses to Incentive Cues

The Ventral Tegmental Area Is Required for the Behavioral and Nucleus Accumbens Neuronal Firing Responses to Incentive Cues The Journal of Neuroscience, March 24, 2004 24(12):2923 2933 2923 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive The Ventral Tegmental Area Is Required for the Behavioral and Nucleus Accumbens Neuronal Firing Responses

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Negative Reinforcing Properties of Morphine-Antagonists in Naive Rhesus Monkeys* **

Negative Reinforcing Properties of Morphine-Antagonists in Naive Rhesus Monkeys* ** Psychopharmacologia (Berl.) 33, 247--258 (1973) 9 by Springer-Verlag 1973 Negative Reinforcing Properties of Morphine-Antagonists in Naive Rhesus Monkeys* ** F. Hoffmeister and W. Wuttke Institute of Pharmacology,

More information

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience Dopamine and Learning Michael Dorris Associate Professor of Physiology & Neuroscience Studies dorrism@biomed.queensu.ca http://brain.phgy.queensu.ca/dorrislab/ NSCI 324 Systems

More information

Accumbal Neurons that are Activated during Cocaine Self-Administration are Spared from Inhibitory Effects of Repeated Cocaine Self-Administration

Accumbal Neurons that are Activated during Cocaine Self-Administration are Spared from Inhibitory Effects of Repeated Cocaine Self-Administration (2007) 32, 1141 1158 & 2007 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0893-133X/07 $30.00 www.neuropsychopharmacology.org Accumbal Neurons that are Activated during Cocaine Self-Administration are Spared

More information

Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons

Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons Supplementary Information for: Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons Steven W. Kennerley, Timothy E. J. Behrens & Jonathan D. Wallis Content list: Supplementary

More information

Journal of Natural Sciences Research ISSN (Paper) ISSN (Online) Vol.2, No.9, 2012

Journal of Natural Sciences Research ISSN (Paper) ISSN (Online) Vol.2, No.9, 2012 Evaluation of Total Brain Acetylcholine in rats treated with inhaled Tetrahydrocannabinol. (A Bioassay Study) Meraiyebu Ajibola. B (Corresponding author) Department of Physiology, College of medicine Bingham

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place

Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place Preference Stephanie Willis, Jonnique Adjmul, Shabaaz Sandhu, Antoniette M. Maldonado-Devincci, Cheryl Kirsten ABSTRACT The present

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback acquisition associative learning in classical conditioning the initial stage when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

Relationship Between Stress and Substance Use Disorders: Neurobiologic Interface

Relationship Between Stress and Substance Use Disorders: Neurobiologic Interface Relationship Between Stress and Substance Use Disorders: Neurobiologic Interface Kathleen Brady, M.D., Ph.D. Professor of Psychiatry Associate Dean of Clinical and Translational Research Medical University

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

The Neurobiology of Addiction

The Neurobiology of Addiction The Neurobiology of Addiction Jodi Gilman, Ph.D. Center for Addiction Medicine Massachusetts General Hospital Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry Harvard Medical School What is Addiction? commonly

More information

Cannabinoid Receptor Activation in the Basolateral Amygdala Blocks the Effects of Stress on the Conditioning and Extinction of Inhibitory Avoidance

Cannabinoid Receptor Activation in the Basolateral Amygdala Blocks the Effects of Stress on the Conditioning and Extinction of Inhibitory Avoidance 11078 The Journal of Neuroscience, September 9, 2009 29(36):11078 11088 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Cannabinoid Receptor Activation in the Basolateral Amygdala Blocks the Effects of Stress on the Conditioning

More information

Basic characteristics

Basic characteristics Learning Basic characteristics The belief that the universe is lawful and orderly The occurrence of phenomena as a function of the operation of specific variables Objective observation Controlled experiments

More information

Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001

Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001 Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001 An Investigation into the Mechanisms of Memory through Hippocampal Microstimulation In rodents, the hippocampus

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction 1 2 3 4 5 Myers Psychology for AP* Unit 6: Learning Unit Overview How Do We Learn? Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Learning by Observation How Do We Learn? Introduction Learning Habituation

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Reinforcement altered Training phase RTs.

Supplementary Figure 1. Reinforcement altered Training phase RTs. Supplementary Figure 1. Reinforcement altered Training phase RTs. To investigate if there were any effects of reinforcement on Simon task performance, the slope of RTs within each block was quantified

More information

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet University of Amsterdam Supplementary Figures and Legends Day 1 Day 2 Day 3

More information

Complexity Made Simple

Complexity Made Simple Complexity Made Simple Bussey-Saksida Touch Screen Chambers for Rats and Mice Tasks translate directly to well established monkey and human touch models e.g. CANTAB High throughput screening of multiple

More information

Appetitive Pavlovian goal-tracking memories reconsolidate only under specific conditions

Appetitive Pavlovian goal-tracking memories reconsolidate only under specific conditions Research Appetitive Pavlovian goal-tracking memories reconsolidate only under specific conditions Amy C. Reichelt 1 and Jonathan L.C. Lee School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham

More information

The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties

The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties Paul J. Meyer*, Elizabeth S. Cogan, Terry E. Robinson Department of Psychology, University

More information

Following the seminal proposal of Hebb1 and many others, acquired learning

Following the seminal proposal of Hebb1 and many others, acquired learning Associative Learning and Long-Term Potentiation in Rodents: José M. Delgado-, MD, PhD Long-Term Potentiation (LTP) and Learning-Dependent Changes in Synaptic Strength Following the seminal proposal of

More information