Predicting the Pursuit and Support of Challenging Life Opportunities

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Predicting the Pursuit and Support of Challenging Life Opportunities"

Transcription

1 708575PSPXXX / Personality and Social Psychology BulletinFeeney et al. research-article2017 Article Predicting the Pursuit and Support of Challenging Life Opportunities Brooke C. Feeney 1, Meredith Van Vleet 1, Brittany K. Jakubiak 1, and Jennifer M. Tomlinson 2 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 2017, Vol. 43(8) by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav DOI: journals.sagepub.com/home/pspb Abstract Deciding to embrace challenging opportunities may present one life context through which individuals may thrive, and these decisions may be influenced by one s significant relationships. Married couples were unobtrusively videotaped as one couple-member was presented with a challenging opportunity and decided whether to accept it. We assessed interpersonal predictors of the decision to accept or forgo the opportunity, predictors of the spouse s support during decision-making, and follow-up thriving outcomes 6 months later. Results indicated that specific support behaviors enacted by the spouse relational catalyst (RC) support provision encouraged decision-makers to accept the challenge and that this decision predicted long-term thriving outcomes for the decision-maker. Results also indicated that the spouse s support behavior was influenced by both chronic and experimentally manipulated motivations for providing support, and these motives provide pathways by which relationship satisfaction and attachment security predict the provision of RC support. Implications and directions for future research are discussed. Keywords challenging life opportunities, social support, thriving, relational catalyst, motivation Received July 13, 2016; revision accepted April 3, 2017 Individuals must often decide whether to embrace or forego challenging life opportunities. These decisions have important implications for the decision-maker, and they are unlikely to occur in a vacuum, independent of external influences. The purpose of this research was to investigate the idea that these decision points (and the subsequent embracing of opportunities) present one life context through which individuals may thrive, and that these choices are influenced by the significant relationships in one s life. Because there is currently a lack of research on interpersonal processes surrounding one s choice to embrace versus forgo challenging opportunities, this investigation addresses two critical questions aimed at understanding these processes: First, we address the question of what predicts the pursuit of challenging opportunities. We make the case that a particular type of social support relational catalyst (RC) support is an important predictor. Second, we address the question of what predicts the responsive support of an individual s embracing of challenging opportunities. We make the case that chronic and experimentally manipulated motivations are key predictors of responsive support provision in this context. The theoretical backdrop for this work is a perspective on thriving through relationships (Feeney & Collins, 2014, 2015a, 2015b), which emphasizes the importance of reconceptualizing social support in terms of the promotion of thriving. This perspective emphasizes that although the social support literature historically has focused on stress buffering (Cohen & Wills, 1985), there is also strong evidence for main effects models of social support, which indicate that supportive relationships are tied to well-being even in the absence of stress/adversity (Lakey & Orehek, 2011). The thriving perspective also emphasizes that the social support literature historically has assessed the presence or absence of negative outcomes associated with acute or chronic stress but has not considered how social relationships can promote (or hinder) positive outcomes. Thus, this perspective raises the question: How do close relationships support individuals not only in their ability to cope with stress/adversity but also in their efforts to learn/grow, explore, achieve goals, cultivate new talents, and find purpose/meaning in life? In other words, how do close relationships facilitate thriving? 1 Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA 2 Colgate University, Hamilton, NY, USA Corresponding Author: Brooke C. Feeney, Carnegie Mellon University, 5000 Forbes Ave., Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA. bfeeney@andrew.cmu.edu

2 1172 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(8) Although thriving has been conceptualized in a variety of ways (see Feeney & Collins, 2015a, for a review), all perspectives agree that it includes flourishing both personally and relationally. The theoretical perspective on thriving through relationships (a) views thriving as including five major components of well-being: eudaimonic well-being (life purpose, growth), hedonic well-being (happiness), psychological well-being (positive self-regard, absence of mental health symptoms), social well-being (deep and meaningful human connections), and physical well-being (absence of illness/disease, health status above expected baselines); (b) has highlighted two major life contexts through which individuals can thrive (coping successfully with life adversities and embracing life opportunities for growth in the absence of adversity); and (c) has specified two corresponding relational support functions that contribute to thriving in each context (Feeney & Collins, 2015a, 2015b). One context in which people may potentially thrive, which is the focus of the current investigation, involves the active pursuit of life opportunities (positive challenges) in times of nonadversity (Feeney & Collins, 2015a). Relationships should be fundamental to the experience of thriving in this life context because they serve an important support function that enables one to embrace positive challenges. This support function is referred to as relational catalyst support (Feeney & Collins, 2015a, 2015b) because support-providers can serve as active catalysts for thriving in this context. A useful metaphor is that, to lift off and successfully accomplish a mission, rockets must have a supportive launchpad that provides services to the vehicle before, during, and after the launch. Similarly, relationships can function as launchpads in the pursuit of opportunities by providing necessary functions that promote thriving. Important components of RC support include nurturing a desire to create or seize opportunities, assisting in perceiving opportunities positively (e.g., as not too difficult to attain), facilitating preparation for engagement in opportunities, and providing the launching function during actual engagement in opportunities (which includes providing a secure base for exploration, Feeney & Thrush, 2010; enabling capitalization on successes, Gable, Gonzaga, & Strachman, 2006; Gable & Reis, 2010; and sensitively responding to setbacks; see Feeney & Collins, 2015a, 2015b, for elaboration). This theoretical perspective also highlights the importance of responsiveness when providing RC support (Feeney & Collins, 2015a). Being responsive involves providing the type and amount of support that is dictated by the situation and the partner s needs (Cohen & Wills, 1985; Cutrona, 1990; Reis, 2012; Reis, Clark, & Holmes, 2004). Responsive support-providers flexibly respond to needs and adjust their behavior in response to the contingencies of the situation (Collins, Guichard, Ford, & Feeney, 2006; Feeney & Collins, 2001). Thus, a responsive RC support-provider encourages the pursuit of opportunities that would likely be attainable and benefit the recipient. This support function is rooted in but extends attachment theory (Bowlby, 1982, 1988) in its focus on the promotion of thriving. In other work, we have referred to the support of a significant other s exploration behavior (e.g., desires to learn, grow, play, discover, accomplish goals) as the provision of a secure base (e.g., Feeney, 2004, 2007; Jakubiak & Feeney, 2016). This is based on attachment theory s notion of a secure base, which functions to support behaviors that involve going out from a relationship for autonomous exploration in the environment (Bowlby, 1988). However, when viewing thriving as the ultimate outcome of receiving social support (instead of just providing a base for exploration, which emphasizes more of a passive, waiting role), the term secure base does not fully capture a support function that promotes thriving in the absence of adversity. Thus, Feeney and Collins (2015a) have expanded attachment theory s notion of a secure base to include the additional components necessary for supporting thriving. The promotion of thriving through life opportunities is the core purpose of this broader support function. The thriving perspective posits that people must fully embrace life and its opportunities to achieve optimal health, happiness, and well-being (to thrive) and that close relationships are integral in this process (Feeney & Collins, 2015a, 2015b). In support of this idea, initial work has shown that RC support for goal-strivings predicts both short- and longterm outcomes indicative of thriving (Tomlinson, Feeney, & Van Vleet, 2015). However, we currently lack any studies that examine people s in-the-moment decisions to pursue or forego opportunities and the long-term effects of these decisions. In the current research, we hypothesized that a specific social support function RC support would predict the pursuit of a challenging opportunity. In addition, we hypothesized that the decision to pursue a specific challenging opportunity (likely representative of typical choices one makes regarding opportunities) would predict important personal and relational outcomes indicative of thriving over time, including growth/learning (eudaimonic well-being), happiness (hedonic well-being), self-perceptions (psychological well-being), and relationship quality (relational wellbeing). That is, the specific, in-the-moment choice participants make regarding accepting/embracing an opportunity presented in the lab (likely reflecting decisions made in one s day-to-day life given stable patterns of RC support in longmarried couples) should predict the extent to which participants are thriving over a 6-month assessment period. These hypotheses were tested using both observational and longitudinal methods. In testing whether RC support would predict the pursuit of a challenging opportunity, we assessed four indicators of pursuit. One important indicator is the choice one makes to embrace the opportunity or not. We hypothesized that individuals who embrace the opportunity would receive greater RC support from their spouse than those who do not. A second indicator involves the decision-maker s reasons for

