Development of Grating Acuity in Children Treated for Unilateral or Bilateral Congenital Cataract

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Development of Grating Acuity in Children Treated for Unilateral or Bilateral Congenital Cataract"

Transcription

1 Development of Grating Acuity in Children Treated for Unilateral or Bilateral Congenital Cataract Terri L. Lewis*-{ Daphne Maurer,*-\ and Henry P. Brent* Purpose. To study the development of grating acuity in children treated for dense congenital unilateral or bilateral cataract and to examine how variations in treatment affect grating acuity during early childhood. Methods. The authors used optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), preferential looking (PL), or both to measure the grating acuity of children treated for congenital cataract in one eye (n = 63) or both eyes (n = 77) whenever possible from the time of treatment until 3 years of age. At each age, the authors compared patients' monocular acuity to that of children with no history of eye disorders. Results. The OKN acuity of treated eyes did not improve with age and was abnormal by 12 months of age. In contrast, PL acuity improved with age, and acuity of most treated eyes was not outside normal limits until 2 to 30 months of age. Nonetheless, at 12 months and at 3 years of age, PL acuity correlated significantly with age at treatment in children who had bilateral cataract. In children who had unilateral cataract, PL acuity correlated significantly with the number of hours per day the good eye had been patched since treatment. Children whose good eye was patched fewer than 3 hours per day did significantly worse than children treated at a comparable age for bilateral congenital cataract. However, children whose good eye was patched at least 3 hours per day had PL acuities similar to those of children treated at a comparable age for bilateral congenital cataract. Conclusions. Children treated for congenital cataract show deficits in grating acuity, with the deficit apparent earlier in OKN acuity than in PL acuity. At least by 1 year of age, visual development has begun to be influenced by the age at treatment and, in children treated for unilateral cataract, by patching of the good eye. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1995; 36: Before the 1980s, numerous studies showed that children treated for unilateral or bilateral congenital cataract have poor letter acuity in affected eyes despite suitable optical correction and, in children who had unilateral cataract, even after regular occlusion of the good eye. 1 As a result, some authors recommended against attempting treatment of unilateral congenital i'uim the ^Def/art merit oj Ophthalmology, The Hospital for Suit Chil/tren, Toronto, and the [Department oj I's^ihology, MiMasler University, llnwiltoii, Ontario Canada. hesented m pint fit the Match 1993 biennial meeting oj the Sonet) foi Research in Child Development (New Oileans, Louisiana) and at the May 1993 annual meeting oj the Association joi Heseanh in Vision and Ophthalmology (Saia.sota, Honda) Supported in part try giants from the National Institutes oj Health (I:Y-O375) and Jwm the Medtial li'eseauh Couruil oj Canada (9975A, MA889, MA 10208, and MTII7I0) Submitted Joi puhlnalion May 31, 199, mused November at(epled April 2, 1995 I'ropnetary interest tategory N Reprint requests Tent I. hints, De\>arlment oj I'syihology, MfMaster University. Hamilton, Ontario, 1.8S KI, Canada cataract. 2 ' However, most of these children had been treated after 1 year of age. Studies during the past decade indicate that children who receive treatment during the first few months of life and, after unilateral cataract, aggressive patching of the good eye throughout early childhood can achieve at least 20/50 linear letter acuity. 1 '" 1:< The advent of methods for measuring acuity in preverbal children prompted the interest in studying the pattern of development of acuity after early deprivation. The first published reports on the development of grating acuity in children treated for congenital cataract used an optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) drum to measure the OKN acuity of two infants treated for unilateral congenital cataract during the first or sixth month of life. M1> Both infants showed an improvement in the OKN acuity of the affected eye during the 3 to months after treatment by surgery and optical correction, with the affected eye always performing 2080 re Ophthalmology & Visual S< lenre, September 1 ( (<):>. Vol 'M\ No 10 Association loi Research in Vision and Ophthalmology

2 Grating Acuity After Congenital Cataract 2081 more poorly than the fellow good eye. Other than these two cases of unilateral cataract and our own publications of preliminary results, 1 '' there have been no reports on the OKN acuity of children treated for congenital cataract. One purpose of the current investigation was to study the development of OKN acuity in children treated for unilateral or bilateral dense congenital cataract. Several investigators have used variants of preferential looking (PL) to monitor grating acuity in children treated for congenital cataract. Generalizations are hampered by the fact that some studies were case reports of a few patients who often were tested only during the first year of life."'" 20 Some studies included patients with other serious problems likely to interfere with visual development, such as retinal detachment 1 ' or persistent hypcrplastic primary vitreous." 21 Some of the studies included patients with better prognoses because the cataract apparently was not dense 1 ' 11 or might not have been congenital." 1 Finally, some studies had procedural problems. 1 "" 20221<VI< - W<<1 '" " Despite methodologic limitations in most studies, the results to date suggest that the PL acuity of the deprived eye of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract is generally within normal limits during the first year of life." 1 " After the first year, PL acuity continues to improve but falls below normal limits by 2 years of age.''- 21 ' 221 "" s<< " 7 Little is known about the development of PL acuity in children treated for bilateral cataract: the only three patients described in the literature had normal PL acuity during the first year of life and were not tested further."" 11 A second purpose of the current study was to compare the development of PL acuity in children treated for unilateral versus bilateral congenital cataract. We chose to monitor the development of grating acuity after visual deprivation with both OKN and PL because our previous studies indicated that the two measures of acuity reveal different developmental patterns in normal children" XM and thus might tap different neural mechanisms. Our versions of both measures have good tcst-retest reliability in normal infants and in infants treated for congenital cataract, 12 ' 2 ' 1 good concurrent validity when compared to Snellen acuity on the same day in older children and adults with amblyopia, 2 * 2 ' and good predictive validity from at least some ages during infancy to later Snellen acuity in children treated for congenital cataract with stable clinical histories. 2 ' Measuring the development of grating acuity in children treated for congenital cataract provides the opportunity to evaluate whether variables known to affect later letter acuity also affect functional vision during infancy and early childhood. Even though the later acuity of children treated for congenital cataract is usually abnormal (reviewed in ref. 11), during the first year of life PL acuity is generally within normal limits."" 1 " This might indicate that tests of PL acuity during the first year of life are insensitive to amblyopia. If so, one would expect no effect on PL acuity of variations in treatment known to affect later acuity, 1 ' namely, age at treatment and, in children treated for unilateral cataract, the extent to which the good eye had been patched. Only two studies have examined systematically the effects of variations in treatment on PL acuity, and both studies included only children treated for unilateral cataract. Birch et al' plotted PL acuity throughout early childhood for eight patients treated for unilateral congenital cataract by 6 weeks of age and for six patients treated between 2 and 8 months of age. The mean PL acuity of the two groups appeared to differ at most ages. However, because the good eye of all 1 patients was usually patched at least 75% of the waking time, Birch et al could not evaluate the contribution of patching the good eye to the PL acuity of the treated eye. Mayer et al 22 evaluated the relative influences of age at treatment and patching in 21 children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. Interocular differences in PL acuity at 12 and 36 (but not 2) months of age were correlated negatively with the total number of hours that the good eye had been patched up to the age of testing. With the amount of patching held constant, the age at surgery did not correlate with PL acuity at any age. However, there may have been insufficient variation in the age at surgery to show an effect because all children had surgery before 5 months of age. Moreover, a better indicator of age at treatment would have been the age at which the affected eye was first given a contact lens because, between surgery and contact lens fitting, the eyes typically were misfocused by +30 D. Another purpose of the current study was to evaluate whether variations in treatment for unilateral or bilateral congenital cataract begin to affect grating acuity during infancy. We examined the effects of these variables on the PL acuity of a sample that was far larger and far more varied in age at treatment than previous studies of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. We are also the first to include children treated for bilateral congenital cataract. We did not have sufficient data to do similar analyses for OKN acuity. In summary, the goals of the research reported here were to use both OKN and PL to study the development of grating acuity in children treated for unilateral versus bilateral dense congenital cataract and to evaluate whether variations in treatment for unilateral or bilateral congenital cataract affect grating acuity during infancy. Patients' OKN and PL data on the development of acuity were compared to reanalyses of our previously published monocular norms. 2 ' 21 Because the norms for PL acuity did not include data from children younger than 6 months of age, we also tested the PL acuity of normal 2- and -month-old

3 2082 Investigative Ophthalmology 8c Visual Science, September 1995, Vol. 36, No. 10 infants. To evaluate any early effects of treatment in patients, we compared age at treatment and, in children treated for unilateral cataract, the mean number of hours per day that the good eye had been patched, to PL acuity at 12 months of age. This was the youngest testing age at which the variance in these factors was sufficient to provide a strong test of their independent and interactive effects on grating acuity. To determine whether the effects of treatment persist throughout childhood, we evaluated the effects of the same variables on PL acuity at 30 to 36 months of age. Compared to our earlier reports, 1 " this study contains a more detailed analysis of the results from a largersample of patients. METHODS Patients All patients younger than 39 months of age who were brought to the Eye Clinic of The Hospital for Sick Children were referred to the study if they had congenital cataracts identified before 6 months of age. Patients were included in the final sample only if, on the first eye examination, an ophthalmologist judged the cataract to interfere seriously with vision because the eye did not fixate or follow a light, because the cataract completely blocked the view of the fundus through an undilated pupil, because no red reflex was visible, or because the cataract looked dense and central. Children with the common associated abnormalities of strabismus, nystagmus, or microcornea were included. We excluded from the final sample the many children with cataracts that were small (<5 mm in diameter), not dense (dull red reflex visible through the cataract), or located only in the periphery of the lens. We also excluded treated eyes with additional serious problems likely to interfere with vision (e.g., glaucoma, retinal scars, detached retina, persistent hyperplastic primar)' vitreous), children who did not wear their optical correction regularly (at least 3 out of days), children treated for unilateral cataract with any abnormalities in the fellow eye that were likely to interfere with vision (e.g., developing cataract, more than minimal refractive error), children whose scores on standardized cognitive tests were >2 SD below the normal mean (because developmental delay may adversely influence grating acuity), 21 ' and children whose medical records were incomplete (because this created doubts about the appropriateness of including them). The final sample consisted of 10 patients, 77 children treated for bilateral cataract and 63 children treated for unilateral cataract. We studied 13 eyes of children who had had bilateral cataract. The remaining 11 eyes of these children were not included in the final sample because they had additional abnormalities likely to interfere with vision (n = 3 with glaucoma; n = 3 with retinal scars), because of procedural errors (n = 2), or because of difficulties in scheduling (n = 3). We studied 63 treated eyes and 60 good eyes of children treated for unilateral cataract. All good eyes of these children were physically normal with minimal refractive error (range = +1.5 to 2.0 D of spherical equivalent). Three of the good eyes were not included in the final sample because of difficulties in scheduling. Because prematurity may retard the development of grating acuity,"' we adjusted to postterm age the ages of the eight patients (five treated for bilateral cataract and three treated for unilateral cataract) who were born before 38 weeks gestation. Table ] provides additional clinical details for the 10 patients in the final sample. Surgery was performed usually within 8 hours of referral to The Hospital for Sick Children. In patients with bilateral cataract, the second eye underwent surgery 2 lo 3 days after the first eye. Either of two ophthalmologists performed the surgery for two thirds of the patients. Approximately 1 week after surgery, the aphakic eyes were measured for contact lenses. Measurements of the eye were made under general anesthesia by one of us (HPB), and the necessary power of the lens to focus the eye at 50 cm was determined by three methods: retinoscopy, autorefraction, and calculation from (he results of keratometry and A-scan axial length. Patients were fitted with custom-made, rigid, gas-permeable contact lenses that parents were instructed to remove at night. Rigid, gas-permeable lenses are used because, compared to soft lenses, parents find them easier to insert, it is easier to perform accurate retinoscopy through them, they compensate for conical astigmatism so that there is no residual cylinder, and they are less easily lost from the eye and less costly to replace. Approximately 1 to 2 weeks after the examination under anesthesia, the child came to the clinic to receive the contact lcns(es) and to have the fit and power checked. Patients returned ever)' I to 2 weeks until it was clear that the parents could cope and that the fit and power of the lens was appropriate. Alterations to the lenses were made as necessary, and, in the few cases in which the parents were not able to handle daily insertion and removal, instructions were given for extended-wear use. Thereafter, the child was checked every 3 months and reexamincd under general anesthesia only if some important aspect of the clinical examination could not be completed. Most children wore their contact lenses successfully and those who did not (eight children treated for bilateral cataract) were prescribed glasses. After treatment for unilateral cataract, the parents were instructed to patch the good eye throughout early childhood. Depending on the ophthalmologist involved and the year of treatment, the recommended

