P1pc, N1pc, N3pc, SPCN

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "P1pc, N1pc, N3pc, SPCN"

Transcription

1 Time-course of hemispheric preference for processing contralateral relevant shapes: P1pc, N1pc, N2pc, N3pc Rolf Verleger 1,, Blandyna Żurawska vel Grajewska 2, and Piotr Jaśkowski 2 1 Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Germany 2 Faculty of Psychology, University of Finance and Management, Warsaw, Poland Abstract KeywordS N2pc, attention, event-related potentials, P1pc, N1pc, N3pc, SPCN A most sensitive and specific electrophysiological indicator of selective processing of visual stimuli is the N2pc component N2pc is a negative EEG potential peaking 250 ms after stimulus onset, recorded from posterior sites contralateral to relevant stimuli Additional deflections preceding or following N2pc have been obtained in previous studies, possibly produced by specific stimulus features or specific prime-target sequences To clarify the entire time-course of the contralateralipsilateral (C-I) difference recorded from the scalp above visual cortex in response to left-right pairs of targets and distracters, C-I differences were here compared between two types of stimuli and between stimuli that were or were not preceded by masked neutral primes The C-I difference waveform consisted of several peaks, termed here P1pc ( ms after target onset), N1pc ( ms), N2pc ( ms), and N3pc ( ms) Being markedly enhanced when stimuli were preceded by the neutral primes, P1pc may indicate a response to stimulus change Also, when stimuli were, N2pc reached its peak earlier, thereby tending to merge with N1pc N3pc seemed to increase when target discrimination was difficult N1pc, N2pc, and N3pc appear as three periods of one process N3pc probably corresponds to L400 or SPCN as described in other studies These observations suggest that the neurophysiological basis of stimulus-driven focusing of attention on target stimuli is a process that lasts for hundreds of milliseconds, with the relevant hemisphere being activated in an oscillating manner as long as required by the task Introduction When two stimuli are simultaneously presented left and right from fixation and the electroencephalogram (EEG) is recorded from the observer s scalp, then the more relevant stimulus will evoke a negative EEG potential at the scalp above the contralateral visual cortex, peaking at about 250 ms after stimulus onset (eg, Eimer, 1996; Hickey, Di Lollo, & McDonald, 2009; Hopf et al, 2006; Wascher & Wauschkuhn, 1996) This potential was termed N2pc by Luck, Fan, and Hillyard (1993), that is, a negative peak in the time range of the N2 peak, posterior contralateral N2pc presumably indicates selective processing of the relevant feature (Eimer, 1996; Eimer & Kiss, 2008; Verleger & Jaśkowski, 2007; Wauschkuhn et al, 1998) N2pc may be conveniently measured in the difference waveform contralateral minus ipsilateral (C I) to the relevant stimulus because everything common to the relevant and irrelevant stimuli will then be subtracted out, leaving what is specific to relevance (Oostenveld, Stegeman, Praamstra, & van Oosterom, 2003; Wascher & Wauschkuhn, 1996) Corresponding author: Rolf Verleger, Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Germany Phone: Fax: RolfVerleger@NeuroUni-Luebeckde ( neurouni-luebeckde) 19 DOI /v

2 Being larger contralateral to the relevant stimulus, N2pc faithfully reflects the side on which the observer perceived something relevant This feature made N2pc a useful tool for disentangling brain responses to stimuli presented in close succession, as for example target stimuli immediately followed by some mask, target stimuli immediately preceded by some priming stimuli, or target stimuli embedded within series of distractors, because the side specificity may provide a unique trace of either of these stimuli (Jaśkowski, Skalska, & Verleger, 2003; Jaśkowski, van der Lubbe, Schlotterbeck, & Verleger, 2002; Verleger & Jaśkowski, 2007; Verleger, Śmigasiewicz, & Möller, 2011; Woodman & Luck, 1999) However, this specificity of N2pc to side of the relevant stimuli does not resolve all difficulties of interpreting overlap of components in the C-I difference Thus, the present study was prompted by questions raised from considering and comparing the results of two studies of ours (Jaśkowski et al, 2002, 2003) In both studies, metacontrastmasking was used, with stimuli presented as primes whose outer contours fit the inner contours of the ensuing visible main stimuli Jaśkowski et al (2002) used diamonds and squares with octagonal inner contours (cf Klotz & Wolff, 1995) while Jaśkowski et al (2003) used simple squares These stimuli and the grand means of the C-I difference waveforms are depicted in Figure 1 In both studies, partici- Figure 1 Contralateral-ipsilateral grand averages P7-P8 and stimuli from Jaśkowski, van der Lubbe, and Schlotterbeck (2002) (top) and Jaśkowski, Skalska, and Verleger (2003) (bottom) The sequence of stimuli in a trial is depicted on the right side of the figure, exemplified for targets presented left of fixation (diamonds and squares with gaps) Primes could be congruent (target shape on the same side as in the main stimulus), or incongruent (target shape on the other side), or neutral (in the diamond experiment only) In the gap-square experiment (lower panel), all four primes had the gap-square on the same side in a given trial and, across blocks, congruent and incongruent trials had frequencies either of 80/20% or of 20/ 80% In the grand averages (of 12 and 11 participants for the two studies, respectively), the x-axes denote milliseconds from (1 st ) prime onset (gray) and from target onset (black) y-axes are in microvolts, with negative polarity contralateral to position of target plotted upwards In the diamond experiment, bold solid lines are from congruent trials, thin lines from neutral, and dotted lines from incongruent trials In the gap-square experiment, solid lines are from congruent trials, dotted lines from incongruent trials, and bold lines are from the 80/20 block, thin lines from the 20/80 block Thin gray vertical arrows point to N160pc evoked by target-like shapes in the prime stimuli Big gray arrows highlight N2pc evoked by target-like shapes in the prime stimuli (Both gray arrows are depicted in lighter gray in the upper panel, reflecting the absence of these deflections) Big black arrows highlight N2pc evoked by targets in the main stimuli Thin black arrows point to N400pc evoked by targets in the main stimuli The sloping arrow (middle panel) points to a peak that may be interpreted either as P300pc evoked by neutral primes or as P130pc evoked by targets preceded by neutral primes SOA = stimulus onset asynchrony between onset of (first) prime and main stimulus 20