3 Feeney et al pursuing the challenge or not. Because RC support should theoretically release one from constraints that may hinder these pursuits, a natural immediate consequence should be an increase in approach versus avoidance motivation toward the opportunity (Elliot, 2008; Feeney & Collins, 2015a). Approach motivation enables one to focus on the potential rewards to be gained by the opportunity instead of focusing on avoiding potentially negative outcomes (e.g., failure or embarrassment). Thus, we hypothesized that receiving greater RC support from one s spouse would predict greater approach and intrinsic (internally driven; Deci & Ryan, 2000) motivation for pursuing the opportunity including accepting the opportunity because they want to be challenged and earn the reward associated with the opportunity, they might enjoy it, and they would disappoint themselves by not trying. In contrast, we predicted that individuals who lack RC support would exhibit greater avoidance motivation and less intrinsic motivation regarding the opportunity including not embracing the opportunity because it would be easier to avoid it, they would not enjoy it, and they do not wish to be challenged or earn the reward associated with the challenge. A third indicator of pursuit involves the decision-maker s self-perceptions of capability (self-appraisals) and perceptions that the spouse views one as capable (reflected appraisals) because feelings of capability should underlie one s willingness to pursue a challenging opportunity. We hypothesized that RC support would predict feelings of capability, and a lack of such support should predict reduced feelings of capability. A final indicator of pursuit involves the decisionmaker s perceptions that support is available. High perceived support availability indicates appraisals of resources as outweighing the demands of the situation. We hypothesized that the provision of RC support (rated by independent observers) would predict decision-maker s perceptions that the spouse encouraged acceptance of the challenge, was emotionally and instrumentally supportive, and that one was able to seek support from the spouse. A lack of RC support was expected to predict decision-maker reports that the spouse was not encouraging or emotionally supportive, and that the spouse was negative, critical, and unsupportive regarding the opportunity. In addition, the theoretical perspective on thriving through relationships (Feeney & Collins, 2015a) emphasizes three prerequisites for being a responsive support-provider. These include having requisite skills (e.g., knowledge of how to support others), resources (e.g., constitutional and situational resources), and motivation (being positively motivated to use one s skills and resources in the service of another; see also Feeney, Collins, Van Vleet, & Tomlinson, 2013). We focused specifically on motivations in this investigation because responsive support provision is unlikely to occur if supportproviders are not appropriately motivated. We hypothesized that general altruistic versus egoistic motivations for supporting a partner s goals would be especially important in predicting the provision of RC support. Specifically, we expected altruistic motives (e.g., helping for enjoyment or to benefit the spouse) to predict greater RC support provision and egoistic motives (e.g., helping to avoid negative consequences, gain rewards, or meet an obligation) to predict less RC support provision. These predictions are consistent with research showing that individuals differ in their motivation to provide support, that support-providers who are motivated by altruistic concerns are more effective than those who are motivated by egoistic concerns (Feeney & Collins, 2001, 2003; Feeney et al., 2013), and that altruistic motives predict increased relatedness, empathy, and more other-oriented goals, whereas egoistic motives predict decreased relatedness, empathy, and more self-oriented goals (Park, Troisi, & Maner, 2011). We also hypothesized that relationship satisfaction and attachment security would predict these motives. People who are happier with their relationships should be more positively motivated to function as an RC for their significant others. In addition, secure individuals may be altruistically motivated to provide RC support because they do not need to focus on having their own attachment needs met (e.g., Feeney & Collins, 2015a). Previous research indeed demonstrates that securely attached individuals have more altruistic motives for helping others than less secure individuals, who report egoistic motives for providing support (Feeney & Collins, 2003; Feeney et al., 2013). Because both types of insecurity (anxiety and avoidance) have been associated with egoistic motives for providing support and with unresponsive support provision (e.g., Feeney & Collins, 2001, 2003; Feeney & Thrush, 2010), we consider an overall index of attachment security versus insecurity in this investigation. We further predicted that these general motives would provide pathways through which partners relationship satisfaction and attachment security predict RC support. Specifically, we expected that less satisfied and less secure individuals might provide less RC support because they are more egoistically (vs. altruistically) motivated. Finally, because less satisfied and less securely attached individuals may need a motivational push to support a partner s opportunity, a final component of this investigation involved an assessment of whether we can experimentally manipulate motives to make these individuals more likely to provide RC support. We predicted that if we experimentally manipulate potential outcomes of engaging in life opportunities to be more rewarding for the support-provider, then less satisfied and less secure people may be more likely to support a partner s challenging opportunity. We tested this hypothesis by experimentally manipulating whether the challenging opportunity would be potentially rewarding for just the person given the opportunity (the decision-maker) or for the partner/support-provider as well (a possible joint benefit).

4 1174 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(8) Method Participants Participants were 163 married, heterosexual couples who were part of a larger investigation of marital relationships. Couples had been married for an average of 9.68 years, known each other for an average of years, and romantically involved for an average of 12 years. All lived with their spouses, and 65.6% had children together. One couple participated at a time. Before arriving for the study, couplemembers were randomly assigned to the roles of decisionmaker and partner. Thus, the couple (not individual) is the unit of analysis. Decision-makers (78 females, 85 males) were, on average, years old, and the majority were White (75.5%) or Black (15.3%); most completed a bachelor s degree or higher (44.8%) or some college credits or an associate s degree (34.3%). Partners were, on average, years old, and the majority were White (77.9%) or Black (16.6%); most completed a bachelor s degree or higher (45.3%) or some college credits or an associate s degree (36.2%). Although this was part of a larger investigation for which sample size was already determined, we calculated power for prototypical analyses to be conducted for this investigation using G*Power 3.1 (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009): For analyses comparing two groups (choice differences in predicted outcomes), we had power of.95 to detect a large effect size (Cohen s d of 0.80) with only 35 participants per group (total sample size of 70). To test an interaction using hierarchical multiple regression with three predictor variables (two main effects and an interaction), we had power of.95 to detect an effect size of.15 with a total sample size of 119 (at p =.05). Thus, a sample size of 163 couples was more than sufficient to test the hypotheses proposed. Procedure Background characteristics. Each couple visited the laboratory to complete background questionnaires which included demographics (e.g., age, relationship length), partners general motivations for supporting the decision-maker s opportunities, relationship satisfaction, and attachment orientation. Specifically, partners completed a measure of their typical motivations for supporting their spouses goals/opportunities (the Motivations for Providing a Secure Base scale; Feeney et al., 2014). Partners were presented with the phrase, On occasions when I encourage or support my spouse s goals, I generally do so because..., and responded to 32 motivation items on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Consistent with prior work (Feeney et al., 2014), a principal components analysis indicated that the items loaded on 11 factors representing the following motives: (a) Avoid Negative Consequences (four items; α =.81, for example, I want to avoid negative consequences from my spouse (e.g., my spouse would get angry or withdraw from me ), (b) Keep Spouse (three items; α =.74, for example, My spouse will be more likely to remain in the relationship if I encourage and support him or her ), (c) Avoid Own Goals (four items; α =.81, for example, When I support my spouse, I don t have to think about my own goals ), (d) Gain Rewards (four items; α =.79, for example, I expect something in return for my encouragement and support later ), (e) Needy Spouse (four items; α =.67, for example, My spouse really needs my encouragement; he or she would be reluctant to do anything otherwise ), (f) Feel Obligated (two items; α =.70, for example, I feel obligated to encourage and support my spouse s goals; it s expected of me ), (g) Enjoy Helping (two items; α =.84, for example, I truly enjoy helping my spouse ), (h) Love Spouse (two items; α =.75, for example, I love my spouse ), (i) Connect With Spouse (three items; α =.79, for example, It helps me to stay connected to my spouse ), (j) Make Spouse Feel Good (two items; α =.55, for example, I want my spouse to feel good about himself or herself ), and (k) Makes Me Feel Good (two items; α =.73, for example, It makes me feel good about myself when I help my spouse ). To further consolidate the motivation variables for use in data analysis, we conducted another principal components analysis on the 11 composite variables. Results indicated that five of the composite variables loaded on one factor representing relatively egoistic motives (i.e., avoid negative consequences, avoid own goals, keep spouse, feel obligated, and gain rewards), and five loaded on a second factor representing relatively altruistic motives (i.e., love spouse, enjoy helping, connect with spouse, make spouse feel good, makes me feel good). Thus, we computed two composite indexes of relatively egoistic motives and relatively altruistic motives by averaging the respective motive scales. One motive, needy spouse, did not conceptually fit on either factor and was retained as a separate scale for use in data analysis. Partners also completed an abbreviated 26-item version of Brennan, Clark, and Shaver s (1998) Experiences in Close Relationships scale, a well-validated measure for assessing adult attachment. The Avoidance subscale measures the extent to which one is comfortable with closeness/intimacy and the degree to which one feels that people can be relied on to be available when needed (α =.89). The Anxiety subscale measures the extent to which one is worried about being rejected, abandoned, or unloved (α =.91). Partners responded to each item on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) in terms of their general orientation toward close relationships. For purposes of this investigation, the avoidance and anxiety dimensions were reverse-scored and averaged into a composite variable representing attachment security (with higher scores reflecting greater attachment security). Partners relationship satisfaction (α =.95) was measured using the four items employed by Van Lange et al. (1997) and two additional items from Collins and Read (1990).

