NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Soc Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 May 1.

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Soc Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 May 1."

Transcription

1 NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Exp Soc Psychol May ; 45(3): doi: /j.jesp A pox on the mind: Disjunction of attention and memory in the processing of physical disfigurement Joshua M. Ackerman, Yale University D. Vaughn Becker, Arizona State University, Polytechnic Campus Chad R. Mortensen, Arizona State University Takao Sasaki, Arizona State University Steven L. Neuberg, and Arizona State University Douglas T. Kenrick Arizona State University Abstract The unfavorable treatment of people with physical disfigurements is well-documented, yet little is known about basic perceptual and cognitive responses to disfigurement. Here, we identify a specialized pattern of cognitive processing consistent with the hypothesis that disfigurements act as heuristic cues to contagious disease. Disfigurements are often invariant across time and difficult to conceal, and thus observers can detect the presence of such cues without necessarily remembering the particular individuals bearing these cues. Indeed, despite the fact that disfigured faces were especially likely to hold disease-sensitive perceivers attention (Study 1), disfigured individuals were often confused with one another and thus not well remembered later (Study 2), revealing a disjunction of the typical relationship between elevated attention and elevated memory. We discuss the implications of our results for stigmatization of people with and without physical abnormalities and suggest the possibility that cognitive mechanisms for processing social information may be functionally tuned to the variant nature of important cues. Keywords threat detection; face perception; disease avoidance; evolutionary psychology; social cognition Shakespeare s Twelfth Night features the memorable quotation In nature there is no blemish but the mind; none can be called deformed but the unkind (1916, p. 68). Today, the thought Corresponding Author: Joshua Ackerman, Yale University, Department of Psychology, PO Box , New Haven, CT , Phone: (203) , Fax: (203) , joshua.ackerman@yale.edu. Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

2 Ackerman et al. Page 2 of treating people with innocuous physical disfigurements negatively because of their appearance is considered more abhorrent than ever. Yet this outcome is far from unusual: People with facial and bodily abnormalities often suffer stigmatization and discrimination, from simple awkwardness in interpersonal interactions to rejection from jobs with high public visibility (e.g., Greenhouse, 2003). Researchers have recently begun to consider these negative responses to disfigurement as emerging from an evolved motivation to avoid contagious disease threats (e.g., Kurzban & Leary, 2001; Park, Faulkner, & Schaller, 2003; Zebrowitz & Collins, 1997). One of the most promising avenues for understanding the consequences of such motivations involves explicating their role in directing basic cognitive processes (e.g., Ackerman et al., 2006; Kenrick, Delton, Robertson, Becker, & Neuberg, 2007; Maner, Galliot, Rouby, & Miller, 2007). Here, we demonstrate a disjunction between visual attention and memory effects that emerges when people view facial disfigurements, and consider the implications of this disjunction for a functional approach to threat management. Interpersonal threat processing and physical disfigurement Inefficient threat management can result in severe harm or death; thus interpersonal threat detection and defense have historically been, and continue to be, of fundamental importance (Daly & Wilson, 1999; Green & Phillips, 2004). Many sources of threat directly signal danger, as with the facial expression of anger, but such signals are rarely perfect indicators. Other sources of threat, like disease-causing pathogens, provide even less precise cues of their presence. This threat uncertainty gives rise to a signal detection problem in which the costs of missing a real danger far outweigh the costs of mistakenly perceiving a false danger. Cognitive threat detection mechanisms may therefore exhibit a bias to minimize costs by over-inferring threat from imperfect cues (Haselton & Nettle, 2006; Nesse, 2005). In the context of a motivation to avoid disease, this bias would have led to negative, avoidant reactions to those exhibiting a wide-range of potential disease cues. Many contagious diseases produce conspicuous physical features such as lesions (Kurzban & Leary, 2001) and, accordingly, perceivers reactions to these features are often strongly negative (Park et al., 2003). To the extent that there exists an overgeneralization bias (Zebrowitz, Fellous, Mignault, & Andreoletti, 2003; Zebrowitz & Rhodes, 2004), one would expect perceivers to react similarly to other physical disfigurements as well. Consistent with this bias, people appear to heuristically associate many benign physical abnormalities with contagious disease (e.g., Park, Schaller, & Crandall, 2007; Schaller, Park, & Faulkner, 2003; Zebrowitz et al., 2003), and when confronting individuals who possess such abnormalities, exhibit the kinds of avoidant behaviors that would minimize contagion risk, if such risk existed (e.g., Heinemann, Pellander, Vogelbusch, & Wojtek, 1981; Houston & Bull, 1994). Specific processing of specific threats Despite a relatively large body of work on the stigmatization of people with physical disfigurements, little is known about the basic cognitive processing of disfigurement. We may gain some insights by considering functional specificity, an attribute common to many psychological mechanisms (e.g., Ackerman & Kenrick, 2008; Cosmides & Tooby, 1994; Kenrick, Li, & Butner, 2003; Barrett & Kurzban, 2006). Functional specificity refers to particular forms of input criteria, processing strategies, and outputs specialized for managing evolutionarily-recurrent problems. Basic social perception mechanisms may be oriented in specialized ways as to facilitate adaptive responses to these problems (Gibson, 1979; McArthur & Baron, 1983). Consider the problem of intergroup aggression: People tend to selectively allocate perceptual and cognitive resources (such as the degree of visual attention) to ingroup members more than to outgroup members (e.g., Becker, Neuberg et al., 2008; Eberhardt, Goff, Purdie, & Davies,

3 Ackerman et al. Page 3 Processing disjunctions Current research 2004; Richeson & Trawalter, 2008; Rodin, 1987). This preferential allocation may account for the better memory typically seen for ingroup targets relative to outgroup targets. However, when displaying signals of directed threat angry expressions outgroup targets are remembered well, eliminating the standard ingroup memory bias (Ackerman et al., 2006). Furthermore, possibly because angry expressions hold attention (Becker, Anderson et al., 2008; Fox et al., 2000; Fox, Russo, Bowles, & Dutton, 2001) and outgroup members are seen as less constrained by the aggression-inhibiting effects of ingroup empathy and interdependence (making them especially dangerous), memory for these outgroup targets can be even better than memory for ingroup targets (Ackerman et al., 2006). These findings suggest that both memory and attention have a specialized sensitivity for the processing of aggressive threats. Similarly, disease-causing organisms have been a recurrent problem throughout human evolutionary history (Gangestad & Buss, 1993; Low, 1994). People therefore may have acquired specific cognitive strategies for managing disease-relevant cues such as disfigurement. What might these processing strategies look like? Certainly, one must detect and encode threats in order to respond properly, and so we would expect that, as with angry expressions, disfigurement holds attention. Unlike angry expressions though, physical disfigurements are relatively stable; they often do not disappear over time and are often difficult to conceal (such features are known in the ecological literature as structural invariants; McArthur & Baron, 1983). The relatively invariant nature of disfigurement suggests that processing individuating information beyond the disfigurement itself may typically be unnecessary, and even cognitively inefficient. That is, whereas the ability to efficiently encode an individual s angry expression will not suffice for reducing a perceiver s later vulnerability to that individual (because the individual may continue to be threatening without displaying it), the ability to efficiently encode an individual s disfigurement may be quite sufficient for reducing a perceiver s later vulnerability to that individual (because the individual will continue to exhibit the contagion-implying disfigurement). One should expect, then, that memory for other information that individuates disfigured targets would be relatively poor. This pattern of cognitive processing represents a processing disjunction a violation of the expected relationship between early and later information processing, one that may serve as a functional solution to the problem of threat management (see Kenrick et al., 2007). Standard cognitive models typically presume a monotonically increasing relationship between attention and memory (e.g., Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Craik & Lockhart, 1972): The more a target is looked at, the better that target will be remembered. We see evidence for this relationship in the processing of ingroup and outgroup faces as described above. In contrast, we are predicting that although disfigurement-based threat cues may lead to elevated attention, these cues will not lead to a corresponding elevation in memory for individuating features over and above the threat cues themselves. Thus, relatively transient threat cues (e.g., angry expressions) and relatively invariant threat cues (e.g., physical disfigurements) may have similar, adaptive effects at one stage of processing (e.g., attention), but much different effects at another stage (e.g., memory). Do attention and memory exhibit specialized strategies for the processing of disease-relevant threats? We conducted two studies to explore how disfigurement influences visual attention (important for immediate threat processing) and sociospatial memory (important for longerterm threat management).