3 pants had to ignore the primes and make speeded left vs right choice responses according to the side of the target in the main stimuli (The visible stimulus pair, which included the target as one of its two stimuli, will be called main stimuli throughout) Both studies focused on N2pc evoked by the target in the main stimuli (black big arrows in Figure 1) and varying as a function of the primes, and on N2pc evoked by the target-like shape in the prime stimuli (gray big arrows in Figure 1) One obvious difference in the latter, prime-evoked N2pc between studies was that in Jaśkowski et al (2002), N2pc was evoked when primes were weakly identifiable (middle panel of Figure 1) but not reliably when primes were unidentifiable (denoted by lighter gray of the big arrow in top panel of Figure 1), whereas a distinct N2pc was evoked by unidentifiable primes in Jaśkowski et al (2003; see gray big arrow in bottom panel of Figure 1) Second, there was a conspicuous primeevoked component at 160 ms already (cf thin gray vertical arrows in Figure 1), both in Jaśkowski et al (2003) and, less distinctly, in the 167 ms SOA (stimulus-onset asynchrony) condition of Jaśkowski et al (2002) and not in the 83 ms SOA condition (denoted by lighter gray of the thin gray vertical arrow in top panel of Figure 1) The question may be asked whether this early peak is also evoked by the following visible target stimuli, remaining unnoticed because of overlapping potentials evoked by the prime Third, targets did not only evoke the N2pc but also a consistent though weak later negative peak at about 400 ms after main-stimulus onset (thin black vertical arrows in Figure 1) 1 This raises the question of how reliable this peak was Fourth, when primes did not contain the target shape (ie, were neutral) and were identifiable, a positivity was evoked in the C-I waveform at about 130 ms after target onset and about 300 ms after prime onset (sloping arrow in middle panel of Figure 1) It was unclear what this deflection reflected These differences between the two studies might be due to differences between stimuli (diamond and square with complex inner contour in one experiment vs simple square with and without gaps in the other), due to differences between the priming sequences (one prime, either at 83 ms or at 167 ms, in one experiment vs a sequence of four primes, at 160 ms, 120 ms, 80 ms, 40 ms, in the other experiment) or due to some other characteristics The purpose of the present experiment was to clarify these issues by focusing on the difference between stimuli and between and un sequences With regard to stimuli, we were interested to see if and how the C-I waveforms would differ between selection for diamonds versus squares and selection for gap-squares versus intact squares In particular, squares with gaps might evoke an earlier contralateral negativity, at 160 ms, like in Jaśkowski et al (2003), whereas diamonds might not Second, with regard to primes, we were interested in how the mere presence of primes would alter these effects Therefore, primes were always neutral in the present study, so as not to prime specific responses or activate attentional focusing on one side, but rather to obtain their alerting and spatial orienting effects simultaneously on both lateral positions (Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002; van der Lubbe, Keuss, & Stoffels, 1996) In comparing the two types of stimuli and their prime sequences, the difficulty arose that the timing of the prime sequences differed between the two studies: Primes were well masked with 83 ms prime-main SOA in Jaśkowski et al (2002) and with 40 ms SOA for each of the intervals of the five prime-prime-prime-prime-main stimuli in Jaśkowski et al (2003; cf the stimulus sequences in the right half of Figure 1) In order to compare these stimulus sequences under conditions that were as equal as possible to each other on the one hand and to the original stimuli on the other hand, we decided to use the same SOA of 80 ms for both types of stimuli between onsets of prime and of main stimulus This meant reducing the number of primes from four to two with the gap-square sequence while maintaining the original 40 ms SOA We did not use only one prime with these stimuli, with an 80 ms SOA before the main stimulus, because we assumed that this longer SOA would alter prime visibility compared to Jaśkowski et al s (2003) original study Earlier peaks than N2pc, as found here at 160 ms after primes, have been occasionally reported in the C-I waveform For example, when presenting relevant stimuli and symmetrically positioned filler stimuli, both Valle-Inclán (1996) and Wascher, Schatz, Kuder, and Verleger (2001) obtained the usual N2pc with a maximum at 250 ms, which changed to an asymmetry of the earlier N1 component in their control experiments where the filler stimuli were omitted This issue was more systematically reconsidered by Wascher and Beste (2010) These authors obtained both N2pc and earlier N1pc components The N1pc was assumed to reflect initial orienting of attention, and the N2pc to reflect a reallocation process following initial orienting Contralateral negativities later than N2pc in the C-I waveform have been more often described, sometimes as a peak (eg, L-400 of Wauschkuhn et al, 1998), sometimes as a tonic surplus of contralateral negativity (Vogel & Machizawa, 2004), and have been called sustained posterior contralateral negativity (SPCN; cf Dell Acqua, Sessa, Jolicœur, & Robitaille, 2006; Jolicœur, Sessa, Dell Acqua, & Robitaille, 2006) or contralateral delayed activity (CDA; Gao, Yin, Xu, Shui, & Shen, 2011; Mazza & Caramazza, 2011) The tonic component has been shown to reflect load and individual capacities of visual working memory (Vogel & Machizawa, 2004; Vogel, McCollough, & Machizawa, 2005) but has also been obtained without any obvious relation to working memory, for instance, when the two searched-for targets were in the same hemifield (Mazza & Caramazza, 2011; Woodman & Luck, 1999) or when the salient stimulus was indeed the relevant one (Seiss, Kiss, & Eimer, 2009; Wauschkuhn et al, 1998), probably just reflecting ongoing relevant stimulation from the same hemi-field Here, we wanted to study whether these components earlier and later than N2pc may also be obtained in simple choice-response tasks and how they depended on the physical nature of stimuli and on the presence of preceding neutral primes Methods Participants Ten students (aged years; one man, nine women) of the University of Bydgoszcz, participated in return for course credit They were seated in a comfortable chair, with 075 m eye-distance to the screen The keyboard for responding was placed on a table in their front 21

4 Stimuli and procedure Black stimuli, as depicted in Figure 2, were presented on a 21 white screen driven by a graphics card working with 75 Hz A fixation dot (03 diameter) was displayed throughout In each trial, a pair of target and distracter stimuli was presented, one left and the other one right of fixation for 200 ms ( main stimulus ) Targets were left or right, in random order across trials According to target side, participants had to press the left or the right key (left Control key or Enter key of the number block) on a keyboard The main stimuli were preceded by primes in a random half of trials Primes always consisted of a pair of stimuli slightly smaller than the main stimuli, and were presented for 13 ms, to be masked by metacontrast, like in Jaśkowski et al (2002, 2003) By always having the shape of distracters, the two primes were neutral with respect to the response required to the target Centers of all stimuli were 23 cm (18 ) away from fixation Each new trial started 840 ms after participant s key-press In one block, the distracter was a square, 22 cm wide and high (17 ), and the target was a diamond (ie, a square rotated by 45 ) Outlines of the shapes were 03 cm wide, and inner contours of these outlines were octagonal, matching both a diamond and a square, as used in metacontrast studies since Klotz and Wolff (1995) Prime stimuli consisted of a pair of smaller squares, 16 cm wide and high, likewise with octagonal inner contours, and were presented 80 ms before the main pair The primes were as in Jaśkowski et al (2002) In the other block, the target was a square with gaps, and the distracter was an intact square Squares were 26 cm (20 ) wide and high, drawn with 01 cm wide lines The gaps were 03 cm (025 ) wide, in the middle of the vertical outlines of the target square Prime stimuli consisted of two successive pairs of intact squares The first pair was presented 80 ms before the main pair and was 22 cm wide and high, the second pair was presented 40 ms later and was 24 cm wide and high This multiple-prime sequence of stimuli was like the stimulus sequence used by Jaśkowski et al (2003) except for the fact that four pairs of primes had been used in that study There were 400 trials in either block Order of blocks alternated between participants EEG recording and processing EEG was recorded from 10 scalp sites (Fz, Cz, Pz, Oz, C3, C4, P7, P8, PO7, PO8) with Ag/AgCl electrodes positioned in an elastic cap (FMS, Munich) An electrode at the nose served as common reference, a forehead electrode as ground Electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded for controlling intrusion of ocular potentials into the EEG from above and below the right eye and from the outer canthi of both eyes EEG and EOG were amplified from 003 Hz to 100 Hz and stored at 250 Hz per channel by a BrainAmp amplifier Off-line, data were low-pass filtered at 20 Hz, segmented from 200 ms before onset of the main stimulus to 900 ms afterwards, and edited for artifacts (rejecting trials with zero lines, followed by correcting ocular artifacts, followed by rejecting tridiamond diamond prime or main 13 ms 67 ms 200 ms gap-square 1 st prime 2 nd prime main or 13 ms 27 ms 13 ms 27 ms 200 ms Figure 2 Stimuli used in the present experiment Targets were in different blocks either diamonds or squares with gaps, in random order either on the left side of the main stimulus (as in the figure) or on the right side In a random half of the trials, stimuli were preceded by primes, which always consisted of pairs of non-target squares 22