5 Feeney et al These items assessed the degree to which partners felt happy/ satisfied with their relationship (e.g., All things considered, how satisfied do you feel with your relationship? ). Partners responded using 9-point Likert-type scales with appropriate anchors. Challenging opportunity. Approximately 1 week later, couples returned to the laboratory to participate in an observational session. Couple-members were informed that they would participate in a series of activities some joint activities that they would complete together, and an individual activity. The individual activity provided a challenging opportunity for the decision-maker and is the focus of this investigation. First, the experimenter instructed the partner as follows: We d like you to participate in an activity that involves the completion of some puzzles. These are paper-and-pencil puzzles like mazes, finding hidden pictures, and so on. They re fun and easy you probably did similar puzzles when you were a child. These puzzles were described such that it was clear to the participant that they would require very little thought or effort. Then, the decision-maker was given a challenging opportunity to consider. A meaningful opportunity in a laboratory situation is one in which (a) an individual is given a choice to accept or decline it (with no pressure to accept or decline from the experimenter), (b) the opportunity involves some degree of challenge/effort such that the choice to accept/ decline is a meaningful one, and (c) there is the potential that embracing the opportunity will be rewarding. Thus, we define an opportunity as something that is available and possible to embrace, and potentially beneficial, although the final outcome is uncertain. In the laboratory context, an opportunity to embrace a challenging activity for which one may or may not win a prize is analogous to a real-life context in which one may or may not get a promotion by pursuing a work opportunity, or one may or may not reap the reward of developing a close relationship by pursuing a social opportunity. Thus, the decision-maker was presented with an opportunity that meets the specified criteria. Specifically, the experimenter said, We re going to give you a choice between two activities. You can pick either one it doesn t matter to us which one you choose. You can either choose to work on some puzzles that are similar to the ones your spouse will be working on, or you can choose to compete in a public speaking activity for a chance to win a prize. The experimenter told the decision-maker that this opportunity was being offered to one member of each couple in the study, that this activity involved giving a speech that would be videotaped and rated by a group of judges, and that he or she would be competing with other couple-members in the study (who select this activity) to win one of three prizes. The decision-maker was shown a list of prizes worth up to US$200 in value. This method is in contrast to laboratory stress tasks such as the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST; Kirschbaum, Pirke, & Hellhammer, 1993) aimed at inducing stress/anxiety and performance pressure by not giving participants a choice, by critically evaluating the participants performance as they are completing the activity, by implying that performance reflects one s intelligence/abilities, and by the absence of a potential reward for engaging in the activity. Social evaluation and uncontrollability have been identified as the key components of stress induction by the TSST (Gruenewald, Kemeny, Aziz, & Fahey, 2004), and both of these elements were missing from our laboratory procedure that presents an opportunity. Our goal was not to create stress/anxiety or performance pressure but instead to mimic real-life opportunities that are challenging and require effort, yet are voluntary and potentially rewarding if embraced. Experimental manipulation. To test hypotheses regarding the influence of partner motivations on support behavior, we experimentally manipulated whether the challenging opportunity was potentially rewarding for only the decision-maker or for the partner as well. We did this by showing the decision-maker, in the presence of the partner, one of two types of prize lists (which manipulated the potential benefit of the prize to the partner). In the individual prize condition, there was no potential benefit of the prizes for the partner. This list contained prizes that would be of interest only to the person given the challenging opportunity (e.g., gift certificates to women s clothing stores and bath shops if the decision-maker was female, and gift certificates to men s clothing stores and hardware stores if the decision-maker was male; all were heterosexual couples). In the joint prize condition, there was a potential benefit to the partner as the prizes could be of interest to both couple-members (e.g., gift certificates to movie theaters, restaurants, general interest stores, and cash prizes). Assessments of opportunity-pursuit. The experimenter then left the room and unobtrusively videotaped the couple for 5 min as the decision-maker considered which activity to select. After 5 min, couple-members were escorted to separate rooms (for their individual activities) where they completed preactivity questionnaires. At this time, the decision-maker recorded his or her choice of activity and answered questions regarding his or her reasons for choosing the selected activity, perceptions of capability, and perceptions of the spouse s support. Specifically, decision-makers rated the extent to which they chose their selected activity for a variety of reasons using a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = I definitely did not pick the activity for this reason, 7 = I definitely did pick the activity for this reason ). Based on the results of a principal components analysis, we computed five composite subscales for use in data analysis representing reasons for selecting one s activity, including Want to Win a Prize (three

6 1176 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(8) items; α =.93, for example, I want to win a prize ), It s Easier (four items; α =.89, for example, It is easier ), Want to Be Challenged (two items; α =.72, for example, It s more challenging ), Will Enjoy It (three items; α =.80, for example, I enjoy doing activities like the one I picked ), and Don t Want to Disappoint Self or Partner (four items; α =.87, for example, I would feel disappointed with myself if I didn t do this one, My spouse would feel disappointed with me if I didn t do this one ). In addition, decision-makers rated their perceptions of their own capability for their selected activity (two items; α =.87, for example, How capable are you of performing this activity well? ) and their perceptions that their spouse views them as capable (two items; α =.88, for example, How capable does your spouse think you are of performing this activity well? ) on 7-point scales with appropriate anchors. Finally, decision-makers reported (on 5-point scales with appropriate anchors) their perceptions of the extent to which their spouses encouraged them to take the challenge ( To what extent did your spouse encourage you to pick the speech activity? ), encouraged them to choose the easier puzzle ( To what extent did your spouse encourage you to pick the puzzle activity? ), provided assistance ( To what extent did your spouse give you assistance or advice about how to perform either activity? ), provided emotional support ( To what extent did your spouse communicate emotional support? By emotional support, we mean affection, compassion, understanding, reassurance, compliments, etc. ), was negative/critical/unsupportive ( Overall, how negative, critical, or unsupportive was your spouse during the time you were together before deciding on an activity? ), and the extent to which the decision-maker sought support from the spouse during the waiting period (three items; α =.72, for example, To what extent did you ask your spouse to help you decide which activity to pick? ). After completing these questionnaires, decision-makers who selected the speech were informed that they would not actually give the speech, and all participants completed puzzles. The names of decision-makers who selected the challenge were entered into a random drawing for the prizes. Participants were fully debriefed and formally asked for permission to use their unobtrusive recordings for research purposes. All procedures were approved by the Carnegie Mellon University Institutional Review Board. Observations of RC support. Independent observers who were blind to study hypotheses and trained to reliability watched each video of the 5-min (unobtrusively recorded) decision period and rated the extent to which the partner exhibited behaviors indicative of responsive RC support. These behaviors were selected to be representative of the theoretical components of RC support, which include nurturing a desire to create or seize opportunities, assisting in perceiving opportunities positively (e.g., as not too difficult to attain), and facilitating preparation for engagement in life opportunities. All behaviors were rated on scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (a great deal) in terms of both the frequency and intensity of occurrence. At least two observers (ratings were averaged, intraclass correlation coefficients [ICCs] reported below) coded each behavior: (a) encouragement of the opportunity (expresses enthusiasm about the opportunity, ICC =.86), (b) confidence in the decision-maker (communicates confidence in the decisionmaker s ability to perform the challenge, ICC =.89), (c) promotion of challenge (suggests that the decision-maker should challenge himself or herself by choosing the speech, ICC =.72), (d) emotional support (validates and reassures the decision-maker, ICC =.88), (e) availability (shows acceptance of requests for support and provides requested support, ICC =.87), (f) persuasion (tries to motivate the decision-maker to try the challenging activity, ICC =.90), and (g) prize (discusses the potential benefits of the prize to the decision-maker, ICC =.89). A composite variable representing RC support was computed by averaging the ratings for each of these behaviors (α =.84). Theoretically, a partner who is functioning as an RC would be expected to exhibit all of these behaviors. Independent observers also coded (on the same 5-point rating scales) a series of behaviors that represent the opposite of an RC. These include (a) discouragement (communicates that the opportunity is not valued, ICC =.88), (b) lack of confidence in the decision-maker (communicates a lack of confidence in decision-maker s ability, ICC =.83), (c) controlling behaviors (tries to control the decision; demands which activity to choose and how to approach it, ICC =.90), (d) minimizing behavior (downplays importance of the decision and/or opportunity, ICC =.78), (e) maximizing behavior (catastrophizes obstacles/concerns related to the opportunity; imagines worst possible outcome, ICC =.54), (f) expressions of negative affect toward the decision-maker (e.g., hostility, ICC =.84), (g) avoidance (avoids discussing the opportunity with the decision-maker, ICC =.93), (h) selffocus (focuses the conversation on one s own desires, needs, or concerns, ICC =.84), and (i) belittling the opportunity (puts down or makes fun of the activity, ICC =.46). A composite variable representing anti-rc support was computed by averaging the ratings for each of these behaviors. The purpose of creating this composite was to assess the extent to which any anti-rc behavior occurred (with higher scores indicating more intense and frequent occurrences). Because some of these unsupportive behaviors are opposites (e.g., minimizing and maximizing; controlling and avoidance behaviors), we did not compute a composite reliability index, as partners who exhibited some of these behaviors would be unlikely to exhibit others. Follow-up assessment. Six months later, couple-members were contacted to complete follow-up assessments of outcomes

