GENDER STEREOTYPES AND WOMEN S REPORTS OF LIKING AND ABILITY IN TRADITIONALLY MASCULINE AND FEMININE OCCUPATIONS

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "GENDER STEREOTYPES AND WOMEN S REPORTS OF LIKING AND ABILITY IN TRADITIONALLY MASCULINE AND FEMININE OCCUPATIONS"

Transcription

1 Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32 (2008), Blackwell Publishing, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright C 2008 Division 35, American Psychological Association /08 GENDER STEREOTYPES AND WOMEN S REPORTS OF LIKING AND ABILITY IN TRADITIONALLY MASCULINE AND FEMININE OCCUPATIONS Debra L. Oswald Marquette University Gender stereotypes were examined for their causal influence on women s reported liking for and perceived ability to succeed in traditionally masculine and feminine occupations. One hundred twenty-one women were assigned to either a gender-stereotype activation or filler task and then completed measures of liking for, and perceived ability to succeed in, traditionally masculine and feminine occupations. Strongly gender-identified women showed significantly greater liking for feminine occupations in the stereotype-activation condition than in the control condition. However, more weakly identified women did not show the same effect. In contrast, women weak in gender identification reported an increase in perceived ability for feminine occupations when stereotypes were activated than in the control condition. Activating gender stereotypes did not shift reported liking or perceived ability in traditionally masculine occupations. These results demonstrate the theoretical and practical importance of gender stereotypes on women s career-related attitudes. Women currently make up almost half of the workforce, yet many occupations remain gender segregated (U.S. Department of Labor, 2005). In terms of educational achievements, college major choices forecast continued occupational gender segregation. For example, women earned only 29% of the bachelor s degrees in math and computer science, 21% of the degrees in engineering, and 42% of the bachelor s degrees in physical science (National Science Foundation, 2007). The goal of this article is to explore one factor that may contribute to college-age women s decisions to pursue traditionally feminine occupations rather than traditionally masculine occupations. Specifically, this article seeks to further delineate how gender stereotypes have a causal impact on female college students perceived ability and reported liking for traditionally masculine and feminine occupations. Social science researchers have sought to understand why women are so underrepresented in math and science fields, and a number of occupational choice models provide insight into this issue (e.g., Eccles, 1987, 1994; Lapan, Shaughnessy, & Boggs, 1996; Lent, Brown, & Hackett, Debra L. Oswald, Department of Psychology, Marquette University. Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Debra L. Oswald, Department of Psychology, Marquette University, P.O. 1881, Milwaukee, WI debra.oswald@ marquette.edu 1994). For example, social cognitive career theory (e.g., Lent et al., 1994) was developed as an integrative framework for examining the factors associated with career and academic choices. This model proposes that occupational choices are directly influenced by occupational interests and expectations for positive outcomes and indirectly by self-efficacy (the belief in one s ability to succeed on the task). The impact of self-efficacy on occupational choices is mediated primarily by interests. This model has been empirically supported with a number of samples, including women and ethnic minorities in math, science, and engineering majors (e.g., Lent et al., 2001; Lent et al., 2003; Lent et al., 2005). Using a similar model, Lapan and colleagues (1996) conducted a longitudinal study following students for 3 years after college entry. They found that math-specific self-efficacy (i.e., expectations to succeed in math fields) and interest in math were predictive of students choosing math and science majors. Furthermore, these two variables accounted for gender differences in decisions about college major. Similarly, Eccles s (1987, 1994) model of achievementrelated choices proposes that to understand women s academic and occupational choices one must understand the variables that influence a person s expectations to succeed and the subjective value of a domain. If women do not expect to succeed and place relatively little value or personal interest on math and science, then they will not choose to take advanced classes or go into these traditionally masculine fields. This model has been used extensively 196

2 Gender Stereotyping and Occupations 197 to understand women s underrepresentation in math- and science-related fields (e.g., Eccles, Barber, & Jozefowicz, 1999; Eccles, Jacobs, & Harold, 1990). According to the model (Eccles, 1987, 1994), expectations for success and task value are influenced by factors such as cultural stereotypes, socializing agents beliefs and behaviors, differential aptitudes, and previous achievement-related experiences. Similarly, social cognitive career theory recognizes that the environment contains both distal and proximal factors (such as financial support, social aspects of gender and ethnicity, and network contacts) that may either support or serve as barriers to occupational choices (see Lent, Brown, & Hackett, 2000). The occupational choice models (e.g., Eccles, 1994; Lent et al., 1994) suggest that women make career choices within a general cultural context of pervasive gender stereotyping that may influence their liking for and perceived abilities in occupations for which gender typing exists. The specific focus of the current research project is to investigate the causal impact of cultural gender stereotypes about occupations on college women s career interests and perceived ability to be successful in traditionally masculine and feminine fields. A common stereotype is that men succeed in math and science domains; women, in English and the arts (e.g., Eccles et al., 1990; Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002; Oswald & Harvey, 2003). Masculine gender stereotypes are associated with being successful in masculine occupations (Cejka & Eagly, 1999). In contrast, feminine gender stereotypes are associated with success in feminine occupations. Researchers have begun to examine how stereotypes can influence the targets own behaviors (e.g., Levy, 1996; O Brien & Hummert, 2006; Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995). Self-stereotyping theory proposes that cues that activate self-relevant group stereotypes lead to assimilation to the stereotypes. When self-stereotyping, a person sees himor herself as possessing the characteristics and behaviors that are associated with the in-group (e.g., Biernat, Vescio, & Green, 1996; Chiu et al., 1998; Levy, 1996, 2003; Pickett, Bonner, & Coleman, 2002). For example, older adults, but not younger adults, who were primed with negative aging stereotypes performed more poorly on memory tasks (Levy, 1996, 2003) and had poorer handwriting (Levy, 2000) than did older adults who were primed with positive aging stereotypes. Steele and Ambady (2006) found that making gender salient, by priming either gender identity or the category female, resulted in women reporting both an implicit and explicit preference for the arts over math. Even situational factors that subtly highlight gender, such as being a numeric minority, have resulted in women s decreased performance on a math task (Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2000). Although negative stereotypes can result in detriments to performance in relevant areas, positive stereotypes can result in a boost to performance on stereotype-relevant tasks (e.g., Levy, 1996; Shih, Ambady, Richeson, Fujita, & Gray, 2002). For example, Asian American women s math performance decreased when their gender identity was salient but improved when their Asian identity was salient (Shih, Pittinsky, & Ambady, 1999). In contrast, Asian American women performed better on a verbal test when their gender identity, not Asian identity, was made salient (Shih, Pittinsky, & Trahan, 2006). Similarly, female participants primed with the achievement-oriented identity of being a private college student performed better on a mental rotation task as compared to the performance of participants who were primed to think about their gender or a task-irrelevant identity (McGlone & Aronson, 2006). Thus, situations that heighten self-relevant stereotypes, both positive and negative, can result in a target assimilating to the stereotyped role. The importance of salient group stereotypes also has been highlighted in research on stereotype threat. Stereotype threat theory (Steele, 1997) proposes that stigmatized individuals experience a psychological burden to not confirm a group stereotype whenever they engage in a stereotype-relevant task. This psychological burden results, through a variety of possible mediators, in decreased performance on the stereotyped task. Importantly, one does not need to personally endorse the stereotype, but simply needs to be aware of the stereotype. To date, a body of empirical research has demonstrated that, when stereotype threat is experimentally induced, women underperform relative to their male counterparts on a math test; however, removing the stereotype relevance eliminates the performance gap after controlling for prior ability scores (e.g., Cadinu, Maass, Rosabianca, & Kiesner, 2005; Keller, 2002; O Brien & Crandall, 2003; Oswald & Harvey, 2000; Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999). Indeed, the impact of stereotype threat on task performance is robust and has been found across a number of stereotyped groups, such as ethnic minorities on academic tests (Gonzalez, Blanton, & Williams, 2002; Steele & Aronson, 1995), women s accuracy for political knowledge (McGlone, Aronson, & Kobrynowicz, 2006), and men when engaging in nurturing behaviors (Leyens, Desert, Croizet, & Darcis, 2000). Thus, research on stereotype threat further demonstrates how experimentally activating group stereotypes can result in decreased performance on stereotyped tasks. However, not all women respond identically to gender stereotypes. Both stereotype threat and self-stereotyping theories argue that the individuals who identify most strongly with the group should be most impacted by group stereotypes (e.g., Levy, 1996; O Brien & Hummert, 2006; Schmader, 2002; Steele, 1997). Strongly identified individuals view group membership as an important part of their self-definition. Thus, for individuals who are strongly identified with the group, activation of the group s stereotype should be more likely to evoke a personally relevant image (Levy, 1996, p. 1104) and therefore have a greater impact on their behaviors. For example, Schmader (2002) found that women who were strongly identified with their gender, compared to those with weaker gender identification, were more vulnerable to the effects of stereotype threat. Similar results have been found for racial minorities