4 Ackerman et al. Page 4 Study 1: Attentional Adhesion Method Study 1 used a dot-probe task to compare the extent to which normal and disfigured faces capture visual attention. To directly investigate the role of contagion concerns in the processing of disfigurement, we also included a condition in which participants were primed to be diseasesensitive before participating in the dot-probe task. We expected that this manipulation would differentially increase attention to disfigured faces, consistent with other research demonstrating a selective effect of primes on relevant targets (e.g., Eberhardt et al., 2004; Faulkner, Schaller, Park, & Duncan, 2004; Ferguson & Bargh, 2004; Maner et al., 2005, 2007; Neuberg, Kenrick, Maner, & Schaller, 2005). Participants Two hundred fifty-five undergraduates (median age = 19) participated in exchange for course credit. Four participants were excluded because of computer malfunctions. Thus, 251 participants (124 female) were included in the reported analyses. Materials Two slideshows were used, purportedly as part of an unrelated experiment, to prime either disease-sensitivity or a control state (between-participants). These slideshows featured 10 slides depicting either images and text related to contagion (e.g., a dirty sponge, a sneezing person) or architecture (e.g., public buildings). The disease-sensitivity slideshow has been effectively used in previous research (e.g., Faulkner et al., 2004). Stimulus photographs included 64 color front-oriented faces, sized to pixels. Target faces were both male and female and of a similar age to our participant sample. For one version of these stimuli, computer software was used to add flat pink coloration to a random area of the face (simulating a port-wine stain) or to adjust the location of one pupil (simulating strabismus), features that are both salient and yet not symptomatic of contagious disease. Which faces bore disfigurements and which did not was counterbalanced between participants. The dot-probe task required participants to view faces appearing in one quadrant of a computer screen and then quickly identify shapes (circle, square) appearing in distinct quadrants, thus providing a measure of the speed with which perceivers disengaged visual attention from target faces (Maner et al., 2007). Each face was viewed twice, for a total of 128 trials. On each trial, a blank screen appeared for 2000ms, followed by a central fixation symbol (+) for 1000ms. Next, this symbol was replaced by a color facial photograph in one of four screen quadrants. Faces varied by sex and by disfiguring cue (none or port wine stain/strabismus) withinparticipants; because these cues do not logically imply contagious disease, any biases in their processing implicate the presence of heuristic threat management processes. After 500ms, the photo was replaced by a shape which appeared either in the same quadrant as the photograph (filler trials; 25% of total) or in a different quadrant (disengagement trials; 75% of total) and remained until a response was given. To do this, participants used two keyboard keys (A and L) labeled with stickers featuring a circle or a square. A sample trial from the task appears in Figure 1. Photographs and trial type (filler or disengagement) were randomly presented withinparticipants. Procedure Participants received initial instructions in small groups of 1 3 and then proceeded to participate using individual computers separated by dividers. Participants were told that the main experimental task (a shape identification game) did not require the entire session time, so they would first evaluate a slideshow being constructed for use in a different study. After viewing the slideshow on their individual computers, participants responded to several filler questions (e.g., How many slides did you see? ).

5 Ackerman et al. Page 5 Results Instructions for the dot-probe task described above were then given on the screen: Participants were told to look at both the central fixation symbol and the photographs when they appeared, and to accurately categorize the shapes. Speed of response on the disengagement trials was the primary dependent variable. All participants were then probed for suspicion, fully debriefed and released. Following standard approaches to such attention-probe tasks (e.g., Fox et al., 2001), response times less than 200ms and greater than 2.5 standard deviations from the mean were excluded as outliers (0.6% of trials). Response times for correct trials were averaged for each of the four target photo types, and log-transformed to correct for skew (data presented in milliseconds for readability). Trials with incorrect responses (3%) were excluded from analysis. This number is comparable to those obtained in other dot-probe studies (e.g., Maner et al., 2007). We also collapsed across both types of target disfigurements, as no significant response time differences emerged between the two in the analyses (ps>.21). Primary analyses A Disease Prime Target Disfigurement Target Sex mixed ANOVA revealed the expected main effect of disfigurement: Disfigured faces held attention longer than normal faces, F(1, 249) = 16.17, p<.001, η p 2 =.06. This was qualified by a Disease Prime Disfigurement interaction, F(1, 249) = 4.86, p<.03, η p 2 =.02, indicating that the diseasesensitivity prime made it harder to disengage from disfigured targets than from normal targets (see panel A of Figure 2). Simple effects tests demonstrated that this difference in attention reached significance within the disease-sensitivity condition, F(1, 249) = 19.30, p<.001, η p 2 =.07, but not within the control condition (p=.20). This functional specificity is consistent with the present theoretical approach and incompatible with alternative explanations built on the simple presumption that people s attention is held by novel cues. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 1. Additionally, a Target Sex Disfigurement interaction, F(1, 249) = 3.74, p=.05, η p 2 =.02, indicated that the effect of disfigurement across priming conditions was significant for male targets, but not for female targets (see Table 1). 1 This sex difference was due to normal female faces holding attention longer than normal male faces, an effect consistent with the relatively higher attention to female faces evident in both infants and adults (e.g., Becker, Neuberg et al., 2008;Quinn, Yahr, Kuhn, Slater, & Pascalis, 2002;Rosenwasser, Adams, & Tansil, 1983). To explore whether the disease-sensitivity manipulation increased attention to both disfigured male and female faces, we investigated the simple effect of disfigurement on attention to faces within each prime condition. In the control condition, attention was captured equally by normal and disfigured female faces (F<1) and marginally more by disfigured male faces than normal male faces, F(1, 249) = 3.09, p=.08, η p 2 =.01. However, in the disease-sensitivity condition, attention was indeed higher for disfigured faces than for normal faces, both with male targets, F(1, 249) = 15.79, p<.001, η p 2 =.06, and (marginally) with female targets, F(1, 249) = 3.48, p=.06, η p 2 =.01. These results suggest a stronger effect of disfigurement on male targets, though when sensitivity to disease is active, both disfigured males and females hold attention. Ancillary analyses Filler trials (when the shape appeared in the same quadrant as the photo) were used to motivate participants to maintain eye contact with the facial photographs. Although not of interest for attentional disengagement, analysis of responses to filler trials revealed only a main effect of target disfigurement: People were faster to respond to cues that 1 Because of the Target Sex effect, we also ran an analysis including Participant Sex in the model. A main effect of Participant Sex did emerge, F(1, 247) = 7.77, p <.01, η p 2 =.03, with women holding ttention to all faces longer than men, but no other effects involving Participant Sex were significant.