7 Feeney et al relevant to thriving. Specifically, the decision-maker reported his or her growth/learning over the last 6 months as an index of eudaimonic well-being (two items; α =.77, for example, Over the last 6 months, to what degree do you feel that you have learned new things and developed new competencies? ), as well as his or her happiness (two items; α =.70, asking how happy and how excited the decision-maker felt during the past 6 months) as an index of hedonic well-being. To assess the decision-maker s positive self-regard (a component of psychological well-being), he or she completed assessments of perceived self-efficacy (a 15-item version of the Self-Efficacy Scale, Sherer et al., 1982; α =.93, for example, When I make plans, I am certain I can make them work ) and perceptions of being smart/talented (five items; α =.82). The decision-maker also reported his or her relationship satisfaction (six items; α =.93, for example, All things considered, how satisfied do you feel with your relationship? ) and communal relationship strength (Mills, Clark, Ford, & Johnson, 2004; 10 items; α =.89, for example, How far would you be willing to go to help your spouse? ) as indicators of relationship thriving. Results Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations among all study variables are shown in Table 1. We first tested hypotheses regarding RC support predicting the pursuit of a challenging opportunity. Second, we tested whether the pursuit of the challenging opportunity predicted thriving outcomes. Finally, we tested hypotheses regarding predictors of RC support. Does RC Support Predict the Pursuit of a Challenging Opportunity? Choice of activity. We examined whether the partner s RC support provision predicted the decision-maker s choice of activity (a dichotomous variable) using both t tests and logistic regression analyses. We first compared the amount of RC support and anti-rc support that decision-makers received during the waiting period between those who accepted the opportunity and those who declined it. As shown in Table 2, decision-makers who accepted the opportunity had partners who provided more RC support than those who declined it. Similarly, decision-makers who declined the opportunity had partners who provided more anti-rc support than those who accepted it. A logistic regression analysis predicting activity choice from both RC and anti-rc behavior simultaneously confirmed that spouses RC and anti-rc behavior predicted the decisionmaker s choice of activity. Decision-makers who received more RC support were more likely to choose the opportunity (B = 1.20, SE = 0.40, Wald = 9.01, p <.01, 95% confidence interval [CI] = [1.52, 7.23]), whereas those who received more anti-rc support were less likely to accept it (B = 0.65, SE = 0.64, Wald = 6.64, p =.01, 95% CI = [0.054, 0.673]). Reasons for choosing activity. Next, we conducted simultaneous regression analyses to determine the extent to which RC behavior and anti-rc behavior predicted the decision-maker s reasons for choosing the selected activity. RC support and anti-rc support were entered simultaneously as predictors of each of the reasons for selecting one s chosen activity. As shown in Table 3, results indicated that individuals who received more RC support during the waiting period (as coded by independent observers) were more likely to report selecting their activity because they wanted to win a prize and because they did not want to disappoint themselves or their partners. In contrast, individuals who received more anti-rc behaviors during the waiting period were more likely to report choosing their activity because it is easier, and not because they wanted to win a prize, be challenged, thought they would enjoy it, or were concerned about disappointing themselves or their partners. These reasons were related to the activity decision-makers chose. As shown in Table 2, compared with decisionmakers who declined the opportunity, decision-makers who chose the opportunity were significantly more likely to report that they wanted to win a prize, wanted to be challenged, and did not want to disappoint themselves or their partners. They were also significantly less likely to report that they selected their activity because it is easier. There were no significant choice differences in expectations for enjoying the activity. Perceived capability. We conducted simultaneous regression analyses to determine the extent to which RC behavior and anti-rc behavior predicted the decision-maker s perceptions that the partner viewed them as capable of succeeding at the challenge, as well as their own perceived capability. As shown in Table 3, results indicated that RC behavior was positively associated with the decision-maker s perceptions that the partner viewed them as capable, and anti-rc behavior was negatively associated with these perceptions. RC behavior and anti-rc behavior were not associated with the decisionmaker s own perceived capability. Additional analyses examining choice differences in perceptions of capability revealed that decision-makers who chose the opportunity perceived that their partners viewed them as more capable than those who declined it (see Table 2). There were no significant choice differences for one s own perceptions of capability. Perceptions of support. Simultaneous regression analyses were conducted to examine the degree to which RC support and anti-rc support predicted decision-makers perceptions of partner support during the waiting period. As shown in Table 4, RC support predicted decision-makers perceptions that their spouse encouraged them to embrace the

8 Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and Zero-Order Correlations for Major Study Variables. Study Variables M (SD) Spouse background 1. Altruistic motives 5.10 (0.61) 2. Egoistic motives 2.73 (0.84).19** 3. Needy motives 3.49 (1.15).08.40*** 4. Relationship satisfaction 6.39 (1.51).41***.32***.36*** 5. Attachment security 4.84 (1.02).04.41***.26***.45*** Observed support 6. RC support 2.03 (0.65).25*** Anti-RC support 1.46 (0.32) *** Opportunity decision 8. Choice 1.73 (0.45) ***.31*** DM reasons for choice 9. To win prize 4.43 (2.26) **.28***.73*** 10. To not disappoint 2.48 (1.85) ***.22**.47***.57*** 11. It is easier 3.40 (1.93) **.71***.59***.28*** 12. To enjoy it 4.22 (1.78) *** 13. To challenge oneself 3.67 (1.86) *.50***.37***.36***.33***.31*** DM perceived capability 14. Capable (self view) 4.81 (1.30) *** Capable (partner view) 5.62 (1.16) **.22**.17* ***.09.60*** DM support perceptions 16. Encouraged opportunity 3.14 (1.58) ***.09.27***.24**.52***.18*.02.16*.04.19* 17. Encouraged easy activity 1.46 (0.97) * Gave assistance 3.11 (1.43).19* ***.12.25***.20*.13.17* *** Gave emotional support 3.80 (1.19) ***.28***.22**.16* ***.27***.00.41*** 20. Negative/critical 1.33 (0.73) *.13.22** * *.20** *** 21. DM sought support 2.73 (1.08) ***.14.29***.26***.25***.21** ***.07.58***.36***.06 Long-term thriving outcomes 22. Growth 3.38 (0.87) * * *.01.22* Happiness 3.64 (0.96) *** 24. Self-efficacy 5.31 (0.94) * * * ***.33*** 25. Feel smart/talented 5.86 (1.00) * **.22*.00.23** ***.46***.66*** 26. CRS 6.46 (1.50) **.22* *.03.26** **.30*** 27. Relationship satisfaction 8.63 (1.40).21* ***.22* *** **.37***.38***.47*** Note. Choice was coded as 1 (did not accept opportunity) or 2 (did accept opportunity). RC = relational catalyst; DM = decision-maker; CRS = communal relationship strength. p <.10. *p <.05. **p <.01. ***p <

