Keywords culture, social support seeking, self-esteem, relational concerns, influence and adjustment

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Keywords culture, social support seeking, self-esteem, relational concerns, influence and adjustment"

Transcription

1 731091JCCXXX / Journal of Cross-Cultural PsychologyIshii et al. research-article2017 Article Cultural Differences in Motivation for Seeking Social Support and the Emotional Consequences of Receiving Support: The Role of Influence and Adjustment Goals Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 2017, Vol. 48(9) The Author(s) 2017 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav DOI: journals.sagepub.com/home/jcc Keiko Ishii 1, Taraneh Mojaverian 2, Kanami Masuno 1, and Heejung S. Kim 2 Abstract Previous research suggests that the benefits of different types of social support depend on cultural background. However, cultural variations in the underlying motivations for seeking social support and the emotional implications of receiving support have not yet been clearly explored. We hypothesized and found that European Americans emphasized the motivation for self-esteem as a factor in deciding to seek explicit social support (e.g., advice, emotional comfort), whereas Japanese emphasized relational concerns as a factor in deciding to seek implicit social support (e.g., the emotional comfort experienced without disclosing one s problems). Furthermore, European Americans anticipated experiencing strong feelings of selfesteem and pride regarding receiving support, whereas Japanese anticipated experiencing strong feelings of shame and guilt. Additionally, influence goals mediated cultural differences in the motivation for self-esteem and the experience of self-esteem and pride, whereas adjustment goals mediated cultural differences in relational concerns and the experience of shame and guilt. Keywords culture, social support seeking, self-esteem, relational concerns, influence and adjustment Social support is believed to help people cope effectively with stressful events and contribute to their psychological well-being and physical health (e.g., Cohen & Wills, 1985; House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988). Although the benefits of social support are ubiquitous, previous research has suggested that appropriate forms of social support differ across cultures. Asians and Asian Americans are less likely than European Americans to seek explicit social support, such as advice or emotional comfort, from close others, and they are also less likely to perceive social support to be effective in resolving stressors (Kim, Sherman, Ko, & Taylor, 2006; Kim, Sherman, & Taylor, 2008; Taylor et al., 2004). Moreover, the factors motivating social support transactions vary across cultures. For example, among European Americans, providing social support is 1 Kobe University, Japan 2 University of California, Santa Barbara, CA, USA Corresponding Author: Keiko Ishii, Faculty of Letters, Kobe University, 1-1 Rokkodai-cho, Nada-ku, Kobe , Japan. ishii@lit.kobe-u.ac.jp

2 Ishii et al associated with the motivation of maintaining the self-esteem of the recipient, whereas this association is not found among Japanese (Chen, Kim, Mojaverian, & Morling, 2012). Although these culture-specific underlying motivations for social support provision have been demonstrated, it is still unclear whether the motivational underpinnings of social support seeking vary across cultures, whether cultures influence individuals emotional responses associated with receiving support, and what factors account for these cultural differences. Following previous research on cultural differences in influence and adjustment motivations (Morling, Kitayama, & Miyamoto, 2002), the present research explores the idea that cultural differences in the motivational underpinnings of seeking social support and the emotional consequences of receiving support are associated with the goals of influence (e.g., exerting personal control and changing the surrounding people and events) and adjustment (e.g., adapting to and accepting the surrounding situations). Culture, Self-Esteem, and Social Support Social support has been defined as information in an interpersonal transaction that leads the recipient to believe that he or she is cared for, loved, esteemed, and a member of a network of communication and mutual obligation (Cobb, 1976). As social support is a multidimensional concept, researchers have argued that the amount of support, the type of support, and the source of support are important dimensions (Thoits, 1982). Moreover, different aspects of support, which are grouped into perceived support (i.e., one s potential access to social support) and received support (i.e., the reported exchange of social support), have been conceptually distinguished, and the latter, in particular, has been examined in terms of the effectiveness of support (e.g., Uchino, 2009). For instance, tangible support, such as giving practical advice, is helpful for resolving concrete problems (e.g., debt-related stress). However, it would be less effective for other sorts of problems (e.g., grieving the death of a parent) than emotional support. Also, depending on how support is received, enacted support may be ineffective. For example, when a support transaction is visible to the recipient, it may be ineffective in alleviating stress as it could damage the recipient s self-esteem and sense of independence. Instead, invisible support, a support transaction achieved without the awareness of the recipient, is more likely to be beneficial for decreasing depression (Bolger & Amarel, 2007; Bolger, Zuckerman, & Kessler, 2000). Although self-esteem is important pan-culturally as a buffer against stress (Corning, 2002), its relative importance varies across cultures. Previous research has found that people are more motivated to maintain and enhance self-esteem by focusing on the uniqueness of their own positive attributes in Western cultures than in Eastern cultures (e.g., Heine, Lehman, Markus, & Kitayama, 1999). Moreover, compared with Western cultures, which place greater value on individual freedom and the sense of personal control conveyed through one s personal actions, Eastern cultures place greater emphasis on social adjustment and accommodation to others (e.g., Markus & Schwartz, 2010; Savani, Markus, & Conner, 2008). Cultural differences in the importance of personal control versus social accommodation suggest that choosing to disclose problems and sharing personal matters with others to cope with one s stress is viewed as less appropriate in Eastern cultures than in Western cultures. Such disclosure implies a demand for help from others, which may disrupt a harmonious relationship. Taylor et al. (2004) sought to understand reasons for not seeking support, and showed that Asians and Asian Americans were less likely than European Americans to seek social support (e.g., getting comfort, advice, or help from other people) to cope with stressful events and that cultural differences in seeking social support were mediated by relational concerns (e.g., disrupting group harmony). That is, Asians and Asian Americans reported greater relational concerns surrounding social support seeking than European Americans did. Another study (Mojaverian & Kim, 2013) showed that, when experiencing unsolicited social support situations (i.e., situations where social

3 1444 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 48(9) support is given without being asked for), Asian Americans felt less stressed and reported higher self-esteem than they did when experiencing solicited social support situations (i.e., situations where support is given only after explicit seeking). By comparison, European Americans were not differentially affected by these types of social support. This research suggests that actively seeking social support has more negative consequences and is less strongly associated with maintaining self-esteem for Asian Americans than for European Americans. Research suggests that support experienced without disclosing one s stressors and problems is more effective among Asian Americans. Taylor, Welch, Kim, and Sherman (2007) examined the effects of explicit social support (defined as the advice, instrumental aid, or emotional comfort given by social networks) and implicit social support (defined as the emotional comfort experienced without disclosing one s problems and stressful experiences) in Asian Americans and European Americans. This study found that implicit social support is more psychologically and physiologically beneficial for Asian Americans than explicit social support whereas the pattern is reversed in European Americans. These findings show that appropriate and effective types of social support depend on culture. Beyond cultural differences in the forms of social support that people prefer to use, there may be cultural differences in why people choose to seek social support. Although studies have shown that relational concerns explain why people would choose not to seek explicit social support, to date, there has been no examination of cultural differences in why people would seek social support. Thus, the possibility that relational concerns might encourage Asians to seek implicit social support more so than European Americans has not been tested. In addition, we suggest the desire to maintain self-esteem as a differing potential motivator, given previous findings showing that European Americans are more likely than Asian Americans to maintain their self-esteem when they decide to seek explicit social support (Mojaverian & Kim, 2013), and that social support provision is associated with motivation for maintaining the self-esteem of the support recipient among European Americans but not among Japanese (Chen et al., 2012). Despite these reported cultural differences in the association between support seeking and self-esteem, it is still unclear to what extent self-esteem is an important goal in deciding to seek social support and whether culture influences social support seeking as an intentional action to enhance self-esteem. In this research, we take a step towards examining cultural differences in the extent to which social support seeking is intended for decreasing relational concerns and maintaining self-esteem. In addition, the emotional consequences of receiving social support have not yet been clearly explored. Given the association between explicit social support use and self-esteem in European Americans, they may experience positive emotions, particularly self-esteem and self-pride, more strongly than Asians would when receiving explicit social support. In contrast, compared with European Americans, Asians may experience negative emotions, particularly guilt and shame related to a failure to maintain group harmony, more strongly when receiving explicit social support. The present research aims to fill the gap in the literature by investigating the underlying motivational mechanisms of seeking explicit and implicit social support and the emotional consequences of receiving such support. The Role of Influence and Adjustment Goals in Social Support Seeking The present research also explores the role of influence and adjustment goals as mediators of cultural differences in motivation for seeking social support and the emotional consequences of receiving social support. At a basic level, people everywhere seek social support in an attempt to resolve their personal issues, and they believe that their close others are able to help them achieve the goal. Thus, they are satisfied when social support provision matches their goal (Horowitz et al., 2001). Previous research shows that people from different cultures vary in broader