6 Ackerman et al. Page 6 appeared behind disfigured faces than behind normal faces, F(1, 249) = 3.89, p =.05, η p 2 =. 02. This supports the conclusion that disfigurement both draws and holds attention. Discussion The results of Study 1 show that physical disfigurements hold attention. This is particularly the case when people were primed with disease threat. The attentional adhesion effect also appeared stronger for male targets than for female targets, which may reflect the relatively stronger heuristic association between males and threat (Daly & Wilson, 1999; Maner et al., 2005; Neuberg et al., 2005), or alternately, a relative disregard for normal male faces (Quinn et al., 2002; Rosenwasser et al., 1983). Additionally, the adhesion effect does not appear to be solely a function of cue novelty as it proved significant only when participants were primed with disease-sensitivity. The elevated attention given to physical disfigurement, as with other types of threat cues, represents an immediate form of threat management. If we expect that attention to disfigurement follows the standard monotonically increasing relationship with memory, we should find that disfigured faces are remembered better than normal faces. This should be especially true when participants are sensitive to disease threats. However, if the processing of disfigured faces is focused on the invariant threat cue and not the individual bearing that cue, we may find evidence for a processing disjunction. That is, memory for disfigured faces may be worse than for normal faces, even in light of elevated attention, and even in light of elevated disease-sensitivity. Study 2: Socicospatial Memory Method Study 2 used a photo matching task to test memory for normal and disfigured faces. This task measures both recognition memory (i.e., the ability to correctly identify previously-seen faces) and memory for the location of faces (Becker, Kenrick, Guerin, & Maner, 2005). We again primed participants with the slideshows used earlier to investigate their role in orienting perceivers toward (heuristic) cues of disease threat. Participants One hundred eleven undergraduates (39 female, median age = 20) participated in exchange for course credit. Materials The card matching task involved a computerized version of the classic Concentration card game (Becker et al., 2005). This game consisted of a 4 16 array with 32 pairs of faces, again varying by sex and disfigurement (only the normal and port wine stain faces from Study 1 were used) with the particular faces bearing disfigurement counterbalanced between participants. At the beginning of the game, all the faces were presented in randomly assigned locations for 10 seconds; this pre-exposure period allowed for the examination of early matches an index of attention as well as simplifying the calculation of matching efficiency by ensuring that all of the face locations have an equal chance of being noted. The faces were then concealed behind tiles, and participants attempted to match pairs of faces by clicking the tiles, one at a time, with a computer mouse. If two sequentially selected tiles matched, they remained face up; otherwise, both were hidden again after 2 seconds. The object of the task is to match the faces in as few as trials as possible. See Figure 3 for a sample game. Procedure Participants completed the study in groups of 1 3 using individual computers, and included the same cover story for evaluating slideshow primes (disease-sensitivity or control) as in Study 1. Participants were told that that the study involved memory for people before and after they underwent cosmetic surgery to correct facial disfigurements. The card

7 Ackerman et al. Page 7 matching game, and instructions for completing it, were then administered. Following the game, all participants were probed for suspicion, fully debriefed and released. Results Primary analyses For each participant, mean error rates for each combination of target sex and disfigurement were calculated, as was the conditional probability that faces were confused with other faces of the same type. These mismatches and within-category errors were calculated with reference to the first face chosen in each turn. That is, if the first face was a diseased male and the second face was not a diseased male, this counted as one mismatch. If that mismatch was a (different) diseased male, this was additionally counted as a within-category error. No participant results necessitated being excluded as outliers. A Disease Prime Target Disfigurement Target Sex mixed ANOVA revealed a main effect of Disfigurement: Despite the tendency for disfigured faces to hold attention in Study 1, participants made more mismatches with disfigured faces than normal faces, F(1, 109) = 94.93, p<.001, η p 2 =.47. This increased mismatch rate resulted from a relatively high number of within-category errors (see Table 2). That is, participants confused disfigured faces with each other more than with normal faces, F(1, 109) = 32.17, p<.001, η p 2 =.23. Strikingly, this occurred despite the fact that the location of the disfigurement on each face was unique, and thus should have provided an additional memory cue, making faces easier to match. A main effect of the priming manipulation revealed that participants receiving the disease prime generated significantly fewer within-category memory errors relative to control participants (see panel B of Figure 2), F(1, 109) = 7.45, p<.01, η p 2 =.06. The conventional monotonic relationship between attention and memory would suggest that this reduction in errors would occur most strongly for disfigured faces (as these were the faces receiving the most attention in Study 1). However, planned comparisons indicated that the reduction in within-category errors was significant with normal faces, F(1, 109) = 4.94, p<.03, η p 2 =.04, but it was not significant with disfigured faces (p=.11). Thus, it appears that although disease-sensitivity generally prompted more effortful processing of target faces, this occurred primarily for normal and not for disfigured faces. This pattern is inconsistent with conventional predictions, but it is consistent with a processing disjunction people concerned with disease-related threats focus attention on targets bearing (invariant) cues heuristically associated with disease, but do not spend the cognitive resources to individuate those targets. In order to determine whether the attentional adhesion found in Study 1 produced any initial encoding advantage for disfigured faces, we also analyzed the first three trials of the game separately. Participants did match significantly more disfigured faces (N = 34) than normal faces (N = 13) over the first three trials (p binomial <.005). This verifies that attention was attracted to and held by disfigurement cues. Yet despite this early advantage, disfigured faces were ultimately remembered (across all trials) only in terms of their location rather than their individual identity. In fact, although the result of the first three trials mathematically increases the probability that future mistakes would involve between-category errors, our earlier analyses instead revealed that people were more likely to confuse disfigured faces with other disfigured faces. Finally, we investigated whether a boost in attention to disfigured male faces relative to nondisfigured male faces (as observed in Study 1) might produce later stage memory effects. In fact, a main effect of Target Sex indicated that participants made more within-category memory errors across all male faces relative to female faces (see Table 2), F(1, 109) = 4.57, p<.05, η p 2 =.04. Thus, although disfigurement led to increased attention to male faces in Study 1,

8 Ackerman et al. Page 8 General Discussion especially when participants were primed with disease-sensitivity, this attention did not produce a corresponding increase in memory. This finding is consistent both with the idea of a processing disjunction, and with previous research indicating that female faces are often more memorable than male faces (e.g., Becker, Neuberg et al. 2008;Maner et al., 2003;Rehnman & Herlitz, 2006). How do people allocate cognitive resources to disease-relevant threats? Unlike many other types of threats, disease-causing agents are not directly detectable and perceivers must rely on heuristic indicators, such as physical disfigurement, that are not always diagnostic of contagion. The present studies revealed that disfigurements did indeed capture attention, especially when people were primed to be sensitive to disease threats. However, this increased attunement to disfigured faces did not result in improved memory for those faces over the long-term. Perceivers instead encoded faces with disfigurements as being relatively homogenous, confusing them with one another. Even when people were primed with disease cues, exactly the time when we should expect that memory for disfigured faces improves relative to memory for normal faces, disfigured targets were not remembered better. These seemingly inconsistent effects of attention and memory are indicative of a threat-cued processing disjunction. Thus, when people attend to individuals with invariant threat cues, such as physical disfigurements, they may, to some degree, be looking without seeing. Adaptively tuned cognitive threat processing Detecting and encoding threats in one s immediate environment represent the first stages of threat management. A number of studies, including the present one, have shown that visual attention is adaptively tuned in such a way as to facilitate automatic and rapid encoding of dangers (e.g., Fox et al., 2000; Lundqvist & Öhman, 2005). As physical disfigurements are often heuristically treated as cues to disease (e.g., Faulkner et al., 2004; Park et al., 2003), their attention-adhering effects make functional sense. Similarly, attentional adhesion is seen when danger-primed people view outgroup males (Eberhardt et al., 2004) and when males display angry expressions (Becker, Anderson et al., 2008; Fox, Russo, & Dutton, 2001), suggesting a commonality in how people attend to different forms of threat. However, the later stages of threat management (including memory for dangerous targets) may allow for a greater variety of specialized, but still adaptive, processing strategies. For example, outgroup male faces are typically remembered relatively poorly (Anthony, Copper & Mullen, 1992), but the addition of angry expressions to these faces boosts recognition for these individuals by decreasing the confusions made with other angry faces (Ackerman et al., 2006). Contrast this with the deficits in memory found here for disfigured faces. The relatively high number of within-category confusions for these faces indicates that perceivers did encode the location of faces with physical disfigurement cues, but it appears that further processing did not occur. We suggest that these two findings, though representing seemingly inconsistent patterns of threat processing, jointly indicate functional attunement to an important feature of threat-relevant cues their degree of invariance. Angry expressions are fleeting, although the interpersonal threat they signal may not be. Therefore, perceivers may need to expend valuable cognitive resources remembering individual features of an angry outgroup member to better remember that person at a later time. Physical disfigurements are not as fleeting. In fact, if such a cue (to contagious disease) were to disappear, it is probable that the underlying threat would have similarly vanished. Thus, if a physical abnormality appears permanent, there may be little immediate need to engage in effortful, individuating processing. Though it is still preliminary to conclude that invariant threat cues make individuals less memorable relative to variant cues, there is other evidence consistent with this conclusion.