9 Feeney et al Table 2. Differences Between DMs Who Accepted and Did Not Accept the Opportunity. DMs who accepted DMs who declined Difference Support and Pursuit Variables M (SD) M (SD) t Cohen s d 95% CI of the difference Observed support from spouse RC support 2.14 (0.66) 1.65 (0.47) t(148) = 4.14*** 0.86 [ 0.73, 0.26] Anti-RC support 1.41 (0.30) 1.65 (0.33) t(161) = 3.98*** 0.76 [0.12, 0.35] Reason for choice To win a prize 5.41 (1.62) 1.60 (1.20) t(156) = 13.87*** 2.67 [ 4.35, 3.27] To not disappoint self or partner 3.93 (1.85) 1.87 (1.20) t(157) = 6.71*** 1.32 [ 2.66, 1.45] It is easier 1.69 (1.12) 4.67 (1.73) t(158) = 12.79*** 2.04 [2.52, 3.45] To enjoy it 4.09 (1.82) 4.47 (1.67) t(158) = 1.18 To challenge oneself 4.21 (1.71) 2.10 (1.34) t(157) = 7.23*** 1.37 [ 2.69, 1.53] Perceived capability Spouse views DM as capable 5.73 (1.10) 5.28 (1.25) t(161) = 2.22* 0.38 [ 0.85, 0.05] DM views self as capable 4.72 (1.32) 5.02 (1.22) t(161) = 1.34 DM support perceptions and behavior Spouse encouraged DM to embrace 3.39 (1.54) 2.43 (1.56) t(161) = 3.54*** 0.62 [ 1.50, 0.43] opportunity Spouse encouraged DM to choose 1.36 (0.84) 1.73 (1.26) t(161) = 2.13* 0.35 [0.03, 0.71] the easier activity Spouse provided assistance or advice 3.34 (1.39) 2.55 (1.41) t(161) = 3.24*** 0.56 [ 1.29, 0.31] Spouse provided emotional support 3.94 (1.13) 3.34 (1.28) t(160) = 2.91** 0.50 [ 1.01, 0.19] Spouse was negative/critical or 1.27 (0.67) 1.45 (0.85) t(161) = 1.45 unsupportive DM sought support from spouse 2.93 (1.06) 2.23 (1.04) t(161) = 3.77*** 0.67 [ 1.07, 0.33] Note. DM = decision-maker; CI = confidence interval; RC = relational catalyst. p <.10. *p <.05. **p <.01. ***p <.001. Table 3. RC Support and Anti-RC Support Predicting Reasons for Choosing Activity and Perceived Capability. Reasons for choosing activity Perceived capability Observed Support To win a prize To not disappoint self or partner It is easier To enjoy it To challenge oneself Spouse views DM as capable DM views self as capable B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) RC support 0.68* (0.28) [0.13, 1.23] 1.00*** (0.23) [0.54, 1.46] 0.16 (0.22) [ 0.60,.27] 0.32 (0.23) [ 0.77, 0.13] 0.09 (0.24) [ 0.38, 0.56] 0.34* (0.14) [0.07, 0.62] 0.09 (0.17) [ 0.42, 0.24] Anti-RC support 1.69** (0.57) [ 2.81, 0.56] 0.85 (0.48) [ 1.79, 0.09] 1.07* (0.45) [0.18, 1.96] 1.01* (0.47) [ 1.93, 0.09] 1.01* (0.49) [ 1.97, 0.05] 0.65* (0.29) [ 1.22, 0.08] 0.34 (0.34) [ 1.02, 0.34] R 2.12***.16***.05* **.01 Note. Unstandardized regression coefficients; 95% confidence intervals for all effects are shown in brackets. RC = relational catalyst; DM = decisionmaker. p <.10. *p <.05. **p <.01. ***p <.001. opportunity, gave assistance, gave emotional support, and that the decision-maker sought support from the spouse. Anti- RC support, in contrast, predicted decision-makers perceptions that the spouse did not give emotional support and that the spouse was negative and unsupportive. Neither RC nor anti-rc behavior predicted decision-makers perceptions that the spouse encouraged them to choose the easier activity. Additional analyses revealed that these perceptions were related to decision-makers choice of activity (see Table 2). Decision-makers who chose the challenge (relative to those who declined) reported that they sought more support, and that the spouse provided greater encouragement for the opportunity, provided less encouragement for the easy puzzle activity, and gave more assistance and emotional support, during the

10 1180 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(8) Table 4. RC and Anti-RC Support Predicting DMs Perceptions of Support During Waiting Period. Observed Support Spouse encouraged DM to embrace opportunity Spouse encouraged DM to choose the easier activity Spouse provided assistance or advice Spouse provided emotional support Spouse was negative, critical, or unsupportive DM sought support from spouse B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) RC support 1.19*** (0.18) [0.84, 1.54] 0.06 (0.12) [ 0.17, 0.28] 0.76*** (0.17) [0.43, 1.10] 0.47*** (0.14) [0.19, 0.74] 0.09 (0.09) [ 0.28, 0.09] 0.47*** (0.13) [0.21, 0.72] Anti-RC support 0.12 (0.36) [ 0.60, 0.83] 0.26 (0.23) [ 0.21, 0.72] 0.11 (0.34) [ 0.79, 0.57] 0.80** (0.28) [ 1.35, 0.24] 0.48** (0.19) [0.11, 0.85] 0.21 (0.27) [ 0.73, 0.32] R 2.24***.01.13***.15***.06**.09*** Note. Unstandardized regression coefficients; 95% confidence intervals for all effects are shown in brackets. DM = decision-maker; RC = relational catalyst. p <.10. *p <.05. **p <.01. ***p <.001. Table 5. Choice Differences in Long-Term Thriving Outcomes. DMs who accepted DMs who declined Difference Long-Term Thriving Outcomes M (SD) M (SD) t Cohen s d Growth and learning (eudaimonic well-being) Happiness (hedonic well-being) Self-efficacy (psychological well-being) Feeling smart/talented (psychological well-being) Communal relationship strength (relational well-being) Relationship satisfaction (relational well-being) Note. DM = decision-maker; CI = confidence interval. p <.10. *p <.05. **p <.01. ***p < % CI of the difference 3.77 (0.86) 3.34 (1.04) t(114) = 2.28* 0.45 [ 0.81, 0.06] 3.47 (0.83) 3.14 (0.83) t(114) = [ 0.67, 0.01] 5.43 (0.88) 4.98 (0.84) t(114) = 2.51* 0.52 [ 0.81, 0.10] 6.03 (0.85) 5.45 (1.08) t(114) = 3.01** 0.60 [ 0.96, 0.20] 8.83 (1.04) 7.97 (2.08) t(112) = 2.92** 0.52 [ 1.44, 0.28] 6.60 (1.33) 6.53 (1.38) t(112) = 0.26 waiting period. There were no choice differences in perceptions of the spouse as negative, critical, or unsupportive. Does the Decision to Pursue a Challenging Opportunity Predict Thriving Over Time? Long-term thriving outcomes. To test the hypothesis that embracing challenging opportunities contributes to longterm thriving, we next examined choice differences in decision-makers personal and relational outcomes indicative of thriving 6 months later. We did not hypothesize changes in thriving outcomes over 6 months, as we expected that the choice participants made in the lab (to embrace the opportunity or not) would be representative of the choices they typically make regarding life opportunities. Instead, our goal was to show the prospective predictability of choice on thriving outcomes (decisions made at Time 1 predicting thriving 6 months later) to establish that these choice points are predictive of thriving over time. As shown in Table 5, decision-makers who accepted the opportunity reported significantly more growth and learning over the last 6 months (greater eudaimonic well-being) compared with those who had declined it, and there was a nearsignificant trend for decision-makers who chose the opportunity to report being happier 6 months later (greater hedonic well-being) than those who had declined it. In addition, with regard to psychological well-being, decision-makers who chose the opportunity reported significantly greater self-efficacy and feelings of being smart/talented than those who had declined it. With regard to relationship well-being over 6 months, results indicated that decision-makers who chose the opportunity reported significantly greater communal relationship strength than those who had declined. However, there was not a long-term difference in reports of

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological needs & implicit motives Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2016 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 3 Psychological

More information

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155 24 Motivation Internalized Motivation in the Classroom Kennon M. Sheldon The motivation that students bring to a classroom setting is critical in determining how much, and how well, they learn. This activity

More information

Interest in Another s Consideration of One s Needs in Communal and Exchange Relationships

Interest in Another s Consideration of One s Needs in Communal and Exchange Relationships JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 34, 246 264 (1998) ARTICLE NO. JS981352 Interest in Another s Consideration of One s Needs in Communal and Exchange Relationships Margaret S. Clark and Parastu

More information

Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs

Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2014 Image source 1 Reeve (2009, pp. 142-143) Psychological need An inherent source

More information

TTI SUCCESS INSIGHTS Personal Interests, Attitudes and Values TM

TTI SUCCESS INSIGHTS Personal Interests, Attitudes and Values TM TTI SUCCESS INSIGHTS Personal Interests, Attitudes and Values TM "He who knows others is learned. He who knows himself is wise." Lao Tse CATHERINE 2-29-2008 Sculpt your Destiny 4545 Contour blvd. #B San

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2014 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 when people find

More information

Relationship Questionnaire

Relationship Questionnaire Relationship Questionnaire The 7 Dimensions of Exceptional Relationships Developed by Gal Szekely, MFT The Couples Center.org Copyright Gal Szekely, 2015. All rights reserved. Permission is granted to

More information

Examining the Role of Self-esteem in the Association between Emotional Vulnerability and Psychological Well-being

Examining the Role of Self-esteem in the Association between Emotional Vulnerability and Psychological Well-being University of Dayton ecommons Honors Theses University Honors Program Spring 4-2015 Examining the Role of Self-esteem in the Association between Emotional Vulnerability and Psychological Well-being Kathryn

More information

Keywords altruistic, communal orientation, communal relationship, egoistic, motivation

Keywords altruistic, communal orientation, communal relationship, egoistic, motivation Article Egoistic versus altruistic concerns in communal relationships J S P R Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 28(3) 315 335 ª The Author(s) 2010 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalspermissions.nav

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2013 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 3 Psychological

More information

THESIS. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University

THESIS. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University End-states vs. Directions: Goals as Objectives vs. Compass Points THESIS Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University

More information

Internalized Motivation in the Classroom

Internalized Motivation in the Classroom Internalized Motivation in the Classroom Motivation Exercise 20-30 min. The motivation that students bring to a classroom setting is critical in determining how much, and how well, they learn. This activity

More information

DISPOSITIONAL POSITIVE EMOTIONS SCALE (DPES) COMPASSION SUBSCALE.

