A Comparison of Monkey Afferent Nerve Spike Rates and Spike Latencies for Classifying Torque, Normal Force and Direction

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "A Comparison of Monkey Afferent Nerve Spike Rates and Spike Latencies for Classifying Torque, Normal Force and Direction"

Transcription

1 A Comparison of Monkey Afferent Nerve Spike Rates and Spike Latencies for Classifying Torque, Normal Force and Direction Stephen J. Redmond *, Antony W. Goodwin, Nigel H. Lovell *, Ingvars Birznieks * University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 252, Australia Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute, Sydney, NSW 23, Australia Department of Anatomy and Cell Biology, University of Melbourne, Australia s.redmond@unsw.edu.au, a.goodwin@unimelb.edu.au, n.lovell@unsw.edu.au, i.birznieks@powmri.edu.au Abstract The movement of primate hands and limbs is governed by a biological control system which utilizes information from tactile sensory afferents. However, it is not entirely understood how complex stimuli, such as force, direction and torque applied to the finger pad, are encoded within the population responses of these afferent mechanoreceptors. This study investigates whether first spike latency after the onset of a torque stimulus, superimposed on an applied normal force, from a population of 25 fast-adapting type-i (FA-I) afferents, can achieve a comparable classification accuracy (using a Parzen window classifier) as the more traditional feature of spike count. Results show that FA-I afferents perform poorly at identifying the three levels of static normal force applied, but achieve % recognition rate within 5 ms when classifying two different magnitudes and directions of torque, with an accuracy of over 9% within 3 ms of the torque onset. This is in line with other reported studies which indicate that afferent first spike latency contains similar information about tactile stimuli as spike count. I. INTRODUCTION The movement of primate hands and limbs is governed by a biological control system which utilizes information from tactile sensory afferents. However, it is not entirely understood how complex stimuli, such as force, direction and torque applied to the finger pad, are encoded within the population responses of these afferent mechanoreceptors. Performing seemingly mundane manipulation of delicate objects, such as grasping, lifting and rotating, becomes a nontrivial task in the absence of tactile sensory input []. Tactile afferents encode critical manipulative parameters, which occur during simple object manipulation, like the grip force or normal force applied to the object to prevent slip and/or rotation; achieving the latter implies that a torque is also being applied. Torque is ubiquitous in everyday tasks and arises when an object s centre of mass does not align with the vector of force application [2, 3]. Human experiments employing a two finger opposition grip [4-6] and tasks where objects are held in a multi-finger grasp [7-] have been used to investigate sensorimotor mechanisms controlling the effects of torque during object manipulation. Predictive strategies based on internal representations which must be updated by sensory inputs, have been shown to be largely responsible for smooth self-paced object manipulation and coordination of fingertip forces [2]. An adequate reaction can only be triggered when errors or unpredictable events occur in the sensory input, which do not align with predicted events. We have recently investigated how such information is encoded in the activity generated by the afferent neurons, or mechanoreceptors, in the finger pads of monkeys [3, 4]. In these studies, a Parzen window classification model was applied to the data ensemble, consisting of accumulated spike counts (summing from the onset of the stimulus) to investigate how accurately the various stimulus attributes, of normal force, torque and direction, were encoded in the population of afferent nerve responses. These analyses attempted to solve how interactions between numerous stimulus features may be disentangled in real-time. While studies, like those mentioned above, have shown that features of the afferent nerve response, such as accumulated spike count, contain relevant information to perform the realtime classification of multiple simultaneous stimuli parameters, others have demonstrated that features, such as the first spike latency also contain the same (if not more) information about the same stimuli [5, 6]. The aim of this study is to compare the real-time classification performance when using spike count features against first spike latency features to discriminate between the normal force and torque applied to the finger pad, and the direction which the torque is applied. The same methodology is applied as was used in [3, 4], which is summarized in the following sections. II. METHODS A. Neural recordings Recordings of afferent nerve responses were made from three anesthetized Macaca nemestrina monkeys (weight: 4.5 to 7.6 kg), with approval from the University of Melbourne Ethics Committee and conforming to the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia s Code of Practice for non-human primate research APSIPA. All rights reserved. 72 Proceedings of the Second APSIPA Annual Summit and Conference, pages , Biopolis, Singapore, 4-7 December 2.