3 198 OSWALD (Davis, Aronson, & Salinas, 2006) and older adults performing memory tasks (e.g., O Brien & Hummert, 2006). In sum, research suggests that gender stereotypes have a pernicious, negative influence on women s math-related behaviors. In contrast, gender stereotypes appear to result in a boost to women s verbal-related behaviors. These stereotypes appear to have the largest effect on women who are strongly gender identified. However, to date, the research has focused primarily on test performance, in areas such as verbal, math, and spatial abilities. Do these stereotype effects extend beyond performance behaviors to more global choices, such as career aspirations? This study examined whether gender stereotype activation influences liking for and perceived ability to be successful in masculine- and feminine-typed occupations. Female participants were assigned to complete either a task that activated feminine gender stereotypes or a nonstereotyperelevant control task. Participants then completed measures of liking for and perceived ability to succeed in traditionally masculine and feminine occupations. It was hypothesized that gender stereotype activation would result in an increase in liking for and perceived ability to succeed in femininetyped fields. Furthermore, it was hypothesized that gender identification would moderate the effect of stereotype activation. Specifically, women who reported that gender was an important part of their self-image were expected to experience the effects of stereotype activation to a greater extent than women who reported weaker gender identification. In contrast, because stereotypes about women lack relevance to masculine-typed occupations, it was hypothesized that activating feminine stereotypes would not influence liking for and perceived ability to succeed in masculine-typed fields. METHOD Participants One hundred twenty-one female college students from a large public Midwestern university participated in this study for partial course credit. The majority of the participants reported their ethnicity as White/Caucasian (n = 93, 77%), 9% (n = 11) as African American, 7% (n = 9) as Asian American, 4% (n = 5) as Hispanic/Latina, and three people did not report their ethnicity. Their median age was 18 years (ranging from 18 to 22 years). Seventy-five percent (n = 91) of the participants were first-year college students, 14% (n = 17) were sophomores, 9% (n = 11) were juniors, and one student was a senior. Participants reported 48 different majors (or intended majors). Almost 50% (n = 61) of participants reported a major in a predominately feminine field, and 25% (n = 30) reported a major in a predominately masculine field (categorization was based on the National Science Foundation s 2007 report of bachelor s degrees awarded by gender). Approximately 11% (n = 13) reported majors in fields that could not be classified as masculine or feminine typed, and 14% (n = 17) did not report a major. Sixteen percent (n = 19) of the students reported being a psychology major. Design The design of this study included a randomly assigned experimental group, for whom gender stereotypes were activated with pretask instructions designed to make gender salient, and an untreated control group. Prior to this manipulation, participants completed a gender identification scale, and following it, women rated their liking for and perceived ability in 20 occupations. Measures Gender identification. The first survey completed was a group identification scale (Pickett, Bonner, & Coleman, 2002, Study 3), modified to assess gender identification. The scale consisted of 16 items and measured both the affective and cognitive aspects of group identification (e.g., I like being a woman, When I talk about women, I usually say we rather than they, I have a number of qualities typical of women ). The items were measured on a 7-point Likert scale, from 1 (strongly disagree) to7(strongly agree). The scale has been used in previous self-stereotyping research to measure identification with a variety of groups and has demonstrated acceptable reliability (Pickett & Brewer, 2001; Pickett et al., 2002). The coefficient alpha for the current study sample was.71. Reported liking for and ability in stereotypically masculine and feminine occupations. Participants were given a list of 20 occupations, half of which previously had been identified as stereotypically masculine (e.g., architect, building contractor, pilot) and half as stereotypically feminine (e.g., nurse, teacher, social worker; Aros, Henly, & Curtis, 1998). For each occupation (presented in alphabetical order), participants indicated the extent to which they would (a) like the job and (b) feel confident in their abilities to be successful in the job. Responses were measured on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 (not like at all/not at all confident in abilities)to5(like a lot/very confident in abilities). Means were computed for each of the subscales. The coefficient alpha for masculine liking was.74, for masculine ability it was.86, for feminine liking it was.61, and for feminine ability it was.79. Demographics. Participants completed a demographic sheet that included age, ethnicity, year in college, and major. Procedures Participants were recruited from a psychology subject pool. The study sessions were conducted in a classroom, and small groups of students participated during each session.

4 Gender Stereotyping and Occupations 199 All materials were provided as paper-and-pencil surveys. At the beginning of the study, session participants were informed they would be completing a series of short and unrelated survey studies. Participants first completed a packet that included the gender identification measure. The experimenter then distributed a second packet that included the experimental manipulation. Each participant s task condition was masked from the experimenter. Participants in the stereotype-activation condition read: For every social group there are a variety of positive and negative traits, characteristics, and behaviors that people assume to describe the group. Below, list the things that people, in general, assume to be true of people the same gender as you. These can be positive or negative assumptions. You do not need to personally believe what you are listing is true, just that people in general might make the assumption. Participants listed a mean of 10.4 items (SD = 5.0; Median = 10.0), which included positive (M = 3.1), negative (M = 5.3), and neutral traits (M = 2.0). The majority of traits were coded as feminine-related personality (M = 4.4), physical (M = 1.33), or cognitive (M =.33) characteristics (coding scheme based on Oswald & Lindstedt, 2006). In the control nonstereotyped condition, participants were asked to list as many uses for a knife as possible. In both conditions, the participants were informed that they had 5 minutes to work on the task, and the experimenter kept time. The third survey packet included the occupational interest and ability measure and the demographic survey. To maintain the cover story of unrelated studies, the three phases were distributed as separate packets and filler measures were included. Upon completion of the study, participants were debriefed about the nature of the study. RESULTS The goal of this study was to examine how genderstereotype activation influences women s ratings of the degree to which they would like and feel confident in their abilities to be successful in masculine- and feminine-typed occupations. Also tested was the extent to which gender identification moderates the impact of gender-stereotype activation. Moderated multiple regressions were computed for each of the dependent variables of interest. Gender identification was centered, and an interaction term between experimental condition and gender identification was created (Aiken & West, 1991). Ratings of ability in and liking for the feminine-typed jobs were correlated, r(119) =.43, p<.01, as were ratings of ability and liking for the masculine-typed jobs, r(119) =.71, p<.01. Given my interest in examining perceptions of ability and liking independently of each other, the rating of liking was controlled when predicting ability by being entered into the first step of the regression model. Gender identification, condition (dummy coded as 1 = stereotype activation and 0 = control condition), and their interaction were entered in the second step of the regression model. Likewise, perceived ability for the occupations was controlled when predicting liking of the occupations. The multiple regression model for the degree to which women reported that they liked the feminine-typed jobs was significant, F(4, 116) = 10.15, p<.01, Adjusted R 2 =.23, above and beyond the significant rating of ability for feminine-typed jobs, B =.37, p <.01. Gender identification, B =.11, p =.52, and condition, B =.02, p =.87, were not significant predictors. However, the anticipated interaction between gender identification and condition was significant, B =.41, p =.03. Figure 1 shows the interaction using gender identification at one standard deviation above and below the mean (Aiken & West, 1991; Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2003). Examination of the graph and simple slope analysis indicated that in the control condition women reported the same level of liking for the femininetyped occupations regardless of their gender identification, simple slope t(116) =.64, p =.52. However, in the stereotype-activation condition the reported liking for the feminine-typed occupations increased with gender identification, simple slope t(116) = 3.02, p<.01. This latter finding indicated that, in the stereotype-activation condition, women who were strongly identified, relative to those who were weakly identified, reported higher levels of liking for feminine-typed occupations. The multiple regression model for reported ability to succeed in feminine-typed jobs also was significant, F(4, 116) = 8.71, p<.01, Adjusted R 2 =.20, above and beyond the significant rating of liking for feminine-typed jobs, B =.50, p<.01. Gender identification was also a significant predictor of perceived ability, B =.46, p =.02, such that gender identification was positively associated with perceived ability in feminine-typed occupations. The experimental condition was not a significant predictor, B =.05, p =.66; however, the anticipated interaction between gender identification and condition was significant, B =.51, p =.02. Examination of the graph (see Figure 2) and simple slope analysis indicated that women in the stereotype-activation condition reported high levels of perceived ability to succeed in feminine-typed fields regardless of their gender identification, simple slope t(116) =.53, p =.60; however, the simple slope was significant for the control condition, simple slope t(116) = 2.44, p<.02. Women who were more weakly gender-identified reported lower levels of perceived ability for feminine-typed occupations than did women who were more strongly gender identified. Notably, women who were weakly gender identified had the lowest predicted perceived ability for feminine-typed occupations in the control condition but were equivalent to the strongly identified women in the stereotype-activation condition. In contrast, stereotype activation, gender identification, and their interaction were not significant predictors of either liking for or perceived ability in the masculine-typed occupations. For ratings of liking for the masculine-typed jobs, the model was statistically significant,