9 Ackerman et al. Page 9 Much of this evidence concerns the ubiquitous outgroup homogeneity (or cross-race) effect, typified by relatively lower recognition rates for outgroup members compared to those for ingroup members (Anthony et al., 1992; Ostrom & Sedikides, 1992). Within cross-race contexts that involve an active threat, we find that memory appears to increase when variant cues can be used to encode outgroup members, and decrease when invariant cues can be used. Consider three empirical illustrations. First, participants primed with threat tend to more strongly categorize neutral outgroup male faces using the invariant cue of race, leading to reduced memory for these faces (Miller, Maner, & Becker, 2008). Second, participants who view outgroup males with angry expressions (a variant cue) remember those faces better than they remember neutrally-expressive outgroup faces (Ackerman et al., 2006), suggesting that the presence of a variant cue trumps the typically poor encoding that the invariant cue of race elicits. Third, the addition of another variant cue has similarly been shown to modulate memory for outgroup faces. Ackerman (2007) showed neutrally-expressive Black and White male targets, varying in high or low-status clothing (a variant cue) to threat-primed White participants. The addition of low-status, but not high-status, clothing significantly improved memory for Black males, making them equally as memorable as low-status White males, presumably because stereotypes about low-status men involve a physical threat component but stereotypes about high-status men do not. Processing variance and invariance more generally We have thus far limited our consideration of cue variance to the context of threat management. However, functionally selective cognitive processing is evident across a range of situations and problems for which cue variance may be important (e.g., Ackerman & Kenrick, 2008; Cosmides & Tooby, 1994; Kenrick et al., 2003; McArthur & Baron, 1983). Consider the role physical attractiveness (a relatively invariant cue) plays in romantic partner choice. Both men and women prefer physically attractive sexual partners (Li & Kenrick, 2006), but female attractiveness is a central indicator of longer-term romantic suitability whereas male attractiveness is less so (Buss, 1989; Gangestad & Simpson, 2000; Li, Bailey, Kenrick, & Linsenmeier, 2002). Interestingly, physically attractive faces draw visual attention regardless of their gender, yet attractive female faces are well remembered whereas attractive male faces are not (Becker et al., 2005; Maner et al., 2003). This is evidence of another processing disjunction, and one that suggests that perceivers can individuate targets according to invariant cues (e.g., female attractiveness) but typically do not expend the cognitive resources to do so when it is relatively unimportant. Thus, people do not remember attractive men under normal circumstances because doing so would produce little bang for the cognitive buck. Suggestively, evidence indicates that people may be better at remembering men of high status (a central indicator of romantic suitability for males; Li et al., 2002) when status is expressed by the variant cue of clothing (Maner, DeWall, & Gailliot, 2008) than when it is expressed by an invariant cue such as physical attractiveness (Maner et al., 2003). These examples suggest that memory for faces is probably not impaired by a limited capacity to process relevant information, but rather a (nonconscious) disinclination to do so (see also Rodin, 1987). Implications for stereotypic processing The current studies suggest a number of implications for intergroup processing and stigmatization. The heuristic association of physical disfigurement with disease may often lead to the social and physical isolation of people bearing such features. Our data indicate that this stigmatization begins at an early stage of cognition. As with individuals defined by other stigmatized characteristics (e.g., race), perceivers tend to confuse such individuals with one another an outgroup homogeneity effect. However, unlike many of the more innocuous cues

10 Ackerman et al. Page 10 that define outgroup membership, the threat associated with disfigurement grabs attention, possibly facilitating further avoidance and segregation. Conclusions Acknowledgments References An active motivation to avoid disease may increase the potential for stigmatization, even in those people without disfigurements. In Study 2, priming disease-sensitivity made nondisfigured faces more memorable. This effect may indicate that active concerns about health promote allocation of cognitive resources toward individuals who bear no physical abnormalities, but may nonetheless be perceived as potential carriers of disease. This possibility is consistent with the finding that people motivated to avoid disease endorse more negative attitudes about, and behaviors toward, targets stereotypically associated with contagion threat, including foreign immigrants (Faulkner et al., 2004) and homosexual men (Crandall, Glor, & Britt, 1997). Basic cognitive mechanisms show specialized and adaptive attunements to threatening stimuli. These attunements may lead to disjunctions in the expected linear relationship between attention and memory depending on the particular qualities of the particular threats. One such quality is the variant nature of the threat cue. Here, we found that people process facial disfigurement (a relatively invariant threat cue) in a functional manner, and yet very differently from the functional manner in which they process angry facial expressions (a relatively variant threat cue). These patterns have important implications outside of the experimental environment. Consider that each year in the U.S., 6,800 children are born with orofacial clefts (CDC, 2006), just one of many physical disfigurements. Understanding basic cognitive reactions to such individuals is an important step in addressing the stigmatization they will one day face. Supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH64734 to Douglas Kenrick and Steven Neuberg. Ackerman, JM. What you see isn t what you get: Differential processing across the cognitive stream. Arizona State University; Tempe, Arizona: Unpublished doctoral dissertation Ackerman JM, Kenrick DT. The costs of benefits: Help-refusals highlight key trade-offs of social life. Personality and Social Psychology Review 2008;12: [PubMed: ] Ackerman JM, Shapiro JR, Neuberg SL, Kenrick DT, Becker DV, Griskevicius, et al. They all look the same to me (unless they re angry): From out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychological Science 2006;17: [PubMed: ] Anthony T, Copper C, Mullen B. Cross-racial facial identification: A social cognitive integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 1992;18: Atkinson, RC.; Shiffrin, RM. Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In: Spence, KW.; Spence, JT., editors. The psychology of learning and motivation. Vol. 8. London: Academic Press; Barrett HC, Kurzban R. Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychological Review 2006;113: [PubMed: ] Becker DV, Anderson US, Mortensen CR, Robertson TE, Tybur J, Delton AW, Shapiro JR, Ackerman JM. Angry Faces Demand Attention: Evidence of Attentional Adhesion Across Four Paradigms Manuscript submitted for publication. Becker DV, Kenrick DT, Guerin S, Maner JK. Concentrating on beauty: Sexual selection and sociospatial memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 2005;12: [PubMed: ]

11 Ackerman et al. Page 11 Becker DV, Neuberg SL, Maner JK, Shapiro JR, Ackerman JM, Schaller M, Kenrick DT. Interpersonal Threats Generate Disease Prime-Specific Encoding Benefits Manuscript submitted for publication. Buss DM. Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1989;12:1 49. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Improved National Prevalence Estimates for 18 Selected Major Birth Defects United States, MMWR 2006;54: [PubMed: ] Crandall CS, Glor J, Britt TW. Aids-related stigmatization: Instrumental and symbolic attitudes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 1997;27: Cosmides, L.; Tooby, J. Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization. In: Hirschfeld, LA.; Gelman, SA., editors. Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; p Craik FIM, Lockhart RS. Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 1972;11: Daly, M.; Wilson, M. An evolutionary psychological perspective on homicide. In: Smith, MD.; Zahn, MA., editors. Homicide: A sourcebook of social research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; p Eberhardt JL, Goff PA, Purdie VJ, Davies PG. Seeing Black: Race, Crime, and Visual Processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2004;87: [PubMed: ] Faulkner J, Schaller M, Park JH, Duncan LA. Evolved Disease- Avoidance Mechanisms and Contemporary Xenophobic Attitudes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 2004;7: Ferguson MJ, Bargh JA. Liking is for doing: The effects of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2004;87: [PubMed: ] Fox E, Lester V, Russo R, Bowles RJ, Pichler A, Dutton K. Facial expressions of emotion: Are angry faces detected more efficiently? Cognition & Emotion 2000;14: [PubMed: ] Fox E, Russo R, Dutton K. Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition & Emotion 2001;16: [PubMed: ] Fox E, Russo R, Bowles R, Dutton K. Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 2001;130: [PubMed: ] Gangestad SW, Buss DM. Pathogen prevalence and human mate preferences. Ethology & Sociobiology 1993;14: Gangestad SW, Simpson JA. The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2000;23: [PubMed: ] Gibson, JJ. The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; Green MJ, Phillips ML. Social threat perception and the evolution of paranoia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2004;28: [PubMed: ] Greenhouse S. Lifetime Affliction Leads to a U.S. Bias Suit. The New York Times 2003 March 30;:A08. Heinemann W, Pellander F, Vogelbusch A, Wojtek B. Meeting a deviant person: Subjective norms and affective reactions. European Journal of Social Psychology 1981;11:1 25. Houston V, Bull R. Do people avoid sitting next to someone who is facially disfigured? European Journal of Social Psychology 1994;24: Kenrick, DT.; Delton, AW.; Robertson, TE.; Becker, DV.; Neuberg, SL. How the mind warps. In: Forgas, JP.; Haselton, MG.; Von Hippel, W., editors. The Evolution of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognition. New York: Psychology Press; p Kenrick DT, Li NP, Butner J. Dynamical evolutionary psychology: Individual decision rules and emergent social norms. Psychological Review 2003;110:3 28. [PubMed: ] Kurzban R, Leary MR. Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin 2001;127: [PubMed: ] Li NP, Bailey JM, Kenrick DT, Linsenmeier JA. The necessities and luxuries of mate preferences: Testing the trade-offs. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 2002;82: [PubMed: ] Li NP, Kenrick DT. Sex similarities and differences in preferences for short-term mates: What, whether, and why. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 2006;90: [PubMed: ]