2 After inducing surgical anesthesia, single cutaneous mechanoreceptive afferents in the median nerve were isolated and recorded from, using standard procedures [7]. Using established criteria, the responses of each afferent to static and rapidly changing stimuli, and the receptive field size, was analyzed [8]. Calibrated von Frey hairs were used to determine the location of each receptive field. Using the results of this analysis of afferent response characteristics and receptive field area, each single fiber was labeled into wellknown categories of slowly adapting type-i (SA-I), fastadapting type-i (FA-I), or fast-adapting type-ii (FA-II) afferents. All 25 responding FA-I afferents innervating the glabrous skin of the distal phalanx of digits 2, 3 and 4 were included in this study; the response of FA-II afferents was not reliable. It is not instructive to use SA-I afferents, since we wish to examine first spike latency after the onset of the torque stimulus, however, the background normal force applied (discussed below) will cause the SA-I afferents to continuously fire, making them unsuitable for first spike latency analysis. In order to securely affix the digits, they were splayed and the dorsal aspect of the hand was embedded in plasticine up to the mid-level of the middle phalanges. The distal phalanges were stabilized by gluing the fingernails to small metal plates which were fixed to a post embedded in the plasticine. The fingertip was free to deform as the glabrous skin of the distal phalanges was not in contact with the plasticine (see Methods in [9]). B. Stimulation procedure ) Stimulator: The same stimulation procedure was applied as in [3, 4], using a custom-made mechanical stimulator, controlled using LabVIEW 5 software (National Instruments, Austin, TX), applying concurrent normal force and torque forces. The forces and torques were measured using a six-axis force-torque transducer (Nano FT; ATI Industrial Automation, Apex, NC). The resolution of the transducer to force was.25 N. The torque resolution was.625 mnm. The stimulus applicator was a flat circular surface (diameter 24 mm) covered with fine grain sandpaper (5 grade). 2) Application of torsional loads: The stimulus was applied to the center of the flat portion of the volar surface of the fingertip on digits 2, 3 and 4. The normal force direction of application was at right angles to the skin surface. Three normal force magnitudes (.8, 2.2 and 2.5 N) and two torque magnitudes (2. and 3.5 mnm) were applied in both clockwise and anticlockwise directions. This gave 2 (3 2 2) force, torque, direction combinations. For each normal force, each torque was applied in ascending order. Once both torque values were applied for a given normal force, the normal force was increased and the torques were reapplied. This was repeated until all three normal forces had been applied for both torque values. This entire process was repeated six times, after which the entire procedure was repeated six more times with the torque applied in the opposite direction. In summary, there were 2 different stimulation combinations, each applied 6 times in total, giving 72 different training examples for the classifier model (described later), when the responses of all afferents were considered as a simultaneously acquired ensemble. For details on the time evolution of the normal force and torque, the reader is referred to [3]. A summary of the force/torque application process is as follows. The normal force was ramped up during a s loading phase; the normal force was held constant for 3.6 s of the plateau phase, followed by a s retraction phase removing the normal force. The application of the torque commenced s after the plateau phase of the normal force had begun and was superimposed on this constant normal force. The torque was loaded over s, held constant at its maximum value for.5 s, and unloaded over a final s duration. Therefore, the normal force was completely retracted s after the unloading of the torque force. C. Classification features A classification of the stimulus parameters (normal force, torque and direction) was performed at ms intervals after the onset of the torque. The classifier model design is described in later sections. This section describes the features used for classification. There are two types of features under comparison; accumulated spike count and first spike latency, as outlined in the following sections. ) Accumulated spike count: The accumulated spike count is simply the total number of spikes which have occurred since the onset of the torque application, evaluated in ms intervals. 2) First spike latency: There are different ways which the first spike latency, τ, could be represented, since before and after the occurrence of the first spike some feature value must be assigned to each afferent response, as dealing with missing features is problematic when training classifier models [2]. Therefore two different feature values are investigated for each afferent to represent the first spike latency: (i) the feature takes the value of before the first spike occurs and τ after; (ii) the feature value is before the first spike occurs and afterwards (acting as a binary indicator of whether the nerve has fired yet, or not). D. Stimulus classification using afferent populations ) Feature vector ensemble: As described in Section II.B.2, there were 2 stimulus combinations applied. Each combination of force, torque and direction was applied six times, leading to 72 training patterns, or feature vectors. Each stimulus combination, i, leads to a 25 element feature vector x i. Each element the feature vector is a spike count, or first spike latency value, from one of the 25 FA-I afferents, as described in Section II.B.2. Note that while only one afferent is recorded from at any one time, the feature vector is constructed as an ensemble, as if all afferents were recorded simultaneously. 2) Parzen window classifier model: A Parzen window classifier model was used to discriminate between each of the stimulus parameter classes [2]. There are three classes for normal force and two for each of torque magnitude and torque 72

3 before, τ after - before, after (a) Normal force Torque magnitude Torque direction (b) - before, τ after - before, after (c) - before, τ after - before, after Fig.. Classification performance using two different first spike latency features. (a) Classification accuracy for normal force, (b) torque and (c) direction of torque application. Classification is performed at ms intervals after the start of torque application. The two different latency features are described in Section II.C.2. Chance performance would be 3, and, respectively, for the three plots. direction, corresponding to each of the stimulus values applied (see Section II.B.2). For example, in the case of classifying normal force there are three classes ω {.8, 2.2, 2.5} N. Separate independent classifiers are designed for the different classification tasks of normal force, torque and direction. Equal a priori probabilities are assumed for each class of each stimulus classification task. A multivariate normal Gaussian window is used for the Parzen window classifier. The radius variable for the Gaussian window, r, which serves to adjust the smoothness of the nonparametric density estimation, is given the value r= after each afferent response feature has been normalized across all training vectors, by removing the mean and dividing by the standard deviation. 3) Cross-fold validation: A leave-one-out cross-fold validation scheme was employed to obtain an unbiased estimate of generalized classification performance [2]. After removing a single test vector from the training data the remaining 7 feature vectors were used to train the Parzen window classifier. The withheld vector was then reintroduced for testing once training was complete. This process was repeated for all 72 training patterns. The results presented below represent the percentage of these 72 test patterns which were correctly classified. E. Classification comparisons ) Comparison of latency features: In order to determine which representation of first spike latency achieves the best classification performance, the two proposed features for representing spike latency are compared. 2) Comparison of spike count and first spike latency: Using the best performing spike latency feature, a comparison is performed between the discriminative powers of first spike latency and accumulated spike count. 3) Combined performance of spike latency and count: Finally, the combined classification accuracy achieved when simultaneously using spike count and latency for the classification task is investigated by appending the two feature vectors together, to give a 5 element feature vector. III. RESULTS A. comparison results Fig. (a), (b) and (c) show the plots of classification accuracy for normal force, torque magnitude and torque direction, respectively. On each graph, the classification accuracy, assessed at ms intervals is shown when using each of the two proposed latency features, described in Section II.C.2. The graphs assess the epoch of time from the onset of the torque until 2 ms after the torque has reached its maximum and is being held constant. We note from Fig. that the first spike latency representation which uses a zero feature value before the occurrence of the first spike, and the latency time afterwards, provides the maximum classification accuracy for two of the three classification tasks at 7 ms (2 ms after the torque has reached its plateau), with the classification performance for torque only marginally worse than using the binary indicator feature. B. versus spike count results Fig. 2 illustrates the comparative performance of the accumulated spike count, and the best first spike latency feature, which uses the latency time as a feature value after the first spike is generated for that afferent. C. Combined latency and count performance 722