4 Ishii et al approaches to how they deal with challenges that they face. In North American contexts, daily practices of influencing one s environment are more emphasized whereas in East Asian contexts, daily practices of adjusting oneself to the surrounding environment are more emphasized (Morling et al., 2002). Given the well-established differences in culture-specific goals, it is anticipated that differences in the relative emphasis placed on influence versus adjustment would lead to cross-cultural differences in why and how people seek support as well as the association of different emotional experiences with receiving support. Influencing behaviors cause people to experience enhanced self-esteem (Morling et al., 2002), and social support, as a way to proactively influence one s social environment, bolsters selfesteem (e.g., Pearlin, Menaghan, Lieberman, & Mullan, 1981). In social support seeking, the motivation to exert influence may thus lead to the act of seeking social support by disclosing problems and asking explicitly for help. Consequently, support seeking may restore and enhance self-esteem. The psychological tendency of maintaining self-esteem through seeking and receiving explicit social support is widespread in North American cultural contexts, and influencing goals may underlie this relationship. In contrast, adjustment goals may make people hesitate to actively mobilize their social network. Rather, people may become more considerate of others needs and sensitive of the potential risk of burdening close others to maintain their self-esteem. This is consistent with existing findings showing that adjusting behaviors bring a feeling of closeness to others (Morling et al., 2002). In social support seeking, adjustment may lower a person s willingness to seek social support given its potential for troubling and burdening others. Adjustment goals may facilitate seeking implicit social support, which carries minimal social risks. Thus, in this study, we explored whether cultural goals mediate cultural differences in the processes of social support use. Overview Figure 1 outlines the theoretical model of how cultural goals of influence and adjustment shape individuals self-esteem, the relational motivations that underlie social support seeking and receipt, and the emotional responses associated with social support use. Comparing European American and Japanese participants, the present research examined willingness to seek emotional and instrumental support (categorized together as explicit social support) and implicit social support along with the emotional experiences anticipated when receiving these types of support. We hypothesized that European Americans would be more likely than Japanese to emphasize self-esteem as a motivating factor in deciding to seek explicit social support. We also hypothesized that Japanese would be more likely than European Americans to emphasize relational concerns as a motivating factor in deciding to seek implicit social support. We predicted that, after seeking and receiving explicit social support, European Americans would be more likely than Japanese to experience positive feelings, particularly self-esteem and pride, whereas Japanese would be more likely than European Americans to experience negative feelings, particularly shame and guilt. Furthermore, we predicted that endorsement of influence goals would mediate cultural differences in the process of explicit support use whereas endorsement of adjustment goals would mediate cultural differences in the process of implicit social support use. Method Participants Eighty-eight Japanese students (53 women and 35 men, M age = 18.60, SD = 2.07) at Kobe University and 70 European American students (52 women and 17 men, one unidentified, M age = 19.28, SD = 1.42) at the University of California, Santa Barbara, participated in the study for partial course credit or monetary compensation. 1

5 1446 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 48(9) Figure 1. The theoretical framework on the association between culturally divergent influence and adjustment goals and the underlying motivation and emotional outcomes of seeking and receiving social support. The proportion of female participants was higher in the European American group than in the Japanese group; χ 2 (1, N = 157) = 4.00, p =.046. Because our initial analysis showed that gender did not influence the cultural differences reported below, we excluded gender in the following analyses. 2 Materials and Procedure Following the methods used in previous research (e.g., Kim et al., 2006; Taylor et al., 2004), participants were initially asked to identify the biggest stressor they had faced within the last three months, describe it briefly, and choose the most relevant type of stressor from nine options (family relationship, friend relationship, romantic relationship, academic, health, financial, job, future, or other). 3 By asking participants to recall these stressors, we could assess their salient responses to concrete stress situations. The participants were then asked to examine their motivations for seeking support in response to stressors like the ones they had described. Specifically, they were asked to imagine seeking three different types of social support and to report what factors would motivate them to seek each type of social support. The three types of social support were (a) emotional support (e.g., encouraging words from a close other), (b) instrumental support (e.g., advice from others), and (c) implicit support (e.g., simply spending more time with close others without talking about a given problem). For each type of support, participants were presented with a list of potential support seeking motivations, and they rated on a 7-point rating scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very important) how important each factor would be for them in deciding to seek support. Motivation for self-esteem was measured with four items ( I want to feel like I am doing better regarding the problem, I want to make myself feel better, I want to feel more confident, and I want to feel good about myself ). We developed the items by adapting those used in Chen et al. (2012) regarding motivations for providing support to the context of seeking support. The reliabilities were reasonably high (Japanese: α =.68,.72, and.67; European Americans: α =.81,.92, and.94. for emotional, instrumental, and implicit support, respectively). Regarding seeking implicit social support, relational concerns were measured with four items ( If I discuss my problems

6 Ishii et al with the people I am close to, it makes it a bigger problem than if I keep it to myself ; I wouldn t want to make the people I am close to feel stressed about my problems ; I can save face by not talking about my stressor ; and I would rather keep my problems to myself than risk criticism from the people I am close to ; Taylor et al., 2004). Cronbach s alpha was.66 for Japanese and.91 for European Americans. 4 For each type of support, after completing the questions regarding motivations for seeking support, participants reported on a 6-point rating scale (1 = not at all, 6 = very strongly) the extent to which they would experience a series of emotions if they received that type of support. The emotions used were adopted from previous studies by Kitayama and colleagues (Ishii, Kitayama, & Uchida, 2014; Kitayama & Park, 2007; Kitayama, Park, Sevincer, Karasawa, & Uskul, 2009). Emotions were sampled from six theoretically derived categories. Some emotions were general in the sense that they could be associated with either independence or interdependence, both positive (happy, elated, and calm) and negative (unhappy). Others were socially disengaging, resulting from either success (proud and self-esteem) or failure regarding independence (angry and frustration). Another set of emotion terms were socially engaging, stemming from either success (friendly feelings and close feelings) or failure regarding interdependence (ashamed and guilty). 5 Cronbach s alpha for general positive emotions, disengaging positive emotions, disengaging negative emotions, engaging positive emotions, and engaging negative emotions was between.78 and.90 among the European Americans and between.74 and.88 among the Japanese. Finally, influence and adjustment were assessed by using the Circumplex Scale of Interpersonal Values (Locke, 2000). The participants were presented with items describing how they would act, appear, or be treated in interpersonal situations and rated the importance of each item on a 5-point rating scale (1 = not important, 5 = extremely important). Following Tsai, Miao, Seppala, Fung, and Yeung (2007), influence was measured with four items (e.g., I have an impact on them ; Cronbach s α was.58 for Japanese and.68 for European Americans) while adjustment was measured with five items (e.g., I go along with what they want to do ; Cronbach s α was.46 for Japanese and.57 for European Americans). 6 All materials were initially developed in English and then translated and back-translated between Japanese and English by two Japanese English bilinguals to ensure cross-cultural equivalence. Results Characteristics of Stressful Events Regarding how participants categorized their own stressors, Japanese (36.4%) were more likely than European Americans (7.1%) to mention stressful events related to friend relationships, χ 2 (1, N = 158) = 18.56, p <.001. In contrast, European Americans (57.1%) were more likely than Japanese (15.9%) to mention stressful events related to academic issues, χ 2 (1, N = 158) = 20.47, p <.001. Although the characteristics of the stressful events described differed across cultures, we asked the participants to rate the extent to which they perceived the events as stressful and negative by using two separate items with a 7-point rating scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very much) and found that there was no cultural difference in the average of the two items (Japanese: M = 5.04, SD = 1.24, European Americans: M = 5.24, SD = 1.28), F(1, 156) = 0.95, p =.33. Cultural differences in the characteristics of stressful events did not influence the ratings for motivations for seeking support and emotional experiences except for the rating of relational concerns regarding implicit social support seeking (see Note 7 for more details).

7 1448 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 48(9) Figure 2. The mean ratings of self-esteem motivation by culture and type of social support. Motivation for Self-Esteem in Seeking Support A 2 (culture) 3 (type of support) ANOVA conducted on the mean average rating of items measuring self-esteem motivation revealed a significant main effect of type of support, F(2, 302) = 70.37, p <.001, η 2 p =.32. Self-esteem motivation was rated as more important in seeking emotional (M = 5.21, SD = 1.14) and instrumental support (M = 4.90, SD = 1.30) compared with implicit support (M = 4.40, SD = 1.27), ts(302) = and 6.54, ps <.001, ds = 1.22 and The difference between emotional and instrumental support was also significant, t(302) = 4.09, p <.001, d = 0.47, with participants reporting greater motivation for self-esteem regarding emotional support seeking compared with instrumental support seeking. Importantly, the culture and type of support interaction was significant, F(2, 302) = 37.99, p <.001, η 2 p =.20. As shown in Figure 2, European Americans (M = 5.59, SD = 1.00) rated self-esteem motivation as more important than Japanese did (M = 4.93, SD = 1.16) in seeking emotional support from others, t(453) = 3.34, p <.001, d = The same tendency was found with motivations for seeking instrumental support (European Americans: M = 5.21, SD = 1.36, Japanese: M = 4.67, SD = 1.22), t(453) = 2.75, p =.006, d = These results were consistent with our prediction. However, European Americans (M = 4.08, SD = 1.35) rated self-esteem motivation as less important than Japanese did (M = 4.63, SD = 1.16) in seeking implicit support from others, t(453) = 2.82, p =.005, d = A main effect of culture was not significant, F(1, 151) = 1.48, p =.23. Relational Concerns in Seeking Implicit Support An ANOVA conducted on the mean average rating of items measuring relational concerns regarding implicit social support seeking revealed a main effect of culture, F(1, 151) = 38.29, p <.001, η p 2 =.20. As expected, relational concerns were significantly higher for Japanese (M = 4.06, SD = 1.30) than for European Americans (M = 2.61, SD = 1.61). Expected Emotional Experiences in Receiving Support The average intensity for each participant was computed for the six categories of emotions for each type of support in each culture. An ANOVA with a between-subjects variable (culture) and three within-subjects variables (valence [positive and negative], social orientation [general,

