Reward-Priming of Location in Visual Search

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Reward-Priming of Location in Visual Search"

Transcription

1 Reward-Priming of Location in Visual Search Clayton Hickey 1,2 *, Leonardo Chelazzi 3,4, Jan Theeuwes 1 1 Department of Cognitive Psychology, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2 Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, Trento, Italy, 3 Department of Neurological and Visual Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy, 4 Italian Institute of Neuroscience, Verona, Italy Abstract Existing visual search research has demonstrated that the receipt of reward will be beneficial for subsequent perceptual and attentional processing of features that have characterized targets, but detrimental for processing of features that have characterized irrelevant distractors. Here we report a similar effect of reward on location. Observers completed a visual search task in which they selected a target, ignored a salient distractor, and received random-magnitude reward for correct performance. Results show that when target selection garnered rewarding outcome attention is subsequently a.) primed to return to the target location, and b.) biased away from the location that was occupied by the salient, task-irrelevant distractor. These results suggest that in addition to priming features, reward acts to guide visual search by priming contextual locations of visual stimuli. Citation: Hickey C, Chelazzi L, Theeuwes J (2014) Reward-Priming of Location in Visual Search. PLoS ONE 9(7): e doi: /journal.pone Editor: Joy J. Geng, University of California, Davis, United States of America Received February 17, 2014; Accepted July 1, 2014; Published July 31, 2014 Copyright: ß 2014 Hickey et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Funding: This work was made possible by a Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) VENI grant ( ) to CH and a European Research Council (ERC) advanced grant (2012-AdG ) to JT. LC is supported by Fondazione Cariverona. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. * clayton.hickey@unitn.it Introduction Attention has commonly been characterized as under the control of a.) endogenous, top-down factors, reflecting goal-driven strategy, and b.) exogenous, bottom-up factors, determined by stimulus characteristics and hard-wired sensitivities in early visual cortex [1 2]. However, this framework fails to account for a class of findings in the literature that index an endogenous state of the system, but are not strategic in nature [3]. Notable in this regard are results demonstrating the influence of reward history on selective control [4]. Reward appears able to prime vision so that objects with reward-associated features become salient and attention-drawing and this can occur in spite of an observer s efforts otherwise. For example, we have shown that when a distractor is defined by a color that has recently characterized a rewarded target, it will disrupt target selection even when participants know that the distractor will appear and do their best to ignore it [5]. Anderson, Laurent, and Yantis [6] have similarly found that entrained association of reward to a color will cause distractors characterized by this hue to disrupt search for a unique shape, even when participants are well aware that stimuli color is no longer task relevant, and Kristjánsson, Sigurjónsdóttir and Driver [7] have shown that reward facilitates selection of a target defined by a repeated feature, even when participants are aware that the stimulus is very unlikely to prove rewarding again. Task-irrelevant objects with reward-associated characteristics appear initially well represented in the visual system [5,8 9] before being attentionally suppressed [8,10], possibly so that the target representation is sheltered from interference [11,12]. Reward thus creates biases in perceptual and attentional processing that are not indicative of the current goal state of the observer. To date, investigations of this non-strategic influence of reward have focused almost exclusively on representations of lowlevel visual features and feature-based selection. Results show that objects with reward-associated features or characteristics are preferentially selected regardless of their location [5,6,8,13 26]. However, visual search clearly takes place within a spatial coordinate system, and the prior experience of targets and distractors is known to have an impact on how attention is deployed to locations in the future. Here we test the idea that reward might impact the deployment of attention to locations in visual search. The study of location priming in search has a rich history. Seminal work from Rabbitt, Cumming and Vyas [27] demonstrated that correct detection of a set of targets in an array of letters was facilitated when identical target letters were presented at the same position in sequential trials. Treisman [28] extended this finding into the study of feature search, showing that participant response to a target defined by a unique visual feature was faster when target-defining feature and location were both repeated. This suggests that location priming might be contingent on repetition of target-defining features, however Maljkovic and Nakayama [29] later observed that location priming and feature priming could be independently elicited. These authors had participants search for a uniquely coloured shape and discriminate the presence or absence of a notch in one corner of this object, with results showing a benefit for targets that reappeared at the same location and a cost for targets that appeared at a location that had previously held a distractor, regardless of whether the target-defining color was repeated. A critical difference between this study and earlier work is that Maljkovic and Nakayama [29] employed a compound search paradigm, in which the response feature is independent of the target-defining feature. This allows one to isolate effects caused by repetition of location from effects caused by repetition of response. Subsequent work using the same paradigm [30] or other types of compound search task [31] have largely reproduced Maljkovic and Nakayama s [29] findings. PLOS ONE 1 July 2014 Volume 9 Issue 7 e103372