4 Fig. 2 also contains a plot of the classification accuracy obtained using a feature vector which employs both spike count and first spike latency. IV. DISCUSSION Examining Fig. we see that there is little discriminative capacity in the first spike latency before 25 ms. After 25 ms, the classification accuracy begins to improve for all classification tasks. For normal force, the classification accuracy does not improve much beyond chance, even when 7 ms is reached. At this point the spike latency measure, (a) Normal force Torque magnitude Torque direction (b) (c) Fig. 2. Comparison of the classification performance of the first spike latency (time elapsed before first spike) and the accumulated spike count on the three classification tasks of (a) normal force, (b) torque magnitude and (c) torque direction. features allow close to % classification accuracy of torque parameters 7 ms after the stimulus onset. Chance performance would be 3, and, respectively, for the three plots. which fixes the feature with the spike latency after the first spike occurs, provides the best performance of both proposed latency features, for all three classification tasks normal force, torque and direction. The response for FA type afferents is not expected to be crucial for discriminating normal force in our analyzed dataset, as FA afferents only respond to dynamically changing features of stimuli, and at the onset of torque application the normal force has already been at its plateau for s, and thus does not per se excite FA afferents. However, normal force information theoretically could still be extracted based on the interaction effect normal force might have on afferent responses to torque; for instance, if normal force was to somehow modulate the FA afferents sensitivity to torque. latency features perform similarly when classifying torque magnitude and direction, obtaining high accuracies by approximately 35 ms after the stimulus onset. Interestingly, using a simple binary indicator feature, which indicates if the afferent has fired at least once since the onset of the torque, shows very similar performance in the early stages of the recognition task (around 25 ms), when compared to the other latency feature, which contains more detailed timing information. Gautrais and Thorpe were the first to identify information encoded in temporal activation order for the visual system [2] although the usefulness of afferent recruitment order was first shown by Johansson and Birznieks [5]. Also, examining the time around 7 ms, when the binary latency feature shows good performance, illustrates that the final activation pattern of the afferents contains enough information to complete the classification task. The dip in classification performance for this feature, around 38 ms when classifying torque magnitude, may be an indicator that the order in which the afferents are activated during this period varies from trial to trial. Since the binary feature removes the actual latency timing information (and only codes the population activation distribution) it is too difficult to perform the discrimination task using so little information during this dynamic phase. Fig. 2 reproduces the curves shown in Fig., for the latency feature, in addition to the performance achieved when using a more traditional feature of the afferent responses the accumulated spike count. The similarity of the curves shows that first spike latency contains enough information to perform the classification task with a comparable accuracy to using spike count. Examining both Fig. (b) and Fig. 2 (b) together, one anomaly to note is the dip in accuracy which occurs around 4 ms. This happens when using the spike count feature (Fig. 2 (b)) and a similar dip in performance is observed in Fig. (b), when using the binary indicator of whether the afferent had fired, or not, at each time interval. This might imply that in fact spike latency is a more robust feature for classifying the stimulus parameters used, when compared to population activation features, of which the binary latency feature and the spike count are approximate surrogates. Finally, also shown in Fig. 2 are results illustrating that combining both the spike latency and spike count features and 723

5 repeating the classification tasks demonstrates no discernable performance improvement over using either feature alone. This indicates that the latency and count contain mostly the same information about the stimulus. However, a bootstrap analysis is required to verify this claim. That is not to say that this will always be the case for all stimulus events not tested here; however, within the constraints of the stimuli applied here, both contain adequate information to perform the classification tasks of recognizing torque magnitude and direction. Similar studies of first spike latency have been performed and appear to concur with the results presented here. An excellent paper by Saal et al. analyzes and compares the entropy information contained in spike latency and spike counts, for single afferent responses under similar stimulation conditions (although torque is not used). They show the information content in spike latency is 2.2 and.6 times that of spike count for the discrimination tasks of determining applicator curvature and force direction, respectively [6]. However, selecting features for classification tasks based on entropy information measures is known to provide no guarantee of improved classification performance, due to what may possibly be complex correlations between features. As a result, there is a sizeable literature on the topic of feature selection for pattern classification which attempts to address this issue [22, 23]. V. CONCLUSION This study has demonstrated that first spike latency after the onset of a torque stimulus, superimposed on an applied normal force, from a population of 25 FA-I afferents recorded from Macaca nemestrina monkeys, achieves similar classification accuracy as using the more traditional feature of spike count to quantify the afferent population response. FA-I afferents perform poorly at identifying the three levels of static normal force applied, but achieve almost % recognition rate on two different magnitudes and directions of applied torque within 5 ms, and an accuracy of over 9% within 3 ms. This is in line with other reported studies which indicate that afferent first spike latency contains similar information about tactile stimuli as the spike count. VI. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This research was supported by the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) and Australian Research Council (ARC) Thinking Systems grant TS The neurophysiology experiments were performed at the Department of Anatomy and Cell Biology, University of Melbourne, Australia. We would like to thank Dr Heather Wheat and Dr Lauren Salo for their contribution to the collection of the neurophysiological data. REFERENCES [] J. R. Flanagan, M. C. Bowman, and R. S. Johansson, "Control strategies in object manipulation tasks," Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., vol. 6, pp , 26. [2] R. S. Johansson and G. Westling, "Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects," Exp. Brain Res., vol. 56, pp , 984. [3] H. Kinoshita, L. Bäckström, J. R. Flanagan, and R. S. Johansson, "Tangential torque effects on the control of grip forces when holding objects with a precision grip," J. Neurophysiol., vol. 78, pp , 997. [4] A. W. Goodwin, P. Jenmalm, and R. S. Johansson, "Control of grip force when tilting objects: effect of curvature of grasped surfaces and of applied tangential torque," J. Neurosci., vol. 8, pp , 998. [5] A. M. Wing and S. J. Lederman, "Anticipating load torques produced by voluntary movements," J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., vol. 24, pp , 998. [6] R. S. Johansson, J. L. Backlin, and M. K. O. Burstedt, "Control of grasp stability during pronation and supination movements," Exp. Brain Res., vol. 28, pp. 2-3, 999. [7] M. K. O. Burstedt, R. Flanagan, and R. S. Johansson, "Control of grasp stability in humans under different frictional conditions during multidigit manipulation," J. Neurophysiol., vol. 82, pp , 999. [8] J. R. Flanagan, M. K. O. Burstedt, and R. S. Johansson, "Control of fingertip forces in multi-digit manipulation," J. Neurophysiol., vol. 8, pp , 999. [9] M. L. Latash, J. K. Shim, F. Gao, and V. M. Zatsiorsky, "Rotational equilibrium during multi-digit pressing and prehension," Motor Control, vol. 8, pp , 24. [] J. K. Shim, M. L. Latash, and V. M. Zatsiorsky, "Prehension synergies in three dimensions," J. Neurophysiol., vol. 93, pp , 25. [] J. K. Shim, J. Park, V. M. Zatsiorsky, and M. L. Latash, "Adjustments of prehension synergies in response to self-triggered and experimentertriggered load and torque perturbations," Exp. Brain Res., vol. 75, pp , 26. [2] D. A. Nowak, S. Glasauer, and J. Hermsdörfer, "Grip force efficiency in long-term deprivation of somatosensory feedback," Neuroreport, vol. 4, pp , 23. [3] I. Birznieks, H. E. Wheat, S. J. Redmond, L. M. Salo, N. H. Lovell, and A. W. Goodwin, "Encoding of tangential torque in responses of tactile afferent fibres innervating the fingerpad of the monkey," The Journal of Physiology, vol. 588, pp , 2. [4] S. J. Redmond, I. Birznieks, N. H. Lovell, and A. W. Goodwin, "Classifying Torque, Normal Force and Direction using Monkey Afferent Nerve Spike Rates," in Eurohaptics Amsterdam, Holland, 2 (in press). [5] R. S. Johansson and I. Birznieks, "First spikes in ensembles of human tactile afferents code complex spatial fingertip events," Nat Neurosci, vol. 7, pp. 7-77, 24. [6] H. P. Saal, S. Vijayakumar, and R. S. Johansson, "Information about Complex Fingertip Parameters in Individual Human Tactile Afferent Neurons," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 29, pp , 29. [7] A. W. Goodwin, A. S. Browning, and H. E. Wheat, "Representation of curved surfaces in responses of mechanoreceptive afferent fibers innervating the monkey's fingerpad," J. Neurosci., vol. 5, pp , 995. [8] A. B. Vallbo and R. S. Johansson, "Properties of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the human hand related to touch sensation," Hum. Neurobiol., vol. 3, pp. 3-4, 984. [9] I. Birznieks, P. Jenmalm, A. W. Goodwin, and R. S. Johansson, "Encoding of Direction of Fingertip Forces by Human Tactile Afferents," J. Neurosci., vol. 2, pp , 2. [2] R. O. Duda, P. E. Hart, and D. G. Stork, Pattern Classification (2nd edition), 2 ed.: John Wiley & Sons, 2. [2] R. V. Rullen and S. J. Thorpe, "Rate Coding Versus Temporal Order Coding: What the Retinal Ganglion Cells Tell the Visual Cortex," Neural Comput., vol. 3, pp , 2. [22] N. Abe and M. Kudo, "Entropy Criterion for Classifier-Independent Feature Selection," in Knowledge-Based Intelligent Information and Engineering Systems, 25, pp [23] P. Pudil, J. Novovičová, and J. Kittler, "Floating search methods in feature selection," Pattern Recogn. Lett., vol. 5, pp. 9-25,

ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS.

ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS. ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS. Alan M. Wing Sensory Motor Neuroscience Centre, The University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, B15 2TT, UK. a.m.wing@bham.ac.uk J. Randall Flanagan

More information

Somatosensory modalities!

Somatosensory modalities! Somatosensory modalities! The somatosensory system codes five major sensory modalities:! 1. Discriminative touch! 2. Proprioception (body position and motion)! 3. Nociception (pain and itch)! 4. Temperature!

More information

Human Ability to Scale and Discriminate Forces Typical of Those Occurring during Grasp and Manipulation

Human Ability to Scale and Discriminate Forces Typical of Those Occurring during Grasp and Manipulation 3394 The Journal of Neuroscience, March 31, 2004 24(13):3394 3401 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Human Ability to Scale and Discriminate Forces Typical of Those Occurring during Grasp and Manipulation Heather

More information

Encoding of Direction of Fingertip Forces by Human Tactile Afferents

Encoding of Direction of Fingertip Forces by Human Tactile Afferents The Journal of Neuroscience, October 15, 2001, 21(20):8222 8237 Encoding of Direction of Fingertip Forces by Human Tactile Afferents Ingvars Birznieks, 1 Per Jenmalm, 1 Antony W. Goodwin, 2 and Roland

More information

Information about Complex Fingertip Parameters in Individual Human Tactile Afferent Neurons

Information about Complex Fingertip Parameters in Individual Human Tactile Afferent Neurons 8022 The Journal of Neuroscience, June 24, 2009 29(25):8022 8031 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Information about Complex Fingertip Parameters in Individual Human Tactile Afferent Neurons Hannes P. Saal,

More information

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch!

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch! Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch! How do we perceive objects held in the hand?! Touch receptors deconstruct objects to detect local features! Information is transmitted in parallel

More information

Somatosensation. Recording somatosensory responses. Receptive field response to pressure

Somatosensation. Recording somatosensory responses. Receptive field response to pressure Somatosensation Mechanoreceptors that respond to touch/pressure on the surface of the body. Sensory nerve responds propotional to pressure 4 types of mechanoreceptors: Meissner corpuscles & Merkel discs

More information

PSY 310: Sensory and Perceptual Processes 1

PSY 310: Sensory and Perceptual Processes 1 Touch PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 33 Why is the Braille system better? Vision and audition involve perception of objects from a distance Safe and dependent on the transfer of energy (light, air pressure)

More information

Touch PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 33. Touch perception

Touch PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 33. Touch perception Touch PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 33 Why is the Braille system better? Touch perception Vision and audition involve perception of objects from a distance Safe and dependent on the transfer of energy (light,

More information

Control of Finger Forces during Fast, Slow and Moderate Rotational Hand Movements

Control of Finger Forces during Fast, Slow and Moderate Rotational Hand Movements Control of Finger Forces during Fast, Slow and Moderate Rotational Hand Movements Hamed Kazemi, Robert E. Kearney, IEEE Fellow, and Theodore E. Milner, IEEE Member Abstract The goal of this study was to

More information

SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEMS

SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEMS SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEMS Schematic diagram illustrating the neural pathways that convey somatosensory information to the cortex and, subsequently, to the motor system. Double arrows show reciprocal connections.