12 Ackerman et al. Page 12 Low BS. Marriage systems and pathogen stress in human societies. American Zoologist 1990;30: Lundqvist D, Öhman A. Emotion regulates attention: The relation between facial configurations, facial emotion, and visual attention. Visual Cognition 2005;12: McArthur LZ, Baron RM. Toward an ecological theory of social perception. Psychological Review 1983;90: Maner JK, DeWall CN, Gailliot MT. Selective attention to signs of success: Social dominance and early stage interpersonal perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 2008;34: [PubMed: ] Maner JK, Gailliot MT, Rouby DA, Miller SL. Can t take my eyes off you: Attentional adhesion to mates and rivals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2007;93: [PubMed: ] Maner JK, Kenrick DT, Becker DV, Delton AW, Hofer B, Wilbur CJ, et al. Sexually selective cognition: Beauty captures the mind of the beholder. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2003;85: [PubMed: ] Maner JK, Kenrick DT, Becker DV, Robertson TE, Hofer B, Neuberg SL, Delton AW, Butner J, Schaller M. Functional projection: How fundamental social motives can bias interpersonal perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2005;88: [PubMed: ] Miller SL, Maner JK, Becker DV. Self-Protective Biases in Group Categorization: What Shapes the Psychological Boundary between Us and Them? Manuscript in preparation. Nesse RM. Natural selection and the regulation of defenses: A signal detection analysis of the smoke detector principle. Evolution and Human Behavior 2005;26: Neuberg, S.; Kenrick, DT.; Maner, JK.; Schaller, M. From evolved motives to everyday mentation: Evolution, goals, and cognition. In: Forgas, J.; Williams, KD.; Laham, SM., editors. Social motivation: Conscious and unconscious processes. New York: Cambridge University Press; p Ostrom TM, Sedikides C. Out-group homogeneity effects in natural and minimal groups. Psychological Bulletin 1992;112: Park JH, Faulkner J, Schaller M. Evolved disease-avoidance processes and contemporary anti-social behavior: Prejudicial attitudes and avoidance of people with physical disabilities. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 2003;27: Park JH, Schaller M, Crandall CS. Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people. Evolution and Human Behavior 2007;28: Quinn PC, Yahr J, Kuhn A, Slater AM, Pascalis O. Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: A preference for female. Perception 2002;31: [PubMed: ] Rehnman J, Herlitz A. Higher face recognition ability in girls: Magnified by own-sex and own-ethnicity bias. Memory 2006;14: [PubMed: ] Richeson JA, Trawalter S. The threat of appearing prejudiced and race-based attentional bias. Psychological Science 2008;19: [PubMed: ] Rodin MJ. Who is memorable to whom: A study of cognitive disregard. Social Cognition 1987;5: Rosenwasser SM, Adams V, Tansil K. Visual attention as a function of sex and apparel of stimulus object: Who looks at whom? Social Behavior and Personality 1983;11: Schaller M, Park JH, Faulkner J. Prehistoric dangers and contemporary prejudices. European Review of Social Psychology 2003;14: Zebrowitz LA, Collins M. Accurate social perception at zero acquaintance: The affordances of a Gibsonian approach. Personality and Social Psychology Review 1997;1: [PubMed: ] Zebrowitz LA, Fellous JM, Mignault A, Andreoletti C. Trait impressions as overgeneralized responses to adaptively significant facial qualities: Evidence from connectionist modeling. Personality and Social Psychology Review 2003;7: [PubMed: ] Zebrowitz LA, Rhodes G. Sensitivity to Bad Genes and the Anomalous Face Overgeneralization Effect: Cue Validity, Cue Utilization, and Accuracy in Judging Intelligence and Health. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 2004;28:

13 Ackerman et al. Page 13 Figure 1. A single disengagement trial in the dot-probe task (Study 1).

14 Ackerman et al. Page 14 Figure 2. Greater attentional adhesion to disfigured targets (in milliseconds for readability, with standard error bars) (A) does not necessarily translate into better long-term memory storage (B).

15 Ackerman et al. Page 15 Figure 3. A single run of the concentration game task (Study 2).

16 Ackerman et al. Page 16 Attentional Adhesion (in milliseconds) Control Prime Table 1 Disease Threat Prime Normal targets Disfigured targets Normal targets Disfigured targets Male targets (117.01) (111.65) (133.67) (145.62) Female targets (116.63) (117.63) (143.83) (140.62) Total a (116.82) ab (114.64) a (138.75) b (143.12) Note. Means in the Total row not sharing a subscript are different within rows at p<.05. Standard deviations appear in parentheses. Analyses were conducted on log-transformed scores, but milliseconds are presented for ease of interpretation.

17 Ackerman et al. Page 17 Table 2 Sociospatial memory (within-category errors) Control Prime Disease Threat Prime Normal targets Disfigured targets Normal targets Disfigured targets Male targets.245 (.090).298 (.103).240 (.098).287 (.120) Female targets.245 (.118).287 (.117).193 (.091).257 (.107) Total.245 a (.104).293 b (.110).217 c (.095).272 b (.114) Note. Means in the Total row not sharing a subscript are different within rows at p<.05. Standard deviations appear in parentheses.

They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They re Angry): From Out-group Homogeneity to Out-group Heterogeneity

They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They re Angry): From Out-group Homogeneity to Out-group Heterogeneity 1 They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They re Angry): From Out-group Homogeneity to Out-group Heterogeneity Joshua M. Ackerman, Jenessa R. Shapiro, Steven L. Neuberg, Douglas T. Kenrick, D. Vaughn Becker,

More information

They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They re Angry) From Out-Group Homogeneity to Out-Group Heterogeneity

They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They re Angry) From Out-Group Homogeneity to Out-Group Heterogeneity PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They re Angry) From Out-Group Homogeneity to Out-Group Heterogeneity Joshua M. Ackerman, 1 Jenessa R. Shapiro, 1 Steven L. Neuberg,

More information

The Basic Cognition of Jealousy: An Evolutionary Perspective. Jon K. Maner. Florida State University. Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

The Basic Cognition of Jealousy: An Evolutionary Perspective. Jon K. Maner. Florida State University. Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University Evolution, cognition 1 RUNNING HEAD: JEALOUSY AND COGNITION The Basic Cognition of Jealousy: An Evolutionary Perspective Jon K. Maner Florida State University Todd K. Shackelford Florida Atlantic University

More information

I only have eyes for you: Ovulation redirects attention (but not memory) to attractive men