5 200 OSWALD Fig. 1. Predicted ratings of liking for traditionally feminine occupations. Fig. 2. Predicted ratings of perceived ability to be successful in traditionally feminine occupations. F(4, 116) = 30.32, p<.01, Adjusted R 2 =.49; however, the only significant variable was perceived ability for masculine-typed jobs, B =.61, p <.01. For ratings of ability in masculine-typed jobs, the model was significant, F(4, 116) = 30.20, p<.01, Adjusted R 2 =.49, but the only significant variable was reported liking for masculine-typed jobs, B =.84, p<.001. DISCUSSION The current study found that women in the gender stereotype salient condition reported higher levels of interest in, and perceived ability to succeed in, feminine-typed fields than did women in the control condition. However, the impact of the stereotype activation was moderated by women s identification with their gender group. In contrast, gender-stereotype salience did not influence women s reports of interest and perceived ability in masculine-typed fields. Specifically, when gender stereotypes were activated, the more strongly gender-identified participants reported more liking for feminine-typed occupations than did the more weakly identified women. This finding is consistent with previous research findings that women who are strongly gender identified are more susceptible to stereotype threat effects (Schmader, 2002). Thus, only the more strongly gender-identified women showed the expected assimilation effects when feminine stereotypes were activated. In contrast to strongly identified women, the women who were weaker in gender identification did not hold their gender

6 Gender Stereotyping and Occupations 201 as central to their identity and therefore should have been less susceptible to gender stereotypes (Levy, 1996; O Brien & Hummert, 2006; Steele, 1997). It may be that the more weakly gender-identified women experienced reactance to the explicit gender stereotypes and, therefore, reported less liking for feminine-typed occupations than did the strongly identified women. A different pattern was found for perceptions of ability to be successful in feminine-typed occupations. In the control condition, more weakly gender-identified women, relative to those with stronger identification, reported lower levels of ability for feminine-typed jobs. However, women in the stereotype-activation condition reported consistently high levels of confidence in their ability to succeed in femininetyped occupations, regardless of their gender identification. Stereotype activation appears to have resulted in the weakly identified women reporting that they could succeed in feminine-typed occupations. In fact, when gender stereotypes were activated, the weakly and strongly genderidentified women displayed equivalent levels of confidence in their abilities to succeed in feminine-typed occupations. It may be that women who were strongly gender identified were chronically aware of their areas of stereotypic feminine skills and responded accordingly. In contrast, more weakly identified women may not immediately think about their skills in terms of gender stereotypes. When feminine stereotypes were activated they experienced a stereotype boost in confidence in their ability to succeed in stereotypically feminine occupations. In contrast, the stereotype activation did not shift women s ratings of their liking for, or perceived ability to be successful in, stereotypically masculine fields. This finding is consistent with self-stereotyping theory because stereotypes about women are relevant for feminine-typed occupations but lack relevance for masculine-typed occupations. Cejka and Eagly (1999) found that masculine traits are judged as necessary for success in stereotypically masculine occupations, and feminine traits are judged as necessary for success in stereotypically feminine occupations. Thus, feminine stereotypes may not be diagnostic when making ratings of liking for and perceived ability in masculine-typed occupations. For example, activating stereotypes of feminine traits, such as nurturing and warm, may result in women reporting more interest and perceived ability in feminine-typed occupations. However, these traits are not relevant for masculine-typed jobs and thus have no effect on reported liking or perceived ability in those fields. In sum, activating feminine stereotypes contributes to women s endorsing interest and perceived ability to be successful in feminine-typed occupations. Because both interest and perceived ability are important factors that predict academic and career choices (e.g., Eccles, 1994; Lapan et al., 1996; Lent et al., 1994), these results suggest that the activation of feminine stereotypes may, in part, explain why women are drawn toward traditionally feminine occupations. Although this was an experimental situation, realworld examples of gender- stereotype activation are easy to identify. Being the solo woman in a group heightens gender stereotypes and can result in stereotyped task performance (e.g., Sekaquaptewa & Thompson, 2002). Sexist jokes and blonde jokes also highlight negative gender stereotypes and create hostile environments for female employees (e.g., LaFrance, & Woodzicka, 1998; Watts, 2007). By their nature, gender-segregated occupations make gender stereotypes highly salient. The current findings demonstrate how gender stereotypes influence women s attitudes that are important when choosing careers. Women may be pulled by others into gender-appropriate fields while simultaneously feeling an internal push from personal awareness of gender stereotypes (see also Schmader, Johns, & Barquissau, 2004). Even when women choose a nontraditional major or career, stereotypes might play a role in the choice to leave the field. For example, female students in traditionally masculine majors, compared to those in traditionally feminine majors, reported experiencing more stereotype threat and discrimination and had a higher intention to leave the major (Steele, James, & Barnett, 2002). The current study found that women may be drawn to feminine occupations when feminine stereotypes are activated. That is, feminine fields might become especially attractive to women who experience stereotype threat or discrimination in a male dominated field. Thus, rather than continually dealing with the anxiety of potentially confirming negative stereotypes, women may choose to pursue a stereotypically consistent field. A goal, then, should be to reduce the harmful impact of gender stereotypes so as to increase women s participation in nontraditional fields. Indeed, research has shown several ways in which gender stereotypes can be negated, resulting in an increase in women s math performance. For example, in experimental research, it has been found that women performed better in math and were more confident about their math skills when there was a female role model who was competent in math (Marx & Roman, 2002) and when women s achievements in masculine-typed fields were highlighted (McIntyre, Paulson, & Lord, 2003). Oswald and Harvey (2003) found that college-age women who reported that they were encouraged by their parents and teachers to do well in math, had female role models, and generally reported that they had been shielded from negative gender stereotypes reported being identified with math and having a positive attitude toward math. Thus, it is important to remember that educators and parents can take proactive steps to reduce the salience of gender stereotypes and that these efforts can have positive implications for women s career aspirations. Although the current study demonstrated the importance of stereotypes in influencing women s reported liking for and perceived ability in feminine-typed occupations, there are questions that remain for future research. It would be useful to also examine the effect of