4 Grating Acuity After Congenital Cataract 2083 TABLE l. Clinical Details for Patients Tested With PL, OKN, or Both Age at surgery (months) Age at first optical correction (months) Age at beginning of patching (months) Initial refractive error (diopters) Unilateral (n = 63) 3.0 ( ).2 ( ).5 ( ) +26 ( ) Bilateral (n = 13 eyes of 77 children) 3.3 ( ). ( ) +28 ( ) Values arc median (range). PI. = preferential looking; OKN = optokinetic nystagmus. amount of patching varied among patients from to 8 hours of waking time per day. Not only was there variation in the amount of recommended patching, there was variation in compliance with the instruction. To monitor as accurately as possible the amount of patching, we questioned parents on every visit to the clinic. We phrased our questions so as to make it easy for a parent to admit to little or no patching (e.g., "Is he still giving you a fight about patching?" or "Would you say that you have been patching about 15 minutes a day?"). Often, we also observed whether parents' reports were consistent with how easily they were able to patch a child's eye for our tests, and we questioned them again if we doubted their initial estimates. Normal Controls The subjects were twenty-four 2-month-old and twentyfive -month-old infants similar to those described previously. 2 ' 1 Briefly, all children had been born at term (gcstational age, 38 to 2 weeks), weighed at least 2500 grams at birth, were tested within 2 weeks of the specified age, and showed no abnormalities on a standard ophthalmologic examination, including cycloplegic refraction, given within 1 month of the PL tests (see 23 for criteria). Two additional children were excluded from the sample because of a procedural error (n = 1) or because the eye examination revealed problems that might interfere with normal vision (n = 1). For all patients and normal control subjects, the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki were followed, and informed consent was obtained from a parent or guardian after the procedures had been explained. This research was approved by the human experimentation committees at The Hospital for Sick Children and at McMaster University. Acuity Testing Preferential looking. For patients and normal controls, PL acuity was tested using procedures similar to those published previously (sec experiment 3 of reference 2). Briefly, the child had one eye patched and sat 50 cm from a grey panel with two 10 portholes, each placed with the closest edge 16 to the left or right of a central observation hole. Two small (0.3 ) lights located immediately above and below the observation hole were flashed at the beginning of each trial to attract the child's attention. During each trial, black-and-white vertical stripes (contrast = 88%; space-average luminance = 62 cd/m 2 ; Intergraphics Precision Photographic Services, Kirkland, WA) were presented through either the left or right porthole, and one of four plain grey stimuli bracketing the luminance of the stripes (range = 55 to 70 cd/m 2 ) was presented through the other porthole. The stimuli were illuminated by a 500-W EHD stage lamp (Standard Products, Mt. Royal, Quebec, Canada) located above and behind the child. Stripe size ranged from 132 to 0.75 arc min, when viewed from 50 cm, in approximately half-octave steps (an octave is a doubling or a halving of a value). When necessary, we moved the child back to cm to create stripes as small as arc min. We began with wide stripes (66 arc min or, if necessary, 132 arc min) and first trained the child to look toward the stripes by projecting a colored picture onto a screen beside them whenever the child made an appropriate response. After training, the observer had to guess the location of the stripes solely on the basis of the child's behavior. After four consecutive correct choices, the acuity test began. In the acuity test, we used a modification of Taylor and Creelman's 2 * PEST staircase procedure (sec ref. 2 for details) to zero in on the narrowest stripes the child/observer could locate accurately 75% of the time. Normal 2- and -month-old infants completed two tests of one eye, with the eye selected for testing (right or left) alternated across subjects. The two tests were conducted by different observers, usually on the same day but always within 1 week of each other. The observer for the second test was unaware of the results from the first test. The protocol for patients is described below in Testing Protocol for Patients. Optokinetic nystagmus. Details of our OKN acuity test have been published. J< Briefly, the child had one eye patched and sat 50 cm (or, to create the smallest stripes, cm) from a large rear projection screen (8 X 8 when viewed from 50 cm). Vertical blackand-white stripes (contrast = 91%; space-average luminance = 0 cd/m 2 ) moved across the screen at 13 /

5 208 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 1995, Vol. 36, No. 10 second (or 6.5 /second when viewed from cm) for periods of 7 seconds. The direction of movement was always from the temporal field toward the nasal field because normal young infants and children with eye problems often show OKN only for stripes moving in this direction. 2 ' 1 " u Stripe size ranged from 800 to 12 arc min, when viewed from 50 cm, in approximately half-octave steps. Observers watched the child's eye movements through peepholes on cither side of the screen and determined the narrowest stripes eliciting OKN. After each stripe size yielding a positive response, the child was given a control trial during which a blank field was projected. If the observers could not differentiate nystagmus elicited by the stripes from any spontaneous nystagmus seen on control trials, the test was aborted. Whenever the child had to be moved back to cm to create smaller stripes, the observers verified that OKN could still be elicited by large stripes despite the reduction in velocity and in the size of the field. Note that because velocity was constant for testing at each distance, increases in spatial frequency simultaneously increased the temporal frequency of changes in luminance on the retina. Hence, our OKN thresholds may be limited by temporal resolution, spatial resolution, or both. In addition, OKN thresholds were limited to 6 arc min, the smallest stripes available when viewed from cm. Testing protocol for patients. Patients were tested repeatedly with PL and OKN throughout early childhood. All tests were monocular, and treated eyes were corrected optically for the testing distance. We divided the data collected between 1 and 39 months of age into the age groups shown in Table 2 (nominal ages represent the approximate median age for each group). The number of eyes in the final analyses varied with age (see Table 2) because we excluded tests for which there were reasons other than the initial deprivation to expect poor acuity (e.g., the presence of a pupillary membrane that impinged on the visual axis, failure to wear the contact lens for several days before the test, and so on). In addition, some children entered the sample late (e.g., received their first optical correction when they were too old for even the 12-month tests), some moved away before the 36- month tests were completed, some could not be tested because of scheduling difficulties, and some were not yet 3 years of age at the end of the study. Of the tests we attempted, a relatively small proportion were unsuccessful because the child would not complete the procedure or because a subsequent ophthalmologic examination showed that the optical correction had been inappropriate for the testing distance. With PL, we completed tests with an optical correction suitable for the testing distance in 7% to % of the treated eyes attempted at each age (see Table 2). However, with OKN, we were successful with only % to 62% of the treated eyes, primarily because many of the children treated for bilateral cataract had a spontaneous nystagmus that we were unable to differentiate from any nystagmus elicited by the stripes. Of the 20 treated eyes tested successfully with PL, each, on average, contributed data to 3.5 age periods and, across all age periods during which the child was part of the active sample, each was tested an average of 1. times per age period. Of the 75 treated eyes tested successfully with OKN each, on average, contributed data to 2.1 age periods and was tested an average of 0.6 times per age period. Age at treatment (defined as age at which the first optical correction was received) was similar for the samples tested successfully with PL and with OKN, and it was similar for those tested successfully versus those who were not. Data Analysis Acuity for PL and OKN was measured as the size of the smallest stripes (expressed in arc min) eliciting the criterion response. For analyses and graphing, the acuities were converted to log 2. The numbers were then reconverted to arc min for reporting, except for the comparisons between PL and OKN acuity, which are reported in octaves (log 2 ). Normal controls. To obtain a better estimate of the variability in normal development, we used parametric, rather than nonparametric, statistics to reanalyze our previously published normative data on the development of PL and OKN acuity. 2 ' 2 ' 1 For each method and age group, we calculated the mean acuity on the first test and the 95% prediction limits. The 95% prediction limits can be used to determine whether an individual patient's acuity falls within the range of values expected from 95% of age-matched normal children" and, for each age group, were calculated using the formula: 95% prediction limits = M ± t rf/, X SD X Vl + 1/n (1) where M = sample mean, {^n = the two-tailed value at the 0.05 level from Student's t distribution with n = 1 df, n = the number of subjects in the age group, and SD = their standard deviation. Norms for the 2- and -month-old infants tested with PL in the current study were calculated in the same way. Each subject's second test of acuity was used only to estimate reliability. Development of acuity in patients. For each eye of patients, we calculated the mean PL acuity and the mean OKN acuity for all tests within each age period, then calculated group means and standard errors separately for eyes treated for bilateral congenital cataract, for the affected eyes of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract, and for the good eyes of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. To compare PL and OKN acuity in treated eyes tested with both measures, we compared the logy ratio of the

6 Grating Acuity After Congenital Cataract 2085 TABLE 2. Size and Representativeness of the Sample at Each Age for PL and OKN Acuity PL Acuity OKN Acuity Thnted Eyes Good Eyes Treated Eyes Good Eyes Age at Test (months) N* %t A' % N % N % Unilateral Bilateral * Number of eyes in the final.sample. t Percentage of eyes conlributing daia of those we atlempled to test. PL = preferential looking; ORN = oplokinetic nystagmus. PL acuity of each eye relative to the normal mean at that age with the log 2 ratio of OKN acuity relative to its normal mean. We then calculated the Pearson correlation between the two ratios at each age. To avoid type II error and for consistency with the ages selcctedfor the analyses on the effects of treatment, we performed these analyses only for data collected at 12 and 36 months of age. Effects of variations in treatment. There were too few data to examine the effects of variations in treatment on OKN acuity. To evaluate whether variations in treatment have any effect on early PL acuity, we compared PL acuity at 12 months of age to age at treatment (age at which the first optical correction after surgery was received) and, in children treated for unilateral cataract, to the amount of patching. We chose 12 months because that was the earliest age at which there was sufficient variation in the age at treatment. To determine whether the effects persist, we examined PL acuity at 30 to 36 months of age. We combined the data from 30 and 36 months because at neither age alone was there sufficient data for the multiple regression analyses used with children treated for unilateral cataract (see below). Because the norms differ for 30- and 36-month-old children (see Fig. 1), wefirstcalculated an acuity ratio for ever)' test using the formula: log 2 acuity ratio acuity of treated eye = log, normal mean for patient's age (2) We then derived one score for each treated eye by calculating the mean of the acuity ratios across the 30- to 36-month periods. For children treated for bilateral cataract, we used simple regression analyses to compare age at treatment to PL acuity. We included one eye per child at each age. For children for whom we had data from both eyes at 12 months, we arbitrarily selected for inclusion the eye with the most data across all age periods. Whenever possible, we selected the same eye for inclusion at 30 to 36 months. Later recognition acuity, when available, indicated that the selected eyes did not systematically achieve better or worse acuity than the fellow treated eyes. For children treated for unilateral cataract, we used multiple regression analysis to compare the independent variables of age at treatment and mean number of hours per day the good eye was patched to the dependent variable of PL acuity of the treated eye at 12 months of age. To calculate a patching score for each child, we computed the mean number of waking hours per day that the good eye was patched from the day the child received the first optical correction until the day of the last acuity' test in the 12-month age period. For example, if a child underwent the last acuity test in the 12-month age period 07 days after receiving the first optical correction and the good eye was patched 6 waking hours per day beginning 7 days after the first optical correction, the child would be credited with 200 hours of patching during the 07 days, or a mean of 5.9 hours per day. (Patching scores