I only have eyes for you: Ovulation redirects attention (but not memory) to attractive men I only have eyes for you: Ovulation redirects attention (but not memory) to attractive men The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Soc Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 March 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Soc Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 March 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Exp Soc Psychol. 2012 March 1; 48(2): 583 686. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.11.009. Who Expressed What Emotion? Men Grab Anger, Women

More information

Goal-Driven Cognition and Functional Behavior: The Fundamental-Motives Framework

Goal-Driven Cognition and Functional Behavior: The Fundamental-Motives Framework Goal-Driven Cognition and Functional Behavior: The Fundamental-Motives Framework Current Directions in Psychological Science 19(1) 63-67 ª The Author(s) 2010 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav

More information

Running head: RESOURCE SCARCITY & RACE 1. Does this Recession Make Me Look Black? The Effect of Resource Scarcity on the

Running head: RESOURCE SCARCITY & RACE 1. Does this Recession Make Me Look Black? The Effect of Resource Scarcity on the Running head: RESOURCE SCARCITY & RACE 1 Manuscript In Press Psychological Science Does this Recession Make Me Look Black? The Effect of Resource Scarcity on the Categorization of Biracial Faces. Christopher

More information

The disengagement of attentive resources from task-irrelevant cues to sexual and emotional. infidelity

The disengagement of attentive resources from task-irrelevant cues to sexual and emotional. infidelity The disengagement of attentive resources from task-irrelevant cues to sexual and emotional infidelity Date: September 20, 2007 Word count: 4,664 Running head: Disengagement of attentive resources Disengagement

More information

Adaptive allocation of attention: effects of sex and sociosexuality on visual attention to attractive opposite-sex faces

Adaptive allocation of attention: effects of sex and sociosexuality on visual attention to attractive opposite-sex faces Evolution and Human Behavior 28 (2007) 359 364 Adaptive allocation of attention: effects of sex and sociosexuality on visual attention to attractive opposite-sex faces Lesley A. Duncan a,, Justin H. Park

More information

Infection Breeds Reticence: The Effects of Disease Salience on Self-Perceptions of Personality and Behavioral Avoidance Tendencies

Infection Breeds Reticence: The Effects of Disease Salience on Self-Perceptions of Personality and Behavioral Avoidance Tendencies Research Article Infection Breeds Reticence: The Effects of Disease Salience on Self-Perceptions of Personality and Behavioral Avoidance Tendencies Psychological Science 21(3) 440 447 The Author(s) 2010

More information

Imagine being invited to a social gathering consisting. Following in the Wake of Anger: When Not Discriminating Is Discriminating

Imagine being invited to a social gathering consisting. Following in the Wake of Anger: When Not Discriminating Is Discriminating Pers Soc Psychol Bull OnlineFirst, published on July 1, 009 as doi:10.1177/01461670933967 Following in the Wake of Anger: When Not Discriminating Is Discriminating Jenessa R. Shapiro University of California,

More information

Testing the Domain-Specificity of the. Disease-Avoidance and Self-Protection Systems. Uriah Steven Anderson

Testing the Domain-Specificity of the. Disease-Avoidance and Self-Protection Systems. Uriah Steven Anderson Testing the Domain-Specificity of the Disease-Avoidance and Self-Protection Systems by Uriah Steven Anderson A Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Soc Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 September 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Soc Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 September 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Exp Soc Psychol. 2008 September ; 44(5): 1322 1327. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.03.006. Attending to Threat: Race-based Patterns of

More information

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Motivated Social Categorization: Fundamental Motives Enhance People's Sensitivity to Basic Social Categories Jon K. Maner, Saul L. Miller, Justin H. Moss, Jennifer

More information

Black 1 White 5 Black

Black 1 White 5 Black PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Black 1 White 5 Black Hypodescent in Reflexive Categorization of Racially Ambiguous Faces Destiny Peery and Galen V. Bodenhausen Northwestern University ABSTRACT Historically,

More information

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44 (2008) 1322 1327 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Social Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Reports

More information

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (1), 159-165 Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions SOWON HAHN AND SCOTT D. GRONLUND University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma Using a visual search

More information

PERSON PERCEPTION September 25th, 2009 : Lecture 5

PERSON PERCEPTION September 25th, 2009 : Lecture 5 PERSON PERCEPTION September 25th, 2009 : Lecture 5 PERSON PERCEPTION Social Information Attribution Self-serving Biases Prediction SOCIAL INFORMATION BEHAVIOURAL INPUT What Goes Into Person Perception?

More information

Infants Differential Processing of Female and Male Faces Jennifer L. Ramsey-Rennels 1 and Judith H. Langlois 2

Infants Differential Processing of Female and Male Faces Jennifer L. Ramsey-Rennels 1 and Judith H. Langlois 2 CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Infants Differential Processing of Female and Male Faces Jennifer L. Ramsey-Rennels 1 and Judith H. Langlois 2 1 University of Nevada, Las Vegas, and 2 The University

More information

Sexually Selective Cognition: Beauty Captures the Mind of the Beholder

Sexually Selective Cognition: Beauty Captures the Mind of the Beholder Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 85, No. 6, 1107 1120 0022-3514/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1107 Sexually

More information

Can t Take My Eyes off You: Attentional Adhesion to Mates and Rivals

Can t Take My Eyes off You: Attentional Adhesion to Mates and Rivals Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 93, No. 3, 389 401 0022-3514/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.389 Can t Take My Eyes

More information

The observation that the mere activation

The observation that the mere activation Preparing and Motivating Behavior Outside of Awareness Henk Aarts,* Ruud Custers, Hans Marien Force (N) 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 The observation that the mere activation of the idea of a behavioral act

More information

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology:

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Defining Psychology Behaviorism: The scientific study of how rewards and punishment in the environment affect human and non-human behavior Empirical approach: vary contingencies of

More information

The Effect of Facial Attractiveness on Recognition Memory

The Effect of Facial Attractiveness on Recognition Memory Rhode Island College Digital Commons @ RIC Honors Projects Overview Honors Projects 2014 The Effect of Facial Attractiveness on Recognition Memory Brandon M. DeSimone Rhode Island College, bdesimone_1628@email.ric.edu

More information

HAPPINESS SUPERIORITY EFFECT FOR SCHEMATIC FACES 1. Different Faces in the Crowd: A Happiness Superiority Effect for Schematic Faces in

HAPPINESS SUPERIORITY EFFECT FOR SCHEMATIC FACES 1. Different Faces in the Crowd: A Happiness Superiority Effect for Schematic Faces in HAPPINESS SUPERIORITY EFFECT FOR SCHEMATIC FACES 1 Different Faces in the Crowd: A Happiness Superiority Effect for Schematic Faces in Heterogeneous Backgrounds Belinda M. Craig, Stefanie I. Becker, &

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

Does Racial Bias in the Identification of Threatening Stimuli Generalize to Older Black Men?

Does Racial Bias in the Identification of Threatening Stimuli Generalize to Older Black Men? University of Iowa Honors Theses University of Iowa Honors Program Spring 2017 Does Racial Bias in the Identification of Threatening Stimuli Generalize to Older Black Men? Gustav Lundberg University of

More information

JOSHUA M. ACKERMAN CURRICULUM VITAE

JOSHUA M. ACKERMAN CURRICULUM VITAE JOSHUA M. ACKERMAN CURRICULUM VITAE Marketing Group Tel: (617) 258-9102 MIT Sloan School of Management Fax: (617) 258-7597 77 Massachusetts Ave., E62-541 joshack@mit.edu Cambridge, MA 02139 web.mit.edu/joshack/www/index.html

More information

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Honors Scholar Theses Honors Scholar Program Spring 5-1-2008 Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible Juemei Yang

More information

Sexual behavior and jealousy: An evolutionary perspective

Sexual behavior and jealousy: An evolutionary perspective Romanian Journal of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy and Hypnosis Volume 5, Issue 1-2, January June 2018 Theoretical Paper Sexual behavior and jealousy: An evolutionary perspective Irina Grigorovici Titu Maiorescu

More information

The Effects of Facial Similarity on the Express of Displaced Aggression

The Effects of Facial Similarity on the Express of Displaced Aggression 1 Running Head: The Effects of Facial Similarity on the Express of Displaced Aggression The Effects of Facial Similarity on the Express of Displaced Aggression Kristen Brookes and Zach Liapis Hanover College

More information

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not?