7 202 OSWALD masculine-stereotype activation on male participants occupational attitudes. Occupational segregation results also because men enter masculine-typed fields more often than they enter feminine-typed fields. One might expect that masculine stereotypes may have a similar influence on men s occupational choices. Furthermore, would cross-stereotype activation (priming women with masculine stereotyped traits) provide one possible intervention? It would also be interesting to examine the impact of stereotype activation on women in traditional and nontraditional majors. Stereotype-threat theory (Steele, 1997) suggests that the women who feel that a domain is important in their lives should experience the largest detriment in performance when stereotypes are relevant to that domain. Thus, it might be the case that women in masculine-typed majors would experience the greatest change in attitudes upon stereotype activation. Unfortunately, relatively few women in the current study reported masculine-typed majors so that these analyses were not possible to compute. Finally, it should be noted that in the current study gender identification was measured prior to the study manipulation and ratings of occupational liking and perceived ability. To a certain extent, all participants may have been somewhat primed to think about their gender, and thus the study manipulation may be comparing women in a stereotype-salient condition to women for whom the stereotype was less salient but not nonexistent. Ideally, future research should obtain a pretest measure of gender identification. In sum, occupational choices are made within a general cultural context of gender stereotypes. This study sought to examine the impact of these contextual stereotypes by experimentally making these stereotypes salient. This research is novel in that it reflects an extension of research on stereotype activation from performance behaviors to more global occupation-related attitudes. The variables of reported liking for (e.g., interest) and perceived ability to be successful in occupations (e.g., self-efficacy) were chosen because they have been highlighted in previous models as central factors when choosing academic major and careers (e.g., Eccles, 1987, 1994; Lapan et al., 1996; Lent et al., 1994). That stereotype activation causally influences liking for and perceived ability to succeed in feminine occupations suggests that gender stereotypes may be one reason why we continue to see women pursing occupations that are gender traditional rather than nontraditional. Initial submission: June 12, 2007 Initial acceptance: November 15, 2007 Final acceptance: December 18, 2007 REFERENCES Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Aros, J. R., Henly, G. A., & Curtis, N. T. (1998). Occupational sextype and sex differences in vocational preference-measured interest relationships. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 53, Biernat, M., Vescio, T. K., & Green, M. L. (1996). Selective selfstereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, Cadinu, M., Maass, A., Rosabianca, A., & Kiesner, J. (2005). Why do women underperform under stereotype threat? Evidence for the role of negative thinking. Psychological Science, 16, Cejka, M. A., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender-stereotypic images of occupations correspond to sex segregation of employment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, Chiu, C., Hong, Y., Lam, I., Fu, J., Tong, J., & Lee, V. (1998). Stereotyping and self-presentation: Effects of gender stereotype activation. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 1, Davis, C., Aronson, J., & Salinas, M. (2006). Shades of threat: Racial identity as a moderator of stereotype threat. Journal of Black Psychology, 32, Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women s achievementrelated decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, Eccles, J. S. (1994). Understanding women s educational and occupational choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, Eccles, J., Barber, B., & Jozefowicz, D. (1999). Linking gender to educational, occupational, and recreational choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. In W. B. Swann, J. H. Langlois, & L. A. Gilbert (Eds.), Sexism and stereotypes in modern society: The gender sciences of Janet Taylor Spence (pp ). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Eccles, J., Jacobs, J., & Harold, R. (1990). Gender role stereotypes, expectancy effects, and parents socialization of gender differences. Journal of Social Issues, 46, Gonzalez, P. M., Blanton, H., & Williams, K. J. (2002). The effects of stereotype threat and double-minority status on test performance of Latino women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2000). A threatening intellectual environment: Why females are susceptible to experiencing problem-solving deficits in the presence of males. Psychological Science, 11, Keller, J. (2002). Blatant stereotype threat and women s math performance: Self-handicapping as a strategic means to cope with obtrusive negative performance expectations. Sex Roles, 47, LaFrance, M., & Woodzicka, J. A. (1998). No laughing matter: Women s verbal and nonverbal reactions to sexist humor. In J. Swim & C. Stangor (Eds.), Prejudice: The target s perspective (pp ). New York: Academic Press. Lapan, R. T., Shaughnessy, P., & Boggs, K. (1996). Efficacy expectations and vocational interests as mediators between sex and choice of math/science college majors: A longitudinal study. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 49, Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., Brenner, B., Chopra, S. B., Davis, T., Talleyrand, R., et al. (2001). The role of contextual supports and barriers in the choice of math/science educational options: A test of social cognitive hypotheses. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48,

8 Gender Stereotyping and Occupations 203 Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Hackett, G. (1994). Toward a unifying social cognitive theory of career and academic interest, choice, and performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 45, Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Hackett, G. (2000). Contextual supports and barriers to career choice: A social cognitive analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47, Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., Schmidt, J., Brenner, B., Lyons, H., & Treistman, D. (2003). Relation of contextual supports and barriers to choice behavior in engineering majors: Test of alternative social cognitive models. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50, Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., Sheu, H., Schmidt, J., Brenner, B. R., Gloster, C. S., et al. (2005). Social cognitive predictors of academic interests and goals in engineering: Utility for women and students at historically Black universities. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, Levy, B. R. (1996). Improving memory in old age through implicit self-stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, Levy, B. (2000). Handwriting as a reflection of aging selfstereotypes. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 33, Levy, B. R. (2003). Mind matters: Cognitive and physical effects of aging self-stereotypes. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 58B, Leyens, J. P., Desert, M., Croizet, J. C., & Darcis, C. (2000). Stereotype threat: Are lower status and history of stigmatization necessary preconditions of stereotype threat? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, Marx, D. M., & Roman, J. S. (2002). Female role models: Protecting women s math test performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, McGlone, M. S., & Aronson, J. (2006). Stereotype threat, identity salience, and spatial reasoning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, McGlone, M. S., Aronson, J., & Kobrynowicz, D. (2006). Stereotype threat and the gender gap in political knowledge. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R. M., & Lord, C. G. (2003). Alleviating women s mathematics stereotype threat through salience of group achievements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, National Science Foundation. (2007). Science and engineering degrees: (NSF Publication No ). Arlington, VA: Author. Nosek, B., Banaji, M., & Greenwald, A. (2002). Math = male, me = female, therefore math not-equal-to me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, O Brien, L., & Crandall, C. (2003). Stereotype threat and arousal: Effects on women s math performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, O Brien, L. T., & Hummert, M. L. (2006). Memory performance of late middle-aged adults: Contrasting self-stereotyping and stereotype threat accounts of assimilation to age stereotypes. Social Cognition, 24, Oswald, D. L., & Harvey, R. D. (2000). Stereotypes, hostile environments, and math performance among undergraduate women. Current Psychology, 19, Oswald, D. L., & Harvey, R. D. (2003). A q-methodological study of women s subjective perspectives on mathematics. Sex Roles, 49, Oswald, D. L., & Lindstedt, K. (2006). The content and function of gender self-stereotypes: An exploratory investigation. Sex Roles, 54, Pickett, C. L., Bonner, B. L., & Coleman, J. M. (2002). Motivated self-stereotyping: Heightened assimilation and differentiation needs result in increased levels of positive and negative self-stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, Pickett, C. L., & Brewer, M. B. (2001). Assimilation and differentiation needs as motivational determinants of perceived in-group and out-group homogeneity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2003). Simple intercepts, simple slopes, and regions of significance in MLR 2-Way Interactions. Retrieved June 27, 2006, from Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Barquissau, M. (2004). The costs of accepting gender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women s experience in the math domain. Sex Roles, 50, Sekaquaptewa, D., & Thompson, M. (2002). Solo status, stereotype threat, and performance expectancies: Their effects on women s performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, Shih, M., Ambady, N., Richeson, J. A., Fujita, K., & Gray, H. M. (2002). Stereotype performance boosts: The impact of selfrelevance and the manner of stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, Shih, M., Pittinsky, T. L., & Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: Identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 10, Shih, M., Pittinsky, T., & Trahan, A. (2006). Domain-specific effects of stereotypes on performance. Self and Identity, 5, Spencer, S., Steele, C., & Quinn, D. (1999). Stereotype threat and women s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, Steele, C., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, Steele, J. R., & Ambady, N. (2006). Math is hard! The effect of gender priming on women s attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, Steele, J., James, J. B., & Barnett, R. C. (2002). Learning in a man s world: Examining the perceptions of undergraduate women in male-dominated academic areas. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2005, May). Women in the labor force: A data book (Table 11). Retrieved June 5, 2007, from pdf Watts, J. (2007). Can t take a joke? Humour as resistance, refuge and exclusion in a highly gendered workplace. Feminism & Psychology, 17,

Testing hypothesized moderators of stereotype threat. Do calculus GPA and math identification moderate the relationship between stereotype threat

Testing hypothesized moderators of stereotype threat. Do calculus GPA and math identification moderate the relationship between stereotype threat Initial Submission: April 8, 2011 Resubmission: December 15, 2011 Resubmission: March 9, 2012 Running Head: Testing hypothesized moderators of stereotype threat Do calculus GPA and math identification

More information

PERCEIVED BARRIERS AND ENDORSEMENT OF STEREOTYPES AMONG ADOLESCENT GIRLS OF COLOR IN STEM. Allison Scott, Ph.D. Alexis Martin, Ph.D.