DISPOSITIONAL POSITIVE EMOTIONS SCALE (DPES) COMPASSION SUBSCALE. DISPOSITIONAL POSITIVE EMOTIONS SCALE (DPES) COMPASSION SUBSCALE. Reference: Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John O. P. (2006). Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality

More information

TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Coaching Report

TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Coaching Report TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Coaching Report "He who knows others is learned. He who knows himself is wise." Lao Tse Mason Roberts District Manager YMCA 8-1-2008 Copyright 2003-2008. Performance

More information

Adjustment to Retirement: The Moderating Role of Attachment. Dikla Segel, Peter Bamberger

Adjustment to Retirement: The Moderating Role of Attachment. Dikla Segel, Peter Bamberger Adjustment to Retirement: The Moderating Role of Attachment Dikla Segel, Peter Bamberger Introduction: Adjustment to Retirement Retirement has become a long and meaningful phase in the older adult s life

More information

Keys to Being a Successful Leader

Keys to Being a Successful Leader FEDERER PERFORMANCE MANAGEMENT GROUP, LLC TRANSFORMING VISIONS INTO RESULTS WHITE PAPER Keys to Being a Successful Leader Introduction Why is it that some people are successful in leadership roles, while

More information

The Paradox of Received Social Support

The Paradox of Received Social Support PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Paradox of Received Social Support The Importance of Natalya C. Maisel 1 and Shelly L. Gable 2 1 University of California, Los Angeles, and 2 University of California,

More information

The Attribute Index - Leadership

The Attribute Index - Leadership 26-Jan-2007 0.88 / 0.74 The Attribute Index - Leadership Innermetrix, Inc. Innermetrix Talent Profile of Innermetrix, Inc. http://www.innermetrix.cc/ The Attribute Index - Leadership Patterns Patterns

More information

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: SOCIAL AFFILIATION CUES PRIME HELP-SEEKING INTENTIONS 1 This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: Rubin, M. (2011). Social

More information

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE TEST-R

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE TEST-R We thank you for taking the test and for your support and participation. Your report is presented in multiple sections as given below: Menu Indicators Indicators specific to the test Personalized analysis

More information

TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Emotional Intelligence Version

TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Emotional Intelligence Version TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Emotional Intelligence Version "He who knows others is learned. He who knows himself is wise." Lao Tse Henry Stein 7-31-2008 Copyright 2004-2008. Target Training International,

More information

Motivation Motivation

Motivation Motivation This should be easy win What am I doing here! Motivation Motivation What Is Motivation? Motivation is the direction and intensity of effort. Direction of effort: Whether an individual seeks out, approaches,

More information

Version The trait emotional intelligence (trait EI) model successfully integrates and extends EIrelated

Version The trait emotional intelligence (trait EI) model successfully integrates and extends EIrelated Version 1.50 The trait emotional intelligence (trait EI) model successfully integrates and extends EIrelated ideas in a general framework that incorporates 15 specific facets. Adaptability Emotion control

More information

Predictors of Caregiving in Adult Intimate Relationships: An Attachment Theoretical Perspective

Predictors of Caregiving in Adult Intimate Relationships: An Attachment Theoretical Perspective Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2001. Vol. 80, No. 6, 972-994 Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. OO22-3514/OI/S5.OO DOI: I0.1037//0022-3514.80.6.972 Predictors

More information

An Indian Journal FULL PAPER ABSTRACT KEYWORDS. Trade Science Inc. Feedback seeking effects on creative performance based on goal self consistency

An Indian Journal FULL PAPER ABSTRACT KEYWORDS. Trade Science Inc. Feedback seeking effects on creative performance based on goal self consistency [Type text] [Type text] [Type text] ISSN : 0974-7435 Volume 10 Issue 16 BioTechnology 2014 An Indian Journal FULL PAPER BTAIJ, 10(16), 2014 [9370-9374] Feedback seeking effects on creative performance

More information

BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory. Resource Report. John Morris. Name: ID: Admin. Date: December 15, 2010 (Online) 17 Minutes 22 Seconds

BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory. Resource Report. John Morris. Name: ID: Admin. Date: December 15, 2010 (Online) 17 Minutes 22 Seconds BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory By Reuven Bar-On, Ph.D. Resource Report Name: ID: Admin. Date: Duration: John Morris December 15, 2010 (Online) 17 Minutes 22 Seconds Copyright 2002 Multi-Health Systems

More information

The eight steps to resilience at work

The eight steps to resilience at work The eight steps to resilience at work Derek Mowbray March 2010 derek.mowbray@orghealth.co.uk www.orghealth.co.uk Introduction Resilience is the personal capacity to cope with adverse events and return

More information

leadership REPORT Sam Sample Other Raters (3), Family/Friends (3), Direct Reports (3), Peers (4), and Manager (3)

leadership REPORT Sam Sample Other Raters (3), Family/Friends (3), Direct Reports (3), Peers (4), and Manager (3) Coach leadership EQ 360 REPORT Sam Sample Other Raters (3), Family/Friends (3), Direct Reports (3), Peers (4), and Manager (3) Sample Report Multi-Health Systems Inc. December 05, 2014 Copyright 2014 Multi-Health

More information

COACH WORKPLACE REPORT. Jane Doe. Sample Report July 18, Copyright 2011 Multi-Health Systems Inc. All rights reserved.

COACH WORKPLACE REPORT. Jane Doe. Sample Report July 18, Copyright 2011 Multi-Health Systems Inc. All rights reserved. COACH WORKPLACE REPORT Jane Doe Sample Report July 8, 0 Copyright 0 Multi-Health Systems Inc. All rights reserved. Response Style Explained Indicates the need for further examination possible validity

More information

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter Lawrence Feinstein Jyllian Call University of California, Los Angeles December, 1986 Further information on the instrument can be obtained

More information

International School of Turin

International School of Turin International School of Turin Adapted from the IB PSE scope and sequence Personal, Social and Physical Education Scope and Sequence Identity An understanding of our own beliefs, values, attitudes, experiences

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

TERMINOLOGY INSECURITY? APPLICATION OF ATTACHMENT THEORY TO THE HUMAN-COMPANION ANIMAL BOND, AND REVIEW OF ITS EFFECTS

TERMINOLOGY INSECURITY? APPLICATION OF ATTACHMENT THEORY TO THE HUMAN-COMPANION ANIMAL BOND, AND REVIEW OF ITS EFFECTS TERMINOLOGY INSECURITY? APPLICATION OF ATTACHMENT THEORY TO THE HUMAN-COMPANION ANIMAL BOND, AND REVIEW OF ITS EFFECTS Stephanie Wilmore, M. Ed. Doctoral Candidate, Washington State University Pre-Doctoral

More information

Selecting Research Participants. Conducting Experiments, Survey Construction and Data Collection. Practical Considerations of Research

Selecting Research Participants. Conducting Experiments, Survey Construction and Data Collection. Practical Considerations of Research Conducting Experiments, Survey Construction and Data Collection RCS 6740 6/28/04 Practical Considerations of Research This lecture will focus on some of the practical aspects of conducting research studies

More information

Acceptance of Help as a Function of Similarity of the Potential Helper and Opportunity To Repay'

Acceptance of Help as a Function of Similarity of the Potential Helper and Opportunity To Repay' Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1974.4, 3, pp. 224-229 Acceptance of Help as a Function of Similarity of the Potential Helper and Opportunity To Repay' MARGARET S. CLARK, CAROLYN C. GOTAY, AND JUDSON

More information

Towson University Center for Adults with Autism Towson, MD Adventure Pursuits for Adults with Autism

Towson University Center for Adults with Autism Towson, MD  Adventure Pursuits for Adults with Autism Towson University Center for Adults with Autism Towson, MD www.towson.edu/chp/caasd Adventure Pursuits for Adults with Autism Project Coordinator: Lisa Crabtree, PhD, OTR/L, Towson University Center for

More information

Building Friendships: Avoid Discounting

Building Friendships: Avoid Discounting Module 3 Part 2 Building Friendships: Avoid Discounting Objectives: 1. Explore the relationship between stress and discounting. 2. Understand what discounting is and how it relates to stress in relationships.

More information

Measuring Psychological Wealth: Your Well-Being Balance Sheet

Measuring Psychological Wealth: Your Well-Being Balance Sheet Putting It All Together 14 Measuring Psychological Wealth: Your Well-Being Balance Sheet Measuring Your Satisfaction With Life Below are five statements with which you may agree or disagree. Using the

More information

Why Do Kids Play Soccer? Why Do You Coach Soccer?