More information

Learning and Decay of Prediction in Object Manipulation

Learning and Decay of Prediction in Object Manipulation Learning and Decay of Prediction in Object Manipulation ALICE G. WITNEY, SUSAN J. GOODBODY, AND DANIEL M. WOLPERT Sobell Department of Neurophysiology, Institute of Neurology, University College London,

More information

Pain and Touch. Academic Press. Edited by Lawrence Kruger. Department of Neurobiology University of California, Los Angeles Los Angeles, California

Pain and Touch. Academic Press. Edited by Lawrence Kruger. Department of Neurobiology University of California, Los Angeles Los Angeles, California Pain and Touch Edited by Lawrence Kruger Department of Neurobiology University of California, Los Angeles Los Angeles, California San Diego New York Sydney Academic Press London Boston Tokyo Toronto Contributors

More information

ABSTRACT. Title of Document: SENSORY FEEDBACK MODULATES MAXIMUM VOLUNTARY FORCE IN MULTI-FINGER PRESSING. Sohit Karol, PhD., 2012

ABSTRACT. Title of Document: SENSORY FEEDBACK MODULATES MAXIMUM VOLUNTARY FORCE IN MULTI-FINGER PRESSING. Sohit Karol, PhD., 2012 ABSTRACT Title of Document: SENSORY FEEDBACK MODULATES MAXIMUM VOLUNTARY FORCE IN MULTI-FINGER PRESSING Sohit Karol, PhD., 2012 Directed By: Dr. Jae Kun Shim, Department of Kinesiology The human hand is

More information

Coding of Sensory Information

Coding of Sensory Information Coding of Sensory Information 22 November, 2016 Touqeer Ahmed PhD Atta-ur-Rahman School of Applied Biosciences National University of Sciences and Technology Sensory Systems Mediate Four Attributes of

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Information and neural computations

Information and neural computations Information and neural computations Why quantify information? We may want to know which feature of a spike train is most informative about a particular stimulus feature. We may want to know which feature

More information

Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang Laboratory of Auditory Neurophysiology Department of Biomedical Engineering Johns Hopkins University web1.johnshopkins.edu/xwang

More information

The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense

The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense www.tutis.ca/senses/ Contents Objectives... 1 Introduction... 2 Muscle Spindles and Golgi Tendon Organs... 3 Gamma Drive... 5 Three Spinal Reflexes...

More information

Skin types: hairy and glabrous (e.g. back vs. palm of hand)

Skin types: hairy and glabrous (e.g. back vs. palm of hand) Lecture 19 revised 03/10 The Somatic Sensory System Skin- the largest sensory organ we have Also protects from evaporation, infection. Skin types: hairy and glabrous (e.g. back vs. palm of hand) 2 major

More information

Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on Adaptation of Multi-Digit Forces to Object Weight for Whole-Hand Manipulation

Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on Adaptation of Multi-Digit Forces to Object Weight for Whole-Hand Manipulation Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on Adaptation of Multi-Digit Forces to Object Weight for Whole-Hand Manipulation Wei Zhang 1, Jamie A. Johnston 2, Mark A. Ross 3, Anthony A. Smith 3, Brandon J. Coakley

More information

An Investigation of Hand Force Distribution, Hand Posture and Surface Orientation

An Investigation of Hand Force Distribution, Hand Posture and Surface Orientation An Investigation of Hand Force Distribution, Hand Posture and Surface Orientation R. FIGUEROA and T. ARMSTRONG Department of Industrial and Operations Engineering, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI

More information

COGS 107B Week 1. Hyun Ji Friday 4:00-4:50pm

COGS 107B Week 1. Hyun Ji Friday 4:00-4:50pm COGS 107B Week 1 Hyun Ji Friday 4:00-4:50pm Before We Begin... Hyun Ji 4th year Cognitive Behavioral Neuroscience Email: hji@ucsd.edu In subject, always add [COGS107B] Office hours: Wednesdays, 3-4pm in

More information

Key words: pattern recognition; texture; roughness; mechanoreceptor; somatosensory; neurophysiology; psychophysics; rhesus

Key words: pattern recognition; texture; roughness; mechanoreceptor; somatosensory; neurophysiology; psychophysics; rhesus The Journal of Neuroscience, October 1, 1997, 17(19):7480 7489 Neural Coding Mechanisms in Tactile Pattern Recognition: The Relative Contributions of Slowly and Rapidly Adapting Mechanoreceptors to Perceived

More information

Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (3) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (3) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang 580.626 Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (3) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang Laboratory of Auditory Neurophysiology Department of Biomedical Engineering Johns

More information

Clinical Neurophysiology

Clinical Neurophysiology Clinical Neurophysiology xxx (2012) xxx xxx Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Clinical Neurophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Effects of carpal tunnel syndrome

More information

Hand Anatomy A Patient's Guide to Hand Anatomy

Hand Anatomy A Patient's Guide to Hand Anatomy Hand Anatomy A Patient's Guide to Hand Anatomy Introduction Few structures of the human anatomy are as unique as the hand. The hand needs to be mobile in order to position the fingers and thumb. Adequate

More information

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Goals for the lecture Be able to: Conceptually understand some of the analysis and jargon encountered in a typical (sensory)

More information

IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 00, NO. 00, Keivan Mojtahedi, Qiushi Fu, and Marco Santello IEEE

IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 00, NO. 00, Keivan Mojtahedi, Qiushi Fu, and Marco Santello IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 00, NO. 00, 2015 1 Extraction of Time and Frequency Features From Grip Force Rates During Dexterous Manipulation Keivan Mojtahedi, Qiushi Fu, and Marco Santello

More information

How strong is it? What is it? Where is it? What must sensory systems encode? 9/8/2010. Spatial Coding: Receptive Fields and Tactile Discrimination

How strong is it? What is it? Where is it? What must sensory systems encode? 9/8/2010. Spatial Coding: Receptive Fields and Tactile Discrimination Spatial Coding: Receptive Fields and Tactile Discrimination What must sensory systems encode? How strong is it? What is it? Where is it? When the brain wants to keep certain types of information distinct,

More information

Spatial Coding: Receptive Fields and Tactile Discrimination

Spatial Coding: Receptive Fields and Tactile Discrimination Spatial Coding: Receptive Fields and Tactile Discrimination What must sensory systems encode? How strong is it? What is it? Where is it? When the brain wants to keep certain types of information distinct,

More information

Lecture 1: Neurons. Lecture 2: Coding with spikes. To gain a basic understanding of spike based neural codes

Lecture 1: Neurons. Lecture 2: Coding with spikes. To gain a basic understanding of spike based neural codes Lecture : Neurons Lecture 2: Coding with spikes Learning objectives: To gain a basic understanding of spike based neural codes McCulloch Pitts Neuron I w in Σ out Θ Examples: I = ; θ =.5; w=. - in = *.