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not? The Effects of Voice Pitch on Attractiveness 1 The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not? Lead Author Katie Leaderbrand Co-Researchers Josh Dekam, and Ashley

More information

Evolutionary Medicine (continued)

Evolutionary Medicine (continued) Evolutionary Medicine (continued) Behavioral immune system: Evolved psychology to detect and avoid pathogen threats disgust group bias (ethnocentrism and xenophobia) Mental Disorders schizophrenia psychopathy

More information

Beauty at the Ballot Box: Disease Threats Predict Preferences for Physically Attractive Leaders

Beauty at the Ballot Box: Disease Threats Predict Preferences for Physically Attractive Leaders Research Article Beauty at the Ballot Box: Disease Threats Predict Preferences for Physically Attractive Leaders Psychological Science XX(X) 1 8 The Author(s) 2013 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav

More information

Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people

Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people Evolution and Human Behavior 28 (2007) 410 414 Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people Justin H. Park a,, Mark Schaller b, Christian S. Crandall c a University of Groningen,

More information

When Memory is Better for Out-group Faces: On Negative Emotions and Gender Roles

When Memory is Better for Out-group Faces: On Negative Emotions and Gender Roles J Nonverbal Behav (2011) 35:51 61 DOI 10.1007/s10919-010-0096-8 ORIGINAL PAPER When Memory is Better for Out-group Faces: On Negative Emotions and Gender Roles Eva G. Krumhuber Antony S. R. Manstead Published

More information

Jealousy: Unconscious processes

Jealousy: Unconscious processes Jealousy: Unconscious processes Karlijn Massar a* and Abraham P. Buunk b a Work & Social Psychology, Maastricht University b Evolutionary Social Psychology, University of Groningen & Royal Netherlands

More information

The Color of Similarity

The Color of Similarity The Color of Similarity Brooke O. Breaux (bfo1493@louisiana.edu) Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, Lafayette, LA 70504 USA Michele I. Feist (feist@louisiana.edu) Institute

More information

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 3, 580 584 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027885 OBSERVATION Templates

More information

Infectious Disease and Imperfections of Self-Image

Infectious Disease and Imperfections of Self-Image 73389PSSXXX10.1177/095679761773389Ackerman et al.infectious Disease and Self-Image research-article017 Research Article Infectious Disease and Imperfections of Self-Image Psychological Science 018, Vol.

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies Emma ter Mors 1,2, Mieneke Weenig 1, Naomi Ellemers 1, Dancker Daamen 1 1 Leiden University,

More information

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT Study Focus Empathy s impact on: O Human Interactions O Consequences of those interactions O Empathy Development Study Purpose to examine the empathetic

More information

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology Preface p. xv Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology p. 3 Sociology, Psychology, or Both? p.

More information

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use Amanda Hahn (achahn30@gmail.com) Takashi Yamauchi (tya@psyc.tamu.edu) Na-Yung Yu (nayungyu@gmail.com) Department of Psychology, Mail

More information

Supplemental Material. Romance, Risk, and Replication: Nisha Hickin, Amanda J. F. Tamman, and Lara M. C. Puhlmann

Supplemental Material. Romance, Risk, and Replication: Nisha Hickin, Amanda J. F. Tamman, and Lara M. C. Puhlmann Supplemental Material Romance, Risk, and Replication: Can Consumer Choices and Risk-Taking be Primed by Mating Motives? David R. Shanks, Miguel A. Vadillo, Benjamin Riedel, Ashley Clymo, Sinita Govind,

More information

Implicit Bias: What Is It? And How Do We Mitigate its Effects on Policing? Presentation by Carmen M. Culotta, Ph.D.

Implicit Bias: What Is It? And How Do We Mitigate its Effects on Policing? Presentation by Carmen M. Culotta, Ph.D. Implicit Bias: What Is It? And How Do We Mitigate its Effects on Policing? Presentation by Carmen M. Culotta, Ph.D. Overview Define Implicit Bias Compare to explicit bias Measurement Effects of Implicit

More information

Healing Otherness: Neuroscience, Bias, and Messaging

Healing Otherness: Neuroscience, Bias, and Messaging Healing Otherness: Neuroscience, Bias, and Messaging Tomorrow s Detroit and Detroit s Tomorrow: The Economics of Race Conference 2016 DATE: November 12, 2016 PRESENTER: john a. powell, Director, Haas Institute

More information

(A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above

(A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above CLEP Biology - Problem Drill 24: Behavior No. 1 of 10 1. Habituation is a type of. (A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above Having a certain drive

More information

Explanations in Psychology Attachment Theory Attribution Theory Availability Heuristic Bystander Effect Classical Conditioning Cognitive Dissonance Th

Explanations in Psychology Attachment Theory Attribution Theory Availability Heuristic Bystander Effect Classical Conditioning Cognitive Dissonance Th Toward a Unified Evolutionary Psychology Many sciences develop for a time as exercises in description and empirical generalization. Only later do they acquire reasoned connections within themselves and

More information

Sex differences (and similarities) in jealousy The moderating influence of infidelity experience and sexual orientation of the infidelity

Sex differences (and similarities) in jealousy The moderating influence of infidelity experience and sexual orientation of the infidelity Evolution and Human Behavior 24 (2003) 17 23 Sex differences (and similarities) in jealousy The moderating influence of infidelity experience and sexual orientation of the infidelity Brad J. Sagarin a,

More information

Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1

Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1 Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1 Stereotype Threat and the Racial Achievement Gap: Race, Salience of Race, and Perception of Race as Possible Contributing Factors Scientist

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Men s visual attention to and perceptions of women s dance movements

Men s visual attention to and perceptions of women s dance movements 1 [in press, Personality and Individual Differences, May 2016] Men s visual attention to and perceptions of women s dance movements Susanne Röder 1,2*, Claus-Christian Carbon 1,2,3, Todd K. Shackelford

More information

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task Olga Lositsky lositsky@princeton.edu Robert C. Wilson Department of Psychology University

More information

Implicit Attitude. Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia. Mahzarin R. Banaji. Harvard University

Implicit Attitude. Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia. Mahzarin R. Banaji. Harvard University 1 Implicit Attitude Brian A. Nosek University of Virginia Mahzarin R. Banaji Harvard University Contact Information Brian Nosek 102 Gilmer Hall; Box 400400 Department of Psychology University of Virginia

More information

Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes

Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes Dr. Susan Boland Lock Haven University of PA Presented at AAUW-PA 88 th Annual Meeting Explicit vs. Implicit Evidence of implicit processes Are we all

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

PSYC 210 Social Psychology

PSYC 210 Social Psychology South Central College PSYC 210 Social Psychology Course Information Description Total Credits 4.00 Pre/Corequisites PSYC100 or consent of instructor. Course Competencies Social Psychology introduces the

More information

Exploring Reflections and Conversations of Breaking Unconscious Racial Bias. Sydney Spears Ph.D., LSCSW

Exploring Reflections and Conversations of Breaking Unconscious Racial Bias. Sydney Spears Ph.D., LSCSW Exploring Reflections and Conversations of Breaking Unconscious Racial Bias Sydney Spears Ph.D., LSCSW Race the Power of an Illusion: The Difference Between Us https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=b7_yhur3g9g

More information

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040 School of Psychology Important Degree Information: B.Sc./M.A. Honours The general requirements are 480 credits over a period of normally 4 years (and not more than 5 years) or part-time equivalent; the

More information

D F 3 7. beer coke Cognition and Perception. The Wason Selection Task. If P, then Q. If P, then Q

D F 3 7. beer coke Cognition and Perception. The Wason Selection Task. If P, then Q. If P, then Q Cognition and Perception 1. Why would evolution-minded cognitive psychologists think it is more likely that the mind consists of many specialized mechanisms rather than a few general-purpose mechanisms?