7 2086 Investigative Ophthalmology 8c Visual Science, September 1995, Vol. 36, No. 10 i. The development of preferential looking acuity (A) and optokinetic nystagmus acuity (B) as a function of age at test in normal children. Each point represents the mean acuity (±1 SE) of the group at a particular age. Sample looking size was 2 to 25 children per group except for the 2-month-old children tested with preferential looking (n = 7) and the 36month-old children tested with optokinetic nystagmus (n = 21). If not shown, the standard error was smaller than the size of the symbol. Shaded areas represent the 95% prediction limits. FICURE OKN «2-8- O c f f Age (months) were rarely this high; even the most compliant children had short lapses in patching because of lost lenses, illness, and odier factors). The precision of these numbers is misleading. Nonetheless, we are confident that the children credited with the most patching were different from the rest and that estimates for the children credited with the least patching are fairly accurate because parents had no reason to understate the amount of patching. We conducted a similar multiple regression analysis for the PL acuity of the treated eye at 30 to 36 months of age. However, for each child, we weighted the patching score according to the number of tests contributed by the treated eye to the 30-month versus 36-month age periods. Specifically, we computed the mean number of waking hours per day that the good eye was patched from the day the child received the first optical correction until (a) the day of the last acuity test in the 30-month age period for all tests in that age period and (b) the day of the last acuity test in the 36-month age period for all tests in diat age period. We then calculated a weighted mean of the patching scores for inclusion in the multiple regression analysis. For example, if a child contributed two tests of the treated eye in the 30-month age period and one test in the 36-month age period, the 30-month patching score counted twice as heavily as the 36mondi patching score in the mean. As discussed previously, we used log,, acuity for all analyses. Age at treatment and patching scores were entered as linear variables. However, when we repeated all analyses using log2 values for age at treatment, the conclusions were identical. Unilateral versus bilateral cataract. Children treated for unilateral congenital cataract would be expected to have worse acuity than those treated at the same age for bilateral congenital cataract because children Age (months) 36 treated for unilateral cataract suffer not only from the initial deprivation of a cataractous eye but also from unbalanced competition between the eyes (reviewed in ref. 35). However, the competitive advantage of the good eye can be offset by occlusion. To determine whether these trends are evident in PL acuity, we used t-tesxs to compare PL acuity at 12 months and at 30 to 36 months in one eye of children treated for bilateral cataract to children treated for unilateral cataract whose good eye was patched less than 3 hours per day or at least 3 hours per day. With this division, there were no significant differences in age at treatment across the three groups in children for whom there were data for PL acuity at 12 months of age (Fy,g0 2.01, P > 0.10) or at 30 to 36 months of age (F2iM = 0.06, P > 0.90). We could not divide the data at hours per day of patching because 12 month-old children whose good eye was patched at least hours per day were treated at a significantly younger age than those treated for bilateral cataract, t7q = 2.01, P < RESULTS Normal Controls The left panel of Figure 1 shows the mean, standard error, and 95% prediction limits for the first test of PL acuity in normal 2- and -month-old infantsj, along with the reanalysis of our previously published % Normal 2- and -month-old infants tested with preferential looking yielded reliable results. For 2-month-old infants, acuity values from the second test were within 1 octave of those from the first test 88% of the time and within 0,5 octaves 83% of the time. For -month-old infants, acuity values from the second test were within 1 octave of the first test 80% of the time and within 0.5 octaves 6% of the time.

8 2087 Grating Acuity After Congenital Cataract PL o u ft CM O a in the good eye of children treated for unilateral cataract (open triangles) was similar to the mean acuity of normal eyes (compare left panel of Fig. 1 to Fig. 2), was within normal limits at all ages, and generally was better than that of treated eyes. The PL acuity of the affected eyes of patients treated for unilateral cataract (filled triangles) was no different than that of patients treated for bilateral cataract (open squares). For both groups of patients, PL acuity improved with age, but the improvement failed to keep pace with that of normal children. In fact; most treated eyes had PL acuity within normal limits before 2 months of age, but more that 70% of the treated eyes had PL acuity outside normal limits by 36 months of age. The same was true when the comparison was restricted to eyes tested longitudinally; Of the 17 eyes treated for unilateral cataract and tested both at 12 and 36 months of age, 88% had normal PL acuity at 12 months of age, but 76% had,abnormal PL acuity at 36 months of age. Similarly, of the eyes treated for bilateral cataract and tested at both 12 and 36 months of age, 91% had normal PL acuity at 12 months of age, but 6% had abnormal PL acuity at 36 months of age. Figure 3 shows the development of OKN acuity as a function of age at test in children treated for bilat- Age (months) FIGURE 2. The development of preferential looking acuity as a function of age at test in children treated for bilateral congenital cataract (open squares) and in the affected {filled, triangles) and good (open triangles) eyes of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. Sample size at each age is provided in Table 2. Graph shows mean and SE plotted -CL against the 95% prediction limits from-normal -childrenr Other details as in Figure OKN 5 norms for older children.2 For comparison, the right panel of Figure 1 shows a similar reanalysis of our previously published norms for OKN acuity." (As in our previous publications, age at test is plotted on a log-2 scale.) The two measures of acuity reveal very different developmental patterns. Although the shape of the functions are similar between 2 and months of age, PL acuity develops more slowly than OKN acuity between and 18 months of age. Although PL acuity appears to develop more rapidly than OKN acuity between 18 and 36 months of age, OKN acuity was limited by a ceiling effect: At 18 and 36 months of age, some children showed OKN to the smallest stripes available (see Fig. 1). Patients Figure 2 shows the development of PL acuity as a function of age at test for patients. For comparison, the shaded area represents the 95% prediction limits obtained from normal children. The mean PL acuity D Age (months) FIGURE 3. The development of optokinetic nystagmus acuity as a function of age at test in children treated for bilateral congenital cataract (open squares) and in the affected (filled triangles) and good (open, triangles) eyes of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. Other details as in Figure 1.

9 2088 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 1995, Vol. 36, No. 10 oral congenital cataract (open squares), in the affected eyes of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract (filled triangles), and in the good eyes of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract (open triangles) compared to the 95% prediction limits (shaded area). (Mainly because of difficulties testing children with spontaneous nystagmus; these data arc from fewer children than the data oii PL acuity; see Table 2.) The mean OKN acuity in the good eyes of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract was below the normal mean after G months of age (compare right panel of Fig. J to Fig. 3) but better than that of fellow treated eyes after months of age. At most ages, OKN acuity was similar in eyes treated for unilateral versus bilateral cataract. Unlike the results for PL acuity, the OKN acuity of treated eyes did not improve with age and was usually abnormal by 12 months of age. The lack of improvement in OKN acuity with age also was evident when the sample was restricted to eyes tested at more than one age. Of the nine eyes treated for unilateral cataract and tested both at 12 and 36 months of age, 78% had abnormal OKN acuity at both ages. Similarly, of the five eyes treated for bilateral cataract and tested at both 12 and 36 months of age, 80% had abnormal acuity at both ages. The different pattern of development revealed by tests of OKN versus PL acuity is emphasized in Figure, which compares the PL and OKN acuity of treated eyes tested at the same age by the two techniques. In each case, acuities are plotted as distance in octaves (log 2 ) from the age norm so that values of 0 equal the age norm, negative values represent acuities worse than the normal mean, and positive values represent acuities better than the normal mean. Agreement was poor at 12 months of age because acuity was worse than the normal mean much more often when measured with OKN than when measured with PL (see Fig., Panel A). Not surprisingly, there was no significant correlation between results from the two measures at 12 months (r = 0.15, I* > 0.0). At 36 months of age, the two measures agreed that the acuity of most treated eyes was worse than the normal mean (see Fig., panel B). However, the correlation between PL and OKN acuity (both expressed relative to normal) was still not significant (r = 0.15, I 3 > 0.50). Effects of Variations in Treatment Bilateral cataract. Figure 5 shows PL acuity at 12 months as a function of the age at treatment for one eye of each child treated for bilateral cataract with data at that age. Figure 6 shows similar results for children tested at 30 to 36 months of age, except that acuities arc represented as a ratio of the normal mean. Simple regression analyses indicated that the earlier the treatment, the better the PL acuity at 12 months (n = 51, r = 0.3, t = 3.32, P < 0.01) and the better o o Z o is u S - -5' B / r//i < OKN Acuity relative to Normal (Octaves) F1GUKK. The relation between preferential looking and optokinetic nystagmus acuity at 12 months (panel A) and 36 months (panel B) of age. Symbols distinguish children treated for bilateral (open circles) or unilateral (filled circles) congenital cataract. Each point represents the acuity for one treated eye expressed as distance, in octaves, from the normal mean. Negative values represent acuities worse than the normal mean, and positive values represent acuities better than the normal mean. The diagonal line represents perfect agreement between the two measures. the PL acuity ratio at 30 to 36 months (n = 51, r = 0.51, t =.19, P= ). Unilateral cataract. For the 2 children treated for unilateral cataract for whom there were data from the treated eye at 12 months of age, multiple regression analysis revealed a significant main effect of patching such that the more the good eye was patched, the better the PL acuity of the treated eye (r = -0.36, t

10 Grating Acuity After Congenital Cataract 2089 CQ I- 5 T 1 - s 8 - > Bilateral Cases CO O y =.161x , r 2 = Age at Treatment (months) FIGURE 5. The relation between preferential looking acuity at 12 months and age at treatment for one eye of each of 51 children treated for bilateral congenital cataract. = 2.18, P < 0.05) (sec Fig. 7). In contrast, PL acuity of the good eye of these children was unrelated to the number of hours per day it had been patched (n = 36, r = -0.0, P> 0.10) and, as shown in Figure 2, was better than the PL acuity of the treated eye. Multiple regression analysis also indicated no significant main effect of the age at treatment on the PL acuity of the treated eye at 12 months (r = 0.12, t = 0.77, P > 0.0) and no significant interaction between age at treatment and patching (t = 1.87, P> 0.05). Multiple regression analysis for the 36 children treated for unilateral cataract for whom there were data at 30 to 36 months of age revealed a significant main effect of patching (r = -0.53, t = 3.55, P = 0.001), no effect of the age at treatment (t = 0.6, P > 0.60), and no interaction between patching and age at treatment (t = 0.85, P = 0.0). The PL acuity ratio of the good eye of these children was unrelated to the number of hours per day that it had been patched (n = 3, r = -0.30, P > 0.05). Moreover, as shown in Figure 2, the PL acuity of the good eye was better than the PL acuity of the treated eye. Figure 8 illustrates the simple correlation between patching and the PL acuity ratio at 30 to 36 months of age. In summary, multiple regression analyses lead to the same conclusions at 12 and at 30 to 36 months of age in children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. The more the good eye is patched, the better the PL acuity of the treated eye. Preferential looking acuity of the good eye was unrelated to the number of hours per day it had been patched. Finally, children treated for unilateral congenital cataract, unlike those treated for bilateral congenital cataract, showed no linear effect of age at treatment on PL acuity. Unilateral versus bilateral cataract. At 12 months of age, PL acuity was significantly better in the 51 children treated for bilateral cataract than in the seven children treated for unilateral cataract whose good eye was patched less than 3 hours per day (t.-,,, = 3.37, P = 0.001). However, there was no significant difference in PL acuity between children treated for bilateral cataract and the 35 children treated for unilateral cataract whose good eye was patched at least 3 hours per day (l M = 0.58, P > 0.50). Results were similar at 30 to 36 months of age. Specifically, the PL acuity ratio was significantly better in the 51 children treated for bilateral cataract than in the 1 children treated for unilateral cataract whose good eye was patched less than 3 hours per day (t<, : < = 3.31, P < 0.01) but not in the 22 children whose good eye was patched at least 3 hours per day (t 71 = 0.36, P > 0.70). DISCUSSION Other than our own preliminary reports, 1 " this study provides the first report of the development of PL O 2 z ex> -<! PN 3 8 fiu >6 J Bilateral Cases o o =.11x+ 0.16, r 2 = >12 Age at Treatment (months) FIGURE 6. Preferential looking acuity ratio at 30 to 36 months of age as a function of age at treatment for one eye of each of 51 children treated for bilateral congenital cataract. The _y-axis represents preferential looking acuity divided by the age norm (see text for details).