PERCEIVED BARRIERS AND ENDORSEMENT OF STEREOTYPES AMONG ADOLESCENT GIRLS OF COLOR IN STEM. Allison Scott, Ph.D. Alexis Martin, Ph.D. PERCEIVED BARRIERS AND ENDORSEMENT OF STEREOTYPES AMONG ADOLESCENT GIRLS OF COLOR IN STEM Allison Scott, Ph.D. Alexis Martin, Ph.D. Presentation Agenda Overview of Underrepresentation and Barriers for

More information

The Costs of Accepting Gender Differences: The Role of Stereotype Endorsement in Women s Experience in the Math Domain

The Costs of Accepting Gender Differences: The Role of Stereotype Endorsement in Women s Experience in the Math Domain Sex Roles, Vol. 50, Nos. 11/12, June 2004 ( C 2004) The Costs of Accepting Gender Differences: The Role of Stereotype Endorsement in Women s Experience in the Math Domain Toni Schmader, 1,2 Michael Johns,

More information

Teacher Influences on Child Interest in STEM Careers

Teacher Influences on Child Interest in STEM Careers Teacher Influences on Child Interest in STEM Meara M. Habashi Iowa Wesleyan College, Mt. Pleasant, USA meara.habashi@iwc.edu William G. Graziano Purdue University, West Lafayette, USA graziano@psych.purdue.edu

More information

REMINDERS OF A POSITIVELY STEREOTYPED IDENTITY WHEN FACING STEREOTYPE THREAT: IDENTITY CONSISTENCY AND IDENTITY ACCESSIBILITY AS MEDIATING MECHANISMS

REMINDERS OF A POSITIVELY STEREOTYPED IDENTITY WHEN FACING STEREOTYPE THREAT: IDENTITY CONSISTENCY AND IDENTITY ACCESSIBILITY AS MEDIATING MECHANISMS REMINDERS OF A POSITIVELY STEREOTYPED IDENTITY WHEN FACING STEREOTYPE THREAT: IDENTITY CONSISTENCY AND IDENTITY ACCESSIBILITY AS MEDIATING MECHANISMS BY 2011 KELLY DANAHER Submitted to the graduate degree

More information

Social Cognitive Predictors of Adjustment to Engineering by Underrepresented Students

Social Cognitive Predictors of Adjustment to Engineering by Underrepresented Students 1 Social Cognitive Predictors of Adjustment to Engineering by Underrepresented Students Robert W. Lent, Matthew J. Miller, Paige E. Smith, University of Maryland; Bevlee A. Watford, Virginia Tech University;

More information

Capitalizing on Multiple Social Identities to Prevent Stereotype Threat: The Moderating Role of Self-Esteem

Capitalizing on Multiple Social Identities to Prevent Stereotype Threat: The Moderating Role of Self-Esteem Capitalizing on Multiple Social Identities to Prevent Stereotype Threat: The Moderating Role of Self-Esteem Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(2) 239 250 2010 by the Society for Personality

More information

The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance

The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance Lauren Byrne, Melannie Tate Faculty Sponsor: Bianca Basten, Department of Psychology ABSTRACT Psychological research

More information

Creating a Diverse Learning Community

Creating a Diverse Learning Community Creating a Diverse Learning Community August 14, 2013 Our Approach A Framework We re here to talk about diversity and to plan out a workshop to encourage healthy social interactions between people without

More information

Do I Belong? It Depends on When You Ask

Do I Belong? It Depends on When You Ask Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 17, No. 4, 432 436 1099-9809/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0025455 Do I Belong? It Depends on When You Ask

More information

Does Stereotype Threat Require Stereotypes? Sarah LeStourgeon & David Phelps. Hanover College

Does Stereotype Threat Require Stereotypes? Sarah LeStourgeon & David Phelps. Hanover College Components of Stereotype Threat 1 Running head: Does Stereotype threat Require Stereotypes? Does Stereotype Threat Require Stereotypes? Sarah LeStourgeon & David Phelps Hanover College Components of Stereotype

More information

The effect of priming intelligence malleability on stereotype threat and performance.

The effect of priming intelligence malleability on stereotype threat and performance. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 2003 The effect of priming intelligence malleability on stereotype threat and performance. Kathleen C.

More information

Relations of Ethnic Stereotype Threat and Mindset to Achievement Goals in Science

Relations of Ethnic Stereotype Threat and Mindset to Achievement Goals in Science Relations of Ethnic Stereotype Threat and Mindset to Achievement Goals in Science Despite efforts to increase the persistence and achievement of underrepresented minority (URM) students in STEM, these

More information

Preserving positive identities: Public and private regard for one s ingroup and susceptibility to stereotype threat

Preserving positive identities: Public and private regard for one s ingroup and susceptibility to stereotype threat Article Group Processes & Intergroup Relations G P I R Preserving positive identities: Public and private regard for one s ingroup and susceptibility to stereotype threat Group Processes & Intergroup Relations

More information

Does Teaching Women About Stereotype Threat Reduce Its Effects on Math Performance?

Does Teaching Women About Stereotype Threat Reduce Its Effects on Math Performance? The College at Brockport: State University of New York Digital Commons @Brockport Senior Honors Theses Master's Theses and Honors Projects 4-9-2013 Does Teaching Women About Stereotype Threat Reduce Its

More information

Signaling Threat. How Situational Cues Affect Women in Math, Science, and Engineering Settings. Research Article

Signaling Threat. How Situational Cues Affect Women in Math, Science, and Engineering Settings. Research Article PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Signaling Threat How Situational Cues Affect Women in Math, Science, and Engineering Settings Mary C. Murphy, Claude M. Steele, and James J. Gross Stanford University

More information

Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1

Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1 Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1 Stereotype Threat and the Racial Achievement Gap: Race, Salience of Race, and Perception of Race as Possible Contributing Factors Scientist

More information

THE ROLE OF MOTIVATION IN BLATANT STEREOTYPE THREAT, SUBTLE STEREOTYPE THREAT, AND STEREOTYPE PRIMING. A dissertation presented. Jeremy Paul Jamieson

THE ROLE OF MOTIVATION IN BLATANT STEREOTYPE THREAT, SUBTLE STEREOTYPE THREAT, AND STEREOTYPE PRIMING. A dissertation presented. Jeremy Paul Jamieson 1 THE ROLE OF MOTIVATION IN BLATANT STEREOTYPE THREAT, SUBTLE STEREOTYPE THREAT, AND STEREOTYPE PRIMING A dissertation presented by Jeremy Paul Jamieson to The Department of Psychology In partial fulfillment

More information

Sexism Predicts Appeal of Gender Stereotypes from a Popular Book on Relationships

Sexism Predicts Appeal of Gender Stereotypes from a Popular Book on Relationships Sexism Predicts Appeal of Gender Stereotypes from a Popular Book on Relationships Jeanna E. Cooper and Margaret L. Signorella 1 The Pennsylvania State University College students and older adults had difficulty

More information

Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members

Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members Margaret E. Brooks, Eyal Grauer, Erin E. Thornbury, and Scott Highhouse Bowling Green State University The scientist-practitioner

More information

When challenging math tests are described as diagnostic measures of. ability or when gender differences are salient, stereotype threat causes women to

When challenging math tests are described as diagnostic measures of. ability or when gender differences are salient, stereotype threat causes women to Ashley R. Wilson, MALLEABLE VIEW OF INTELLIGENCE AS INTERVENTION FOR STEREOTYPE THREAT: OVERCOMING MATH UNDERPERFORMANCE IN WOMEN (Under the direction of Dr. Marion Eppler) Department of Psychology, December

More information

Gender and Discipline Specific Differences in Mathematical Self-Efficacy of Incoming Students at a Large Public University

Gender and Discipline Specific Differences in Mathematical Self-Efficacy of Incoming Students at a Large Public University Gender and Discipline Specific Differences in Mathematical Self-Efficacy of Incoming Students at a Large Public University Ulrike Genschel, Hien X. Nguyen Iowa State University This study investigates

More information

Stereotype Threat and Women's Perceptions of Leadership Self-Efficacy

Stereotype Threat and Women's Perceptions of Leadership Self-Efficacy Clemson University TigerPrints All Theses Theses 8-2008 Stereotype Threat and Women's Perceptions of Leadership Self-Efficacy Phillip Lipka Clemson University, plipka@clemson.edu Follow this and additional

More information

Implicit stereotypes and women s math performance: How implicit gender-math stereotypes inxuence women s susceptibility to stereotype threat

Implicit stereotypes and women s math performance: How implicit gender-math stereotypes inxuence women s susceptibility to stereotype threat Journal of Experimental Social Psychology xxx (2006) xxx xxx www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Implicit stereotypes and women s math performance: How implicit gender-math stereotypes inxuence women s susceptibility

More information

Constraints into Preferences: Gender, Status, and Emerging Career Aspirations

Constraints into Preferences: Gender, Status, and Emerging Career Aspirations Constraints into Preferences: Gender, Status, and Emerging Career Aspirations Shelley J. Correll Cornell University This study presents an experimental evaluation of a model that describes the constraining

More information

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels A peer-reviewed electronic journal. Copyright is retained by the first or sole author, who grants right of first publication to Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation. Permission is granted to distribute

More information

Supplementary Study A: Do the exemplars that represent a category influence IAT effects?