More information

Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking

Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking Miki Matsumuro (muro@cog.human.nagoya-u.ac.jp) Kazuhisa Miwa (miwa@is.nagoya-u.ac.jp) Graduate School of Information

More information

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: SOCIAL AFFILIATION CUES PRIME HELP-SEEKING INTENTIONS 1 This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: Rubin, M. (2011). Social

More information

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Sarah J. Miles (smiles25@uwo.ca) and John Paul Minda (jpminda@uwo.ca) Department of Psychology The University of Western Ontario London, ON N6A 5C2 Abstract

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Computers in Human Behavior

Computers in Human Behavior Computers in Human Behavior 24 (2008) 2965 2971 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Computers in Human Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh Social facilitation and human

More information

The Positivity Effect in Attributions of Praise and Blame. Jamie Hughes and Daniel Krohn. New Mexico State University

The Positivity Effect in Attributions of Praise and Blame. Jamie Hughes and Daniel Krohn. New Mexico State University Running Head: ATTRIBUTIONS ABOUT PRAISE AND BLAME The Positivity Effect in Attributions of Praise and Blame Jamie Hughes and Daniel Krohn New Mexico State University Attributions about Praise and Blame

More information

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Michael Romano (mrr2@nyu.edu) Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place New York, NY 1000 USA Abstract Traditional

More information

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Uri Hasson (uhasson@princeton.edu) Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 USA Geoffrey P. Goodwin (ggoodwin@princeton.edu)

More information

Class, Race, and the Face: Social Context Modulates the Cross-Race Effect in Face Recognition

Class, Race, and the Face: Social Context Modulates the Cross-Race Effect in Face Recognition Class, Race, and the Face: Social Context Modulates the Cross-Race Effect in Face Recognition Edwin R. Shriver Steven G. Young Kurt Hugenberg Michael J. Bernstein Jason R. Lanter Miami University The current

More information

The Purpose-Driven Life: Commentary on Kenrick Et Al.

The Purpose-Driven Life: Commentary on Kenrick Et Al. The Purpose-Driven Life: Commentary on Kenrick Et Al. The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation As Published Publisher

More information

My Notebook. A space for your private thoughts.

My Notebook. A space for your private thoughts. My Notebook A space for your private thoughts. 2 Ground rules: 1. Listen respectfully. 2. Speak your truth. And honor other people s truth. 3. If your conversations get off track, pause and restart. Say

More information

The world is a scary place: Individual differences in belief in a dangerous world predict specific intergroup prejudices

The world is a scary place: Individual differences in belief in a dangerous world predict specific intergroup prejudices 670024GPI0010.1177/1368430216670024Group Processes & Intergroup RelationsCook et al. research-article2016 Article Group Processes & Intergroup Relations G P I R The world is a scary place: Individual differences

More information

Cross-Racial Identification

Cross-Racial Identification Cross-Racial Identification Robert K. Bothwell University of Texas at El Paso John C. Brigham Florida State University Roy S. Malpass State University of New York at Plattsburgh This article reviews theresearch

More information

The "Aha! moment: How prior knowledge helps disambiguate ambiguous information. Alaina Baker. Submitted to the Department of Psychology

The Aha! moment: How prior knowledge helps disambiguate ambiguous information. Alaina Baker. Submitted to the Department of Psychology The A-ha! Moment 1 The "Aha! moment: How prior knowledge helps disambiguate ambiguous information Alaina Baker Submitted to the Department of Psychology of Northeastern University for the degree of Bachelor

More information

The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance

The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance Lauren Byrne, Melannie Tate Faculty Sponsor: Bianca Basten, Department of Psychology ABSTRACT Psychological research

More information

Intersexual Competition

Intersexual Competition (in press, October 2012). In P. Whelan & A. Bolin (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Human Sexuality. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Blackwell. Intersexual Competition Valerie G. Starratt, Nova Southeastern University, valerie.starratt@nova.edu

More information

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? University of Iowa Honors Theses University of Iowa Honors Program Spring 2017 Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? Ann Walsh University of Iowa Follow this and additional works at:

More information

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

CANTAB Test descriptions by function CANTAB Test descriptions by function The 22 tests in the CANTAB battery may be divided into the following main types of task: screening tests visual memory tests executive function, working memory and

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 6, Number 1 Submitted: February 3, 2010 Revisions: March 7, 2010 Accepted: March 8, 2010 Publication Date: March 10, 2010 Sex Differences in Romantic Jealousy:

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Functional Perspectives on Emotion, Behavior, and Cognition

Functional Perspectives on Emotion, Behavior, and Cognition Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 536 540; doi:10.3390/bs3040536 OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci/ Editorial Functional Perspectives on Emotion, Behavior, and Cognition Heather

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Thinking and Intelligence

Thinking and Intelligence Thinking and Intelligence Learning objectives.1 The basic elements of thought.2 Whether the language you speak affects the way you think.3 How subconscious thinking, nonconscious thinking, and mindlessness

More information

Final Exam PS 217, Fall 2010

Final Exam PS 217, Fall 2010 Final Exam PS 27, Fall 200. Farzin, et al. (200) wrote an article that appeared in Psychological Science titled: Spatial resolution of conscious visual perception in infants. The abstract read: Humans

More information

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 11 Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Plous Chapters 10, 11, 12 and 14

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 11 Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Plous Chapters 10, 11, 12 and 14 Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 11 Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Plous Chapters 10, 11, 12 and 14 Still important ideas Contrast the measurement of observable actions (and/or characteristics)

More information

Sawtooth Software. The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? RESEARCH PAPER SERIES

Sawtooth Software. The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? RESEARCH PAPER SERIES Sawtooth Software RESEARCH PAPER SERIES The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? Dick Wittink, Yale University Joel Huber, Duke University Peter Zandan,

More information

EFFECTS OF SOCIAL ANXIETY AND GROUP MEMBERSHIP OF POTENTIAL AFFILIATES ON SOCIAL RECONNECTION AFTER OSTRACISM

EFFECTS OF SOCIAL ANXIETY AND GROUP MEMBERSHIP OF POTENTIAL AFFILIATES ON SOCIAL RECONNECTION AFTER OSTRACISM Submitted: March 17, 2015 Revision: May 22, 2015 Accepted: June 8, 2015 EFFECTS OF SOCIAL ANXIETY AND GROUP MEMBERSHIP OF POTENTIAL AFFILIATES ON SOCIAL RECONNECTION AFTER OSTRACISM Kenta Tsumura Koji

More information

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining 8 the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining items was included. Participants were asked to decide whether a different item was included in the array,

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Evolutionary Processes

Evolutionary Processes 5 Evolutionary Processes Mark Schaller, Lucian Gideon Conway III, and K. Michelle Peavy ABSTRACT Two kinds of evolutionary processes inform psychological research on stereotypes and prejudices. One is

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Behavioural Indicators That Could Be Used To Detect Interviewee Deception In The Conduct Of Investigative Interviews

Behavioural Indicators That Could Be Used To Detect Interviewee Deception In The Conduct Of Investigative Interviews Behavioural Indicators That Could Be Used To Detect Interviewee Deception In The Conduct Of Investigative Interviews Presenter: Martin TAYLOR Position: Senior Legal Specialist Australian Criminal Intelligence

More information

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction Imitation and anticipation 1 It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction Roland Pfister 1, David Dignath 1, Bernhard Hommel 2, & Wilfried Kunde 1 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials for Wisneski and Skitka Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction Table of Contents 2 Pilot Studies 2 High Awareness

More information

EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY. Pages Social Identity 4:22

EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY. Pages Social Identity 4:22 EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY Pages 106 108 Social Identity 4:22 HENRI TAJFEL S SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY Individuals strive to improve their self image by trying to enhance their selfesteem, based on:

More information

Fukuoka University of Education

Fukuoka University of Education Tomoko Sugimura sugitomo@fukuoka-edu.ac.jp Fukuoka University of Education 18 5 6 facial perception, gender-discrimination, young children Developmental studies have demonstrated that young children inaccurately

More information