11 2090 Investigative Ophthalmology 8e Visual Science, September 1995, Vol. 36, No. 10 >63Q 0 1 Unilateral Cases y =.268x +.012, r 2 =.17 Patching (hours/day) FIGURE 7. The relation between preferential looking acuity at 12 months and the number of hours per clay that the good eye was patched. Data are for the affected eye of each of 2 children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. acuity in groups of children treated for bilateral cataract and the first report of the development of OKN acuity in groups of children treated for bilateral or unilateral cataract. We found that the grating acuity of children treated for congenital cataract exhibited very different developmental patterns when measured by PL versus OKN. Preferential looking acuity was within normal limils during early infancy. Then, although it continued to improve throughout early childhood, the improvement failed to keep pace with that of normal children by 2 to 30 months of age. In contrast, OKN acuity did not improve with age and was usually outside the normal range by 12 months of age. Optokinetic Nystagmus Acuity One goal of the current study was to measure the development of OKN acuity in children treated for unilateral or bilateral congenital cataract. We expected OKN to be a useful measure of grating acuity because the results are reliable 1 2 * and have good concurrent and predictive validity. 2 ' 2> M> However, primarily because of spontaneous nystagmus that the observers could not distinguish from elicited nystagmus, results were obtained for only 60% of the patients treated for unilateral cataract and 10% of the patients treated for bilateral cataract. One way to overcome this problem might be to use electro-oculographic recordings rather than human observers, as has been done in some previous studies of OKN in infants." t2 The low success rate in measuring OKN acuity raises the concern that the tested sample may be biased. However, PL acuity was no different in the treated eyes of children who could be tested successfully with OKN than in those who could not. For example, this comparison revealed no differences at 12 months of age (bilateral: t S(( = 1.55, P > 0.10; unilateral: t 1() = 0.02, P > 0.90) or at 36 months of age (bilateral: t 7() = 0.09, P > 0.90; unilateral: t,,, = 0.75, P > 0.0). Another concern is that the observers may have been insensitive because these patients arc difficult to lest. However, we have found the results from patients with similar clinical histories to be reliable with good concurrent and predictive validity. 1 2:v "' There arc several reasons why the OKN acuity of treated eyes may have been below normal limits at most ages. Structural abnormalities such as microcornea are an unlikely explanation because there is no reason to assume that these factors affected OKN acuity any more than PL acuity. Nystagmus is also an unlikely explanation. As discussed, most children with nystagmus were excluded from the OKN sample. For those who were included, any underlying oscillations '3 > O o Unilateral Cases O y =.35x , r 2 =.27 Patching (hours/day) KIGUKK8. Preferential looking acuity ratio at 30 to 36 months of age as a function of the number of hours per day that the good eye was patched. The )>-axis represents preferential looking acuity divided by the age norm (see text for details). Data are for the affected eye of each of 3(5 children treated for unilateral congenital cataract.

12 Grating Acuity After Congenital Cataract 2091 of the eye would increase and decrease temporal frequency on the retina, with the amount of change dependent on the amplitude and frequency of the oscillations relative to the stimulus motion. We do not know the effect of any such oscillations on the ability to elicit OKN. It is difficult to evaluate the extent to which strabismus might have contributed to poor OKN acuity. We could not analyze statistically the effects of strabismus on OKN acuity because the OKN sample is small, some patients have esotropia and some have exotropia, the degree of strabismus often changed over time, and many patients underwent surgery for strabismus. Nonetheless, it is unlikely that the presence or degree of strabismus accounted for a worsening of OKN acuity with age relative to normal. For example, although children treated for bilateral cataract performed worse relative to normal at 36 months of age than at 12 months of age, the degree of strabismus across these patients was no greater at 36 months than at 12 months of age. However, oculomotor factors other than nystagmus or strabismus might have contributed to poor OKN acuity. This possibility is supported by evidence that the OKN system is abnormal in children treated for congenital cataract. For example, such children show good OKN to low spatial frequencies moving from the temporal visual field to the nasal visual field, but little or no OKN to comparable stimuli moving in the opposite direction. 10 <(l In children treated for unilateral congenital cataract, this is true not only for the treated eye but also for the fellow eye. Although the stripes in this study always moved toward the nasal visual field, it is possible that after deprivation the OKN system is not completely normal for any direction of motion. Perhaps partly for that reason, OKN acuity failed to improve with age, arrd even the acuity of the good eye of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract was below the normal mean after 6 months of age (compare right panel of Fig. 1 to Fig. 3). Sensory factors likely also contributed to the poor OKN acuity of treated eyes. Several lines of evidence support this hypothesis. First, children treated for congenital cataract rarely obtairr 20/20 vision on later tests of linear recognitiorr acuity.' (>s " 12 ' ( '"""" ' Thus, it would rrot be surprising if they also had abnormal acuity in infancy. Second, the test was sufficiently sensitive to reveal differences in OKN acuity between the treated eye and the good eye after months of age in children who had unilateral cataract (see Fig. 3). Third, we have shown our version of OKN acuity to be both reliable and valid. Specifically, results from our version of the test (i) are reliable in normal infants and in infants treated for congenital cataract, 1 ~ ( (ii) have good concurrent validity when compared to Snel- Icn acuity orr the same day in older children and adults with amblyopia, 2 ' (iii) have good predictive validity from at least some ages during infancy to the later Snellen acuity of children treated for congenital cataract with stable clinical histories, and (iv) have good concurrent validity when compared to electro-oculographic recordings in a normal subject. ll> In summary, the poor OKN acuity of children treated for congenital cataract was likely not caused by microcornea, nystagmus, or strabismus. Rather, the rrrost likely explanation is a sensor)' deficit with the possible contribution of an abnormal OKN system. Preferential Looking Acuity As did previous investigators 1 ' '" '' 2l --'- Mml "" ' and consistent with our owrr preliminary findings, 1 " we found that the PL acuity of the affected eye of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract is within normal limits during the first year of life and, although it continues to improve, it falls below normal limits by 2 years of age. We found a similar pattern in children treated for bilateral congenital cataract. Such good vision during the first 1 to 2 years of life is surprising because children treated for congenital cataract rarely attain 20/20 vision on later tests of linear letter acuchildren treated for unilateral versus bilateral cataract at similar ages was virtually identical at every age tested. Yet, the linear letter acuity of eyes treated for unilateral congenital cataract is usually worse than that of eyes treated at the same age for bilateral corrgerrital cataract. 1 " Finally, during the first year, most infants had PL acuity well within the normal range despite considerable variation in age at treatment and the extent to which the good eye was patched. The linear letter acuity of eyes treated for corrgerrital cataract is usually better after earlier treatment than after later treatment arrd, in children treated for unilateral congenital cataract, it is better after more patching of the good eye. 11 '"' " It is tempting to conclude either that PL acuity is not a sensitive measure of functional visioir during infancy or that it takes several years for deficits in PL acuity to develop, longer than it takes for deficits irr OKN acuity to emerge. Several lines of evidence argue against the hypothesis that PL is an insensitive measure during infancy. First, even though most eyes had normal PL acuity during the first 18 to 2 months of life, variables krrowrr to influence the later recognition acuity of children treated for congenital cataract and to influence the grating acuity of visually deprived animals were correlated significantly with PL acuity at 12 months of age. Specifically, for children treated for bilateral congenital cataract, there was a significant correlation between age at treatment and PL acuity at 12 months such that the earlier the treatment, the better the PL acuity. Moreover, tests at 30 to 36 months of age indicated that this relationship persisted throughout early

13 2092 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 1995, Vol. 36, No. 10 childhood. These results complement previous reports of better Snellen acuity after earlier treatment in binocularly deprived children 1 and of better grating acuity after shorter deprivation in binocularly deprived cats and monkeys, ww although the effects are not always linear in monkeys. The significant correlations underscore the need for early treatment in children born with bilateral cataracts. For children treated for unilateral congenital cataract, we found that the more the good eye had been patched, the better the PL acuity of the treated eye at 12 months and at 30 to 36 months of age. This result is similar to that reported by Mayer ct al, 22 who studied a more homogeneous sample and complements similar studies of monocularly deprived cats and aphakic monkeys."'"" Moreover, PL acuity at 12 and at 30 to 36 months of age was significantly better in children treated for bilateral cataract than in children treated for unilateral cataract who had been poor patchers, even though the age at treatment was comparable across groups. Finally, although the PL acuity of the affected eye of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract was usually within normal limits until 18 to 2 months of age, it was generally worse than that of the good eye (see Fig. 2). This finding is consistent with previous reports of significant interocular differences in the PL acuity of most children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. 1 ' 2122 The bulk of evidence suggests that PL is a sensitive measure of acuity during infancy and that it takes several years for deficits in PL acuity to develop, longer than it takes for deficits in OKN to emerge. As did Mayer ct al, 22 we found no evidence after monocular deprivation for a linear relationship between age at treatment and PL acuity at 12 or at 30 to 36 months of age and no evidence for an interaction between age at treatment and patching on PL acuity. One possibility is that the relation between age at treatment and PL acuity is not linear. Rather, with patching held constant, PL acuity might be significantly better for eyes treated very early than for eyes treated at any later time. Consistent with this hypothesis is the finding of Birch ct al' that older children whose good eye was patched 6 to 8 waking hours per day throughout early childhood had significantly better recognition acuity in the affected eye if it had been treated before 6 weeks of age than if it had been treated after 2 months of age. We also found evidence to support this hypothesis in our data. Specifically, the 21 children treated for unilateral congenital cataract whose good eye was patched at least waking hours per day (we had too few data to test only children whose good eye was patched longer) had significantly better PL acuity at 12 months if treatment occurred by 6 weeks of age (n = 6) than if it occurred later (n = 15) (t «i = 2.3, P < 0.05). (There were too few data for a similar analysis of PL acuity at 30 to 36 months.) We did not emphasize this result because children with later treatment whose good eye was patched only 3 to waking hours per day tended to do as well as those who had earlier treatment and more patching. Nonetheless, studies of later recognition acuity emphasize the value of both very early treatment and aggressive occlusion of the good eye of children with unilateral congenital cataract.' Preferential Looking Versus Optokinetic Nystagmus Acuity Our finding that PL acuity at 12 months is related to variations in treatment and that the effects persist throughout early childhood suggests that PL is a valid measure of functional vision during infancy. Why then, docs it take so much longer for deficits in PL acuity to develop than deficits in OKN acuity? And why is there no relationship between PL acuity and OKN acuity even at 36 months of age, an age at which both tests agree that the acuity of most treated eyes is abnormal? As discussed, OKN acuity might be affected not only by sensory factors but also by oculomotor deficits that would have little or no effect on PL acuity. However, oculomotor factors cannot easily explain our finding that, in normal infants, PL acuity develops more slowly than OKN acuity between and 18 months of age. A second and not mutually exclusive explanation is that the two tests differ in the extent to which they are influenced by central versus peripheral vision. For PL acuity, decreasing the size of the striped field causes poorer performance in infants with general retinal degeneration,' 12 in older children with amblyopia,' 1 * 1 ' 1 and sometimes in normal children." However, PL acuity is essentially normal in infants with no anatomic fovea.' 1 " Together these results suggest that measurements of PL acuity during infancy are dominated by input from the peripheral retina. In contrast, OKN acuity appears to be dominated by input from the central retina. Optokinetic nystagmus gain is degraded by occluding the central 5 of the stimulus' 1 ' 1 "' and in patients with a central scotoma."' However, eliminating the influence of most of the peripheral retina by decreasing the size of the field even to 5 has little or no effect on OKN gain.' 1 ' 1 "' 1 ' Studies of school-aged children treated for congenital cataract in infancy reveal deficits in both peripheral and central vision, although the deficits in central vision are far more severe. 1S ~' 1 If a similar pattern of deficits is present in young infants treated for cataract, it would result in relatively good PL acuity (which, during infancy, appears to depend mainly on input from the peripheral retina) and relatively poor OKN acuity (which appears to depend mainly on input from the central retina). In patients, the deficits in PL acuity that emerge after approximately 18 months of age may reflect an increasing influence of