8 Ishii et al Table 1. Correlations of the Measures for Japanese and American Participants. Measure Self-esteem motivation for emotional support seeking.82***.69***.32**.27*.27*.35**.21.50***.25* Self-esteem motivation.78***.79***.31**.26*.23.42***.22.49*** for instrumental support seeking 3. Self-esteem motivation for implicit support seeking.56***.63***.46***.25*.22.47***.23.48***.25* Relational concerns for implicit support seeking * ***.14.25* 5. General positive emotions *.78***.24*.62*** General negative emotions.23*.18.28**.30**.27*.18.70***.07.47*** Disengaging positive emotions.14.28**.30**.10.35*** ***.19.26* Disengaging negative emotions.25*.31**.29**.15.22*.56***.35***.20.40*** Engaging positive emotions.19.25* ***.28** Engaging negative emotions *.51***.03.41***.38***.40*** Influence.25*.28**.44*** *.43***.24*.14.27* Adjustment.21* **.30**.33** Note. Correlations for Americans (n = 70) are presented above the diagonal, and correlations for Japanese (n = 88) are presented below the diagonal. *p <.05. **p <.01. ***p <.001. disengaging, and engaging], and type of support [emotional, instrumental, and implicit]) was then performed on the average intensity. The interaction between culture and social orientation was significant, F(2, 302) = 13.47, p <.001, η 2 p =.08. Moreover, the interaction was qualified by valence, F(2, 302) = 6.76, p =.001, η 2 p =.04. Although no significant difference between the two cultures was found in either general positive emotion (European Americans: M = 3.49, SD = 1.09, Japanese: M = 3.61, SD = 1.03), t(453) = 0.76, p =.45, or engaging positive emotion (European Americans: M = 4.25, SD = 1.21, Japanese: M = 4.37, SD = 1.10), t(453) = 0.82, p =.41, European Americans (M = 3.11, SD = 1.19) reported that they would experience disengaging positive emotions more strongly than Japanese did (M = 2.48, SD = 1.07), t(453) = 4.21, p <.001, d = However, Japanese (M = 2.22, SD = 1.00) reported that they would experience engaging negative emotions more strongly than European Americans did (M = 1.91, SD = 1.16), t(453) = 2.11, p =.04, d = No significant difference between the two cultures was found in either general negative emotion (European Americans: M = 2.02, SD = 1.22, Japanese: M = 1.87, SD = 1.26), t(453) = 1.02, p =.31, or disengaging negative emotion (European Americans: M = 1.99, SD = 1.17, Japanese: M = 1.88, SD = 0.95), t(453) = 0.77, p =.44. The Culture Social Orientation Valence interaction was not qualified by type of support, F(4, 604) = 1.02, p = Influence and Adjustment as Cultural Mediators As expected, influence was significantly higher in European Americans (M = 3.48, SD = 0.69) than in Japanese (M = 3.04, SD = 0.71), F(1, 156) = 15.69, p <.001, η p 2 =.09. In contrast, adjustment was significantly higher in Japanese (M = 2.94, SD = 0.53) than in European Americans (M = 2.18, SD = 0.61), F(1, 156) = 70.28, p <.001, η p 2 =.31. Correlations among the measurements reported above in each culture are presented in Table 1. In both cultures, influence was positively associated with self-esteem motivation for emotional and instrument support seeking (Japan: rs =.25 and.28; United States: rs =.20 and.22) and expected experiences of disengaging positive emotions (Japan: r =.43; United States: r =.26),

9 1450 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 48(9) Figure 3. The relationship between culture and motivation in support seeking, mediated by influence and adjustment. Note. Regression coefficients are unstandardized. Standard errors are given in parentheses. *p <.05. **p <.01. ***p <.001. and adjustment was positively associated with relational concerns for implicit support seeking (Japan: r =.12; United States: r =.25) and expected experiences of disengaging positive emotions (Japan: r =.20; United States: r =.19). These patterns are consistent with our predictions. We then examined whether influence and adjustment mediate cultural differences in motivation for support seeking through structural equation modeling. Testing emotional and instrumental support separately yielded similar results, so we combined these two types of support into a measure of explicit support for our main analysis. A model based on our predictions (Figure 3) yielded an adequate fit, df = 1, χ 2 = 2.41, p =.12, comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.990, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = Culture (0 = Japan, 1 = United States) significantly predicted influence (b =.45, SE =.11, z = 4.08, p <.001) and adjustment (b =.79, SE =.09, z = 8.66, p <.001). Influence significantly predicted the motivation for self-esteem in seeking explicit support (b =.35, SE =.13, z = 2.73, p =.006), although the direct path between culture and the motivation for self-esteem (b =.63, SE =.22, z = 2.83, p =.005) was also significant. In addition, adjustment significantly predicted the motivation related to relational concerns in seeking implicit support (b =.45, SE =.20, z = 2.23, p =.03), although the direct path between culture and the motivation related to relational concerns (b = 1.18, SE =.29, z = 4.06, p <.001) was also significant. 9 We also examined whether influence and adjustment mediate cultural differences in expected experiences of disengaging positive emotions and engaging negative emotions. A model led by our prediction (Figure 4) yielded an adequate fit, df = 1, χ 2 = 1.59, p =.21, CFI = 0.995, RMSEA = Influence significantly predicted disengaging positive emotions in receiving support (b =.46, SE =.10, z = 4.55, p <.001), although the direct path between culture and disengaging positive emotion (b =.66, SE =.18, z = 4.55, p <.001) was also significant. In addition, adjustment significantly predicted engaging negative emotions in receiving support (b =.30, SE =.13, z = 2.36, p =.02) while it also unexpectedly significantly predicted disengaging positive emotions (b =.30, SE =.13, z = 2.43, p =.02). Moreover, the direct path between culture and engaging negative emotion was nonsignificant, b =.16, SE =.18, z = 0.93, p =.35. Discussion We found that European Americans emphasized maintaining self-esteem when deciding to seek explicit social support whereas Japanese emphasized relational concerns when deciding to seek implicit social support. Moreover, European Americans reported more self-esteem and pride when receiving social support than did Japanese whereas Japanese reported more shame and

10 Ishii et al Figure 4. The relationship between culture and emotions evoked by social support, mediated by influence and adjustment. Note. Regression coefficients are unstandardized. Standard errors are given in parentheses. *p <.05. **p <.01. ***p <.001. guilt in receiving social support than did European Americans. Furthermore, we demonstrated that influence goals, which were higher in European Americans than in Japanese, mediated cultural differences in self-esteem motivation for seeking explicit social support, whereas adjustment goals, which were higher in Japanese than in European Americans, mediated cultural differences in relational concerns in seeking implicit social support. In addition, influence and adjustment goals mediated the cultural differences in emotional experiences of receiving support. One benefit of the present study is that it suggests that there are cultural differences in the underlying mechanisms of seeking explicit and implicit social support. We demonstrated that culture interacted with self-esteem motivations for seeking explicit social support and relational concern motivations for seeking implicit social support. These mechanisms have been suggested as potential mediators in previous research but were never directly tested. Additionally, the present study, to our knowledge, offers the first evidence that influence and adjustment goals are linked to psychological factors leading individuals to seek appropriate and beneficial types of social support. The current findings suggest that motivation for bolstering self-esteem by seeking and receiving explicit emotional support and instrumental advice are more encouraged in a cultural environment where control and efficacy are emphasized (manifested as influence goals). By contrast, relational concerns leading individuals to seek implicit social support are more encouraged in a cultural environment where the values of connectedness and accommodation are emphasized (manifested as adjustment goals). These findings contribute to the literature on the psychological factors that influence culturally divergent ways of seeking social support. In seeking implicit social support, Japanese rated both relational concerns and self-esteem as more important than European Americans did. This suggests that the motivation for self-esteem is salient even among Japanese, reflecting the importance of maintaining self-esteem in our social lives (e.g., Leary, Tambor, Terdal, & Downs, 1995). At the same time, this result also suggests that ways to bolster self-esteem vary depending on culturally normative perceptions of voluntary interpersonal responsibilities (e.g., Miller & Bersoff, 1998; Miller, Bersoff, & Harwood, 1990) and how frequently individuals engage in self-disclosure (e.g., Schug, Yuki, & Maddux, 2010). Moreover, implicit social support may increase the recipient s feelings of closeness to the provider, which may be related to increased self-esteem, particularly among Japanese. Indeed, a recent article (Chen, Kim, Sherman, & Hashimoto, 2015) reported that high relationship quality is more strongly associated with an indirect form of support provision, such as being attentive of