5 als with voltage differences within the trial of more than 200 µv or voltage steps from one data-point to the next of more than 20 µv) The first 100 ms (200 ms 100 ms before onset of the main stimulus) were used as baseline To obtain averaged contralateral-ipsilateral difference potentials separately for each condition and participant, data were averaged across artifact-free trials in which participants correctly responded, separately for trials with left- and right-side targets Next, the contra-ipsilateral difference was formed in either average (eg, for PO7 and PO8: PO8 PO7 for the left-side average and PO7 PO8 for the right-side average) and these two differences were averaged (termed PO7 PO8 ) Grand means over participants were calculated for illustrating the results Data analysis Response times were measured by using the response-triggered markers set by the control program onto the EEG recording and referring these values to target onset Mean response times were calculated as averages across correct responses Percentages of correct responses were determined as ratios of correct responses relative to all trials Repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were computed with two 2-level factors: Stimulus (gap-square vs diamond) and Prime (absent, present) For an analysis of the PO7-PO8 waveforms averaged for each participant and condition, mean amplitudes were formed over successive 20 ms windows from target onset to 600 ms afterwards Each window s activity was analyzed with an ANOVA with the same design as for behavior (2 stimuli 2 prime conditions) The analysis first focused on whether activity significantly deviated from baseline across all four stimulus and prime conditions This comprehensive analysis was extended by t-tests for deviation from zero, separately for each of the four conditions Then, effects of the Stimulus and Prime factors were analyzed In addition to the 20 ms windows, peaks of the deflections were determined, as described in the Results, and their latencies were submitted to the Stimulus Prime ANOVA Finally, in addition to the PO7-PO8 waveforms, nose-referenced waveforms of the visual evoked potential recorded at PO8 were analyzed and compared to the PO7-PO8 waveforms Degrees of freedom in ANOVAs were 1/9 throughout } 10 µv + un un } 10 µv ms ms ms ms Figure 3 Grand means (N = 10 participants) of contra-ipsilateral waveforms recorded from PO7 PO8 Upper panels are from blocks with diamonds as targets, lower panels from blocks with gap-squares Left panels are from un trials, right panels from trials where the main stimuli were preceded by primes (prime onset at -80 ms) x-axes denote milliseconds from onset of main stimuli (0 ms), with small ticks every 20 ms y-axes are in microvolts, with negative polarity contralateral to position of target shape plotted upwards and ticks in 1 µv intervals Gray shadings denote time windows where deviation from baseline was significant as a main effect across all four panels Grey shades in the horizontal bars denote time windows where deviation from baseline was significant for the waveform depicted in the particular panel 23

6 } 10 µv + prime effect un ms ms } 10 µv + stimulus effect un ms ms Figure 4 Rearrangement of the waveforms depicted in Figure 3, in two ways In the upper panels, and un waveforms are overlaid (left and right panels of Figure 3) Gray shadings denote time windows where the main effect of prime was significant across stimuli Gray shades in the horizontal bars denote time windows where and un waveforms differed in the particular panel In the lower panels, waveforms are overlaid from diamonds and gap-squares (upper and lower panels of Figure 3) Gray shadings denote time windows where the main effect of stimulus was significant across and un waveforms Gray shades in the horizontal bars denote time windows where waveforms differed between diamonds and gap-squares in the particular panel x-axes denote milliseconds from onset of main stimuli (0 ms), with small ticks every 20 ms y-axes are in microvolts, with negative polarity contralateral to position of target shape plotted upwards and ticks in 1 µv intervals Results Behavior Mean response time was 386 ms and mean error rate was 57% Primed responses were 15 ms faster than un responses, F = 123, p = 007, without difference between the two stimuli (Prime Stimulus: F = 15, ns; main effect of Stimulus: F = 06, ns) The faster responses after primes were more error-prone, with 34% more errors committed in than un responses (F = 108, p = 009) Thus, the non-informative primes obviously lowered the criterion for responding Further, diamonds led to 32% more errors than gap-squares (F = 102, p = 01) Apparently, diamonds and squares with their complex and equal inner contours were harder to discriminate from each other than were thin gap-squares from squares The interaction was not significant (F = 04) EEG potentials Grand means of the C-I difference waveforms at PO7 PO8 are displayed in Figure 3 Windows in which the ANOVA s constant term differed from zero are illustrated in Figure 3 by gray background shading underlying the waveforms (equal for all four waveforms), and windows where t-tests displayed deviations from baseline for a given condition are marked by shading in the horizontal bars displayed beneath each waveform As is evident, there was an early contralateral positivity from 60 ms to 100 ms (activity in the two windows ms and ms: F 111, p 009) Then, three periods of contralateral negativity occurred: the first one at ms (F 124, p 007), which was followed after a plateau phase ( ms: F 92, p 01) by a second, longest period at ms ( ms: F 492, p 001; ms: F = 189, p = 002), and a third one at about ms ( ms: F = 74, p = 02), with t-tests being significant in this third 24

7 period for diamonds only In shorthand notation, the early contralateral positivity will be called P1pc, and the following three periods of negativity will be called N1pc, N2pc, and N3pc The following effects of priming were noted in the ANOVAs (see top panels of Figure 4, where the and un waveforms from Figure 3 have been overlaid) P1pc was larger with than with un diamonds ( ms: main effect of Prime: F = 57, p = 04; Prime Stimulus: F = 50, p = 052) delaying the rise of N1pc ( ms: main effect of Prime: F = 75, p = 02) The following Prime Stimulus interactions ( ms: F = 59, p = 04; ms: F = 287, p < 001) on the one hand indicated this delayed rise of N1pc with diamonds (thereby producing more negativity for than for un diamonds in this period) and, on the other hand, indicated that N1pc was larger for un than for gap-squares A second effect of priming, on N2pc, started at 160 ms and lasted until 240 ms: N2pc was larger with stimuli, due to an earlier rising or larger amplitudes (main effect of Prime: ms: F 246, p 001; ms: F = 132, p = 005; for gapsquares also at ms: Prime Stimulus: F = 59, p = 04) Finally, of less interest, there was a main effect of Prime at ms (F = 140, p = 005) Thus, there were two major effects of priming: P1pc was larger, affecting the following N1pc, and N2pc rose earlier and/or was larger There were only a few though possibly interesting differences between stimuli, as illustrated in the bottom panels of Figure 4 N1pc was larger with gap-squares than with diamonds, with un stimuli at ms already (resolving the above-noted Prime Stimulus interaction to an effect of stimulus in un trials, F = 155, p = 003) and generally at ms (main effect of Stimulus: F = 65, p = 03) In contrast, N3pc was larger with diamonds than with gap-squares (main effect of Stimulus: F = 127, p = 006 at ms; F = 55, p = 04 at ms), and at ms more so when (Stimulus Prime: F = 136, p = 005) No effects of Stimulus were significant during the large N2pc (different from what inspection of Figure 4 might suggest) 2 To obtain measures that are more sensitive to differences in timing, peak latencies of the described components were determined P1pc was defined as the most positive peak ms post-stimulus It was earlier with diamonds (83 ms) than with gap-squares (94 ms; Stimulus: F = 97, p = 01) The first negative peak, N1pc, had its mean latency at 135 ms (range ms) without significant differences between prime conditions and stimuli N2pc varied between 185 ms and 280 ms and peaked earlier with than with un stimuli (226 ms vs 240 ms; Prime: F = 67, p = 03) N3pc varied between 290 ms and 470 ms and peaked earlier with gap-squares than with diamonds (352 ms vs 375 ms; Stimulus: F = 73, p = 02) } 25 µv + un un N1 N2 N3 N1 to prime N1 } 25 µv + N2 N3 P1 P1 P1 to prime ms ms N1 N2 N3 N1 N1 to prime N2 P1 N3 P ms P1 to prime ms Figure 5 Grand means recorded from PO8 (gray), overlaid on grand means of contra-ipsilateral waveshapes recorded from PO7 PO8, separately for each condition Conditions are arranged like in Figure 3 Tick marks on y-axes denote 25 µv intervals (different from Figure 3) 25