14 Grating Acuity After Congenital Cataract 2093 input from the central retina on PL acuity. Similarly, in normal children, the rapid development of PL acuity between 18 and 36 months of age may reflect an increasing influence of input from the central retina on PL acuity. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that the PL acuity of normal children begins to exceed the resolution limit of the peripheral retina. Note, however, that we have no direct evidence that our measure of OKN acuity depends mainly on input from the central retina, and perhaps even the best OKN acuity that we can measure (6 arc min) is mediated by the peripheral retina. Clinical Usefulness Because our results are based on group data, they cannot be used to address the issue of whether clinicians should use grating acuity to monitor treatment in individual patients. However, they do indicate that obtaining PL acuity within normal limits during the first 1 to 2 years of life is not necessarily indicative of a good prognosis and that obtaining OKN acuity outside normal limits at the same age is to be expected in eyes treated for congenital cataract. Note that monocular tests of OKN acuity using human observers are not likely to be completed successfully in children with spontaneous or latent nystagmus. Perhaps electrophysiological recordings of eye movements, in combination with computer analysis of the eye movement records, would be able to distinguish OKN from other forms of nystagmus in these patients. The results from preferential looking arc relevant for determining the best treatment in patients with congenital cataract. Specifically, in children with bilateral cataract, the earlier the treatment, the better the outcome. For children treated for unilateral congenital cataract, the achievement of optimal acuity during infancy requires aggressive patching of the good eye. We do not yet know the maximum amount of patching that promotes good vision in the treated eye and yet avoids occlusion amblyopia in the good eye. In our sample, the good eye was patched for as long as 8 hours per day throughout early childhood, and virtually all good eyes had PL acuity within normal limits at least until 30 months of age. Moreover, Birch ct al 7 reported normal contrast sensitivity and normal recognition acuity in the good eye of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract if it was patched 8 hours per day from early infancy until at least 5 years of age. (Although we have reported subtle deficits in linear letter acuity and in contrast sensitivity for the good eye of children treated for unilateral cataract, the amount of deficit was unrelated to the number of hours per day the good eye was patched.)'' 2 Interestingly, monocularly deprived cats and aphakic monkeys appear to achieve the best outcome in both eyes if the nondeprived eye is occluded 70% to 75% of the time.*' 1 " Together, these results indicate that patching for as long as 8 waking hours per day promotes development of the deprived eye without damaging the good eye. Even when treatment has been delayed in children with unilateral cataract, our results indicate that a better outcome can be promoted by aggressive patching of the good eye. The results also suggest that even by 12 months of age, patching can offset unbalanced competition between the eyes sufficiently to eliminate differences in grating acuity between children treated for unilateral versus bilateral cataract. Specifically, at 12 months of age, children treated for bilateral cataract had significantly better PL acuity than children treated for unilateral cataract whose good eye was patched less than 3 hours per day. However, differences in PL acuity after binocular versus monocular deprivation disappeared if, after monocular deprivation, the good eye was patched at least 3 hours per day. In conclusion, both OKN acuity and PL acuity revealed effects of abnormal visual experience by the end of the first year of life. Thus, even though the vision of normal infants is "amblyopic" when compared to adult vision, it is influenced by the clarity of visual input and by the balance of input to the two eyes. Key Words congenital cataract, infant vision, optokinetic nystagmus, preferential looking, visual development Acknowledgments The authors arc grateful to the late Dr. J. Donald Morin, former Ophthalmologist-in-Chicf at The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto for his encouragement and support. The authors also thank Michcline Wyldc and Marian Tighe for helping to collect and analyze the data and Dr. Pat Harvey for conducting ophthalmologic examinations of the normal children. Finally, the authors thank the volunteers at The Hospital for Sick Children and at McMastcr University. Earlier versions of some of the data have been published. 1 " References 1. Maurcr D, Lewis TL 1, Brent HP. The effects of deprivation on human visual development: Studies of children treated for cataracts. In: Morrison FJ, Lord C, Keating DP, eds. Applied Developmental Psychology. Vol 3. Psychological Development in Infamy. San Diego: Academic Press; 1989: Costenbader FD, Albert DC Conservatism in the management of congenital cataract. Am Med Assoc Arch Ophlhalmol. 1957; 58: Francois J. Late results of congenital cataract surgery. J Pediatr Ophlhalmol. 1970; 7: Ryan SJ, Maumenee AE. Unilateral congenital cataracts and their management. Ophthalmic Siirg. 1977; 8: o. lieller R, Hoyt CS, Marg li, Odom V. Congenital monocular cataracts: Good visual function with neonatal surgery. Am J Ophthalmol. 1981; 91:

15 209 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 1995, Vol. 36, No Birch EE, Stager DR. Prevalence of good visual acuity following singer)' for congenital unilateral cataract. Arch Ophthalmol. 1988; 106: Birch EE, Swanson WH, Stager D, Woody M, Everett M. Outcome after very early treatment of dense congenital unilateral cataract. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sa. 1993; 3: Drummond GT, Scott WE, Keech RV. Management of monocular congenital cataracts. Arch Ophthalmol. 1989; 107: Gelbart SS, Hoyt CS, Jastrcbski G, Marg E. Long-term visual results in bilateral congenital cataracls. Am ] Ophthalmol. 1982; 93: Lewis TL, Maurcr D, Brent HP. Effects on perceptual development of visual deprivation during infancy. Br J Ophthalmol. 1986;70: Maurer D, Lewis TL. Visual outcomes after infantile cataract. In: Simons K, cd. Early Visual Development, Normal and Abnormal. Committee on Vision, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Research Council. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993: Pratt-Johnson J, Tillson G. Visual results in congenital cataract surgery performed under the age of one year. CanJ Ophthalmol. 1981; 16: Robb RM, Mayer DL, Moore BD. Results of early treatment of unilateral congenital cataracts. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1987; 2: Enoch JM, Rabinowicz IM. Early surgery and visual correction of ah infant born with unilateral eye lens opacity. Doc Ophthalmol. 1976;1: Enoch JM, Rabinowicz IM, Campos EC. Post-surgical contact lens correction of infants with sensory depr ivation amblyopia associated with unilateral congenital cataract, ///m Contact ljms Soc. 1979;21: Catalano RA, Simon JW, Jenkins PL, Kadel GL. Preferential looking as a guide for amblyopia therapy in monocular infantile cataracts. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1987; 2: Jacobson SG, Mohindra I, Held R. Development of visual acuity in infants with congenital cataracts. Br J Ophthalmol. 1981;65: Jacobson SG, Mohindra I, Held R. Visual acuity of infants with ocular diseases. Am J Ophthalmol. 1982; 93: Mohindra I, Jacobson SG, Held R. Binocular vision form deprivation in human infants. Doc Ophthalmol. 1983; 55: Rogers GL, Tischler CL, Tsou BH, Hertle RW, Fellows RR. Visual acuities in infants with congenital cataracts operated on prior to 6 months of age. Arch Ophthalmol. 1981; 99: Birch EE, Stager DR, Wright WW. Grating acuity development after early surgery for congenital unilateral cataract. Arch Ophthalmol. 1986; 10: Mayer DL, Moore B, Robb RM. Assessment of vision and amblyopia by preferential looking tests after early surgery for unilateral congenital cataracts. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1989;26: Lewis TL, Maurer D, Brent HP. The development of visual resolution in infants and toddlers tested inonocularly with optokinetic nystagmus. Clin Vis Sci. 1990; 5: Lewis TL., Maurcr D. Preferential looking as a measure of visual resolution in infants and toddlers: A comparison of psychophysical methods. Child Dev. 1986;57: Maurer D, Lewis TL, Brent HP. Preferential looking and optokinetic nystagmus: Concurrent and predictive validity. ARVO Abstracts. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sa. 1989; 30: Wyngaarden PA, Maurer, D, Lewis TL, Harvey P, Rosenbaum P. The relationship between developmental delay and grating acuity. ARVO Abstracts. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sa. 1991; 32: van Hof-van Duin J, Mohn G. The development of visual acuity in normal fullterm and pretcrm infants. Vision lies. 1986; 26: Taylor MM, Creelman CD. PEST: Efficient estimates on probability functions. / Acoust Soc Am. 1967;1: Atkinson J. Development of optokinetic nystagmus in the human infant and monkey infant: An analogue to development in kittens. In: Freeman RD, ed. Developmental Neurof/iology of Vision. New York: Plenum; 1979: Lewis TL, Maurer D, Brent HP. Optokinetic nystagmus in normal and visually deprived children: Implications for cortical development. Can J Psychol. 1989; 3: Naegele JR, Held R. The postnatal development of monocular optokinetic nystagmus in infants. Vision Res. 1982;22: Naegele JR, Held R. Development of optokinetic nystagmus and effects of abnormal visual experience during infancy. In: Jeannerod M, Hcin A, eds. Spatially Oriented Behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1983; van Hof-van Duin J, Mohn G. Visual field measurements, optokinetic nystagmus and the visual threatening response: Normal and abnormal development. Doc Ophthalmol Proc Ser. 1986;5: Whitemore GA. Prediction limits for a universal normal observation. Am Statistician. 1986;0: Boothe RG, Dobson V, Teller DY. Postnatal development of vision in human and nonhuman primates. Annu Ilex) Neurosci. 1985;8: Reed MJ. The Development oj Oplokinetic Nystagmus and Contrast Sensitivity in Eye Enucleated and Stralnsmic Subjects. Toronto: York University; Thesis. Dissertation Abstracts International: 1993;53(series B): Harwerth RS, Smith EL III, Paul AD, Crawford MLJ, von Noorden GK. Functional effects of bilateral form deprivation in monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sa. 1991; 32: Smith DC, Lorber CR, Stanford LR, Loop MS. Visual acuity following binocular deprivation in the cat. Brain Res. 1980; 183:1-I I. 39. Mitchell DE. The long-term effectiveness of different regimens of occlusion on recovery from early monocular deprivation in kittens'. Philos Trans R Soc Ijmd Biol. 1991; 333: O'Dcll CD, Gammon JA, Fernandes A, Wilson JR,