More information

Active sensing. Ehud Ahissar 1

Active sensing. Ehud Ahissar 1 Active sensing Ehud Ahissar 1 Active sensing Passive vs active touch Comparison across senses Basic coding principles -------- Perceptual loops Sensation-targeted motor control Proprioception Controlled

More information

LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS

LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS WHAT WE DO Present studies and future directions Our laboratory is currently involved in two major areas of research. The first

More information

Psychophysical laws. Legge di Fechner: I=K*log(S/S 0 )

Psychophysical laws. Legge di Fechner: I=K*log(S/S 0 ) Psychophysical laws Legge di Weber: ΔS=K*S Legge di Fechner: I=K*log(S/S 0 ) Sensory receptors Vision Smell Taste Touch Thermal senses Pain Hearing Balance Proprioception Sensory receptors Table 21-1 Classification

More information

Mechanosensation. Central Representation of Touch. Wilder Penfield. Somatotopic Organization

Mechanosensation. Central Representation of Touch. Wilder Penfield. Somatotopic Organization Mechanosensation Central Representation of Touch Touch and tactile exploration Vibration and pressure sensations; important for clinical testing Limb position sense John H. Martin, Ph.D. Center for Neurobiology

More information

Single cell tuning curves vs population response. Encoding: Summary. Overview of the visual cortex. Overview of the visual cortex

Single cell tuning curves vs population response. Encoding: Summary. Overview of the visual cortex. Overview of the visual cortex Encoding: Summary Spikes are the important signals in the brain. What is still debated is the code: number of spikes, exact spike timing, temporal relationship between neurons activities? Single cell tuning

More information

Peripheral Nervous System

Peripheral Nervous System Peripheral Nervous System 1 Sensory Receptors Sensory Receptors and Sensation Respond to changes (stimuli) in the environment Generate graded potentials that can trigger an action potential that is carried

More information

Quantification of Behavioral Consequences of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome: Insights from Biomechanical Analysis of Grasping and Manipulation

Quantification of Behavioral Consequences of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome: Insights from Biomechanical Analysis of Grasping and Manipulation In: Carpal Tunnel Syndrome ISBN: 978-1-63321-142-1 Editor: Morton Ledford 2014 Nova Science Publishers, Inc. No part of this digital document may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted

More information

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Asymmetry in learning in the reverse direction Full recovery from UP using DOWN: initial return to naïve values within 10 minutes,

More information

راما ندى أسامة الخضر. Faisal Muhammad

راما ندى أسامة الخضر. Faisal Muhammad 22 راما ندى أسامة الخضر Faisal Muhammad Revision Last time we started talking about sensory receptors, we defined them and talked about the mechanism of their reaction. Now we will talk about sensory receptors,

More information

Somatic Sensory System I. Background

Somatic Sensory System I. Background Somatic Sensory System I. Background A. Differences between somatic senses and other senses 1. Receptors are distributed throughout the body as opposed to being concentrated at small, specialized locations

More information

Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience. November 11, 2015 SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEM. Ralph Adolphs

Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience. November 11, 2015 SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEM. Ralph Adolphs Bi/CNS/NB 150: Neuroscience November 11, 2015 SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEM Ralph Adolphs 1 Menu for today Touch -peripheral -central -plasticity Pain 2 Sherrington (1948): senses classified as --teloreceptive

More information

Learned Manipulation at Unconstrained Contacts Does Not Transfer across Hands

Learned Manipulation at Unconstrained Contacts Does Not Transfer across Hands Learned Manipulation at Unconstrained Contacts Does Not Transfer across Hands Qiushi Fu 1, Jason Y. Choi 1, Andrew M. Gordon 2, Mark Jesunathadas 1, Marco Santello 1 * 1 School of Biological and Health

More information

Choice of Contact Points during Multidigit Grasping: Effect of Predictability of Object Center of Mass Location

Choice of Contact Points during Multidigit Grasping: Effect of Predictability of Object Center of Mass Location 3894 The Journal of Neuroscience, April 4, 2007 27(14):3894 3903 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Choice of Contact Points during Multidigit Grasping: Effect of Predictability of Object Center of Mass Location

More information

Sensory information processing, somato-sensory systems

Sensory information processing, somato-sensory systems mm? Sensory information processing, somato-sensory systems Recommended literature 1. Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessel TM (2000) Principles of Neural Science, McGraw-Hill, Ch. xx. 2. Berne EM, Levy MN, Koeppen

More information

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 05 Why do you need bright light to read? Rods and cones Photoreceptors are not evenly distributed across the retina 1 Rods and cones Cones are most dense in

More information

CHAPTER 10 THE SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEM

CHAPTER 10 THE SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEM CHAPTER 10 THE SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEM 10.1. SOMATOSENSORY MODALITIES "Somatosensory" is really a catch-all term to designate senses other than vision, hearing, balance, taste and smell. Receptors that could

More information

Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making

Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making Leo P. Surgre, Gres S. Corrado and William T. Newsome Presenter: He Crane Huang 04/20/2010 Outline Studies on neural

More information

Neural Coding Mechanisms Underlying Perceived Roughness of Finely Textured Surfaces

Neural Coding Mechanisms Underlying Perceived Roughness of Finely Textured Surfaces The Journal of Neuroscience, September 1, 2001, 21(17):6905 6916 Neural Coding Mechanisms Underlying Perceived Roughness of Finely Textured Surfaces Takashi Yoshioka, Barbara Gibb, Andrew K. Dorsch, Steven

More information

Nervous system Reflexes and Senses

Nervous system Reflexes and Senses Nervous system Reflexes and Senses Physiology Lab-4 Wrood Slaim, MSc Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology University of Al-Mustansyria 2017-2018 Nervous System The nervous system is the part of an

More information

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15 s PSY 200 Greg Francis Lecture 06 How do you recognize your grandmother? Action potential With enough excitatory input, a cell produces an action potential that sends a signal down its axon to other cells

More information

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Christopher J. Rozell, Don. H. Johnson and Raymon M. Glantz Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering Department of

More information

Overview of Questions

Overview of Questions Overview of Questions What are the sensors in the skin, what do they respond to and how is this transmitted to the brain? How does the brain represent touch information? What is the system for sensing

More information

The Somatosensory System

The Somatosensory System The Somatosensory System Reading: BCP Chapter 12 cerebrovortex.com Divisions of the Somatosensory System Somatosensory System Exteroceptive External stimuli Proprioceptive Body position Interoceptive Body