8 The only effect on amplitudes of these peak measures was on N1pc amplitude which was largest with un gap-squares (Prime Stimulus: F = 94, p = 01) The prime effect on P1pc amplitude with diamonds, which was significant with the mean-amplitude measure, only tended to significance with peak amplitude (p = 06 for main effect of Prime; p = 10 for the interaction of Prime Stimulus) No effect was significant on N2pc and N3pc peak amplitudes To provide some relationship between the C-I differences and the usual ERP waveforms, Figure 5 displays recordings from the PO8 site against the nose reference, together with the C-I differences The nose-referenced recordings show the usual visually evoked potentials, starting with the P1-N1 complex (Note that in trials, there is first a P1-N1 complex to the prime, and the relevant consecutive P1-N1 complex to the main stimulus is grossly altered, much more than the C-I waveforms) Comparing C-I differences to the nose-referenced recordings shows that all components of the C-I difference-waveform were roughly time-locked to some conventional component, but when looked at in detail there seemed to be relevant differences: P1pc roughly coincided with the P1 component evoked by the main stimuli But the match was better with squares, whereas with diamonds, P1pc occurred with the first, positive-going phase of P1, therefore peaked earlier than P1 N1pc coincided with the N1 component evoked by the main stimuli, except for un diamonds, where N1pc peaked earlier than N1 N2pc had a good match with some minor N2 component seen in the PO8 waveforms, except for un gap-squares where N2 occurred earlier N3pc was consistently aligned with some negative N3 peak Discussion We delineated the entire C-I difference waveform evoked by pairs of target and distracter stimuli presented left and right from fixation This C-I waveform indicates the extent of preference for the side of the relevant stimulus at any given moment In addition to the well-known N2pc component, three other deflections could be discerned, which we called P1pc, N1pc, and N3pc Of primary interest were differences between stimuli Attempting to resolve conflicts between previous results (Jaśkowski et al, 2002, 2003), we used pairs of diamonds and squares with octagonal inner contours in one block, and of gap-squares and simple squares in the other block Indeed, there was a difference between these pairs of well visible main stimuli (lower panels of Figure 4): Gap-square targets evoked larger C-I differences than diamond targets in the first negative deflection, N1pc, and smaller C-I differences in the final deflection, N3pc Since gap-squares were somewhat easier to identify than diamonds, as indicated by percentages of erroneous key-presses, this result suggests that stimuli that are easier to identify evoke more early and less late contralateral negativity than stimuli that are hard to identify The second independent variable was the presence or absence of neutral primes The major effects of primes were to enhance P1pc and to accelerate the rise of N2pc (upper panels of Figure 4) Few studies have so far noticed and described the P1pc, N1pc, and N3pc components The components will be discussed in the temporal order of their occurrence P1pc P1pc was well visible in the data of Jaśkowski et al (2002; see Figure 1, sloping arrow) but at that time we were uncertain how to interpret this effect, not being aware of previous occurrences of P1pc in published literature Yet actually a similar effect on P1 was already obtained in the ground-breaking study by Luck and Hillyard (1994) ascribed by those authors to neuronal dishabituation (because the target stimulus was less frequent than the distracters) rather than to relevance This view has more recently been resumed and elaborated by Kimura and colleagues (eg, Kimura, Katayama, & Murohashi, 2006, 2008) In line with this view, data displayed in Wauschkuhn et al (1998, their Figure 4) show a dissociation between P1pc and N2pc: In each trial, a gray circle was presented opposite to a colored circle, which was either red or blue In different blocks, either the gray circles or the colored circles were defined as targets N2pc was always negative contralateral to the relevant circle In contrast, P1pc was always (and significantly, as shown by present reanalysis of those data) positive contralateral to the colored circle, thus was affected either simply by color salience or by the change of color between trials (red or blue, whereas gray was presented in each trial) This is in line with Luck and Hillyard s (1994) dishabituation assumption The enhancing effect of neutral primes on P1pc in the present study is in line with those findings, reflecting the difference of the target shape from the preceding prime squares Based on this, it makes sense that P1pc after neutral primes in Jaśkowski et al (2002; see Figure 1) was specific to neutral primes, because when primes were congruent there was no change at all, and when primes were incongruent there was change on both sides, yielding a zero effect in the C-I difference N1pc N1pc, as noted in the Introduction, is, trivially, obtained when stimulation is unilateral (Valle-Inclán, 1996; Wascher et al, 2001; Wascher & Beste, 2010) but there are several papers where N1pc was reliably obtained with bilateral stimulation as well It appears instructive to compare the results of Praamstra and Plat (2001), Praamstra and Oostenveld (2003), van der Lubbe and Verleger (2002), and Experiment 3 of Wascher et al (2001), all of whom obtained a well-delimited N1pc, to the results of Wascher and Wauschkuhn (1996); van der Lubbe, Jaśkowski, Wauschkuhn, and Verleger (2001); and Experiment 1 of Wascher et al (2001) all of whom did not At first sight, this diffe-rence cannot be due to stimulus material and task, because all those studies used the same material (the letters A and B, presented left or right, opposite from fixation and a filler consisting of three horizontal lines) and the same task (left-right choice-responses to A vs B) Also at second sight, we did not find a variable that could account for these differences: The two types of results could be distinguished neither by speed of responses, nor by color, size, or eccentricity of stimuli; nor by features of the fixation cross or of screen background; nor by length of intertrial interval, nor by participants age 26