Why Do Kids Play Soccer? Why Do You Coach Soccer? COMMUNICATION MOTIVATION Why Do Kids Play Soccer? Why Do You Coach Soccer? Why Do Kids Play Soccer? Because they want to have fun Because they want to learn, develop, and get better Because they want to

More information

Personal Growth Interpretation of Goal Attainment as a New Construct Relevant to Well-being

Personal Growth Interpretation of Goal Attainment as a New Construct Relevant to Well-being Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 185 ( 2015 ) 244 249 3rd World Conference on Psychology and Sociology, WCPS- 2014 Personal Growth Interpretation

More information

Attachment and daily sexual goals: A study of dating couples

Attachment and daily sexual goals: A study of dating couples Personal Relationships, 15 (2008), 375 390. Printed in the United States of America. Copyright Ó 2008 IARR. 1350-4126=08 Attachment and daily sexual goals: A study of dating couples EMILY A. IMPETT, a

More information

The Power to Change Your Life: Ten Keys to Resilient Living Robert Brooks, Ph.D.

The Power to Change Your Life: Ten Keys to Resilient Living Robert Brooks, Ph.D. The Power to Change Your Life: Ten Keys to Resilient Living Robert Brooks, Ph.D. The latest book I co-authored with my colleague Dr. Sam Goldstein was recently released. In contrast to our previous works

More information

Categories of Strengths

Categories of Strengths Cognitive and appraisal skills (problem-solving) Sees the world as most other people in the same culture see it. Can understand the causes and effects of own and others actions. Can describe facts, events,

More information

Healing Trauma Evaluation Year 1 Findings

Healing Trauma Evaluation Year 1 Findings 2551 Galena Avenue #1774 Simi Valley, CA 93065 310-801-8996 Envisioning Justice Solutions, Inc. Determining the Programs, Policies, and Services Needed to Rebuild the Lives of Criminal Justice Involved

More information

The Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire (MLQ) measures a broad range of leadership types from passive leaders, to leaders who give contingent rewards

The Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire (MLQ) measures a broad range of leadership types from passive leaders, to leaders who give contingent rewards Published by: Mind Garden, Inc. www.mindgarden.com info@mindgarden.com Copyright 1998, 2007, 2011, 2015 by Bernard M. Bass and Bruce J. Avolio. All rights reserved. Permission is hereby granted to reproduce

More information

He l p i n g Be h a v i o r s

He l p i n g Be h a v i o r s Helping Behaviors in Relationships 799 Further Readings Berkman, L. F., & Kawachi, I. (Eds.). (2000). Social epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press. Bowlby, J. (1969/1999). Attachment and loss:

More information

Managing Your Emotions

Managing Your Emotions Managing Your Emotions I love to ask What s your story? (pause) You immediately had an answer to that question, right? HOW you responded in your mind is very telling. What I want to talk about in this

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING: A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF EXECUTIVES OF GOVERNMENT AND PRIVATE ORGANIZATIONS OF JAMMU CITY

PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING: A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF EXECUTIVES OF GOVERNMENT AND PRIVATE ORGANIZATIONS OF JAMMU CITY GJBM ISSN: 0973-8533 Vol. 2 No. 1, June 2008 Psychological Well-Being: A Comparative Study of Executives of Government... PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING: A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF EXECUTIVES OF GOVERNMENT AND PRIVATE

More information

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students Mia Bonner Millersville University Abstract Previous research (Elion, Slaney, Wang and French, 2012) found

More information

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Heather Dudley What influences people s willingness to interact with someone of a different cultural background from

More information

Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins

Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins Columbia University ABSTRACT Where does value come from? I propose a new answer to this classic question. People experience

More information

PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE?

PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE? Submitted: February 25, 2017 Revision: March 30, 2017 Accepted: April 27, 2017 PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE? Masataka Takebe Koji Murata Graduate

More information

Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation

Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation Hsiang Chen, Rolf T. Wigand and Michael Nilan School of Information Studies, Syracuse University Syracuse, NY 13244 Email: [ hchen04, rwigand, mnilan]@mailbox.syr.edu

More information

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use ACM, forthcoming. This is the author s version of the work. It is posted here by permission of ACM for your personal use. Not for redistribution. The definitive version will be published soon. Citation:

More information

The Relationship between YouTube Interaction, Depression, and Social Anxiety. By Meredith Johnson

The Relationship between YouTube Interaction, Depression, and Social Anxiety. By Meredith Johnson The Relationship between YouTube Interaction, Depression, and Social Anxiety By Meredith Johnson Introduction The media I would like to research is YouTube with the effects of social anxiety and depression.

More information

12 The biology of love

12 The biology of love The biology of love Motivation Neurological origins of passionate love begin in infancy when infants attach to mother. Certain neurotransmitters and hormones involved in pleasure and reward are activated

More information

Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI)

Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) Scale Description The Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) is a multidimensional measurement device intended to assess participantsõ subjective experience related to

More information

Evaluating you relationships

Evaluating you relationships Evaluating you relationships What relationships are important to you? What are you doing today to care for them? Have you told those concerned how you feel? Most of us regularly inspect the health of our

More information

Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS

Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS INTEREST and ENJOYMENT TASK VALUE GOALS (Purposes for doing) INTRINSIC vs EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION EXPECTANCY BELIEFS SELF-EFFICACY SELF-CONCEPT (Self-Esteem) OUTCOME

More information

Survey Methods in Relationship Research

Survey Methods in Relationship Research Purdue University Purdue e-pubs Department of Psychological Sciences Faculty Publications Department of Psychological Sciences 1-1-2009 Survey Methods in Relationship Research Christopher Agnew Purdue

More information

Happiness, Pleasure, and Satisfaction

Happiness, Pleasure, and Satisfaction Happiness, Pleasure, and Satisfaction Christopher Rice Lynn University In The Nature and Value of Happiness, Christine Vitrano defines happiness as a mental state in which people feel satisfaction about

More information

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE Previous chapter namely Review of the Literature was concerned with the review of the research studies conducted in the field of teacher education, with special reference

More information

CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK

CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK 2.0.0 INTRODUCTION The details about introduction, rationale of the present study, statement of the problem objectives of the study, hypotheses of the study, delimitation

More information

The Satisfaction in the doctor-patient relationship: the communication assessment

The Satisfaction in the doctor-patient relationship: the communication assessment EUROPEAN ACADEMIC RESEARCH Vol. IV, Issue 2/ May 2016 ISSN 2286-4822 www.euacademic.org Impact Factor: 3.4546 (UIF) DRJI Value: 5.9 (B+) The Satisfaction in the doctor-patient relationship: the LEJDA ABAZI

More information

Subjective Well-Being and Adjustment

Subjective Well-Being and Adjustment Subjective Well-Being and Adjustment LP 10b happiness 1 Subjective well-being is the scientific term for how people evaluate their lives in terms of their happiness and life satisfaction. It is important

More information

Enhancing Support for Special Populations through Understanding Neurodiversity

Enhancing Support for Special Populations through Understanding Neurodiversity Enhancing Support for Special Populations through Understanding Neurodiversity Dr. Ann P. McMahon annpmcmahon@gmail.com Custom K 12 Engineering customk 12engineering.com Select a puzzle from the container

More information

Foundations for Success. Unit 3

Foundations for Success. Unit 3 Foundations for Success Unit 3 Know Yourself Socrates Lesson 1 Self-Awareness Key Terms assessment associate cluster differentiate introspection What You Will Learn to Do Determine your behavioral preferences

More information

Flourishing and floundering students: Implications for identification and engagement

Flourishing and floundering students: Implications for identification and engagement Flourishing and floundering students: Implications for identification and engagement Pat Esplin, Larry Nelson, Steve Wygant Brigham Young University Annual Conference on The First-Year Experience San Antonio

More information

Healthy Self. Lesson 1 Self Esteem

Healthy Self. Lesson 1 Self Esteem Healthy Self Lesson 1 Self Esteem Starter Self Esteem Quiz FALSE 1. A person has high self-esteem if they are always concerned about what other people think of them. TRUE 2. Self-esteem means being proud

More information

GROUP REPORT. Insert Personalized Title SAMPLE. Assessments Completed Between: December 18, 2013 and December 20, 2013

GROUP REPORT. Insert Personalized Title SAMPLE. Assessments Completed Between: December 18, 2013 and December 20, 2013 GROUP REPORT Insert Personalized Title SAMPLE Assessments Completed Between: December 18, 2013 and December 20, 2013 Report Generated on: January 13, 2014 Total in Group: 10 fellipelli.com.br EQ-i 2.0

More information

MOTIVATIONAL INTERVIEWING

MOTIVATIONAL INTERVIEWING MOTIVATIONAL INTERVIEWING Facilitating Behaviour Change Dr Kate Hall MCCLP MAPS Senior Lecturer in Addiction and Mental Health School of Psychology, Faculty of Health, Deakin University. Lead, Treatment

More information

C O N T E N T S ... v vi. Job Tasks 38 Job Satisfaction 39. Group Development 6. Leisure Activities 41. Values 44. Instructions 9.