16 Grating Acuity After Congenital Cataract 2095 Boothe RG. Development of acuity in a primate model of human infantile unilateral aphakia. Invest Ophthalmol VisSa. 1989; 30: Boothe RG, Lambert SR, Bradley DV, Brown RJ, Morns M, Louden T. Visual function following treatment for unilateral infantile cataract: Operant assessments of acuity in a monkey model. ARVO Abstracts. Invest Ophlhaimol Vis Sa. 199;35: Mayer DL, Fulton AB, Hansen RM. Visual acuity of infants and children with retinal degenerations. Ophthalmic Paediatr Genet. 1985;5: Mayer DL. Acuity of amblyopic children for small Held gratings and recognition stimuli. Invest Ophthalrnol Vis Sa. 1986; 27: Katz LM, Levi DM, Bedell HE. Central and peripheral contrast sensitivity in amblyopia with varying field si/.e. Doc Ophthalrnol. 198; 58: Howard IP, Ohmi M. The efficiency of the central and peripheral retina in driving human optokinetic nystagmus. Vision lias. 198;2: Van Die GC, Collewijn H. Control of human optokinetic nystagmus by the central and peripheral retina: Effects of partial visual field masking, scotopic vision and central retinal scoptoma. Brain Res. 1986;383: Van Die GC, Collewijn H. Optokinetic nystagmus in man." Hum h'eurolnol. 1982; 1: Bowering E, Maurer D, Lewis TL, Brent P, Brent HP. Development of the visual field in normal and binocularly deprived children. ARVO Abstracts. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1989; 30: Bowcrjng E, Maurer D, Lewis TL, Brent HP. Detection thresholds in the periphery of normal and visually deprived children. ARVO Abstracts. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Set. 1991; 32: Bowering ER, Maurer D, Lewis TL, Brent HP. Sensitivity in the nasal and temporal hemifields in children treated for cataract. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sa. 1993; 3: Tytla ME, Lewis TL, Maurer D, Brent HP. Peripheral contrast sensitivity in children treated for congenital cataract. ARVO Abstracts. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sa. 1991; 32: Lewis TL, Maurer D, Tytla ME, Bowering ER, Brent HP. Vision in the 'good' eye of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. Ophthalmology. 1992; 99:

Early Treatment of Congenital Unilateral Cataract Minimizes Unequal Competition

Early Treatment of Congenital Unilateral Cataract Minimizes Unequal Competition Early Treatment of Congenital Unilateral Cataract Minimizes Unequal Competition 12 Eileen E. Birch, 12 David Stager? Joel Leffler? and David Weakley 23 PURPOSE. Dense congenital unilateral cataracts may

More information

Vision Care for Connecticut Children

Vision Care for Connecticut Children Vision Care for Connecticut Children EXECUTIVE SUMMARY November 2003 Prepared by: Judith Solomon, JD Mary Alice Lee, PhD Children s Health Council With funding from: Children s Fund of Connecticut, Inc.

More information

Effects on perceptual development of visual deprivation during infancy

Effects on perceptual development of visual deprivation during infancy British Journal of Ophthalmology, 1986, 7, 214-22 Effects on perceptual development of visual deprivation during infancy TERR L LEWS, DAPHNE MAURER, AND HENRY P BRENT From the Hospitalfor Sick Children,

More information

Bilateral Refractive Amblyopia Treatment Study

Bilateral Refractive Amblyopia Treatment Study 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Bilateral Refractive Amblyopia Treatment Study 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 May 24, 2004 Version 1.1 ATS7 Protocol 5-24-04.doc 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38

More information

Criteria For Monocular Acuity Deficit in Infancy and Early Childhood

Criteria For Monocular Acuity Deficit in Infancy and Early Childhood Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, Vol. 29, No. 4, April 1988 Copyright Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Criteria For Monocular Acuity Deficit in Infancy and Early Childhood

More information

Shedding Light on Pediatric Cataracts. Kimberly G. Yen, MD Associate Professor of Ophthalmology Texas Children s Hospital

Shedding Light on Pediatric Cataracts. Kimberly G. Yen, MD Associate Professor of Ophthalmology Texas Children s Hospital Shedding Light on Pediatric Cataracts Kimberly G. Yen, MD Associate Professor of Ophthalmology Texas Children s Hospital A newborn infant presents with bilateral white cataracts. What is the best age to

More information

Clinical Pearls: Infant vision examination Deborah Orel-Bixler, PhD, OD University of California, Berkeley School of Optometry

Clinical Pearls: Infant vision examination Deborah Orel-Bixler, PhD, OD University of California, Berkeley School of Optometry Clinical Pearls: Infant vision examination Deborah Orel-Bixler, PhD, OD University of California, Berkeley School of Optometry Recommended ages for examinations Recommended populations Recommendations

More information

Paediatric Ophthalmology Assessment. Justin Mora 2017

Paediatric Ophthalmology Assessment. Justin Mora 2017 Paediatric Ophthalmology Assessment Justin Mora 2017 History Visual developmental milestones Aware of people in the room, reaching for objects, following toys, alignment should be central and stable

More information

Monocular Acuity Norms for the Teller Acuity Cards Between Ages One Month and Four Years

Monocular Acuity Norms for the Teller Acuity Cards Between Ages One Month and Four Years Monocular Acuity Norms for the Teller Acuity Cards Between Ages One Month and Four Years D. L. Mayer* A. S. Beiser,] A. F. Warner* E. M. Pratt* K. N. Raye* and]. M. Lang\ Purpose. To derive norms for monocular

More information

The Critical Period for Surgical Treatment of Dense Congenital Unilateral Cataract

The Critical Period for Surgical Treatment of Dense Congenital Unilateral Cataract The Critical Period for Surgical Treatment of Dense Congenital Unilateral Cataract Eileen E. Birch*~\ and David R. Stager-fX Purpose. Early treatment of dense congenital unilateral cataract is associated

More information

MiSight 1 day - Live Webinar Q&A

MiSight 1 day - Live Webinar Q&A What age does the child stop needing treatment? Our current published research tracks children up to 15 years of age and the data shows that myopia is still progressing in both MiSight and single vision

More information

FINAL RESULTS OF THE EARLY TREATMENT FOR RETINOPATHY OF PREMATURITY (ETROP) RANDOMIZED TRIAL

FINAL RESULTS OF THE EARLY TREATMENT FOR RETINOPATHY OF PREMATURITY (ETROP) RANDOMIZED TRIAL FINAL RESULTS OF THE EARLY TREATMENT FOR RETINOPATHY OF PREMATURITY (ETROP) RANDOMIZED TRIAL BY William V. Good MD,* on behalf of the Early Treatment for Retinopathy of Prematurity Cooperative Group ABSTRACT

More information

Richman Face Dot Test Paddle

Richman Face Dot Test Paddle Introduction and Purpose Richman Face Dot Test Paddle Developed by Jack Richman, OD, FAAO, FCOVD Professor, Pediatric Optometry/ Binocular Vision Services New England Eye Institute New England College

More information

Pitfalls in testing children's vision by the Sheridan Gardiner single

Pitfalls in testing children's vision by the Sheridan Gardiner single Brit. J. Ophthal. (I 972) 56, I 35 Pitfalls in testing children's vision by the Sheridan Gardiner single optotype method A. F. HILTON AND J. C. STANLEY Southampton Eye Hospital, Hampshire, England During

More information

Contact Lenses for Infants: Indication, Evaluation, and Technique

Contact Lenses for Infants: Indication, Evaluation, and Technique Contact Lenses for Infants: Indication, Evaluation, and Technique Elaine Chen, OD, FAAO, FSLS Southern California College of Optometry Marshall B. Ketchum University Maureen Plaumann, OD, FAAO The Ohio

More information

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: OCULAR PHOTOSCREENING. POLICY NUMBER: CATEGORY: Technology Assessment

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: OCULAR PHOTOSCREENING. POLICY NUMBER: CATEGORY: Technology Assessment MEDICAL POLICY Clinical criteria used to make utilization review decisions are based on credible scientific evidence published in peer reviewed medical literature generally recognized by the medical community.

More information

Interobserver Reliability of the Teller Acuity Card Procedure in Pediatric Patients

Interobserver Reliability of the Teller Acuity Card Procedure in Pediatric Patients Interobserver Reliability of the Teller Acuity Card Procedure in Pediatric Patients Lisa M. Getz,* Velma Dobson,*-\- Beatriz Luna,^ and Clay Mashj Purpose. To compare interobserver agreement for Teller

More information

BINOCULAR DEPTH PERCEPTION IN SMALL-ANGLE

BINOCULAR DEPTH PERCEPTION IN SMALL-ANGLE Brit. J. Ophthal. (1959) 43, 662. BINOCULAR DEPTH PERCEPTION IN SMALL-ANGLE STRABISMUS* BY E. J. NAYLOR AND A. STANWORTH Department of Ophthalmology, University of Manchester MEASUREMENTS of the binocular

More information

IOL Power Calculation for Children

IOL Power Calculation for Children 1 IOL Power Calculation for Children Rupal H. Trivedi, MD MSCR M. Edward Wilson, MD The authors have no financial interest in the subject matter of this presentation. Intraocular lens (IOL) implantation

More information

2. The clinician will know how to manage common pediatric ocular diseases

2. The clinician will know how to manage common pediatric ocular diseases Ida Chung, OD, MSHE, FCOVD, FAAO Western University College of Optometry Associate Professor/Assistant Dean of Learning 309 E. Second Street, Pomona, CA 91766 Office: 909 938 4140 Email: ichung@westernu.edu

More information

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity S. B. Stevenson *, L. K. Cormack Vision Research 40, 2881-2884. (2000) * University of Houston College of Optometry, Houston TX 77204

More information

Retinoscopy. Contributors to this document include:

Retinoscopy. Contributors to this document include: Optometric Extension Program Foundation, Inc. 1921 East Carnegie Avenue, Suite 3-L Santa Ana, CA 92705 949 250-8070 - Phone 949 250-8157 Fax Retinoscopy Contributors to this document include: Paul Harris,

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Gao TY, Guo CX, Babu RJ, et al; the BRAVO Study Team. Effectiveness of a binocular video game vs placebo video game for improving visual functions in older children, teenagers,

More information

Indicators for Prescribing Spectacles in Normal Preschool Children. The author has no financial interest in any optical product or company.

Indicators for Prescribing Spectacles in Normal Preschool Children. The author has no financial interest in any optical product or company. Indicators for Prescribing Spectacles in Normal Preschool Children Sean P. Donahue, M.D., Ph.D. Vanderbilt University Medical Center Nashville, Tennessee Kaiser Symposium June 2008 The author has no financial

More information

Visual Conditions in Infants and Toddlers

Visual Conditions in Infants and Toddlers Visual Conditions and Functional Vision: Early Intervention Issues Visual Conditions in Infants and Toddlers Brief Overview of Childhood Visual Disorders Hatton, D.D. (2003). Brief overview of childhood

More information

Public Health and Eye Care

Public Health and Eye Care Public Health and Eye Care Rohit Varma, MD, MPH Professor and Chair USC Department of Ophthalmology Director, USC Eye Institute Associate Dean, Keck School of Medicine of USC Los Angeles, CA 1 Prevalence

More information

Facilitation of Amblyopia Management by Laser In situ Keratomileusis in Children with Myopic Anisometropia

Facilitation of Amblyopia Management by Laser In situ Keratomileusis in Children with Myopic Anisometropia Facilitation of Amblyopia Management by Laser In situ Keratomileusis in Children with Myopic Anisometropia Athens 2018 Amblyopia Amblyopia is a decrease in visual acuity in one eye due to abnormal visual

More information

Pediatric cataract. Nikos Kozeis MD, PhD, FICO, FEBO, MRCOphth. Surgical challenges and postoperative complications

Pediatric cataract. Nikos Kozeis MD, PhD, FICO, FEBO, MRCOphth. Surgical challenges and postoperative complications Pediatric cataract Surgical challenges and postoperative complications Nikos Kozeis MD, PhD, FICO, FEBO, MRCOphth Consultant Paediatric Ophthalmologist Thessaloniki, Greece Pediatric Cataract 2.4 / 10000

More information

Children's Eye Assessment

Children's Eye Assessment Children's Eye Assessment Dr Antony Bedggood, Children s Specialist Centre Paediatric Ophthalmologist, Cataract & Strabismus Surgeon Why kids need early referral Children s eye problems are often subtle:

More information

10/4/2016. Organic (systemic) Form deprivation (structural) Strabismic Refractive Isometric Anisometric

10/4/2016. Organic (systemic) Form deprivation (structural) Strabismic Refractive Isometric Anisometric Marc B. Taub, OD, MS, FAAO, FCOVD Chief, Vision Therapy and Rehabilitation Southern College of Optometry Editor in Chief, Optometry & Visual Performance A difference in the VA of the two eyes of at least

More information

The Management of Infant Aphakia

The Management of Infant Aphakia The Management of Infant Aphakia Christina Twardowski O.D., FAAO Please silence all mobile devices and remove items from chairs so others can sit. Unauthorized recording of this session is prohibited.