9 The present data suggest that N1pc is enhanced by particular stimuli (gap-squares in the present case) and may be better distinguished as a component of its own when there is no preceding prime, because N1pc formed a well-defined peak with un stimuli only, possibly because priming accelerated the N2pc, merging it with N1pc One might suspect that the biphasic form of N1pc and N2pc in the present grand averages is an artifact of averaging across participants: Possibly some have an N1pc, others an N2pc Yet, inspection of individual data unambiguously showed this biphasic structure (and even triphasic, including N3pc) as a prevailing pattern in many participants One might further suspect that this pattern in individuals is an artifact of averaging across trials: Possibly some trials have an N1pc, others an N2pc To tackle this suspicion, one may dichotomize data according to response times and expect that fast-response trials would be characterized by N1pc, slow-response trials by N2pc When we did this analysis in the present data (results not reported), this expectation was not borne out, nor was it the case when we reanalyzed some older data (from Wascher et al, 2001) Praamstra and Oostenveld (2003) assumed, as did Jaśkowski et al (2003), that N1pc was due to asymmetries of their stimulus material, as a low-level exogenous effect In line with this, participants strategy induced by frequency variation of congruent and incongruent primes affected N2pc but not N1pc in the study by Jaśkowski et al (2003; the effect is displayed above, as the difference between bold and thin lines in the lower part of Figure 1) On the other hand, while certainly being a necessary condition for N1pc to emerge, physical differences do not explain why N1pc has the same polarity as N2pc: If it was due to physical difference only, the negative deflection might be evoked on the other side as well Therefore relevance appears to be a decisive factor Thus, at present, the best guess is that N1pc may be affected both by exogenous asymmetries and by factors that also affect N2pc N3pc N3pc did not show up as distinctly in the present data as N1pc and N2pc However, it reliably differed from zero, differed between stimuli (being larger with diamonds than with gap-squares) and furthermore could be well distinguished in many participants individual averages, forming a triphasic pattern with N1pc and N2pc It seems that latency variation between participants made this component appear flatter in the grand mean N3pc can also be unambiguously seen in Experiment 1 of Wascher et al (2001, their Figures 2 and 3, collapsed data ) In one earlier paper of ours, Wauschkuhn et al (1998), N3pc was analyzed (termed L400, by its peak latency) and found to be more selective than N2pc (L250): Whereas N2pc was always evoked by relevant stimuli, N3pc was only evoked if these stimuli were also targets of the saccade defined by the relevant stimulus (similarly: Mazza, Turatto, Umiltà, & Eimer, 2007; Seiss et al, 2009) N3pc might have been overlooked in previous data, often being pulled below baseline by its riding on the posterior contralateral positivity that is evoked by manual key-presses, for example, in Wascher and Wauschkuhn (1996, their Figure 2) N1pc, N2pc, N3pc N2pc probably reflects attentional capture by relevant stimuli (Eimer & Kiss, 2008; Lien, Ruthruff, Goodin, & Remington, 2008; Seiss et al, 2009) The present data suggest that this capture by and selection of relevant stimuli might proceed in several cycles The differences that were obtained between gap-square and diamond stimuli may mean that stimuli that are easier to identify evoke more N1pc and less N3pc than stimuli that are hard to identify Thus, a single pass may be sufficient if stimulation is unilateral, producing N1pc, but multiple passes may be needed if the decision has to be made on the basis of features of bilateral stimuli, producing the periodic structure of N1pc, N2pc, and N3pc FOOTNotes 1 The assumption that this component reflected target-elicited activity was based on the observation that its peak was constant with respect to target onset (black x-axes) and shifted with respect to prime onset (gray x-axes) by the difference of about 80 ms between primetarget interval conditions depicted in the upper versus middle panel 2 One referee of this paper argued that differences between stimuli might as well reflect differences between the two different prime sequences: One prime with the diamond-square stimuli, two primes with the gap-squares Two points can be made about this argument: First, it appears reasonable to assume that these differences in prime sequence would not play any role in no-prime trials But the early N1pc difference between stimuli was more marked in un than in trials Second, it appears plausible (though admittedly not compelling) that such effects of the physical prime structure should produce early effects if any Thus, it appears not probable that the late N3pc effects were produced by those differences Author Note Piotr Jaśkowski, our friend, colleague, and teacher, passed away on January 6, 2011 Blessed be his memory In writing this paper, Rolf Verleger was supported by grant no Ve110/15-1 from Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, awarded as part of the package proposal Neuro-Cognitive Mechanisms of Conscious and Unconscious Visual Perception References Dell Acqua, R, Sessa, P, Jolicœur, P, & Robitaille, N (2006) Spatial attention freezes during the attentional blink Psychophysiology, 43, Eimer, M (1996) The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, Eimer, M, & Kiss, M (2008) Involuntary attentional capture is determined by task set: Evidence from event-related brain potentials Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, Fan, J, McCandliss, B D, Sommer, T, Raz, A, & Posner, M I (2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14,

10 Gao, Z, Yin, J, Xu, H, Shui, R, & Shen, M (2011) Tracking object number or information load in visual working memory: Revisiting the cognitive implication of contralateral delay activity Biological Psychology, 87, Hickey, C, Di Lollo, V, & McDonald, J J (2009) Electrophysiological indices of target and distractor processing in visual search Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, Hopf, J-M, Luck, S J, Boelmans, M A, Schoenfeld, M A, Boehler, C N, Rieger, J, & Heinze, H-J (2006) The neural site of attention matches the spatial scale of perception The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, Jaśkowski, P, Skalska, B, & Verleger, R (2003) How the self controls its automatic pilot when processing subliminal information Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, Jaśkowski, P, van der Lubbe, R H J, Schlotterbeck, E, & Verleger, R (2002) Traces left on visual selective attention by stimuli that are not consciously identified Psychological Science,13, Jolicœur, P, Sessa, P, Dell Acqua, R, & Robitaille, N (2006) On the control of visual spatial attention: Evidence from human electrophysiology Psychological Research, 70, Kimura, M, Katayama, J, & Murohashi, H (2006) Probabilityindependent and -dependent ERPs reflecting visual change detection Psychophysiology, 43, Kimura, M, Katayama, J, & Murohashi, H (2008) Involvement of memory-comparison-based change detection in visual distraction Psychophysiology, 45, Klotz, W, & Wolff, P (1995) The effect of a masked stimulus on the response to the masking stimulus Psychological Research, 58, Lien, M-C, Ruthruff, E, Goodin, Z, & Remington, R W (2008) Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: Converging evidence from event-related potentials Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, Luck, S J, Fan, S, & Hillyard, S A (1993) Attention-related modulation of sensory-evoked brain activity in a visual search task Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, Luck, S J, & Hillyard, S A (1994) Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search Psychophysiology, 31, Mazza, V, & Caramazza, A (2011) Temporal brain dynamics of multiple object processing: The flexibility of individuation PLoS One, 6, e17453 Mazza, V, Turatto, M, Umiltà, C, & Eimer, M (2007) Attentional selection and identification of visual objects are reflected by distinct electrophysiological responses Experimental Brain Research, 181, Oostenveld, R, Stegeman, D F, Praamstra, P, & van Oosterom, A (2003) Brain symmetry and topographic analysis of lateralized event-related potentials Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, Praamstra, P, & Plat, F M (2001) Failed suppression of direct visuomotor activation in Parkinson s disease Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, Praamstra, P, & Oostenveld, R (2003) Attention and movementrelated motor cortex activation: A high-density EEG study of spatial stimulus-response compatibility Cognitive Brain Research, 16, Seiss, E, Kiss, M, & Eimer, M (2009) Does focused endogenous attention prevent attentional capture in pop-out visual search? Psychophysiology, 46, Valle-Inclán, F (1996) The locus of interference in the Simon effect: An ERP study Biological Psychology, 43, van der Lubbe, R H J, Jaśkowski, P, Wauschkuhn, B, & Verleger, R (2001) Influence of time pressure in a simple detection task, a choice-by-location task, and the Simon task Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, van der Lubbe, R H J, Keuss, P J G, & Stoffels, E-J (1996) Threefold effect of peripheral precues: Alertness, orienting, and response tendencies Acta Psychologica, 94, van der Lubbe, R H J, & Verleger, R (2002) Aging and the Simon task Psychophysiology, 39, Verleger, R, & Jaśkowski, P (2007) Disentangling neural processing of masked and masking stimulus by means of eventrelated contralateral: Ipsilateral differences of EEG potentials, 3, Verleger, R, Śmigasiewicz, K, & Möller, F (2011) Mechanisms underlying the left visual-field advantage in the dual stream RSVP task: Evidence from N2pc, P3, and distractor-evoked VEPs Psychophysiology, 48, Vogel, E K, & Machizawa, M G (2004) Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity Nature, 428, Vogel, E K, McCollough, A W, & Machizawa, M G (2005) Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory Nature, 438, Wascher, E, & Beste, C (2010) Tuning perceptual competition Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, Wascher, E, Schatz, U, Kuder, T, & Verleger, R (2001) Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, Wascher, E, & Wauschkuhn, B (1996) The interaction of stimulusand response-related processes measured by event-related lateralisations of the EEG Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, Wauschkuhn, B, Verleger, R, Wascher, E, Klostermann, W, Burk, M, Heide, W, & Kömpf, D (1998) Lateralised human cortical activity for shifting visuospatial attention and initiating saccades Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, Woodman, G F, & Luck, S J (1999) Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search Nature, 400, Received Accepted