More information

Group Redundancy Measures Reveal Redundancy Reduction in the Auditory Pathway

Group Redundancy Measures Reveal Redundancy Reduction in the Auditory Pathway Group Redundancy Measures Reveal Redundancy Reduction in the Auditory Pathway Gal Chechik Amir Globerson Naftali Tishby Institute of Computer Science and Engineering and The Interdisciplinary Center for

More information

Chapter 14: The Cutaneous Senses

Chapter 14: The Cutaneous Senses Chapter 14: The Cutaneous Senses Somatosensory System There are three parts Cutaneous senses - perception of touch and pain from stimulation of the skin Proprioception - ability to sense position of the

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 5: Data analysis II Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis II 6 Single

More information

Deconstructing the Receptive Field: Information Coding in Macaque area MST

Deconstructing the Receptive Field: Information Coding in Macaque area MST : Information Coding in Macaque area MST Bart Krekelberg, Monica Paolini Frank Bremmer, Markus Lappe, Klaus-Peter Hoffmann Ruhr University Bochum, Dept. Zoology & Neurobiology ND7/30, 44780 Bochum, Germany

More information

Neuronal Circuits and Neuronal Pools

Neuronal Circuits and Neuronal Pools Neuronal Circuits and Neuronal Pools Anatomical Orientation Clinical Orientation Atlas Fig. 6-24 The Reality of the Clinical Environment Atlas Fig. 6-24A One Cardinal Plane to Learn AND Understand

More information

Development and application of a hand force measurement system

Development and application of a hand force measurement system Development and application of a hand force measurement system B.D. Lowe a, Y. Kong b, and J. Han c a National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health, Cincinnati, OH, USA b Systems Management Engineering,

More information

Tactile spatial sensitivity and anisotropy

Tactile spatial sensitivity and anisotropy Perception & Psychophysics 2005, 67 (6), 1061-1079 Tactile spatial sensitivity and anisotropy GREGORY O. GIBSON and JAMES C. CRAIG Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana A gap detection task was examined

More information

The cutaneous contribution to adaptive precision grip

The cutaneous contribution to adaptive precision grip Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.27 No. October 4 The cutaneous contribution to adaptive precision grip Alice G. Witney 1, Alan Wing 2, Jean-Louis Thonnard 3 and Allan M. Smith 4 1 Department of Zoology,

More information

Multi-Digit Coordination in Absence of Cutaneous Sensory Feedback During Grasping Tasks

Multi-Digit Coordination in Absence of Cutaneous Sensory Feedback During Grasping Tasks City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Graduate Center 6-2016 Multi-Digit Coordination in Absence of Cutaneous Sensory Feedback During Grasping

More information

Active and passive movements give rise to different judgments of coldness

Active and passive movements give rise to different judgments of coldness Perception, 2006, volume 35, pages 573 ^ 575 DOI:10.1068/p5439 Last but not least Active and passive movements give rise to different judgments of coldness Abstract. When the right index fingertip of twelve

More information

Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks. Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology

Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks. Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology Applying the coding scheme to dynamic cue combination (Experiment, Kording&Wolpert,2004) Dynamic sensorymotor

More information

Physiology of Tactile Sensation

Physiology of Tactile Sensation Physiology of Tactile Sensation Objectives: 1. Describe the general structural features of tactile sensory receptors how are first order nerve fibers specialized to receive tactile stimuli? 2. Understand

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

Neural Coding. Computing and the Brain. How Is Information Coded in Networks of Spiking Neurons?

Neural Coding. Computing and the Brain. How Is Information Coded in Networks of Spiking Neurons? Neural Coding Computing and the Brain How Is Information Coded in Networks of Spiking Neurons? Coding in spike (AP) sequences from individual neurons Coding in activity of a population of neurons Spring

More information

Grip and load force coordination in cyclical isometric manipulation task is not affected by the feedback type

Grip and load force coordination in cyclical isometric manipulation task is not affected by the feedback type Pedão et al. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 2013, 10:34 JOURNAL OF NEUROENGINEERING JNERAND REHABILITATION METHODOLOGY Open Access Grip and load force coordination in cyclical isometric

More information

Ch. 47 Somatic Sensations: Tactile and Position Senses (Reading Homework) - Somatic senses: three types (1) Mechanoreceptive somatic senses: tactile

Ch. 47 Somatic Sensations: Tactile and Position Senses (Reading Homework) - Somatic senses: three types (1) Mechanoreceptive somatic senses: tactile Ch. 47 Somatic Sensations: Tactile and Position Senses (Reading Homework) - Somatic senses: three types (1) Mechanoreceptive somatic senses: tactile and position sensations (2) Thermoreceptive senses:

More information

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Exp Brain Res (1998) 123:346±350 Springer-Verlag 1998 RESEARCH NOTE Jinsung Wang George E. Stelmach Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Received: 1 April

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing Categorical Speech Representation in the Human Superior Temporal Gyrus Edward F. Chang, Jochem W. Rieger, Keith D. Johnson, Mitchel S. Berger, Nicholas M. Barbaro, Robert T. Knight SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

More information

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical Short Communication TheScientificWorldJOURNAL (2006) 6, 1805 1809 ISSN 1537-744X; DOI 10.1100/tsw.2006.291 Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical V. Frak 1,2, *, D. Bourbonnais 2, I. Croteau

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

SENSORY FUNCTIONS OF THE SKIN OF HUMANS

SENSORY FUNCTIONS OF THE SKIN OF HUMANS SENSORY FUNCTIONS OF THE SKIN OF HUMANS SENSORY FUNCTIONS OF THE SKIN OF HUMANS EDITED BY DAN R. KENSHALO Florida State University Tallahassee, Florida PLENUM PRESS NEW YORK AND LONDON Library of Congress

More information

Neural Integration I: Sensory Pathways and the Somatic Nervous System

Neural Integration I: Sensory Pathways and the Somatic Nervous System 15 Neural Integration I: Sensory Pathways and the Somatic Nervous System PowerPoint Lecture Presentations prepared by Jason LaPres Lone Star College North Harris An Introduction to Sensory Pathways and

More information

Tactile Speed Scaling: Contributions of Time and Space

Tactile Speed Scaling: Contributions of Time and Space J Neurophysiol 99: 1422 1434, 2008. First published January 16, 2008; doi:10.1152/jn.01209.2007. Tactile Speed Scaling: Contributions of Time and Space Alexandra Dépeault, 1, * El-Mehdi Meftah, 1, * and