Supplementary Study A: Do the exemplars that represent a category influence IAT effects? Supplement A to Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method Variables and Construct Validity. Personality and Social Psychology

More information

Stereotype threat and achievement motivation :: a mediational analysis.

Stereotype threat and achievement motivation :: a mediational analysis. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 2000 Stereotype threat and achievement motivation :: a mediational analysis. David R. Fellner University

More information

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students Mia Bonner Millersville University Abstract Previous research (Elion, Slaney, Wang and French, 2012) found

More information

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Heather Dudley What influences people s willingness to interact with someone of a different cultural background from

More information

Breaking the Bias Habit. Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute

Breaking the Bias Habit. Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute Breaking the Bias Habit Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute P e r c e n t W o m e n Percent Women Bachelor's Degrees, Selected Fields

More information

The Effects of Stereotype Threat on State Levels of Stigma Consciousness and Overall Performance on a Stereotype-Relevant Task

The Effects of Stereotype Threat on State Levels of Stigma Consciousness and Overall Performance on a Stereotype-Relevant Task Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Summer 2011 The Effects of Stereotype Threat on State Levels

More information

STRIDE. STRATEGIES and TACTICS for RECRUITING to IMPROVE DIVERSITY and EXCELLENCE. at The University of Tennessee

STRIDE. STRATEGIES and TACTICS for RECRUITING to IMPROVE DIVERSITY and EXCELLENCE. at The University of Tennessee STRATEGIES and TACTICS for RECRUITING to IMPROVE DIVERSITY and EXCELLENCE at The University of Tennessee stride@utk.edu We are grateful for the support and inspiration we have received from the committee

More information

Few people would disagree with the idea that role models

Few people would disagree with the idea that role models PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN Marx, Roman / FEMALE ROLE MODELS Female Role Models: Protecting Women s Math Test Performance David M. Marx Jasmin S. Roman Harvard University Recent theory and

More information

Talent versus Effort: Effects of Gender Differences. In Music Education. Jasmine Carey. Ohio State University

Talent versus Effort: Effects of Gender Differences. In Music Education. Jasmine Carey. Ohio State University Talent Vs. Effort 1 Talent versus Effort: Effects of Gender Differences In Music Education Jasmine Carey Ohio State University Address correspondence: Jasmine Carey 5065 Cambrian Dr. Columbus, OH 43220

More information

ENTREPRENEURSHIP AND STEREOTYPES: ARE ENTREPRENEURS FROM MARS OR FROM VENUS?

ENTREPRENEURSHIP AND STEREOTYPES: ARE ENTREPRENEURS FROM MARS OR FROM VENUS? ENTREPRENEURSHIP AND STEREOTYPES: ARE ENTREPRENEURS FROM MARS OR FROM VENUS? VISHAL K. GUPTA University of Missouri Department of Management Columbia, MO 65211-2600 Phone: (573) 882-7659 DANIEL B. TURBAN

More information

Positive Feedback From Male Authority Figures Boosts Women s Math Outcomes

Positive Feedback From Male Authority Figures Boosts Women s Math Outcomes 74131PSPXXX10.1177/01461671774131Personality and Social Psychology BulletinPark et al. research-article017 Article Positive Feedback From Male Authority Figures Boosts Women s Math Outcomes Lora E. Park

More information

Discovering Diversity Profile Group Report

Discovering Diversity Profile Group Report Discovering Diversity Profile Group Report Sample Report (5 People) Friday, June 18, 2010 This report is provided by: Intesi! Resources 14230 N. 20th Way Phoenix, AZ 85022 Phone: 602-482-6422 Toll Free:

More information

Whitewater, WI, USA b Psychology Department, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada. Available online: 17 May 2012

Whitewater, WI, USA b Psychology Department, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada. Available online: 17 May 2012 This article was downloaded by: [The University of British Columbia] On: 17 May 2012, At: 17:14 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered

More information

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44 (2008) 1187 1193 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Social Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Can fixed

More information

We Can Do It: The Interplay of Construal Orientation and Social Comparisons Under Threat

We Can Do It: The Interplay of Construal Orientation and Social Comparisons Under Threat Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 88, No. 3, 432 446 0022-3514/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.432 We Can Do It: The

More information

Running head: AFFECTIVE FORECASTING AND OBJECTIFICATION 1

Running head: AFFECTIVE FORECASTING AND OBJECTIFICATION 1 Running head: AFFECTIVE FORECASTING AND OBJECTIFICATION 1 The Effect of Demographics, Body Shame, Ambivalent Sexism, and Enjoyment of Sexualization on Anticipated Emotion in Response to Sexual Objectification

More information

Performance in competitive Environments: Gender differences

Performance in competitive Environments: Gender differences Performance in competitive Environments: Gender differences Uri Gneezy Technion and Chicago Business School Muriel Niederle Harvard University Aldo Rustichini University of Minnesota 1 Gender differences

More information

The Stereotyped Task Engagement Process: The Role of Interest and Achievement Motivation

The Stereotyped Task Engagement Process: The Role of Interest and Achievement Motivation Journal of Educational Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 99, No. 1, 99 114 0022-0663/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.99 The Stereotyped Task Engagement

More information

Self-Efficacy in the Prediction of Academic Performance and Perceived Career Options

Self-Efficacy in the Prediction of Academic Performance and Perceived Career Options Journal of Counseling Psychology 1986, Vol. 33, No. 3, 265-269 Copyright 1986 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. F 0022-0167/86/0.75 Self-Efficacy in the Prediction of Academic Performance

More information

The Effect of Stereotype Threat on Women s Mathematical Performance and Motivation

The Effect of Stereotype Threat on Women s Mathematical Performance and Motivation i The Effect of Stereotype Threat on Women s Mathematical Performance and Motivation Vincent Fogliati Bachelor of Arts Psychology (Hons) Macquarie University Psychology Department Submitted for the degree

More information

Fear of Success in Males and Females Effects of Developmental Level and Sex-Linked Course of Study ~

Fear of Success in Males and Females Effects of Developmental Level and Sex-Linked Course of Study ~ Sex Roles, Vol. 2, No. 3, 1976 Fear of Success in Males and Females Effects of Developmental Level and Sex-Linked Course of Study ~ Betsy Kimball and Robert L. Leahy 2 The Catholic University of America

More information

WHAT IS CULTURAL COMPETENCE?

WHAT IS CULTURAL COMPETENCE? WHAT IS CULTURAL COMPETENCE? A culturally competent professional is one who is actively in the process of becoming aware of his or her own assumptions about human behavior, values, biases, preconceived

More information

A Metacognitive Perspective on the Cognitive Deficits Experienced in Intellectually Threatening Environments

A Metacognitive Perspective on the Cognitive Deficits Experienced in Intellectually Threatening Environments A Metacognitive Perspective on the Cognitive Deficits Experienced in Intellectually Threatening Environments The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits

More information

Changing the Graduate School Experience: Impacts on the Role Identity of Women

Changing the Graduate School Experience: Impacts on the Role Identity of Women Changing the Graduate School Experience: Impacts on the Role Identity of Women Background and Purpose: Although the number of women earning Bachelor s degrees in science, technology, engineering and mathematic

More information

The Effects of Stereotype Threat/Boost and Personality on a Verbal Reasoning Task. Kelly L. Arnold Mary A. Ford and Ursula Goldstein

The Effects of Stereotype Threat/Boost and Personality on a Verbal Reasoning Task. Kelly L. Arnold Mary A. Ford and Ursula Goldstein The Effects of Stereotype Threat/Boost and Personality on a Verbal Reasoning Task By Kelly L. Arnold Mary A. Ford and Ursula Goldstein An Honors Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements

More information

Stereotype threat for the scholar-athlete in the classroom

Stereotype threat for the scholar-athlete in the classroom Stereotype threat for the scholar-athlete in the classroom Academic stereotypes about college-athletes Research indicates that academic faculty and traditional student peers perceive college-athletes to

More information

THE ROLE OF BELIEFS OF CONTROLLABILITY IN FAT STIGMA

THE ROLE OF BELIEFS OF CONTROLLABILITY IN FAT STIGMA Journal of Integrated Social Sciences www.jiss.org, 2013-3(1): 92-105 Original Article: THE ROLE OF BELIEFS OF CONTROLLABILITY IN FAT STIGMA Erin D. Solomon, M.S.(R) Saint Louis University, USA Amy M.