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:296 301 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0353-4 BRIEF REPORT Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Risa Sawaki & Steven J. Luck Published online: 20 December 2012 #

More information

The Roles of Feature-Specific Task Set and Bottom-Up Salience in Attentional Capture: An ERP Study

The Roles of Feature-Specific Task Set and Bottom-Up Salience in Attentional Capture: An ERP Study Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 5, 1316 1328 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015872 The Roles of Feature-Specific

More information

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 827 831. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research BRIEF REPT Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the

More information

The initial stage of visual selection is controlled by top-down task set: new ERP evidence

The initial stage of visual selection is controlled by top-down task set: new ERP evidence Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:113 122 DOI 10.3758/s13414-010-0008-3 The initial stage of visual selection is controlled by top-down task set: new ERP evidence Ulrich Ansorge & Monika Kiss & Franziska

More information

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture Psychophysiology, 53 (2016), 650 662. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright VC 2016 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12615 The role of top-down spatial attention

More information

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (4), 951-962 doi:10.3758/app.72.4.951 Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

More information

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Clayton Hickey 1,2, Vincent Di Lollo 2, and John J. McDonald 2 Abstract & Attentional selection of a target presented among

More information

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1 Current Biology 24, 193 198, January 20, 2014 ª2014 The Authors http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.001 Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects Report Martin

More information

Contingent Attentional Capture by Top-Down Control Settings: Converging Evidence From Event-Related Potentials

Contingent Attentional Capture by Top-Down Control Settings: Converging Evidence From Event-Related Potentials Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8, Vol. 3, No. 3, 59 53 Copyright 8 by the American Psychological Association 9-153/8/$1. DOI: 1.137/9-153.3.3.59 Contingent Attentional

More information

Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology

Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology 1 Running head: FIXED-FEATURE SEARCH Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology Ali Jannati, John M. Gaspar, and John J. McDonald Department

More information

to the concept of direct parameter specification attentional capture in near-threshold priming of motor reactions ABSTRACT

to the concept of direct parameter specification attentional capture in near-threshold priming of motor reactions ABSTRACT 2006 volume 2 no 1 47-59 Advances in Cognitive Psychology The role of direct parameter specification and attentional capture in near-threshold priming of motor reactions Blandyna Skalska 1, Piotr Jaśkowski

More information

All set, indeed! N2pc components reveal simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colours

All set, indeed! N2pc components reveal simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colours Running head: Control of multiple-colour search All set, indeed! N2pc components reveal simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colours Anna Grubert and Martin Eimer Department of

More information

An Electrophysiological Study of Attention Capture by Salience: Does Rarity Enable Capture?

An Electrophysiological Study of Attention Capture by Salience: Does Rarity Enable Capture? An Electrophysiological Study of Attention Capture by Salience: Does Rarity Enable Capture? Noesen, B., Lien, M. C., & Ruthruff, E. (2014). An electrophysiological study of attention capture by salience:

More information

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL Visual working memory load disrupts template-guided attentional selection during visual search Nick Berggren* and Martin Eimer Department

More information

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Running Head: Visual Working Memory and the CDA Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Nick Berggren and

More information

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search Steven Phillips (steve@ni.aist.go.jp) Neuroscience Research Institute (AIST), Tsukuba Central 2, 1-1-1 Umezono, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8568

More information

Attention selection, distractor suppression and N2pc

Attention selection, distractor suppression and N2pc cortex 45 (2009) 879 890 available at www.sciencedirect.com journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Research report Attention selection, distractor suppression and N2pc Veronica Mazza a,b, *,

More information

Top-down control of audiovisual search by bimodal search templates

Top-down control of audiovisual search by bimodal search templates Psychophysiology, 50 (013), 996 1009. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright 013 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.1086 Top-down control of audiovisual search by

More information

Redundancy gains in pop-out visual search are determined by top-down task set: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

Redundancy gains in pop-out visual search are determined by top-down task set: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence Journal of Vision (2011) 11(14):10, 1 10 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/14/10 1 Redundancy gains in pop-out visual search are determined by top-down task set: Behavioral and electrophysiological

More information

ERP INDICES OF ATTENTIONAL DISENGAGEMENT AND FILTERING FOLLOWING REFLEXIVE ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE. Cassie Barasch Ford. Chapel Hill

ERP INDICES OF ATTENTIONAL DISENGAGEMENT AND FILTERING FOLLOWING REFLEXIVE ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE. Cassie Barasch Ford. Chapel Hill ERP INDICES OF ATTENTIONAL DISENGAGEMENT AND FILTERING FOLLOWING REFLEXIVE ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE Cassie Barasch Ford A thesis submitted to the faculty of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill in

More information

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Nick Berggren and Martin Eimer Abstract During the retention of

More information

Mental Rotation affects Spatial Attention: an EEG study. Introduction

Mental Rotation affects Spatial Attention: an EEG study. Introduction How does Mental Rotation affect T2 Spatial Attention in a Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm: Behavioural and Neurophysiological Measures Merel M. Pannebakker, Wessel van Dam, Guido P. H. Band, K.

More information

Attentional and anatomical considerations for the representation of simple stimuli in visual short-term memory: evidence from human electrophysiology

Attentional and anatomical considerations for the representation of simple stimuli in visual short-term memory: evidence from human electrophysiology Psychological Research (2009) 73:222 232 DOI 10.1007/s00426-008-0214-y ORIGINAL ARTICLE Attentional and anatomical considerations for the representation of simple stimuli in visual short-term memory: evidence

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

Perceptual Grouping and Visual Enumeration

Perceptual Grouping and Visual Enumeration Perceptual Grouping and Visual Enumeration Veronica Mazza 1,2 *, Alfonso Caramazza 1,3 1 Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy, 2 Department of Cognitive Sciences

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Item and Category-Based Attentional Control During Search for Real-World Objects: Can You Find the Pants Among the Pans? Rebecca Nako,

More information

The control of attentional target selection in a colour/colour conjunction task

The control of attentional target selection in a colour/colour conjunction task Atten Percept Psychophys (2016) 78:2383 2396 DOI 10.3758/s13414-016-1168-6 The control of attentional target selection in a colour/colour conjunction task Nick Berggren 1 & Martin Eimer 1 Published online:

More information

What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010

What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010 What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010 You are trying to determine if a small amplitude signal is a consistent marker of a neural process. How might you design an experiment to

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 834-839 Brief Reports Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex Jeffrey S. Johnson, Geoffrey F. Woodman, Elsie Braun, and Steven