11 1452 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 48(9) close others need, among Japanese than Americans. Future research should examine additional mechanisms underlying implicit and explicit support seeking and provision, such as one s feelings for the support provider and one s evaluation of the relationship with the support provider. As explicit social support is more appropriate and psychologically and physiologically effective for European Americans than for Asians and Asian Americans (Taylor et al., 2007), we expected that explicit social support would increase positive feelings (particularly, self-esteem and pride) for European Americans and increase negative feelings (particularly, guilt and shame) for Japanese. However, the results showed that, regardless of the types of support, European Americans reported more self-esteem and pride than did Japanese, and Japanese reported more guilt and shame than did European Americans. Although the types of support moderated an interaction between culture and valence (see Note 8), the four-way interaction was not significant. Because the emotional responses measured in the study were anticipated reactions to a hypothetical situation, the results might reflect only weak effects of the types of social support in the expected emotional experience of receiving support. Investigating emotional responses to actual social support situations may lead to a clearer understanding of the emotional consequences of culturally appropriate (or inappropriate) methods of social support. The present study has several limitations. First, it used a cross-sectional design. The possibility cannot be ruled out that self-esteem motivations and relational concern motivations lead people to engage in goals of influence and adjustment. Although the model with self-esteem motivations and relational concern motivations as mediators fits the data poorly (see Note 9), future research with a longitudinal design will be needed to determine the nature of the association between goals of influence and adjustment and motivations for seeking social support. Second, by adopting the items used by Chen et al. (2012), which examined an association between concern for a recipient s self-esteem and support provision, we measured self-esteem motivations for seeking social support. However, the items used in the current study might have differing connotations reflecting how effective and successful explicit social support is perceived to be based on past experiences. Additional work with revised items to measure self-esteem motivations will be needed to confirm cultural differences related to self-esteem motivations and social support seeking. Third, although no cultural differences existed in the extent to which participants perceived the events described as stressful and negative, the characteristics of the stressors differed across cultures. Japanese mentioned more stressful events related to friendship whereas European Americans mentioned more stressful events related to academic issues. We adopted the approach to recall the stressors to assess participants salient responses to concrete stress situations. Indeed, the situational differences did not influence cultural differences in motives for seeking social support and the emotional consequences (see Note 7). However, in future research, it will be important to generalize the current findings to a common type of stressful event across cultures. Fourth, the present research did not examine the extent to which cultural differences in motivational aspects of seeking social support and the resulting emotional consequences would be associated with differences in communal norms regarding taking care of mutual needs versus exchange norms that involve expectations of immediate repayment in return for providing a benefit (Clark & Mills, 1979). Recently, Miller, Akiyama, and Kapadia (2017) showed that, although Japanese and Americans are more likely than Indians to endorse exchange norms in receiving help, Japanese, who are more motivated to maintain harmony and restore balance in relationships, are more likely to be concerned with compensating for the costs that the helper paid to provide benefits. In contrast, Americans are more likely to focus on conveying appreciation (e.g., expressing gratitude) to the helper, which leads the receiver to view herself or himself positively. On the contrary, Indians, who are more likely than Japanese and Americans to endorse communal norms, are more likely to feel obligations to in-group members. The findings of Miller et al. suggest that the cultural differences in motivational aspects of seeking social support and the

12 Ishii et al emotional consequences found in the present research might involve exchange norms. It also suggests that the findings of the present research may not generalize to all Asian cultures. Compared with Japanese, cultural groups endorsing communal norms (e.g., Indians) would less likely have relational concerns about receiving help and, accordingly, would be less motivated to seek implicit social support and would feel less guilt and shame from the experience of receiving social support (see also Campos & Kim, 2017). Finally, the present study did not assess how the potential costs of receiving social support would be perceived or examine cultural differences in the perceived cost. Miller et al. (2017) found that Japanese reported more relational concerns in the case of high-cost compared with low-cost help whereas cost did not influence Americans estimation of relational concerns. Given this research, the findings of the present research might partially result from the fact that, compared with European Americans, Japanese might recall stressors which include a high cost to the support provider. Comparing stressful events solved via high-cost versus low-cost help and examining the impact of cost in the cultural differences in motivational aspects of seeking social support and emotional consequences will be crucial in future work. Despite these limitations, the present research contributes to our understanding of culturally appropriate forms of support and the emotional consequences of receiving support, reflecting differences in the relative emphasis of influence versus adjustment goals. This evidence for culturally divergent mechanisms that underlie seeking explicit and implicit social support has implications for the types of counseling services and interventions used to cope with stress effectively and enhance well-being in different cultures. The present study also poses interesting new directions for future research. Additional insight provided by further investigations using various behavioral and physiological measures would be beneficial in achieving a better understanding of the psychological factors that underlie seeking social support in divergent cultural contexts. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: The research was supported by the Uehiro Foundation on Ethics and Education. Notes 1. Mojaverian and Kim (2013, Study 1) found that European Americans (M = 3.90, SD = 0.56) reported higher state self-esteem than did Asian Americans (M = 3.61, SD = 0.62) after receiving social support. Because to our knowledge there was no direct evidence which the present research could consult to determine sample size, we referred to the ratings reported by the study of Mojaverian and Kim and estimated that the sample size was 65 for each culture, assuming a value for desired power as The significant effects of gender were only found when expected emotional experiences were analyzed. The interaction between gender and social orientation was significant, F(2, 296) = 13.80, p < , η p =.09. Moreover, it was qualified by valence, F(2, 296) = 5.41, p =.005, η p =.04. The 2 interaction between gender and type of support was also significant, F(2, 296) = 3.82, p =.02, η p = A given stressor may be categorized into more than one type. As we were interested in how participants themselves subjectively construe the situation, we chose to have participants categorize into the most salient type themselves, rather than imposing an objective categorization. 4. To examine metric equivalence, a multiple group analysis was performed for self-esteem motivation for emotional, instrumental, and implicit support and for relational concerns for implicit support, respectively. For relational concerns, metric invariance was tested by a model constraining factor

13 1454 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 48(9) loadings equal across Japanese and Americans (df = 7, χ 2 = 14.03, Akaike Information Criterion [AIC] = 2,312.9, Bayesian Information Criterion [BIC] = 2,376.5) and was established, df difference = 3, χ 2 difference = 6.82, p =.08. On the contrary, for self-esteem motivation for emotional, instrumental, and implicit support, partial intercept invariance was accordingly tested (emotional: df = 9, χ 2 = 23.98, AIC = 2,116.5, BIC = 2,174.7; instrumental: df = 9, χ 2 = 30.54, AIC = 2,076.4, BIC = 2,134.5; implicit: df = 9, χ 2 = 48.53, AIC = 2,132.0, BIC = 2,192.6) and was established (emotional: df difference = 2, χ 2 difference = 0.17, p =.91; instrumental: df difference = 2, χ 2 difference = 0.08, p =.96; implicit: df difference = 1, χ 2 difference = 2.51, p =.11). 5. This research examined the effect of these interpersonal goals in interpersonally disengaged or engaged emotions (e.g., Kitayama, Mesquita, & Karasawa, 2006). Emotional experiences related to the underlying mechanism of seeking social support may overlap the affective states that people want to feel. However, these states would not be limited to a basic dimension of affect, such as arousal. The present research thus focuses on a set of emotions related more closely to social interactions rather than emotions related to emotional reactivity. 6. To examine metric equivalence, a multiple group analysis was performed for influence and adjustment, respectively. For influence, metric invariance was tested by a model constraining factor loadings equal across Japanese and Americans (df = 7, χ 2 = 4.34, AIC = 1,752.9, BIC = 1,817.0) and was established; df difference = 3, χ 2 difference = 1.72, p =.63. In contrast, for adjustment, a model constraining residual variances in addition to factor loadings and intercepts to be equal was supported (df = 22, χ 2 = 63.20, AIC = 2,129.9, BIC = 2,184.6), df difference = 5, χ 2 difference = 4.83, p =.44. The result of invariance testing for adjustment suggests that caution is needed in interpreting this cultural difference. Moreover, the level of internal consistency for the scale used to assess influence and adjustment goals was relatively low, specifically with regards to Japanese participants. As interpreted by Tsai, Miao, Seppala, Fung, and Yeung (2007), reporting on the same tendency, interpersonal goals might likely depend on situations in Japanese contexts that lead to the low level of internal consistency. Future research, including experimentally manipulated goals of influence and adjustment, will be useful for confirming the results of this study. 7. Although there were no cultural differences in participants perceptions of the events as negative and stressful, cultural differences in the characteristics of stressful events might influence the ratings for motivations for seeking support and emotional experiences. To examine this, we categorized the participants into a group of personal stressors (academic, health, financial, job, and future) and a group of interpersonal stressors (family relationship, friend relationship, and romantic relationship) and performed ANOVAs on motivations for seeking support, emotional experiences, and influence and adjustment goals by including the stressor variable. The personal stressors group consisted of 37 Japanese and 50 European Americans whereas the interpersonal stressors group had 41 Japanese and 17 European Americans. Thirteen participants (10 Japanese and three European Americans) who categorized their stressors as other were not included in either of the stressor groups. A significant main effect of stressors was only found for the rating of relational concerns regarding implicit social support seeking, F(1, 136) = 5.13, p =.03, η p 2 =.04. Participants with interpersonal stressors (M = 3.96, SD = 1.56) were higher in relational concerns than were participants with personal stressors (M = 3.03, SD = 1.56). However, any interaction indicating that stressors moderate cultural differences in the measurements of motivation, emotion, and influence and adjustment goals did not prove to be significant, Fs < 3.59, p > In addition, an interaction including culture, valence, and type of support was significant, F(2, 302) = 3.77, p =.02, η p 2 =.02. European Americans (M = 3.70, SD = 1.23) reported that they would experience positive emotions more strongly than did Japanese (M = 3.40, SD = 1.30) if receiving emotional support, t(453) = 1.99, p =.047, d = The same pattern was found for instrumental support, although it was not significant (European Americans: M = 3.60, SD = 1.22, Japanese: M = 3.37, SD = 1.25), t(453) = 1.55, p =.12. No significant cultural difference was found for implicit support (European Americans: M = 3.54, SD = 1.32 Japanese: M = 3.68, SD = 1.39), t(453) = 0.94, p =.35. Japanese reported that they would experience positive emotions more strongly in the case of implicit support (M = 3.68, SD = 1.39) compared with emotional (M = 3.40, SD = 1.30) and instrumental support (M = 3.37, SD = 1.25), t(302) = 2.80 and 3.11, ps =.005 and.002. European Americans positive emotions did not depend on the types of support. There was no significant effect of culture on the experience of negative emotions.