More information

Florida State University Libraries

Florida State University Libraries Florida State University Libraries 2016 The Effect of Brief Staff-Assisted Career Service Delivery on Drop-In Clients Debra S. Osborn, Seth W. Hayden, Gary W. Peterson and James P. Sampson Follow this

More information

WHY THE OPPORTUNITY/ACHIEVEMENT GAPS ARE SO PERSISTENT AND WHAT WE CAN DO TO CLOSE THEM

WHY THE OPPORTUNITY/ACHIEVEMENT GAPS ARE SO PERSISTENT AND WHAT WE CAN DO TO CLOSE THEM WHY THE OPPORTUNITY/ACHIEVEMENT GAPS ARE SO PERSISTENT AND WHAT WE CAN DO TO CLOSE THEM MCPS/NAACP Parent s Council Kickoff Meeting October 16, 2017 Karl W. Reid, Ed.D. Executive Director National Society

More information

Problems in the pipeline: Stereotype threat and women's achievement in high-level math courses

Problems in the pipeline: Stereotype threat and women's achievement in high-level math courses Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 17 28 Problems in the pipeline: Stereotype threat and women's achievement in high-level math courses Catherine

More information

Evaluating Others: How Our Beliefs Become Reality

Evaluating Others: How Our Beliefs Become Reality Evaluating Others: How Our Beliefs Become Reality Business 38811 Summer, 2008 Well, we needed the rain. 2 Why? Levy & Langer (1994): Beliefs create aging reality? Two Independent Variables: 1. Age: Young

More information

Stereotyping From the Perspective of Perceivers and Targets

Stereotyping From the Perspective of Perceivers and Targets Unit 5 Social Psychology and Culture Subunit 1 Stereotypes and Prejudice Article 1 3-1-2012 Stereotyping From the Perspective of Perceivers and Targets Saera R. Khan University of San Francisco, srkhan@usfca.edu

More information

Threat By Association: Minimal Group Affiliation And Its Outcome For Stereotype Threat

Threat By Association: Minimal Group Affiliation And Its Outcome For Stereotype Threat Wayne State University Wayne State University Theses 1-1-2010 Threat By Association: Minimal Group Affiliation And Its Outcome For Stereotype Threat Eric W. Fuller Wayne State University Follow this and

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 4, Number 1 Submitted: August 20, 2008 Revisions: October 16, 2008 Accepted: October 17, 2008 Publication Date: October 20, 2008 Start Today or the Very

More information

Does the Relationship between Motivation and Performance Differ with Ability?

Does the Relationship between Motivation and Performance Differ with Ability? James Madison University JMU Scholarly Commons Department of Graduate Psychology - Faculty Scholarship Department of Graduate Psychology 4-1999 Does the Relationship between Motivation and Performance

More information

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment Marshall University Marshall Digital Scholar Management Faculty Research Management, Marketing and MIS Fall 11-14-2009 Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment Wai Kwan

More information

Marisa Puckett and Geraldine E. Hynes, Ph.D. Sam Houston State University Huntsville, Texas

Marisa Puckett and Geraldine E. Hynes, Ph.D. Sam Houston State University Huntsville, Texas Feminine Leadership in Commercial Aviation: Success Stories of Women Pilots and Captains Marisa Puckett and Geraldine E. Hynes, Ph.D. Sam Houston State University Huntsville, Texas Abstract This research

More information

Career Counseling and Services: A Cognitive Information Processing Approach

Career Counseling and Services: A Cognitive Information Processing Approach Career Counseling and Services: A Cognitive Information Processing Approach James P. Sampson, Jr., Robert C. Reardon, Gary W. Peterson, and Janet G. Lenz Florida State University Copyright 2003 by James

More information

Unconscious Knowledge Assessment

Unconscious Knowledge Assessment Unconscious Knowledge Assessment The Unconscious Knowledge Assessment is a Go/No Go Association Task (GNAT; Nosek & Banaji, 2001), which is a measure of implicit association. That is, the unconsciously

More information

Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes

Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes Hidden Bias Implicit Bias, Prejudice and Stereotypes Dr. Susan Boland Lock Haven University of PA Presented at AAUW-PA 88 th Annual Meeting Explicit vs. Implicit Evidence of implicit processes Are we all

More information

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution 1 Robert D. Mather University of Central Oklahoma Charles M. Mather University of Science

More information

Measuring Perceived Social Support in Mexican American Youth: Psychometric Properties of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support

Measuring Perceived Social Support in Mexican American Youth: Psychometric Properties of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support Marquette University e-publications@marquette College of Education Faculty Research and Publications Education, College of 5-1-2004 Measuring Perceived Social Support in Mexican American Youth: Psychometric

More information

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective Academic Leadership Journal in Student Research Volume 4 Spring 2016 Article 4 2016 Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective Kevin L. Kenney Fort Hays State University Follow this

More information

How Stereotypes Stifle Performance Potential

How Stereotypes Stifle Performance Potential Social and Personality Psychology Compass 5/10 (2011): 792 806, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00390.x How Stereotypes Stifle Performance Potential Toni Schmader* and Alyssa Croft University of British Columbia

More information

Chapter 6 Topic 6B Test Bias and Other Controversies. The Question of Test Bias

Chapter 6 Topic 6B Test Bias and Other Controversies. The Question of Test Bias Chapter 6 Topic 6B Test Bias and Other Controversies The Question of Test Bias Test bias is an objective, empirical question, not a matter of personal judgment. Test bias is a technical concept of amenable

More information

PSYCHOLOGY (413) Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D.

PSYCHOLOGY (413) Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D. PSYCHOLOGY (413) 662-5453 Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D. Email: S.Claffey@mcla.edu PROGRAMS AVAILABLE BACHELOR OF ARTS IN PSYCHOLOGY BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS MINOR PSYCHOLOGY MINOR TEACHER LICENSURE PSYCHOLOGY

More information

overview of presentation

overview of presentation how STEM faculty contribute to inclusive classrooms: exploring our own biases and assumptions jen s. schoepke, ph.d. postdoctoral researcher wisconsin program for scientific teaching uw-madison jsschoepke@wisc.edu

More information

The Relationship of Competitiveness and Achievement Orientation to Participation in Sport and Nonsport Activities

The Relationship of Competitiveness and Achievement Orientation to Participation in Sport and Nonsport Activities The Relationship of Competitiveness and Achievement Orientation to Participation in Sport and Nonsport Activities By: Diane L. Gill, David A. Dzewaltowski, and Thomas E. Deeter Gill, D.L., Dzewaltowski,

More information

Assessing the Creativity and Its Obstacles based on the Individual Characteristics

Assessing the Creativity and Its Obstacles based on the Individual Characteristics Assessing the Creativity and Its Obstacles based on the Individual Characteristics Vahid Rouhollahi, Mansoorehossat Rozan, Akhil Mehrotra Department of Physical Education, Banaras Hindu University, Varanasi,

More information

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science Ghali Hassan Faculty of Education, Queensland University of Technology, Australia Abstract The purpose of this exploratory study was

More information

Gill, D.L. (1986). Competitiveness among females and males in physical activity classes. Sex Roles, 15,

Gill, D.L. (1986). Competitiveness among females and males in physical activity classes. Sex Roles, 15, Competitiveness Among Females and Males in Physical Activity Classes By: Diane L. Gill Gill, D.L. (1986). Competitiveness among females and males in physical activity classes. Sex Roles, 15, 233-257. Made

More information

Scientist-Practitioner Interest Changes and Course Outcomes in a Senior Research Psychology Course

Scientist-Practitioner Interest Changes and Course Outcomes in a Senior Research Psychology Course Scientist-Practitioner Interest Changes and Course Outcomes in a Senior Research Psychology Course Terry F. Pettijohn II Arsida Ndoni Coastal Carolina University Abstract Psychology students (N = 42) completed