C O N T E N T S ... v vi. Job Tasks 38 Job Satisfaction 39. Group Development 6. Leisure Activities 41. Values 44. Instructions 9. C O N T E N T S LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES v vi INTRODUCTION TO THE FIRO-B INSTRUMENT 1 Overview of Uses 1 THEORY OF INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 3 The Interpersonal Needs 3 Expressed and Wanted Needs 4 The

More information

Defining principles of Strategic family therapy

Defining principles of Strategic family therapy Roselyne Kattar Psychologist and coach http://www.coaching-kattar.com 1 Defining principles of Strategic family therapy Strategic therapy focuses on present observable behavioral interaction and uses deliberate

More information

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question Trivia Question Where did win one for the gipper come from? Chapter 9 What are the 3 components of motivation? 3 major categories of motivation. Major theories of motivation. How the theories are applied

More information

Whole Person Coaching Defined

Whole Person Coaching Defined coach & transform: whole person coaching foundations Whole Person Coaching Defined Whole Person Coaching is an advanced, holistic coaching method dedicated to helping clients create fulfillment by fostering

More information

Creating A Culture of Wellbeing

Creating A Culture of Wellbeing Creating A Culture of Wellbeing Enabling our community to flourish. Rāroa Normal Intermediate School Stephen Eames, Marian Williams, Matt Tilley Why? Changing student needs. To have the best possible teachers

More information

MALE LIBIDO- EBOOKLET

MALE LIBIDO- EBOOKLET MALE LIBIDO- EBOOKLET Hi there, Thank you for ordering this Native Remedies ebooklet! ebooklets are modified from consultations with real people and cover some of the most frequently dealt with problems

More information

Advanced Code of Influence. Book 10

Advanced Code of Influence. Book 10 Advanced Code of Influence Book 10 Table of Contents BOOK 10: SOCIAL IDENTITY, AFFILIATION & ATTRACTION... 3 Determinants of Helpful Behavior... 4 Affiliation... 7 Determinants of Affiliation... 8 Determinants

More information

Journey of Personal Development (Part 3): Transcend Yourself. Paul T. P. Wong. Personal development is a process

Journey of Personal Development (Part 3): Transcend Yourself. Paul T. P. Wong. Personal development is a process 1 Journey of Personal Development (Part 3): Transcend Yourself Paul T. P. Wong Congratulations for coming to the last installment of this 3- part series. If Part (1) Know Yourself serves as the foundation

More information

Introduction to Positive Psychology 23 rd February Jo Hennessy

Introduction to Positive Psychology 23 rd February Jo Hennessy Introduction to Positive Psychology 23 rd February 2018 Jo Hennessy Objectives To introduce you to the following ideas, with exercises, reflection and discussion: The Health Disease Continuum Aims of Positive

More information

SELF-ESTEEM, LOVE, AND THE INTERNET: IS SELF-ESTEEM A FACTOR IN CHOOSING TO PURSUE LOVE THROUGH ONLINE COMMUNICATION?

SELF-ESTEEM, LOVE, AND THE INTERNET: IS SELF-ESTEEM A FACTOR IN CHOOSING TO PURSUE LOVE THROUGH ONLINE COMMUNICATION? SELF-ESTEEM, LOVE, AND THE INTERNET: IS SELF-ESTEEM A FACTOR IN CHOOSING TO PURSUE LOVE THROUGH ONLINE COMMUNICATION? JACQUELINE C. PIRONE COLGATE UNIVERSITY Abstract Because the internet relationship,

More information

USING ASSERTIVENESS TO COMMUNICATE ABOUT SEX

USING ASSERTIVENESS TO COMMUNICATE ABOUT SEX Chapter 5: Sexual Health Exercise 1 USING ASSERTIVENESS TO COMMUNICATE ABOUT SEX Aggressive Passive Manipulative/manipulation Assertive Balance of power Sex Sexual coercion 1. To build learners communication

More information

To Do It or Not to Do It? How Communally Motivated People Navigate. Sexual Interdependence Dilemmas

To Do It or Not to Do It? How Communally Motivated People Navigate. Sexual Interdependence Dilemmas 580129PSPXXX10.1177/0146167215580129Personality and Social Psychology BulletinDay et al. research-article2015 Article To Do It or Not to Do It? How Communally Motivated People Navigate Sexual Interdependence

More information

13 Things Mentally Strong People Don t Do.

13 Things Mentally Strong People Don t Do. SUPPORTING SERVING AND FORMER MEMBERS OF THE ARMED FORCES, EMERGENCY 13 Things Mentally Strong People Don t Do. Mental strength isn't often reflected in what you do. It's usually seen in what you don't

More information

MODULE 7 CLIENT CENTRED THERAPY Quadrant 1

MODULE 7 CLIENT CENTRED THERAPY Quadrant 1 MODULE 7 CLIENT CENTRED THERAPY Quadrant 1 1. Introduction Client Centered Therapy (CCT) established by Carl Rogers is also known by terms such as Rogerian psychotherapy, person-centered psychotherapy,

More information

Step 2 Challenging negative thoughts "Weeding"

Step 2 Challenging negative thoughts Weeding Managing Automatic Negative Thoughts (ANTs) Step 1 Identifying negative thoughts "ANTs" Step 2 Challenging negative thoughts "Weeding" Step 3 Planting positive thoughts 'Potting" Step1 Identifying Your

More information

ANNEX ON THE QIF BROCHURE

ANNEX ON THE QIF BROCHURE ANNEX ON THE QIF BROCHURE Why getting better at work increases your HAPPINESS and WELL-BEING as well as engenders FUN A Short Treatise on Why Getting Better at Work increases your Happiness and Well-Being

More information

THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION

THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION 92 THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION 1. Motivation is a highly dynamic construct that is constantly changing in reaction to life experiences. 2. Needs and goals are constantly growing and changing. 3. As individuals

More information

Choosing Life: Empowerment, Action, Results! CLEAR Menu Sessions. Health Care 3: Partnering In My Care and Treatment

Choosing Life: Empowerment, Action, Results! CLEAR Menu Sessions. Health Care 3: Partnering In My Care and Treatment Choosing Life: Empowerment, Action, Results! CLEAR Menu Sessions Health Care 3: Partnering In My Care and Treatment This page intentionally left blank. Session Aims: Partnering In My Care and Treatment

More information

STAGES OF PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT Developed by: Dr. Kathleen E. Allen

STAGES OF PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT Developed by: Dr. Kathleen E. Allen STAGES OF PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT Developed by: Dr. Kathleen E. Allen Ownership Engaged Willing to help build organizations Be a good steward Individual ownership Territorialism Ownership over the tasks

More information

1. Fun. 2. Commitment

1. Fun. 2. Commitment 25 Healthy Relationship Traits Checklist Courtesy of InfidelityFirstAidKit.com What characteristics does it take to keep a couple together for a long time? These traits tend to lead towards harmony in

More information

section 6: transitioning away from mental illness

section 6: transitioning away from mental illness section 6: transitioning away from mental illness Throughout this resource, we have emphasized the importance of a recovery perspective. One of the main achievements of the recovery model is its emphasis

More information

Addressing the Opposition to Change

Addressing the Opposition to Change Addressing the Opposition to Change There is no single way to deal with making change, but understanding resistance helps. Being patient and determined goes a long way to making it happen. By Preston Ingalls

More information

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect Abstract This article reports the effects of hedonic versus utilitarian consumption goals on consumers choices between

More information

10 steps to create more personal relationships and help your clients meet their goals

10 steps to create more personal relationships and help your clients meet their goals 10 steps to create more personal relationships and help your clients meet their goals Individuals are unique and so too are their retirement journeys. The Science of Happiness is about understanding what

More information

COUNSELING INTERVIEW GUIDELINES

COUNSELING INTERVIEW GUIDELINES Dr. Moshe ben Asher SOC 356, Introduction to Social Welfare CSUN, Sociology Department COUNSELING INTERVIEW GUIDELINES WHAT DISTINGUISHES A PROFESSIONAL FROM OTHER KINDS OF WORKERS? Education and training

More information

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals SOCIAL INFLUENCE 2011, 6 (1), 15 21 The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals Esther S. Jeong 1, Yue Shi 1, Anna Baazova 1, Christine

More information

Paul Figueroa. Washington Municipal Clerks Association ANNUAL CONFERENCE. Workplace Bullying: Solutions and Prevention. for

Paul Figueroa. Washington Municipal Clerks Association ANNUAL CONFERENCE. Workplace Bullying: Solutions and Prevention. for Workplace Bullying: Solutions and Prevention for Washington Municipal Clerks Association ANNUAL CONFERENCE Paul@PeaceEnforcement.com 206-650-5364 Peace Enforcement LLC Bullying described: Why people do

More information

COPING WITH SCLERODERMA

COPING WITH SCLERODERMA COPING WITH SCLERODERMA Any chronic disease is life changing. Symptoms demand your attention. You have to adjust your schedule to accommodate medications, doctors appointments and treatments of various

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information