14 Ishii et al An alternative model with self-esteem motivation and relational concerns as mediators to predict influence and adjustment indicated a poor fit, df = 1, χ 2 = 6.41, p =.01, comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.960, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = References Bolger, N., & Amarel, D. (2007). Effects of social support visibility on adjustment to stress: Experimental evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, doi: / Bolger, N., Zuckerman, A., & Kessler, R. C. (2000). Invisible support and adjustment to stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, doi: / Campos, B., & Kim, H. S. (2017). Incorporating the cultural diversity of family and close relationships into the study of health. American Psychologist, 72, Chen, J. M., Kim, H. S., Mojaverian, T., & Morling, B. (2012). Culture and social support provision: Who gives what and why. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, doi: / Chen, J. M., Kim, H. S., Sherman, D. K., & Hashimoto, T. (2015). Cultural differences in support provision: The importance of relationship quality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, doi: / Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1979). Interpersonal attraction in exchange and communal relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, doi: / Cobb, S. (1976). Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 38, Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, doi: / Corning, A. F. (2002). Self-esteem as a moderator between perceived discrimination and psychological distress among women. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 49, doi: / Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106, doi: / x Horowitz, L. M., Krasnoperova, E. N., Tatar, D. G., Hansen, M. B., Person, E. A., Galvin, K. L., & Nelson, K. L. (2001). The way to console may depend on the goal: Experimental studies of social support. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, doi: /jesp House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science, 241, doi: /science Ishii, K., Kitayama, S., & Uchida, Y. (2014). Voluntary settlement and its consequences on predictors of happiness: The influence of initial cultural context. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, doi: / fpsyg Kim, H. S., Sherman, D. K., Ko, D., & Taylor, S. E. (2006). Pursuit of comfort and pursuit of harmony: Culture, relationships, and social support seeking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, doi: / Kim, H. S., Sherman, D. K., & Taylor, S. E. (2008). Culture and social support. American Psychologist, 63, doi: / x Kitayama, S., Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2006). Cultural affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, doi: / Kitayama, S., & Park, H. (2007). Cultural shaping of self, emotion, and well-being: How does it work? Social & Personality Psychology Compass, 1, doi: /j x Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K. (2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, doi: /a Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, doi: / Locke, K. D. (2000). Circumplex scales of interpersonal values: Reliability, validity, and applicability to interpersonal problems and personality disorders. Journal of Personality Assessment, 75, doi: /s jpa7502_6

Negative events such as failing an exam or finding. Pursuit of Comfort and Pursuit of Harmony: Culture, Relationships, and Social Support Seeking

Negative events such as failing an exam or finding. Pursuit of Comfort and Pursuit of Harmony: Culture, Relationships, and Social Support Seeking Pursuit of Comfort and Pursuit of Harmony: Culture, Relationships, and Social Support Seeking Heejung S. Kim David K. Sherman Deborah Ko University of California, Santa Barbara Shelley E. Taylor University

More information

A Hierarchical Comparison on Influence Paths from Cognitive & Emotional Trust to Proactive Behavior Between China and Japan

A Hierarchical Comparison on Influence Paths from Cognitive & Emotional Trust to Proactive Behavior Between China and Japan A Hierarchical Comparison on Influence Paths from Cognitive & Emotional Trust to Proactive Behavior Between China and Japan Pei Liu School of Management and Economics, North China Zhen Li Data Science

More information

Development and Psychometric Properties of the Relational Mobility Scale for the Indonesian Population

Development and Psychometric Properties of the Relational Mobility Scale for the Indonesian Population Development and Psychometric Properties of the Relational Mobility Scale for the Indonesian Population Sukaesi Marianti Abstract This study aims to develop the Relational Mobility Scale for the Indonesian

More information

Mutual and Non-Mutual Social Support: Cultural Differences in the Psychological, Behavioral, and Biological Effects of Support Seeking

Mutual and Non-Mutual Social Support: Cultural Differences in the Psychological, Behavioral, and Biological Effects of Support Seeking 592967JCCXXX10.1177/0022022115592967Journal of Cross-Cultural PsychologyWang and Lau research-article2015 Article Mutual and Non-Mutual Social Support: Cultural Differences in the Psychological, Behavioral,

More information

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: SOCIAL AFFILIATION CUES PRIME HELP-SEEKING INTENTIONS 1 This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: Rubin, M. (2011). Social

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

Dampening or Savoring Positive Emotions: A Dialectical Cultural Script Guides Emotion Regulation

Dampening or Savoring Positive Emotions: A Dialectical Cultural Script Guides Emotion Regulation Emotion 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 11, No. 6, 1346 1357 1528-3542/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0025135 Dampening or Savoring Positive Emotions: A Dialectical Cultural Script Guides Emotion

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Cultural Differences in the Impact of Social Support on Psychological and Biological Stress Responses Shelley E. Taylor, 1 William T. Welch, 1 Heejung S. Kim, 2 and

More information

Expressions of the Self in Individualistic vs. Collective Cultures: a cross-cultural-perspective teaching module

Expressions of the Self in Individualistic vs. Collective Cultures: a cross-cultural-perspective teaching module Psychology Learning and Teaching Volume 11 Number 3 2012 www.wwwords.co.uk/plat Expressions of the Self in Individualistic vs. Collective Cultures: a cross-cultural-perspective teaching module BERNARDO

More information

Culture and Social Support: Who Seeks It and Why?

Culture and Social Support: Who Seeks It and Why? INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES Culture and Social Support: Who Seeks It and Why? Shelley E. Taylor University of California, Los Angeles David K. Sherman and Heejung S. Kim University of California,

More information

Self-Related and Other-Related Pathways to Subjective Well-Being in Japan and the United States

Self-Related and Other-Related Pathways to Subjective Well-Being in Japan and the United States J Happiness Stud (2014) 15:995 1014 DOI 10.1007/s10902-013-9460-9 RESEARCH PAPER Self-Related and Other-Related Pathways to Subjective Well-Being in Japan and the United States Sheida Novin Ivy F. Tso

More information

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model 50 Educational Measurement and Evaluation Review (2012), Vol. 3, 50-59 2012 Philippine Educational Measurement and Evaluation Association Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model Adonis

More information

SEEKING HELP FROM CLOSE, SAME-SEX FRIENDS 2. Seeking Help From Close, Same-Sex Friends:

SEEKING HELP FROM CLOSE, SAME-SEX FRIENDS 2. Seeking Help From Close, Same-Sex Friends: SEEKING HELP FROM CLOSE, SAME-SEX FRIENDS 2 Seeking Help From Close, Same-Sex Friends: Relational Costs for Japanese and Personal Costs for European Canadians Kenichi Ito Institute on Asian Consumer Insight,

More information

1. Family context. a) Positive Disengaged

1. Family context. a) Positive Disengaged Online Supplementary Materials for Emotion manuscript 015-197 Emotions and Concerns: Situational Evidence for their Systematic Co-Occurrence. by Jozefien De Leersnyder, Peter Koval, Peter Kuppens, & Batja