More information

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance 1 Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Erica Behrens Winona State University Abstract This study examines the effects of self-consciousness

More information

Discovering Diversity Profile Individual Report

Discovering Diversity Profile Individual Report Individual Report Respondent Name Monday, November 13, 2006 This report is provided by: Integro Leadership Institute 1380 Wilmington Pike, Suite 113B West Chester PA 19382 Toll Free (866) 468-3476 Phone

More information

Agenda. Introduction and Overview. Self Assessment. The Experiences of Male Students of Color (MOC) Attitudes Towards Mental Health Services

Agenda. Introduction and Overview. Self Assessment. The Experiences of Male Students of Color (MOC) Attitudes Towards Mental Health Services Agenda Introduction and Overview Self Assessment The Experiences of Male Students of Color (MOC) Attitudes Towards Mental Health Services Recommendations to Address the Issue Case Examples... Questions,

More information

IMPLICIT GENDER STEREOTYPES AT THE NEGOTIATION TABLE: THE MODERATING ROLE OF GENDER AND POWER ROLE MADELIENE ALGER. A thesis submitted to the

IMPLICIT GENDER STEREOTYPES AT THE NEGOTIATION TABLE: THE MODERATING ROLE OF GENDER AND POWER ROLE MADELIENE ALGER. A thesis submitted to the IMPLICIT GENDER STEREOTYPES AT THE NEGOTIATION TABLE: THE MODERATING ROLE OF GENDER AND POWER ROLE By MADELIENE ALGER A thesis submitted to the Graduate School-Camden Rutgers, The State University of New

More information

Stereotype Threat in America. Morgan Watkins. University of Kentucky

Stereotype Threat in America. Morgan Watkins. University of Kentucky Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT 1 Stereotype Threat in America Morgan Watkins University of Kentucky STEREOTYPE THREAT 2 Abstract Stereotype threat refers to being at risk of confirming, as self-characteristic,

More information

Math is Hard! The evect of gender priming on women s attitudes

Math is Hard! The evect of gender priming on women s attitudes Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 42 (2006) 428 436 www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Math is Hard! The evect of gender priming on women s attitudes Jennifer R. Steele a,, Nalini Ambady b a Department

More information

The Dual Impact of Stereotype Threat and Solo Status on the Intellectual Performance of African- Americans

The Dual Impact of Stereotype Threat and Solo Status on the Intellectual Performance of African- Americans Southern Methodist University SMU Scholar Collection of Engaged Learning Engaged Learning 4-15-2014 The Dual Impact of Stereotype Threat and Solo Status on the Intellectual Performance of African- Americans

More information

Gender Differences in Experiences with Sexual Objectification

Gender Differences in Experiences with Sexual Objectification Gender Differences in Experiences with Sexual Objectification Morgan Harris: McNair Scholar Dr. Eric Landrum and Dr. Mary Pritchard: Mentors Psychology Abstract Sexual objectification is a prevalent societal

More information

Running head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AS REGULATORY MISMATCH. Lisa R. Grimm. Arthur B. Markman. W. Todd Maddox. Grant C. Baldwin

Running head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AS REGULATORY MISMATCH. Lisa R. Grimm. Arthur B. Markman. W. Todd Maddox. Grant C. Baldwin Running head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AS REGULATORY MISMATCH Stereotype Threat Reinterpreted as a Regulatory Mismatch Lisa R. Grimm Arthur B. Markman W. Todd Maddox Grant C. Baldwin University of Texas at Austin

More information

Black 1 White 5 Black

Black 1 White 5 Black PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Black 1 White 5 Black Hypodescent in Reflexive Categorization of Racially Ambiguous Faces Destiny Peery and Galen V. Bodenhausen Northwestern University ABSTRACT Historically,

More information

Explain the formation of stereotypes and their effect on behaviour. Done by: Lynn and Daeun

Explain the formation of stereotypes and their effect on behaviour. Done by: Lynn and Daeun Explain the formation of stereotypes and their effect on behaviour Done by: Lynn and Daeun Social Categorization -Society is very complex and thus it presents us with too much information -Since our capacity

More information

Using Multiple Methods to Distinguish Active Delay and Procrastination in College Students

Using Multiple Methods to Distinguish Active Delay and Procrastination in College Students Using Multiple Methods to Distinguish Active Delay and Procrastination in College Students Suzanne F Lindt Midwestern State University Danya M Corkin Houston ISD Shirley L Yu The Ohio State University

More information

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES Correlational Research Correlational Designs Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are

More information

See Your Way to Success: Imagery Perspective Influences Performance under Stereotype Threat DISSERTATION

See Your Way to Success: Imagery Perspective Influences Performance under Stereotype Threat DISSERTATION See Your Way to Success: Imagery Perspective Influences Performance under Stereotype Threat DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the

More information

Exploring the Impact of Financial Incentives on Stereotype Threat: Evidence from a Pilot Study

Exploring the Impact of Financial Incentives on Stereotype Threat: Evidence from a Pilot Study American Economic Review: Papers & Proceedings 2008, 98:2, 370 375 http://www.aeaweb.org/articles.php?doi=10.1257/aer.98.2.370 Exploring the Impact of Financial Incentives on Stereotype Threat: Evidence

More information

Male and Female College Students Educational Majors: The Contribution of Basic Vocational Confidence and Interests

Male and Female College Students Educational Majors: The Contribution of Basic Vocational Confidence and Interests Psychology Publications Psychology 2010 Male and Female College Students Educational Majors: The Contribution of Basic Vocational Confidence and Interests Lisa M. Larson Iowa State University, lmlarson@iastate.edu

More information

A Mediated-Moderation Model of Stereotype Threat and Performance Pressure Effects. Jimmy Louis Johnson

A Mediated-Moderation Model of Stereotype Threat and Performance Pressure Effects. Jimmy Louis Johnson A Mediated-Moderation Model of Stereotype Threat and Performance Pressure Effects by Jimmy Louis Johnson A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Auburn University in partial fulfillment of

More information

The Role of Prevention Focus Under Stereotype Threat: Initial Cognitive Mobilization Is Followed by Depletion

The Role of Prevention Focus Under Stereotype Threat: Initial Cognitive Mobilization Is Followed by Depletion Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2012 American Psychological Association 2012, Vol. 102, No. 6, 1239 1251 0022-3514/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027678 The Role of Prevention Focus Under Stereotype

More information

Sexism 11/15/2011. Sex and Gender Biological sex Anatomical sex Genetic sex. Gender Gender roles & expectations. Gender Identity.

Sexism 11/15/2011. Sex and Gender Biological sex Anatomical sex Genetic sex. Gender Gender roles & expectations. Gender Identity. Sexism A Tutorial American River College Professor Lori Hokerson Video Link Sex and Gender Biological sex Anatomical sex Genetic sex Gender Gender roles & expectations Gender Identity Gender Expression

More information

Stereotypes and the Achievement Gap: Stereotype Threat Prior to Test Taking

Stereotypes and the Achievement Gap: Stereotype Threat Prior to Test Taking Educ Psychol Rev (2012) 24:609 635 DOI 10.1007/s10648-012-9200-4 REVIEW ARTICLE Stereotypes and the Achievement Gap: Stereotype Threat Prior to Test Taking Markus Appel & Nicole Kronberger Published online:

More information

ELEMENTARY TEACHERS SCIENCE SELF EFFICACY BELIEFS IN THE EAST AZERBAIJAN PROVINCE OF IRAN

ELEMENTARY TEACHERS SCIENCE SELF EFFICACY BELIEFS IN THE EAST AZERBAIJAN PROVINCE OF IRAN ELEMENTARY TEACHERS SCIENCE SELF EFFICACY BELIEFS IN THE EAST AZERBAIJAN PROVINCE OF IRAN Eskandar Fathi-Azar College of Education and Psychology, University of Tabriz, Iran This study measures the self-efficacy

More information

The Woman Scientist: Communal Goals as Predictors for Women s Interest in STEM

The Woman Scientist: Communal Goals as Predictors for Women s Interest in STEM University of Colorado, Boulder CU Scholar Undergraduate Honors Theses Honors Program Spring 2014 The Woman Scientist: Communal Goals as Predictors for Women s Interest in STEM Christopher Cavazos University

More information