More information

Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography

Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography by Allison Elisabeth Connell A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction

More information

Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects

Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects Roy Luria 1, Paola Sessa 1, Alex Gotler 2, Pierre Jolicœur 3, and Roberto DellʼAcqua 1 Abstract Does the capacity of visual short-term memory

More information

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set In press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set Timothy J. Wright 1, Walter

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

International Journal of Psychophysiology

International Journal of Psychophysiology International Journal of Psychophysiology 86 (2012) 152 159 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect International Journal of Psychophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho

More information

Human Variation in Overriding Attentional Capture

Human Variation in Overriding Attentional Capture 8726 The Journal of Neuroscience, July 8, 2009 29(27):8726 8733 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Human Variation in Overriding Attentional Capture Keisuke Fukuda and Edward K. Vogel Department of Psychology,

More information

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task Psychophysiology, 44 (2007), 913 917. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2007 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00561.x BRIEF REPORT Reward prediction

More information

Disentangling neural processing of masked and masking stimulus by means of event-related contralateral ipsilateral differences of EEG potentials

Disentangling neural processing of masked and masking stimulus by means of event-related contralateral ipsilateral differences of EEG potentials 2007 volume 3 no 12 193210 Advances in Cognitive Psychology Disentangling neural processing of masked and masking stimulus by means of eventrelated contralateral ipsilateral differences of EEG potentials

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article Research Article DISSOCIATIONS AMONG ATTENTION, PERCEPTION, AND AWARENESS DURING OBJECT-SUBSTITUTION MASKING Geoffrey F. Woodman 1 and Steven J. Luck 2 1 Vanderbilt University and 2 University of Iowa

More information

The speed of voluntary and priority-driven shifts of visual attention. *Corresponding Author. Malet Street, London, WC1E 7HX, UK

The speed of voluntary and priority-driven shifts of visual attention. *Corresponding Author. Malet Street, London, WC1E 7HX, UK The speed of voluntary and priority-driven shifts of visual attention Michael Jenkins 1 *, Anna Grubert, & Martin Eimer 1 *Corresponding Author 1 Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University

More information

The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations

The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations Franka Roorda First supervisor: Rob van der Lubbe Second supervisor: Suzanne Vosslamber

More information

COMPETITION AND SELECTIVITY IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM: EVIDENCE FROM EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIALS

COMPETITION AND SELECTIVITY IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM: EVIDENCE FROM EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIALS COMPETITION AND SELECTIVITY IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM: EVIDENCE FROM EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIALS A Dissertation Presented to The Academic Faculty by Matthew R. Hilimire In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

More information

Lateralisation of the Event-Related Brain Potential Reveals Neural Correlates of Attention, Distractor Suppression, and Visual Short-Term Memory

Lateralisation of the Event-Related Brain Potential Reveals Neural Correlates of Attention, Distractor Suppression, and Visual Short-Term Memory Lateralisation of the Event-Related Brain Potential Reveals Neural Correlates of Attention, Distractor Suppression, and Visual Short-Term Memory Paul Corballis School of Psychology and Centre for Brain

More information

Do Rare Stimuli Evoke Large P3s by Being Unexpected? A Comparison of Oddball Effects Between Standard-Oddball and Prediction-Oddball Tasks

Do Rare Stimuli Evoke Large P3s by Being Unexpected? A Comparison of Oddball Effects Between Standard-Oddball and Prediction-Oddball Tasks Do Rare Stimuli Evoke Large P3s by Being Unexpected? A Comparison of Oddball Effects Between Standard-Oddball and Prediction-Oddball Tasks Rolf Verleger 1,2 and Kamila Śmigasiewicz 1 1 Department of Neurology,

More information

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source

More information

When memory is not enough: Electrophysiological evidence for goal-dependent use. of working memory representations in guiding visual attention

When memory is not enough: Electrophysiological evidence for goal-dependent use. of working memory representations in guiding visual attention When memory is not enough: Electrophysiological evidence for goal-dependent use of working memory representations in guiding visual attention Nancy B. Carlisle and Geoffrey F. Woodman Vanderbilt University

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

Electrophysiological Markers of Visual Dimension Changes and Response Changes

Electrophysiological Markers of Visual Dimension Changes and Response Changes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2008, Vol. 34, No. 3, 531 542 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.531

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article. MECHANISMS OF PRIMING BY MASKED STIMULI: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article. MECHANISMS OF PRIMING BY MASKED STIMULI: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials Research Article MECHANISMS OF PRIMING BY MASKED STIMULI: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials Humboldt-University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany Abstract A metacontrast procedure was combined with

More information

Attentional capture during visual search is attenuated by target predictability: Evidence from the N2pc, Pd, and topographic segmentation

Attentional capture during visual search is attenuated by target predictability: Evidence from the N2pc, Pd, and topographic segmentation Psychophysiology, 50 (2013), 422 430. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2013 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12019 Attentional capture during visual search

More information

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Submitted to Current Biology Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Anna Plodowski, Rachel Swainson, Georgina M. Jackson, Chris Rorden and Stephen R. Jackson School of Psychology

More information

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Biological Psychology 66 (2004) 1 20 The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Bettina Forster, Martin Eimer School of

More information

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention Tineke Grent- t-jong 1,2, Marty G. Woldorff 1,3* PLoS BIOLOGY 1 Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University,

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy DOI 10.1007/s00426-014-0611-3 ORIGINAL ARTICLE The reversal of perceptual and motor compatibility effects differs qualitatively between metacontrast and random-line masks Anne Atas Estibaliz San Anton

More information

Context homogeneity facilitates both distractor inhibition and target enhancement

Context homogeneity facilitates both distractor inhibition and target enhancement Journal of Vision (2013) 13(3):11, 1 12 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/11 1 Context homogeneity facilitates both distractor inhibition and target enhancement Experimental and Biological Psychology,

More information

Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR, USA b University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR, USA b University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [National Cheng Kung University] On: 24 January 2 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 79473938] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

In the first section, Introduction, we present our experimental design.

In the first section, Introduction, we present our experimental design. Occipital and left temporal EEG correlates of phenomenal consciousness Abstract In the first section, Introduction, we present our experimental design. In the second section, we characterize the grand

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF

AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF Birken Noesen for the degree of Master of Arts in Interdisciplinary Studies in Psychology, Psychology, and Gerontology presented on May 13, 2015. Title: An Electrophysiological

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Attentional Blink Paradigm

Attentional Blink Paradigm Attentional Blink Paradigm ATTENTIONAL BLINK 83 ms stimulus onset asychrony between all stimuli B T D A 3 N P Z F R K M R N Lag 3 Target 1 Target 2 After detection of a target in a rapid stream of visual

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential Title of Thesis Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential 2014 September Yang Weiping The Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology (Doctor s Course)

More information

Title change detection system in the visu

Title change detection system in the visu Title Attention switching function of mem change detection system in the visu Author(s) Kimura, Motohiro; Katayama, Jun'ich Citation International Journal of Psychophys Issue Date 2008-02 DOI Doc URLhttp://hdl.handle.net/2115/33891

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Clinical Neurophysiology 120 (2009) 1087 1095 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Clinical Neurophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Effects of attentional filtering demands

More information

Quick Minds Slowed Down Martens, Alexander; Korucuoglu, Ozlem; Smid, H; Nieuwenstein, Mark

Quick Minds Slowed Down Martens, Alexander; Korucuoglu, Ozlem; Smid, H; Nieuwenstein, Mark University of Groningen Quick Minds Slowed Down Martens, Alexander; Korucuoglu, Ozlem; Smid, H; Nieuwenstein, Mark Published in: PLoS ONE DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013509 IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised

More information

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences from the N170 adaptation effect Xiao-hua Cao 1, Chao Li 1, Carl M Gaspar 2, Bei Jiang 1 1. Department of Psychology,Zhejiang Normal

More information

Nature Methods: doi: /nmeth Supplementary Figure 1. Activity in turtle dorsal cortex is sparse.