More information

Person-Centered and Invisible Support A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY. Angela Sigl Janson

Person-Centered and Invisible Support A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY. Angela Sigl Janson The Impact of Emotional Distress on the Perceived Effectiveness of Person-Centered and Invisible Support A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY Angela

More information

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment Marshall University Marshall Digital Scholar Management Faculty Research Management, Marketing and MIS Fall 11-14-2009 Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment Wai Kwan

More information

An Empirical Study of the Roles of Affective Variables in User Adoption of Search Engines

An Empirical Study of the Roles of Affective Variables in User Adoption of Search Engines An Empirical Study of the Roles of Affective Variables in User Adoption of Search Engines ABSTRACT Heshan Sun Syracuse University hesun@syr.edu The current study is built upon prior research and is an

More information

Stability and Change of Adolescent. Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study. Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill. Division of Psychology

Stability and Change of Adolescent. Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study. Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill. Division of Psychology Stability and Change of Adolescent Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill Division of Psychology The Australian National University Paper presented to the

More information

Personality and Individual Differences

Personality and Individual Differences Personality and Individual Differences 50 (2011) 1243 1248 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid An examination

More information

Emotional Acculturation. topic of this chapter. We conceive of emotional acculturation as the process by which

Emotional Acculturation. topic of this chapter. We conceive of emotional acculturation as the process by which 1 Emotional Acculturation Jozefien De Leersnyder, Batja Mesquita, & Heejung Kim Do emotions acculturate when people move from one culture to the next? That is the topic of this chapter. We conceive of

More information

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2014, 42(7), 1105-1116 Society for Personality Research http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2014.42.7.1105 ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN

More information

Depression among human care service professionals and positive/negative work life events: a structural equation modeling analysis

Depression among human care service professionals and positive/negative work life events: a structural equation modeling analysis Kawasaki Medical Journal ( ) doi. /KMJ-E ( ) 53 Regular Article Depression among human care service professionals and positive/negative work life events: a structural equation modeling analysis Hiromichi

More information

PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE?

PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE? Submitted: February 25, 2017 Revision: March 30, 2017 Accepted: April 27, 2017 PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE? Masataka Takebe Koji Murata Graduate

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION In the format provided by the authors and unedited. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION VOLUME: 1 ARTICLE NUMBER: 0056 Online Supplement On the benefits of explaining herd immunity in vaccine advocacy Cornelia Betsch

More information

Cultural variation in implicit independence: An extension of Kitayama et al.

Cultural variation in implicit independence: An extension of Kitayama et al. University of Massachusetts Amherst From the SelectedWorks of Jiyoung Park Winter February 25, 2015 Cultural variation in implicit independence: An extension of Kitayama et al. Jiyoung Park, University

More information

Sense-making Approach in Determining Health Situation, Information Seeking and Usage

Sense-making Approach in Determining Health Situation, Information Seeking and Usage DOI: 10.7763/IPEDR. 2013. V62. 16 Sense-making Approach in Determining Health Situation, Information Seeking and Usage Ismail Sualman 1 and Rosni Jaafar 1 Faculty of Communication and Media Studies, Universiti

More information

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Subhrasikha Naik Senior Study and research chronic diseases Participate in gathering data for Project Cope, which is focused

More information

Original Article. Relationship between sport participation behavior and the two types of sport commitment of Japanese student athletes

Original Article. Relationship between sport participation behavior and the two types of sport commitment of Japanese student athletes Journal of Physical Education and Sport (JPES), 17(4), Art 267, pp. 2412-2416, 2017 online ISSN: 2247-806X; p-issn: 2247 8051; ISSN - L = 2247-8051 JPES Original Article Relationship between sport participation

More information

Title. Author(s)Schug, Joanna; Yuki, Masaki; Maddux, William W. CitationPsychological Science, 21(10): Issue Date Doc URL.

Title. Author(s)Schug, Joanna; Yuki, Masaki; Maddux, William W. CitationPsychological Science, 21(10): Issue Date Doc URL. Title Relational Mobility Explains Between- and Within-Cul Author(s)Schug, Joanna; Yuki, Masaki; Maddux, William W. CitationPsychological Science, 21(10): 1471-1478 Issue Date 2010-10 Doc URL http://hdl.handle.net/2115/47193

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 6, Number 1 Submitted: February 3, 2010 Revisions: March 7, 2010 Accepted: March 8, 2010 Publication Date: March 10, 2010 Sex Differences in Romantic Jealousy:

More information

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect Motiv Emot (2006) 30:165 169 DOI 10.1007/s11031-006-9001-5 ORIGINAL PAPER Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect David K. Sherman Traci

More information

The Paradox of Received Social Support

The Paradox of Received Social Support PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Paradox of Received Social Support The Importance of Natalya C. Maisel 1 and Shelly L. Gable 2 1 University of California, Los Angeles, and 2 University of California,

More information

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter Lawrence Feinstein Jyllian Call University of California, Los Angeles December, 1986 Further information on the instrument can be obtained

More information

BELL WORK. List three words that you think describe the "helping process. Be ready to share

BELL WORK. List three words that you think describe the helping process. Be ready to share BELL WORK List three words that you think describe the "helping process. Be ready to share CLASSROOM NORMS Be prepared Turn off cell phones or put them on vibrate Be respectful Clean up after yourself

More information

When Visibility Matters: Short-Term Versus Long-Term Costs and Benefits of Visible and Invisible Support

When Visibility Matters: Short-Term Versus Long-Term Costs and Benefits of Visible and Invisible Support 497802PSPXXX10.1177/0146167213497802Personality and Social Psychology BulletinGirme et al. research-article2013 Article When Visibility Matters: Short-Term Versus Long-Term Costs and Benefits of Visible

More information

Reports of Emotional Expression and Control in. Indian and American College Students

Reports of Emotional Expression and Control in. Indian and American College Students Reports of Emotional Expression and Control in Indian and American College Students ` A thesis submitted to the Miami University Honors Program in partial fulfillment of the requirements for University

More information

C O N T E N T S ... v vi. Job Tasks 38 Job Satisfaction 39. Group Development 6. Leisure Activities 41. Values 44. Instructions 9.

C O N T E N T S ... v vi. Job Tasks 38 Job Satisfaction 39. Group Development 6. Leisure Activities 41. Values 44. Instructions 9. C O N T E N T S LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES v vi INTRODUCTION TO THE FIRO-B INSTRUMENT 1 Overview of Uses 1 THEORY OF INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 3 The Interpersonal Needs 3 Expressed and Wanted Needs 4 The

More information

An International Study of the Reliability and Validity of Leadership/Impact (L/I)

An International Study of the Reliability and Validity of Leadership/Impact (L/I) An International Study of the Reliability and Validity of Leadership/Impact (L/I) Janet L. Szumal, Ph.D. Human Synergistics/Center for Applied Research, Inc. Contents Introduction...3 Overview of L/I...5

More information

Personal Talent Skills Inventory

Personal Talent Skills Inventory Personal Talent Skills Inventory Sales Version Inside Sales Sample Co. 5-30-2013 Introduction Research suggests that the most effective people are those who understand themselves, both their strengths

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological needs & implicit motives Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2016 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 3 Psychological

More information

Verification of the Structural Model concerning Selfesteem, Social Support, and Quality of Life among Multicultural Immigrant Women

Verification of the Structural Model concerning Selfesteem, Social Support, and Quality of Life among Multicultural Immigrant Women , pp.57-62 http://dx.doi.org/10.14257/astl.2015.91.12 Verification of the Structural Model concerning Selfesteem, Social Support, and Quality of Life among Multicultural Immigrant Women Rack In Choi 1

More information

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect. David K. Sherman

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect. David K. Sherman 1 Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect David K. Sherman University of California, Santa Barbara Traci Mann University of California,

More information

Questioning the utility of self efficacy measurements for Indians

Questioning the utility of self efficacy measurements for Indians Questioning the utility of self efficacy measurements for Indians Purva J. Rushi This study examined the influence of academic self-efficacy and social support on the academic success of Indian-American

More information

Examining the Role of Self-esteem in the Association between Emotional Vulnerability and Psychological Well-being

Examining the Role of Self-esteem in the Association between Emotional Vulnerability and Psychological Well-being University of Dayton ecommons Honors Theses University Honors Program Spring 4-2015 Examining the Role of Self-esteem in the Association between Emotional Vulnerability and Psychological Well-being Kathryn

More information

The Mediating Role of Parental Expectations in Culture and Well-Being

The Mediating Role of Parental Expectations in Culture and Well-Being The Mediating Role of Parental Expectations in Culture and Well-Being Shigehiro Oishi University of Virginia Helen W. Sullivan University of Minnesota, Twin Cities Campus ABSTRACT In two studies, we examined

More information

Interpersonal coping styles and couple relationship quality: Similarity versus complementarity hypotheses

Interpersonal coping styles and couple relationship quality: Similarity versus complementarity hypotheses European Journal of Social Psychology, Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 44, 175 186 (2014) Published online 28 January 2014 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2000 Research article

More information

Cultural Variations in the Self and Underlying Neural Mechanisms: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation.