2 Other studies have demonstrated that it is the relative position of a target and distractors that is critical regardless of a change in absolute retinal position [32], suggesting a link between location priming and contextual cueing [33]. In spite of this long interest in location priming in the vision research community, and in spite of the plethora of recent studies investigating the impact of reward on visual features, to our knowledge only 2 existing papers have discussed the impact of reward on location during search. As noted above, Anderson and colleagues [6] used a training task to associate reward to a discrete color, showing that search was disrupted by the presence of distractors characterized by this hue during a subsequent compound search task. Performance in this study was particularly degraded when the target appeared at a location that had held the distractor with reward-associated color in the immediately preceding trial. This suggests that the distractor with rewardassociated color drew attention before being strongly suppressed, and that this suppression had a residual impact on the subsequent deployment of attention to the distractor location even when it no longer contained a distractor. While clearly an example of an impact of reward on location, this effect is indirect: it relies on the association of reward to a color. Camara, Manohar and Husain [34] have recently investigated the possibility that reward may have a more direct influence on location. In the dual-task paradigm adopted in this eye-tracking study each trial began with participants moving their eyes to one of two locations identified with circles of identical color. Selection of one of these locations resulted in reward, selection of the other garnered punishment, and participants had no way to determine outcome prior to making the eye movement (see Experiment 2). Following reward feedback participants were required to complete a second visual search task where they made an eye movement to a green target while ignoring a pink distractor. Results showed an increased likelihood that the eyes would be deployed to the pink distractor when it appeared at the location that had garnered reward in the immediately preceding task. Results from this graceful study are thus in line with the idea that reward can prime locations (independent of its impact on features), but aspects of the experimental design leave room for further investigation. Perhaps most importantly, in all experiments reported in this study reward outcome was contingent on the nature of overt participant behaviour. This opens the possibility that reward may have primed the saccadic behaviour rather than the covert deployment of attention or perceptual representation. Here we further investigate the effect of reward on location priming in search. Participants completed a compound visual search task described in earlier papers [5,18 19]. While maintaining eye fixation they were required to covertly select a target defined by unique shape and discriminate the orientation of a line segment contained within it. In many trials they had to ignore a distractor defined by unique color and after each correctly performed trial they received 1 or 10 points (see Figure 1). The number of points thus accumulated determined earnings at the conclusion of the experiment. We analyzed performance on a given trial as a function of a.) the magnitude of point reward received in the preceding trial, and b.) whether target and distractor locations were repeated. The design has two important characteristics. First, as a compound search task, it decouples the visual feature that defines a target from the visual feature that defines response. As noted above, this allows for repetition effects on perception and selection to be distinguished from repetition effects on response. Second, the magnitude of reward feedback received on any correctly completed trial was randomly determined. There was thus no motivation or opportunity for participants to establish a strategic attentional set for target characteristics like color, form, or location. We approached the data with the general idea that selective attention relies on both facilitatory mechanisms that act on targets (and their locations) and inhibitory mechanisms that act on distractors (and their locations) [35 36]. From this, we generated 4 central experimental hypotheses: reward should: a.) create a benefit when the target reappears at the same location, b.) create a cost when the target appears at the location that previously held the distractor, c.) create a benefit when the distractor reappears at the same location, and d.) create a cost when the distractor appears at the location that previously held the target. Method Ethics statement All procedures were approved by the VU University Amsterdam psychology department ethics review board and adhered to the principles detailed in the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants gave written informed consent before participation. Summary of approach To test the hypothesis outlined in the introduction we first reanalyzed existing results from 78 participants who took part in one of a set of three existing experiments (see details below). Each of these experiments was designed to examine the impact of reward on the priming of visual features, an issue that is separate from the possible impact of reward on the priming of locations that is the topic of the current study. The primary result from this reanalysis of existing data was a 3-way interaction in RT. We confirmed this 3-way interaction in a new sample of 17 participants before collapsing across all four experiments to create a 95-person sample. Follow-up statistics designed to identify the specific effects underlying the 3-way interaction were conducted on this large sample. This somewhat complicated approach was adopted for two reasons. First, it provided the opportunity to confirm the 3-way interaction identified in reanalysis of old data in a new sample. Second, by collapsing across these samples before conducting follow-up contrasts we were afforded maximal statistical power to detect the sometimes-subtle effects that underlie this core pattern. In the remainder of the Methods section we describe the general paradigm adopted in all four experiments before providing details specific to each of the individual experiments. General design Participants viewed visual search arrays consisting of a number of shape outlines presented in a circle formation (see Figure 1). The shapes were unfilled diamonds (4.2u64.2u visual angle) and circles (1.7u radius) outlined in red or green (0.3u line thickness). Each was presented equidistant from a central fixation point (9.1u) and each other and contained a grey line (0.3u61.5u) that was randomly oriented to be vertical or horizontal. In every trial one object was a circle with all other objects diamonds; this shape singleton was the target of search and participants were required to report the orientation of the line contained within this object. An additional color singleton was defined in many trials by giving one of the diamonds unique color. Target and salient distractor locations were randomized with the sole confine that they could not coincide at one location. Each trial began with the presentation of a fixation cross (400 to 1400 ms, rectangular distribution) which was followed by the search array. Correct responses to the search display were PLOS ONE 2 July 2014 Volume 9 Issue 7 e103372