More information

Amblyopia 101: How to use Current Amblyopia Research in Clinical Practice

Amblyopia 101: How to use Current Amblyopia Research in Clinical Practice Amblyopia 101: How to use Current Amblyopia Research in Clinical Practice Valerie M. Kattouf O.D. Chief, Pediatric/Binocular Vision Service FAAO, FCOVD Illinois College of Optometry Associate Professor

More information

Normal and amblyopic contrast sensitivity functions in central and peripheral retinas

Normal and amblyopic contrast sensitivity functions in central and peripheral retinas Normal and amblyopic contrast sensitivity functions in central and peripheral retinas Joseph Thomas Contrast sensitivity functions (CSF's)for temporally modulated sine wave gratings were established at

More information

Retinopathy Of Prematurity (or) Retrolental Fibroplasia )

Retinopathy Of Prematurity (or) Retrolental Fibroplasia ) Medical Information Document On Retinopathy Of Prematurity (or) Retrolental Fibroplasia ) What we see is made in the brain from signals given to it by the eyes. What we see is in fact made in the brain.

More information

Amblyopia Definition 9/25/2017. Strabismic Amblyopia. Amblyopia 101: How to use Current Amblyopia Research in Clinical Practice

Amblyopia Definition 9/25/2017. Strabismic Amblyopia. Amblyopia 101: How to use Current Amblyopia Research in Clinical Practice Amblyopia 101: How to use Current Amblyopia Research in Clinical Practice Valerie M. Kattouf O.D. Chief, Pediatric/Binocular Vision Service FAAO, FCOVD Illinois College of Optometry Associate Professor

More information

Contact Lenses for Infants: Indication, Evaluation, and Technique

Contact Lenses for Infants: Indication, Evaluation, and Technique Contact Lenses for Infants: Indication, Evaluation, and Technique Maureen Plaumann, OD, FAAO The Ohio State University Elaine Chen, OD, FAAO, FSLS Southern California College of Optometry Marshall B. Ketchum

More information

Treating Amblyopia in Aphakic and Pseudophakic Children

Treating Amblyopia in Aphakic and Pseudophakic Children Treating Amblyopia in Aphakic and Pseudophakic Children Scott R. Lambert, M.D. ABSTRACT Introduction Amblyopia is the leading cause of reduced vision in children following cataract surgery. It may develop

More information

Characterizing Parental Adherence To Amblyopia Therapy At Menelik Ii Referral Hospital In Addis Ababa, Ethiopia

Characterizing Parental Adherence To Amblyopia Therapy At Menelik Ii Referral Hospital In Addis Ababa, Ethiopia Yale University EliScholar A Digital Platform for Scholarly Publishing at Yale Yale Medicine Thesis Digital Library School of Medicine January 2016 Characterizing Parental Adherence To Amblyopia Therapy

More information

3/16/2018. Perimetry

3/16/2018. Perimetry Perimetry The normal visual field extends further away from fixation temporally and inferiorly than superiorly and nasally. From the center of the retina this sensitivity decreases towards the periphery,

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

TUMBLING E RESOLUTION PERIMETRY IN GLAUCOMA

TUMBLING E RESOLUTION PERIMETRY IN GLAUCOMA Tumbling E resolution perimetry in glaucoma 179 TUMBLING E RESOLUTION PERIMETRY IN GLAUCOMA FERGAL A. ENNIS 1, ROGER S. ANDERSON 1, WINSTON S. McCLEAN 1 and SIMON J.A. RANKIN 2 1 Vision Science Research

More information

Comparison of Pattern VEPs and Preferential-Looking Behavior in 3-Month-Old Infants

Comparison of Pattern VEPs and Preferential-Looking Behavior in 3-Month-Old Infants Comparison of Pattern VEPs and Preferential-Looking Behavior in 3-Month-Old Infants Samuel Sokol ond Anne Moskowirz Studies of visual acuity in human infants between 1 and 6 months of age using the visual-evoked

More information

Visual acuity in a national sample of 10 year old children

Visual acuity in a national sample of 10 year old children Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 1985, 39, 107-112 Visual acuity in a national sample of 10 year old children SARAH STEWART-BROWN AND NEVILLE BUTLER From the Department of Child Health, University

More information

INFANTS WITH birth weights less

INFANTS WITH birth weights less CLINICAL SCIENCES of Retinopathy of Prematurity Michael X. Repka, MD; Earl A. Palmer, MD; Betty Tung, MS; for the Cryotherapy for Retinopathy of Prematurity Cooperative Group Objective: To report the timing

More information

LEA Color Vision Testing

LEA Color Vision Testing To The Tester Quantitative measurement of color vision is an important diagnostic test used to define the degree of hereditary color vision defects found in screening with pseudoisochromatic tests and

More information

THE OUTCOME OF STRABISMUS SURGERY IN CHILDHOOD EXOTROPIA

THE OUTCOME OF STRABISMUS SURGERY IN CHILDHOOD EXOTROPIA THE OUTCOME OF STRABISMUS SURGERY IN CHILDHOOD EXOTROPIA J. M. KEENAN and H. E. WILLSHAW Birmingham SUMMARY The results of squint surgery in 42 children with primary, non-paralytic, childhood are analysed.

More information

Consent for Cataract Surgery

Consent for Cataract Surgery Consent for Cataract Surgery Introduction This information is given to you so that you can make an informed decision about having an eye operation, before you sign the INFORMED CONSENT DOCUMENT. You have

More information

Clinical decision upon resection or observation of ocular surface dermoid lesions with the visual axis unaffected in pediatric patients

Clinical decision upon resection or observation of ocular surface dermoid lesions with the visual axis unaffected in pediatric patients DOI 10.1186/s40064-015-1326-7 RESEARCH Open Access Clinical decision upon resection or observation of ocular surface dermoid lesions with the visual axis unaffected in pediatric patients Toshihiko Matsuo

More information

Double Vision as a Presenting Symptom in Adults Without Acquired or Long- Standing Strabismus

Double Vision as a Presenting Symptom in Adults Without Acquired or Long- Standing Strabismus Double Vision as a Presenting Symptom in Adults Without Acquired or Long- Standing Strabismus Sara Shippman, C.O. Larisa Heiser, C.O. Kenneth R. Cohen, M.D., F.A.C.S. Lisabeth Hall, M.D. ABSTRACT Background:

More information

KPA PFIZER EDUCATION GRANT

KPA PFIZER EDUCATION GRANT KPA PFIZER EDUCATION GRANT What every Paediatrician needs to know in Paediatric Ophthalmology Dr. Njambi Ombaba Paediatricians knowledge in ophthalmology Outline Visual development in a child Amblyopia

More information

Analysis of eye movements during myopic laser in situ keratomileusis

Analysis of eye movements during myopic laser in situ keratomileusis 15th International SCHWIND User Meeting, Vancouver 2014 Analysis of eye movements during myopic laser in situ keratomileusis Thomas Kohnen Department of Ophthalmology Goethe-University, Frankfurt, Germany

More information

Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia. Hai-Yan He, Baisali Ray, Katie Dennis and Elizabeth M.

Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia. Hai-Yan He, Baisali Ray, Katie Dennis and Elizabeth M. Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia Hai-Yan He, Baisali Ray, Katie Dennis and Elizabeth M. Quinlan a 3. 2.5 2. 1.5.5 Deprived eye Non-deprived VCtx * * b 8.

More information

CASE REPORT Visual acuity and Contrast sensitivity improvement in a case of Congenital Nystagmus using NeuroVision Technology: a Case Report.

CASE REPORT Visual acuity and Contrast sensitivity improvement in a case of Congenital Nystagmus using NeuroVision Technology: a Case Report. CASE REPORT Visual acuity and Contrast sensitivity improvement in a case of Congenital Nystagmus using NeuroVision Technology: a Case Report. Yaniv Ben Hamo, B.Optom., MBA. The LightHouse Israel- Low Vision

More information

IMPORTANCE OF INFANT EYE CARE (OTHER)

IMPORTANCE OF INFANT EYE CARE (OTHER) Dr. Silvia Han graduated from University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) with a Bachelor of Science in Marshall B. Ketchum University. Upon graduation from SCCO, Dr. Han completed a residency in Pediatric

More information

RETINOSCOPY HANDBOOK FOR CLINICIANS

RETINOSCOPY HANDBOOK FOR CLINICIANS RETINOSCOPY HANDBOOK FOR CLINICIANS Author: Sarah Wassnig B.Optom(OcTher), MPH New England College of Optometry created this handbook for the use of Orbis International trainees. This publication is the

More information

EYE EXAMINATION SUMMARY

EYE EXAMINATION SUMMARY Vision Services 1000 Schoolhouse Lane Greeley, CO 80000 Phone: (987) 654-3210 Fax: (987) 654-0123 EYE EXAMINATION SUMMARY Patient s Name Jason Date January 15, 2010 Date of Birth May 12, 2003 Social Security

More information

Title: Fitting Gas Permeable Contact Lenses on Aphakic Infants with Congenital Cataracts: Case Report

Title: Fitting Gas Permeable Contact Lenses on Aphakic Infants with Congenital Cataracts: Case Report Title: Fitting Gas Permeable Contact Lenses on Aphakic Infants with Congenital Cataracts: Case Report I. Case History Patient demographics: 9-month-old Caucasian female Purpose of visit/chief Complaint:

More information

Think Outside the Box. Strabismus & Amblyopia. Prescribing. Amblyopia 5/9/2017. Goals of today s lecture: Kacie Monroe, OD, FCOVD. Peripheral Movement

Think Outside the Box. Strabismus & Amblyopia. Prescribing. Amblyopia 5/9/2017. Goals of today s lecture: Kacie Monroe, OD, FCOVD. Peripheral Movement Think Outside the Box Strabismus & Kacie Monroe, OD, FCOVD Goals of today s lecture: Define success in treatment Determine the best path to get there Spoiler: it may not be what you think Provide specific

More information

Optical Treatment Reduces Amblyopia in Astigmatic Children Who Receive Spectacles Before Kindergarten

Optical Treatment Reduces Amblyopia in Astigmatic Children Who Receive Spectacles Before Kindergarten Optical Treatment Reduces Amblyopia in Astigmatic Children Who Receive Spectacles Before Kindergarten Velma Dobson, PhD, 1,2 Candice E. Clifford-Donaldson, MPH, 1 Tina K. Green, MS, 1 Joseph M. Miller,

More information

The Royal College of Ophthalmologists. Parent Information Squint/Strabismus

The Royal College of Ophthalmologists. Parent Information Squint/Strabismus Parent Information Squint/Strabismus This leaflet is designed to answer some of the general queries relating to squint in childhood. The Children s Eye Care Team An OPHTHALMOLOGIST is a doctor who specialises

More information

Conventional visual acuity tests with vision charts rely

Conventional visual acuity tests with vision charts rely Clinical and Epidemiologic Research Objective Measurement of Distance Visual Acuity Determined by Computerized Optokinetic Nystagmus Test Joon Young Hyon, 1,2 Hwan Eok Yeo, 3 Jong-Mo Seo, 4 In Bum Lee,

More information

Financial Disclosures. Amblyopia: What the Studies Show. Acknowledgements. Development of PEDIG. PEDIG Protocols. Amblyopia Treatment Dogma Pre-ATS

Financial Disclosures. Amblyopia: What the Studies Show. Acknowledgements. Development of PEDIG. PEDIG Protocols. Amblyopia Treatment Dogma Pre-ATS Amblyopia: What the Studies Show Zachary S. McCarty, OD Financial Disclosures Acknowledgements Development of PEDIG PEDIG is a network dedicated to conducting multi-center studies in strabismus, amblyopia,

More information

Amblyopia: is visual loss permanent?