Cultural Variations in the Self and Underlying Neural Mechanisms: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation. Cultural Variations in the Self and Underlying Neural Mechanisms: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation by Jiyoung Park A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements

More information

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Honors Scholar Theses Honors Scholar Program Spring 5-1-2008 Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible Juemei Yang

More information

Relationships between stage of change for stress management behavior and perceived stress and coping

Relationships between stage of change for stress management behavior and perceived stress and coping Japanese Psychological Research 2010, Volume 52, No. 4, 291 297 doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2010.00444.x Short Report Relationships between stage of change for stress management behavior and perceived stress

More information

Running Head: SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 1

Running Head: SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 1 Running Head: SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 1 Does Cultural Differences in Emotion Impact how People Rate their Subjective Well-Being? A Literature Review Aimee C. Cross Grenfell Campus, Memorial University SUBJECTIVE

More information

Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The. Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp ). Blackwell Publishing.

Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The. Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp ). Blackwell Publishing. Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp. 864-867). Blackwell Publishing. Self-Compassion Self-compassion is an open-hearted way

More information

Is the power of weak ties universal? A crosscultural comparison of social interaction in Argentina and Canada

Is the power of weak ties universal? A crosscultural comparison of social interaction in Argentina and Canada Published in Revue Tranel (Travaux neuchâtelois de linguistique) 68, 107-112, 2018 which should be used for any reference to this work Is the power of weak ties universal? A crosscultural comparison of

More information

WHAT IS SELF? MODULE-IV OBJECTIVES 16.1 CONCEPT OF SELF. What is Self? Self and Personality. Notes

WHAT IS SELF? MODULE-IV OBJECTIVES 16.1 CONCEPT OF SELF. What is Self? Self and Personality. Notes What is Self? MODULE-IV 16 WHAT IS SELF? Self is focus of our everyday behaviour and all of us do have a set of perceptions and beliefs about ourselves. This kind of self concept plays important role in

More information

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Adam Hahn 1 Bertram Gawronski 2 Word count: 20,754 excluding acknowledgements, abstract,

More information

Patients as Partners Co-Creation in Health Care

Patients as Partners Co-Creation in Health Care Patients as Partners Co-Creation in Health Care Michael Bartl 1. Introduction Most of the time we discuss concepts like consumer centric-design, co-creation or open innovation in the context of business

More information

Behavioral EQ MULTI-RATER PROFILE. Prepared for: By: Session: 22 Jul Madeline Bertrand. Sample Organization

Behavioral EQ MULTI-RATER PROFILE. Prepared for: By: Session: 22 Jul Madeline Bertrand. Sample Organization Behavioral EQ MULTI-RATER PROFILE Prepared for: Madeline Bertrand By: Sample Organization Session: Improving Interpersonal Effectiveness 22 Jul 2014 Behavioral EQ, Putting Emotional Intelligence to Work,

More information

Culture & Psychology Mini Conference University of Kent 16 September 2016 KLT4, Keynes College Talks will be 20 min + 10 min for Q&A

Culture & Psychology Mini Conference University of Kent 16 September 2016 KLT4, Keynes College Talks will be 20 min + 10 min for Q&A Culture & Psychology Mini Conference University of Kent 16 September 2016 KLT4, Keynes College Talks will be 20 min + 10 min for Q&A 09:30 10:00 Arrival + Coffee/Tea 10:00 11:00 Anat Bardi Biculturals

More information

A Study of a Diet Improvement Method for Controlling High Sodium Intake Based on Protective Motivation Theory

A Study of a Diet Improvement Method for Controlling High Sodium Intake Based on Protective Motivation Theory pissn 1229-1153 / eissn 2465-9223 J. Food Hyg. Saf. Vol. 33, No. 2, pp. 89~93 (2018) https://doi.org/10.13103/jfhs.2018.33.2.89 Journal of Food Hygiene and Safety Available online at http://www.foodhygiene.or.kr

More information

CULTURE DIFFERENCES IN FEEDBACK EXCHANGE: SHOULD I SEND A OR? Xiaxin Zhong

CULTURE DIFFERENCES IN FEEDBACK EXCHANGE: SHOULD I SEND A OR? Xiaxin Zhong CULTURE DIFFERENCES IN FEEDBACK EXCHANGE: SHOULD I SEND A OR? by Xiaxin Zhong A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the University of Delaware in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

More information

Path Analysis of a Self-Determination Model of Work Motivation in Vocational Rehabilitation

Path Analysis of a Self-Determination Model of Work Motivation in Vocational Rehabilitation Path Analysis of a Self-Determination Model of Work Motivation in Vocational Rehabilitation Timothy N. Tansey Jill Bezyak Kanako Iwanaga Cayte Anderson Nicole Ditchman This presentation is being offered

More information

A Modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to Include an Assessment of Amotivation

A Modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to Include an Assessment of Amotivation JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2004, 26, 191-196 2004 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. A Modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to Include an Assessment of Amotivation

More information

THE CUSTOMER SERVICE ATTRIBUTE INDEX

THE CUSTOMER SERVICE ATTRIBUTE INDEX THE CUSTOMER SERVICE ATTRIBUTE INDEX Jane Doe Customer Service XYZ Corporation 7-22-2003 CRITICAL SUCCESS ATTRIBUTES ATTITUDE TOWARD OTHERS: To what extent does Jane tend to maintain a positive, open and

More information

Volitional Autonomy and Relatedness: Mediators explaining Non Tenure Track Faculty Job. Satisfaction

Volitional Autonomy and Relatedness: Mediators explaining Non Tenure Track Faculty Job. Satisfaction Autonomy and : Mediators explaining Non Tenure Track Faculty Job Satisfaction Non-tenure track (NTT) faculty are increasingly utilized in higher education and shoulder much of the teaching load within

More information

Never Good Enough: Advising Perfectionist Students. Katie Dunn, Senior Academic Advisor, Brandeis University NACADA Region

Never Good Enough: Advising Perfectionist Students. Katie Dunn, Senior Academic Advisor, Brandeis University NACADA Region Never Good Enough: Advising Perfectionist Students Katie Dunn, Senior Academic Advisor, Brandeis University Think of a perfectionistic student you worked with recently 1) What thoughts, feelings or behaviors

More information

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect Abstract This article reports the effects of hedonic versus utilitarian consumption goals on consumers choices between

More information

Comparing normative influences as determinants of environmentally conscious behaviours between the USA and Japan

Comparing normative influences as determinants of environmentally conscious behaviours between the USA and Japan Asian Journal of Social Psychology (2007), 10, 171 178 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2007.00223.x Comparing normative influences as determinants of environmentally conscious behaviours between the USA and Japan

More information

Relationship of Stress Coping Strategies and Life Satisfaction among Students

Relationship of Stress Coping Strategies and Life Satisfaction among Students IPA International Journal of Psychology Vol. 8, No. 1, Winter & Spring 2014 PP. 156-165 Iranian Psychological Association Relationship of Stress Coping Strategies and Life Satisfaction among Students Bahman

More information

Supplementary material. Cross-Cultural Differences and Similarities in Proneness to Shame: An Adaptationist and Ecological Approach

Supplementary material. Cross-Cultural Differences and Similarities in Proneness to Shame: An Adaptationist and Ecological Approach Supplementary material Cross-Cultural Differences and Similarities in Proneness to Shame: An Adaptationist and Ecological Approach Shame Proneness scale (Friend version) Note: Across the samples, Cronbach

More information

DISPOSITIONAL POSITIVE EMOTIONS SCALE (DPES) COMPASSION SUBSCALE.