More information

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences Marc Pomplun 1, Yueju Liu 2, Julio Martinez-Trujillo 2, Evgueni Simine 2, and John K. Tsotsos 2 1 Department

More information

SA1 and RA Afferent Responses to Static and Vibrating Gratings

SA1 and RA Afferent Responses to Static and Vibrating Gratings J Neurophysiol 95: 77 782, 26. First published October 9, 25; doi:.52/jn.877.25. and Afferent Responses to Static and Vibrating Gratings S. J. Bensmaïa,,2 J. C. Craig, 3 T. Yoshioka, and K. O. Johnson,2

More information

HUMAN MOTOR CONTROL. Emmanuel Guigon

HUMAN MOTOR CONTROL. Emmanuel Guigon HUMAN MOTOR CONTROL Emmanuel Guigon Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique Université Pierre et Marie Curie CNRS / UMR 7222 Paris, France emmanuel.guigon@upmc.fr e.guigon.free.fr/teaching.html

More information

Models of Attention. Models of Attention

Models of Attention. Models of Attention Models of Models of predictive: can we predict eye movements (bottom up attention)? [L. Itti and coll] pop out and saliency? [Z. Li] Readings: Maunsell & Cook, the role of attention in visual processing,

More information

Neurobiology Biomed 509 Sensory transduction References: Luo , ( ), , M4.1, M6.2

Neurobiology Biomed 509 Sensory transduction References: Luo , ( ), , M4.1, M6.2 Neurobiology Biomed 509 Sensory transduction References: Luo 4.1 4.8, (4.9 4.23), 6.22 6.24, M4.1, M6.2 I. Transduction The role of sensory systems is to convert external energy into electrical signals

More information

Humerus. Ulna. Radius. Carpals

Humerus. Ulna. Radius. Carpals Posture Analysis Exercise T. Armstrong M. Ebersole 1.0 Objectives: 1. Improve skill for rating over all job and identifying specific force and posture problems 2. Learn how to characterize posture 3. Learn

More information

How is the stimulus represented in the nervous system?

How is the stimulus represented in the nervous system? How is the stimulus represented in the nervous system? Eric Young F Rieke et al Spikes MIT Press (1997) Especially chapter 2 I Nelken et al Encoding stimulus information by spike numbers and mean response

More information

Lecture overview. What hypothesis to test in the fly? Quantitative data collection Visual physiology conventions ( Methods )

Lecture overview. What hypothesis to test in the fly? Quantitative data collection Visual physiology conventions ( Methods ) Lecture overview What hypothesis to test in the fly? Quantitative data collection Visual physiology conventions ( Methods ) 1 Lecture overview What hypothesis to test in the fly? Quantitative data collection

More information

Adventures into terra incognita

Adventures into terra incognita BEWARE: These are preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. Chapter VI. Adventures into terra incognita In primary visual cortex there are neurons

More information

Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons

Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons Yoonsuck Choe Department of Computer Sciences The University of Texas at Austin Austin, TX 78712 USA Risto Miikkulainen Department

More information

A general error-based spike-timing dependent learning rule for the Neural Engineering Framework

A general error-based spike-timing dependent learning rule for the Neural Engineering Framework A general error-based spike-timing dependent learning rule for the Neural Engineering Framework Trevor Bekolay Monday, May 17, 2010 Abstract Previous attempts at integrating spike-timing dependent plasticity

More information

III: To define the mechanoreceptive and thermoreceptive sensations.

III: To define the mechanoreceptive and thermoreceptive sensations. Somatic Sensations I: To explain the proper way of sensory testing. II: To describe the somatic sensations. III: To define the mechanoreceptive and thermoreceptive sensations. SOMATIC SENSATIONS Somatic

More information

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14 CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines Assignment 5: Neural mechanisms of object-based attention Due: Friday, April 14 This Assignment is a guided reading of the 2014 paper, Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based

More information

Cortical Control of Movement

Cortical Control of Movement Strick Lecture 2 March 24, 2006 Page 1 Cortical Control of Movement Four parts of this lecture: I) Anatomical Framework, II) Physiological Framework, III) Primary Motor Cortex Function and IV) Premotor

More information

PHGY Physiology. SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY Sensory Receptors. Martin Paré

PHGY Physiology. SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY Sensory Receptors. Martin Paré PHGY 212 - Physiology SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY Sensory Receptors Martin Paré Assistant Professor of Physiology & Psychology pare@biomed.queensu.ca http://brain.phgy.queensu.ca/pare Sensory Systems Question:

More information

The Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on the Kinematics of Reach-to-Pinch Function

The Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on the Kinematics of Reach-to-Pinch Function The Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on the Kinematics of Reach-to-Pinch Function Raviraj Nataraj, Peter J. Evans, MD, PhD, William H. Seitz, MD, Zong-Ming Li. Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH, USA. Disclosures:

More information

2/3/17. Visual System I. I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex

2/3/17. Visual System I. I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex 1 Visual System I I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex 2 1 2/3/17 Window of the Soul 3 Information Flow: From Photoreceptors

More information

The How of Tactile Sensation

The How of Tactile Sensation The How of Tactile Sensation http://neuroscience.uth.tmc.edu/s2/chapter02.html Chris Cohan, Ph.D. Dept. of Pathology/Anat Sci University at Buffalo Objectives 1. Understand how sensory stimuli are encoded

More information

Frequency-dependence of Psychophysical and Physiological Responses to Hand-transmitted Vibration

Frequency-dependence of Psychophysical and Physiological Responses to Hand-transmitted Vibration Industrial Health 2012, 50, 354 369 Review Article Frequency-dependence of Psychophysical and Physiological Responses to Hand-transmitted Vibration Michael J. GRIFFIN 1 1 Human Factors Research Unit, Institute

More information

Sensory coding and somatosensory system

Sensory coding and somatosensory system Sensory coding and somatosensory system Sensation and perception Perception is the internal construction of sensation. Perception depends on the individual experience. Three common steps in all senses

More information

Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on adaptation of multi-digit forces to object mass distribution for whole-hand manipulation

Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on adaptation of multi-digit forces to object mass distribution for whole-hand manipulation Zhang et al. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 2012, 9:83 JOURNAL OF NEUROENGINEERING JNERAND REHABILITATION RESEARCH Open Access Effects of Carpal Tunnel Syndrome on adaptation of multi-digit

More information