Amblyopia: is visual loss permanent? 952 Ophthalmology and Vision Science, Queen s University, Royal Victoria Hospital, Belfast BT 12 6BA M K El Mallah U Chakravarthy P M Hart Corrrespondence to: Usha Chakravarthy u.chakravarthy@qub.ac.uk

More information

HYPOTHESIS INTRODUCTION. Trans Am Ophthalmol Soc 2006;104:

HYPOTHESIS INTRODUCTION. Trans Am Ophthalmol Soc 2006;104: THE INFLUENCE OF REFRACTIVE ERROR MANAGEMENT ON THE NATURAL HISTORY AND TREATMENT OUTCOME OF ACCOMMODATIVE ESOTROPIA (AN AMERICAN OPHTHALMOLOGICAL SOCIETY THESIS) BY BRADLEY CHARLES BLACK MD ABSTRACT Purpose:

More information

Visual Function Following Congenital Cataract Surgery

Visual Function Following Congenital Cataract Surgery Visual Function Following Congenital Cataract Surgery Misao Yamamoto, Murat Dogru, Makoto Nakamura, Hiroko Shirabe, Yasutomo Tsukahara, Yoshibumi Sekiya Department of Ophthalmology, Kobe University School

More information

Adaptation of the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Amblyopia

Adaptation of the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Amblyopia Adaptation of the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Amblyopia Carol A. Wesfall and Clifton M. Schor Adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) is demonstrated by changes in gain in response to discrepancies

More information

Optical Treatment of Amblyopia in Astigmatic Children

Optical Treatment of Amblyopia in Astigmatic Children Optical Treatment of Amblyopia in Astigmatic Children The Sensitive Period for Successful Treatment Erin M. Harvey, PhD, 1,2 Velma Dobson, PhD, 1,3 Candice E. Clifford-Donaldson, MPH, 1 Joseph M. Miller,

More information

The WORST-CASE SCENARIO Survival Handbook

The WORST-CASE SCENARIO Survival Handbook The WORST-CASE SCENARIO Survival Handbook I certify: I have no proprietary interest in the tested product I have no equity interest or significant payments by the sponsor of a covered study Lecture content

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

Pediatric Ophthalmology. Strabismus

Pediatric Ophthalmology. Strabismus Pediatric Ophthalmology Strabismus Strabismus is an eye alignment condition when both eyes do not look at the same point at the same time. Strabismus most often begins in early childhood. It is sometimes

More information

Case Example BE 6 year old male

Case Example BE 6 year old male Goals for this lecture Understand how to properly diagnose amblyopia Understand how to utilize patching and atropine in therapy Learn about the role of vision therapy Amblyopia: To See or Not To See Discuss

More information

STANDARD AUTOMATED PERIMETRY IS A GENERALLY

STANDARD AUTOMATED PERIMETRY IS A GENERALLY Comparison of Long-term Variability for Standard and Short-wavelength Automated Perimetry in Stable Glaucoma Patients EYTAN Z. BLUMENTHAL, MD, PAMELA A. SAMPLE, PHD, LINDA ZANGWILL, PHD, ALEXANDER C. LEE,

More information

Complication and Visual Outcome after Peadiatric Cataract Surgery with or Without Intra Ocular Lens Implantation

Complication and Visual Outcome after Peadiatric Cataract Surgery with or Without Intra Ocular Lens Implantation Original Article Complication and Visual Outcome after Peadiatric with or Without Intra Ocular Lens Implantation Mazhar-ul-Hasan, Umair A. Qidwai, Aziz-ur-Rehman, Nasir Bhatti, Rashid H. Alvi Pak J Ophthalmol

More information

Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension

Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension Christopher W. Tyler Temporal modulation sensitivity for a 5 flickering field was measured in central and peripheral retina

More information

Binocular Luminance Summation in Humans With Defective Binocular Vision

Binocular Luminance Summation in Humans With Defective Binocular Vision Binocular Luminance Summation in Humans With Defective Binocular Vision Ruxandra Sirereanu Subjects with no functional binocularity (no stereopsis and no peripheral fusion) show much less binocular summation

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J AAPOS. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 April 25.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J AAPOS. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 April 25. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J AAPOS. 2005 December ; 9(6): 542 545. The Effect of Amblyopia Therapy on Ocular Alignment Michael X. Repka, MD a, Jonathan M. Holmes,

More information

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 05 Why do you need bright light to read? Rods and cones Photoreceptors are not evenly distributed across the retina 1 Rods and cones Cones are most dense in

More information

Use of the Arden grating test for screening

Use of the Arden grating test for screening British Journal of Ophthalmology, 19, 64, 591-596 Use of the rden grating test for screening ROBERT G. WETHERHED From Moorfields Eye Hospital, City Road, London EC] V PD SUMMRY One hundred and forty patients

More information

Paediatric cataract pathogenesis and management

Paediatric cataract pathogenesis and management Paediatric cataract pathogenesis and management Dr. Kavitha Kalaivani. N Paediatric ophthalmology Sankara Nethralaya February 28-2017 Incidence... 1 to 13 per 10 000 live births 1 200,000 children blind

More information

Help! My Baby s Eyes Are Crossed (or Something!)

Help! My Baby s Eyes Are Crossed (or Something!) Help! My Baby s Eyes Are Crossed (or Something!) Madhuri Chilakapati, MD Ophthalmology Chief Complaint My baby has a lazy eye The eyes move funny The eyes don t move together The eyes get stuck The eyes

More information

Factors Influencing the Prevalence of Amblyopia in Children with Anisometropia

Factors Influencing the Prevalence of Amblyopia in Children with Anisometropia pissn: 1011-8942 eissn: 2092-9382 Korean J Ophthalmol 2010;24(4):225-229 DOI: 10.3341/kjo.2010.24.4.225 Factors Influencing the Prevalence of Amblyopia in Children with Anisometropia Original Article Chong

More information

Clinical Approach To Refractive Errors. Dr. Faizur Rahman Associate Professor Peshawar Medical College

Clinical Approach To Refractive Errors. Dr. Faizur Rahman Associate Professor Peshawar Medical College Clinical Approach To Refractive Errors Dr. Faizur Rahman Associate Professor Peshawar Medical College Learning objectives By the end of this lecture the students would be able to; Correlate optics with

More information

Lessons learned about cataract surgery in infants from the Infant Aphakia Treatment Study

Lessons learned about cataract surgery in infants from the Infant Aphakia Treatment Study 1 April 4, 2017 Lessons learned about cataract surgery in infants from the Infant Aphakia Treatment Study Summary: The workshop will present the latest findings from the Infant Aphakia Treatment Study

More information

Visual Impairment & Eye Health in Children. Susan Cotter, OD, MS So CA College of Optometry Marshall B Ketchum University Fullerton, CA

Visual Impairment & Eye Health in Children. Susan Cotter, OD, MS So CA College of Optometry Marshall B Ketchum University Fullerton, CA Visual Impairment & Eye Health in Children Susan Cotter, OD, MS So CA College of Optometry Marshall B Ketchum University Fullerton, CA Consequences of Childhood VI Social Emotional Physical Educational

More information

Unilateral amblyopia is a monocular reduction in bestcorrected. Accommodative Performance of Children With Unilateral Amblyopia

Unilateral amblyopia is a monocular reduction in bestcorrected. Accommodative Performance of Children With Unilateral Amblyopia Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology Accommodative Performance of Children With Unilateral Amblyopia Vivian Manh, 1 Angela M. Chen, 2 Kristina Tarczy-Hornoch, 1 Susan A. Cotter,

More information

Vision Science III Handout 15

Vision Science III Handout 15 Vision Science III Handout 15 NYSTAGMUS Nystagmus describes a pattern of eye movements in which the eyes move to and fro, usually with alternating Slow and Fast phases. Nystagmus occurs normally in some

More information

Sensitivity in the Nasal and Temporal Hemifields in Children Treated for Cataract

Sensitivity in the Nasal and Temporal Hemifields in Children Treated for Cataract Sensitivity in the Nasal and Temporal Hemifields in Children Treated for Cataract Elizabeth R. Bowering* Daphne Maurer,-\ Terri L. Lewis,,f and Henry P. Brentf Purpose. To determine if form-deprived aphakes,

More information

Cataract. What is a Cataract?

Cataract. What is a Cataract? Cataract What is a Cataract? We all have a lens in our eye. This is positioned just behind the iris, which is the coloured ring in the eye that gives your eye its colour. The lens s function is to focus

More information

Reena Patel, OD, FAAO

Reena Patel, OD, FAAO Reena Patel, OD, FAAO 5 to 10% of all preschool-aged children o Significant refractive error o Amblyopia Poor vision o Strabismus Misalignment of the eyes Myopia o nearsightedness Hyperopia o farsightedness

More information

Visual Fields Shawn L. Cohen, M.D. Part 2 of 4. Definitions / Tables (Part 2 of 2) Static Perimetry (Humphrey, Octopus)

Visual Fields Shawn L. Cohen, M.D. Part 2 of 4. Definitions / Tables (Part 2 of 2) Static Perimetry (Humphrey, Octopus) Visual Fields Shawn L. Cohen, M.D. Part 2 of 4 Definitions / Tables (Part 2 of 2) Static Perimetry (Humphrey, Octopus) Normal Visual Field: Components: General Information Reliability Indices Raw Data

More information

A mblyopia is the commonest childhood vision disorder

A mblyopia is the commonest childhood vision disorder 1552 EXTENDED REPORT Refractive adaptation in amblyopia: quantification of effect and implications for practice C E Stewart, M J Moseley, A R Fielder, D A Stephens, and the MOTAS cooperative... See end

More information

Cataracts in babies and children

Cataracts in babies and children Manchester Royal Eye Hospital Paediatric Ophthalmology Information for Patients Cataracts in babies and children It can be a difficult and anxious time when you discover that your child has visual difficulties,

More information

VISION CARE BENEFIT LIST 2012

VISION CARE BENEFIT LIST 2012 NON-INSURED HEALTH BENEFITS First Nations and Inuit Health Branch VISION CARE BENEFIT LIST 2012 The Non-Insured Health Benefits (NIHB) Program provides supplementary health benefits, including vision care

More information

Sensitivity to global form in glass patterns after early visual deprivation in humans

Sensitivity to global form in glass patterns after early visual deprivation in humans Vision Research 42 (2002) 939 948 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Sensitivity to global form in glass patterns after early visual deprivation in humans Terri L. Lewis a,b,c, *, Dave Ellemberg b,d, Daphne

More information

Cataract. What is a Cataract?

Cataract. What is a Cataract? Cataract What is a Cataract? We all have a lens in our eye. This is positioned just behind the iris, which is the coloured ring in the eye that gives your eye its colour. The lens function is to focus

More information

Visual Deficits in Amblyopia

Visual Deficits in Amblyopia Human Amblyopia Lazy Eye Relatively common developmental visual disorder (~2%) Reduced visual acuity in an otherwise healthy and properly corrected eye Associated with interruption of normal early visual

More information

PAPER The role of early visual input in the development of contour interpolation: the case of subjective contours

PAPER The role of early visual input in the development of contour interpolation: the case of subjective contours Developmental Science (2016), pp 1 14 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12379 PAPER The role of early visual input in the development of contour interpolation: the case of subjective contours Bat-Sheva Hadad, 1 Daphne

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect, so keep your eyes foveated on your own exam. I tend to think of a point as a minute, so be sure to spend the appropriate amount

More information

Test Bank for Medical Surgical Nursing An Integrated Approach 3rd Edition by White

Test Bank for Medical Surgical Nursing An Integrated Approach 3rd Edition by White Test Bank for Medical Surgical Nursing An Integrated Approach 3rd Edition by White Link full download : http://testbankair.com/download/test-bank-for-medical-surgical-nursing-anintegrated-approach-3rd-edition-by-white/

More information