DISPOSITIONAL POSITIVE EMOTIONS SCALE (DPES) COMPASSION SUBSCALE. DISPOSITIONAL POSITIVE EMOTIONS SCALE (DPES) COMPASSION SUBSCALE. Reference: Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John O. P. (2006). Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality

More information

The original reason that influences international students help-seeking behavior: Familiarity

The original reason that influences international students help-seeking behavior: Familiarity The original reason that influences international students help-seeking behavior: Familiarity Zhoujun Sun University Preparatory Program in Tufts University, Medford, Massachusetts Abstract Mental illness

More information

A TEST OF SHARKEY AND SINGELIS (1995) MODEL OF SELF-CONSTRUAL AND EMBARRASSABILITY: SITUATIONAL VERSUS DISPOSITIONAL FACTORS 1

A TEST OF SHARKEY AND SINGELIS (1995) MODEL OF SELF-CONSTRUAL AND EMBARRASSABILITY: SITUATIONAL VERSUS DISPOSITIONAL FACTORS 1 A TEST OF SHARKEY AND SINGELIS (1995) MODEL / G. H. J. KRUGER A TEST OF SHARKEY AND SINGELIS (1995) MODEL OF SELF-CONSTRUAL AND EMBARRASSABILITY: SITUATIONAL VERSUS DISPOSITIONAL FACTORS 1 GERT H. J. KRUGER

More information

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Heather Dudley What influences people s willingness to interact with someone of a different cultural background from

More information

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY http://www.uiowa.edu/crisp Submitted: May 3, 2016 First Revision: June 3, 2016 Second Revision: August 26, 2016 Accepted: August 30, 2016 WHY DO EXTRAVERTS FEEL MORE

More information

Supplementary Materials. Worksite Wellness Programs: Sticks (Not Carrots) Send Stigmatizing Signals

Supplementary Materials. Worksite Wellness Programs: Sticks (Not Carrots) Send Stigmatizing Signals Supplementary Materials Worksite Wellness Programs: Sticks (Not Carrots) Send Stigmatizing Signals David Tannenbaum, Chad J. Valasek, Eric D. Knowles, Peter H. Ditto Additional Mediation Analyses for Study

More information

Athletic Identity and Life Roles of Division I and Division III Collegiate Athletes

Athletic Identity and Life Roles of Division I and Division III Collegiate Athletes ATHLETIC IDENTITY AND LIFE ROLES OF DIVISION I AND DIVISION III COLLEGIATE ATHLETES 225 Athletic Identity and Life Roles of Division I and Division III Collegiate Athletes Katie A. Griffith and Kristine

More information

THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION

THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION 92 THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION 1. Motivation is a highly dynamic construct that is constantly changing in reaction to life experiences. 2. Needs and goals are constantly growing and changing. 3. As individuals

More information

C H A P T E R. Leadership. Chapter 9: Leadership

C H A P T E R. Leadership. Chapter 9: Leadership C H A P T E R 9 Leadership Chapter 9: Leadership A boss creates fear, a leader confidence. A boss fixes blame, a leader corrects mistakes. A boss knows all, a leader asks questions. A boss makes work

More information

BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory. Resource Report. John Morris. Name: ID: Admin. Date: December 15, 2010 (Online) 17 Minutes 22 Seconds

BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory. Resource Report. John Morris. Name: ID: Admin. Date: December 15, 2010 (Online) 17 Minutes 22 Seconds BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory By Reuven Bar-On, Ph.D. Resource Report Name: ID: Admin. Date: Duration: John Morris December 15, 2010 (Online) 17 Minutes 22 Seconds Copyright 2002 Multi-Health Systems

More information

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping Amanda Palmer, Ken Sinclair and Michael Bailey University of Sydney Paper prepared for presentation

More information

VALUES OF THE PAST AND THE FUTURE: CULTURAL DIFFERENCES IN TEMPORAL VALUE ASYMMETRY

VALUES OF THE PAST AND THE FUTURE: CULTURAL DIFFERENCES IN TEMPORAL VALUE ASYMMETRY VALUES OF THE PAST AND THE FUTURE: CULTURAL DIFFERENCES IN TEMPORAL VALUE ASYMMETRY by Tieyuan Guo A thesis submitted to the Department of Psychology in conformity with the requirements for the degree

More information

Interconnectedness - The key to personal and collective well-being

Interconnectedness - The key to personal and collective well-being Interconnectedness - The key to personal and collective well-being Day: Wednesday 11th July 2018 Time: 9:00 10:15 am Track: Philosophical and Dharma Underpinnings In Buddhism, interconnectedness, or the

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 4, Number 1 Submitted: August 20, 2008 Revisions: October 16, 2008 Accepted: October 17, 2008 Publication Date: October 20, 2008 Start Today or the Very

More information

Goal of Cross-Cultural Research

Goal of Cross-Cultural Research Cross Cultural Research Dr. K. A. Korb Goal of Cross-Cultural Research Examine differences or similarities of psychological variables in two or more cultures Determine the meaning of similarities or differences

More information

The role of self-aspects in emotions elicited by threats to physical health

The role of self-aspects in emotions elicited by threats to physical health University of Kent From the SelectedWorks of Ayse K Uskul 2014 The role of self-aspects in emotions elicited by threats to physical health Ayse K Uskul, University of Kent Michaela Hynie, York University

More information

Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale. Interpretive Report. Paul L. Hewitt, Ph.D. & Gordon L. Flett, Ph.D.

Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale. Interpretive Report. Paul L. Hewitt, Ph.D. & Gordon L. Flett, Ph.D. Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale Paul L. Hewitt, Ph.D. & Gordon L. Flett, Ph.D. Interpretive Report This Interpretive Report is intended for the sole use of the test administrator and is not to be

More information

Study of relationship between Emotional Intelligence and Social Adjustment

Study of relationship between Emotional Intelligence and Social Adjustment Third 21st CAF Conference at Harvard, in Boston, USA. September 2015, Vol. 6, Nr. 1 ISSN: 2330-1236 Study of relationship between Emotional Santwana G. Mishra Dr. Babasaheb Ambedkar Marathwada University

More information

Chapter 9. Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS)

Chapter 9. Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS) Chapter 9 Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS) Background Purpose The Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS) is a measure of perceived effectiveness of a specific counseling session. In general, measures

More information

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Procrastination Assessment Scale for Students

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Procrastination Assessment Scale for Students 611456SGOXXX10.1177/2158244015611456SAGE OpenYockey and Kralowec research-article2015 Article Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Procrastination Assessment Scale for Students SAGE Open October-December

More information

12/31/2016. PSY 512: Advanced Statistics for Psychological and Behavioral Research 2

12/31/2016. PSY 512: Advanced Statistics for Psychological and Behavioral Research 2 PSY 512: Advanced Statistics for Psychological and Behavioral Research 2 Introduce moderated multiple regression Continuous predictor continuous predictor Continuous predictor categorical predictor Understand

More information

Black 1 White 5 Black

Black 1 White 5 Black PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Black 1 White 5 Black Hypodescent in Reflexive Categorization of Racially Ambiguous Faces Destiny Peery and Galen V. Bodenhausen Northwestern University ABSTRACT Historically,

More information

TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Coaching Report

TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Coaching Report TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Coaching Report "He who knows others is learned. He who knows himself is wise." Lao Tse Mason Roberts District Manager YMCA 8-1-2008 Copyright 2003-2008. Performance

More information

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto Session VI, September 20 2017 Learning objectives 1. Get familiar with the basic idea

More information

Running head: ANGER AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 1. The Effect of Third-party Anger on Prosocial Behavior. Bachelor s Thesis. Tilburg University

Running head: ANGER AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 1. The Effect of Third-party Anger on Prosocial Behavior. Bachelor s Thesis. Tilburg University Running head: ANGER AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 1 The Effect of Third-party Anger on Prosocial Behavior Bachelor s Thesis Tilburg University Pien Vingerhoets 714473 Supervised by Janne van Doorn 2 February

More information

National Culture Dimensions and Consumer Digital Piracy: A European Perspective

National Culture Dimensions and Consumer Digital Piracy: A European Perspective National Culture Dimensions and Consumer Digital Piracy: A European Perspective Abstract Irena Vida, irena.vida@ef.uni-lj.si Monika Kukar-Kinney, mkukarki@richmond.edu Mateja Kos Koklič, mateja.kos@ef.uni-lj.si

More information

Exam 3 PS 306, Spring 2005

Exam 3 PS 306, Spring 2005 Exam 3 PS 306, Spring 2005 1. In a classic study, Tulving and Gold (1963) studied word identification under conditions of amounts of relevant and irrelevant context. Let s conceive of their study as a

More information

The Impact of Culture on Consumers Perceptions of Service Recovery Efforts

The Impact of Culture on Consumers Perceptions of Service Recovery Efforts The Impact of Culture on Consumers Perceptions of Service Recovery Efforts Anna S. Mattila, Pennsylvania State University And Paul Patterson University of Southern Wales, Australia Dissatisfaction with

More information

Kent Academic Repository

Kent Academic Repository Kent Academic Repository Full text document (pdf) Citation for published version Uskul, Ayse K. and Kikutani, Mariko (2014) Concerns about losing face moderate the effect of visual perspective on health-related

More information

What Affects the Way Individuals Cope with Stress?

What Affects the Way Individuals Cope with Stress? LP 8D hardiness 1 What Affects the Way Individuals Cope with Stress? Hardiness (Commitment, Control and Challenge) Optimism versus pessimism (see chapter 3) Coping options o Change environments o Change

More information

PHASE 1 OCDA Scale Results: Psychometric Assessment and Descriptive Statistics for Partner Libraries

PHASE 1 OCDA Scale Results: Psychometric Assessment and Descriptive Statistics for Partner Libraries Running head: PHASE I OCDA RESULTS PHASE 1 OCDA Scale Results: Psychometric Assessment and Descriptive Statistics for Partner Libraries Paul J. Hanges, Juliet Aiken, Xiafang Chen & Hali Chambers University

More information

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question Trivia Question Where did win one for the gipper come from? Chapter 9 What are the 3 components of motivation? 3 major categories of motivation. Major theories of motivation. How the theories are applied

More information