Nature Methods: doi: /nmeth Supplementary Figure 1. Activity in turtle dorsal cortex is sparse. Supplementary Figure 1 Activity in turtle dorsal cortex is sparse. a. Probability distribution of firing rates across the population (notice log scale) in our data. The range of firing rates is wide but

More information

Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene

Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene Marissa L. Gamble and Marty G. Woldorff Abstract To make sense of our dynamic and complex auditory environment, we must be able

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Sleep Deprivation and Selective Attention An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Logan T. Trujillo, PhD 1 ; Steve Kornguth,

More information

Title of Thesis. Studies on Mechanisms of Visual and Auditory Attention in Temporal or Spatial Cueing Paradigm

Title of Thesis. Studies on Mechanisms of Visual and Auditory Attention in Temporal or Spatial Cueing Paradigm Title of Thesis Studies on Mechanisms of Visual and Auditory Attention in Temporal or Spatial Cueing Paradigm 2015 September Xiaoyu Tang The Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology (Doctor s

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction

More information

Key questions about attention

Key questions about attention Key questions about attention How does attention affect behavioral performance? Can attention affect the appearance of things? How does spatial and feature-based attention affect neuronal responses in

More information

Perceptual load modulates the processing of distractors presented at task-irrelevant locations during the attentional blink

Perceptual load modulates the processing of distractors presented at task-irrelevant locations during the attentional blink Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (8), 2106-2114 doi:10.3758/app.72.8.2106 Perceptual load modulates the processing of distractors presented at task-irrelevant locations during the attentional

More information

Two-object attentional interference depends on attentional set

Two-object attentional interference depends on attentional set International Journal of Psychophysiology 53 (2004) 127 134 www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho Two-object attentional interference depends on attentional set Maykel López, Valia Rodríguez*, Mitchell Valdés-Sosa

More information

The observation of contingent negative variation with reaction times in non-human. primates. Nam Soo Kim. Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty of the

The observation of contingent negative variation with reaction times in non-human. primates. Nam Soo Kim. Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty of the The observation of contingent negative variation with reaction times in non-human primates By Nam Soo Kim Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Vanderbilt University in Partial fulfillment

More information

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Cognitive Brain Research 18 (2004) 255 272 Research report Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Audrey Duarte a, *, Charan Ranganath

More information

Delayed working memory consolidation during the attentional blink

Delayed working memory consolidation during the attentional blink Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2002, 9 (4), 739-743 Delayed working memory consolidation during the attentional blink EDWARD K. VOGEL and STEVEN J. LUCK University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa After the detection

More information

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) Introduction 17 subjects were tested to measure effects of a Brain.fm focus session on cognition. With 4 additional subjects, we recorded EEG data during baseline and while

More information

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Rui Cao (caorui.beilia@gmail.com) Thomas A. Busey (busey@indiana.edu) Robert M. Nosofsky (nosofsky@indiana.edu) Richard

More information

Selection history alters attentional filter settings persistently and beyond top-down control

Selection history alters attentional filter settings persistently and beyond top-down control Psychophysiology, 54 (07), 736 754. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright VC 07 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 0./psyp.830 Selection history alters attentional filter settings

More information

FEATURE ARTICLE Top-Down Dimensional Weight Set Determines the Capture of Visual Attention: Evidence from the PCN Component

FEATURE ARTICLE Top-Down Dimensional Weight Set Determines the Capture of Visual Attention: Evidence from the PCN Component Cerebral Cortex July 2012;22:1554 1563 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr231 Advance Access publication September 7, 2011 FEATURE ARTICLE Top-Down Dimensional Weight Set Determines the Capture of Visual Attention:

More information

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003)

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003) Cognition 97 (2005) 89 97 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003) Filip Van Opstal a, Bert Reynvoet b, Tom Verguts a, * a Department

More information

Consciousness and Cognition

Consciousness and Cognition Consciousness and Cognition 21 (2012) 961 968 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Consciousness and Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/concog Short Communication The P3

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Early Selection Versus Late Correction: Age-Related Differences in Controlling Working Memory Contents

Early Selection Versus Late Correction: Age-Related Differences in Controlling Working Memory Contents Psychology and Aging 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 31, No. 4, 000 0882-7974/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000103 Early Selection Versus Late Correction: Age-Related Differences

More information

Follow this and additional works at: Part of the Neurosciences Commons

Follow this and additional works at:   Part of the Neurosciences Commons University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Open Access Dissertations 9-2012 Gradients and Ranges of Visually Selective Attention Based on Location, Objects, Color, and Size: Gradients

More information

Selective manipulation of target identification demands in visual search: The role of stimulus contrast in CDA activations

Selective manipulation of target identification demands in visual search: The role of stimulus contrast in CDA activations Journal of Vision (2013) 13(3):23, 1 13 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/23 1 Selective manipulation of target identification demands in visual search: The role of stimulus contrast in CDA activations

More information

Neural Correlates of Auditory Attention in an Exogenous Orienting Task. A Thesis. Presented to

Neural Correlates of Auditory Attention in an Exogenous Orienting Task. A Thesis. Presented to Neural Correlates of Auditory Attention in an Exogenous Orienting Task A Thesis Presented to The Division of Philosophy, Religion, Psychology, and Linguistics Reed College In Partial Fulfillment of the

More information

Deconstructing my thesis topic

Deconstructing my thesis topic Deconstructing my thesis topic The central claim of interest to me is this: The meaning of word or sentence depends, at least in part, on the automatic activation of sensory-motor representations. In the

More information

The distractor positivity (Pd) signals lowering of attentional priority: Evidence from event-related potentials and individual differences

The distractor positivity (Pd) signals lowering of attentional priority: Evidence from event-related potentials and individual differences Psychophysiology, 51 (014), 685 696. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright 014 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.115 The distractor positivity (Pd) signals lowering

More information

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study Armita Faghani Jadidi, Raheleh Davoodi, Mohammad Hassan Moradi Department of Biomedical Engineering Amirkabir University

More information

Does a contralateral decrease in alpha power during the orienting phase predict visual. awareness? Hendrik Bartenbach, s

Does a contralateral decrease in alpha power during the orienting phase predict visual. awareness? Hendrik Bartenbach, s Does a contralateral decrease in alpha power during the orienting phase predict visual awareness? Hendrik Bartenbach, s1221086 University of Twente, September 2017 Department Cognitive Psychology and Ergonomics

More information

Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming

Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming Biological Psychology 64 (2003) 7 26 Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming Martin Eimer a,, Friederike Schlaghecken b a School of Psychology, Birkbeck College, University of London,

More information

Pushing around the Locus of Selection: Evidence for the Flexible-selection Hypothesis

Pushing around the Locus of Selection: Evidence for the Flexible-selection Hypothesis Pushing around the Locus of Selection: Evidence for the Flexible-selection Hypothesis Edward K. Vogel 1, Geoffrey F. Woodman 2, and Steven J. Luck 3 Abstract & Attention operates at an early stage in some

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information