3 Figure 1. Experimental paradigm. doi: /journal.pone g001 immediately followed by a central indication of the number of points acquired in the completed trial, either +1 or +1. The magnitude of reward following correct performance was randomly determined for each trial. Incorrect trials resulted in 10, indicating the loss of 10 points. Feedback was presented to participants for 1000 ms and the search display remained onscreen during the this interval. Participants were instructed to maximize earnings by responding accurately and were paid based on the number of points they accumulated throughout the experiment, but, because reward magnitude was randomly determined and accuracy was high for all participants, there was little variability in pay: no one earned less than 8.00 euro per hour or more than Participants were asked to maintain eye fixation throughout each experimental block. Trials in which response occurred sooner than 100 ms after stimulus onset or later than 2500 ms after were discarded from all analyses (0.8% +/21.6% of trials, mean +/2 SD) and incorrect trials were excluded from calculation of reaction time (RT). Stimuli were presented on a CRT monitor located,60 cm from the observer s eyes. Feedback regarding response PLOS ONE 3 July 2014 Volume 9 Issue 7 e103372

4 latency, average accuracy, and total number of points earned to that point was provided at the end of every block. For all analyses involving intertrial contingencies the immediately preceding trial had to have occurred in the same block, have been correctly completed, and have involved a search display containing a distractor singleton. Performance in this kind of additional singleton task is substantially more variable in trials where the distractor singleton is present in the display because there is variability in the strength with which this stimulus will capture attention [37]. With this in mind, primary analyses of target location are based on trials where the target was presented in the absence of a salient distractor, with analysis of distractor location necessarily based on trials where both the current and preceding search display contained a salient distractor. To foreshadow, results look much the same if this constraint is not adopted (see Results). In all analyses average per-subject RT reflects the median and average per-subject accuracy reflects the mean. Details specific to Experiment 1 Fourteen neurologically typical students of the VU Amsterdam completed this experiment and other analyses of the data formed the basis for a prior report [5]. Participants (21+/23 years, mean +/2 SD; all right handed; 6 women) completed the search task described above where the search array contained 10 shape outlines and the additional color singleton was defined in 75% of trials by giving one of the diamonds unique color, either saturated red while all other objects were saturated green or vice versa. Response was unimanual using the right index and middle fingers on a standard two-button mouse and participants completed 45 blocks of 30 trials. Eye movements were monitored via electrooculogram (EOG). All trials with eye movements identified in an interval beginning 500 ms before stimulus onset and ending 1 s. after were removed from analysis (8+/24% of trials, mean +/2 SD). Details specific to Experiment 2 Thirty-seven neurologically typical students of the VU Amsterdam completed this experiment and other analyses of the data formed the basis for a prior report [18]. Data from three participants was removed from analysis due to low accuracy (,2 SD from the mean). Participants (20+/22 years, mean +/2 SD; two left handed; 7 men) completed the search task described above where the search array contained 10 shape outlines and the additional color singleton was defined in 75% of trials by giving one of the diamonds unique color, either saturated red while all other objects were saturated green or vice versa. Response was bimanual, using the left and right index fingers to press the z and m keys on a standard keyboard, and participants completed 30 blocks of 30 trials. Details specific to Experiment 3 Thirty-two neurologically typical students of the VU Amsterdam completed this experiment and other analyses of the data formed the basis for a prior report [19]. Data from two participants was removed from analysis due to low accuracy (,2 SD from the mean). Participants (20+/22 years, mean +/2 SD; 4 left-handed; 11 men) completed a variation of the search task described above where the search array contained 6 shape outlines. For fifteen of these participants the target and homogenous distractors could be characterized by red or green color, with a salient distractor defined in 75% of trials by giving one of the distractors blue color. For the other fifteen this reversed: the target and homogenous distractors were always blue, but a salient distractor was defined in 75% of trials by giving one of the distractors red or green color. Response was bimanual, using the left and right index fingers to press the z and m keys on a standard keyboard, and participants completed 30 blocks of 30 trials. Details specific to Experiment 4 Seventeen neurologically typical students of the VU Amsterdam completed this experiment. In contrast to Experiments 1 through 3, no analysis of this data has been reported elsewhere. Participants (20+/22 years, mean +/2 SD; 4 left-handed; 2 women) completed the search task described above where the search array contained 10 shape outlines and the additional color singleton was defined in 75% of trials by giving one of the diamonds unique color, either saturated red while all other objects were saturated green or vice versa. Response was bimanual, using the left and right index fingers to press the z and m keys on a standard keyboard, and participants completed 15 blocks of 30 trials. Results Analysis began with consideration of the combined results from Experiments 1, 2 and 3. A RANOVA of RT in this 78-person sample had three factors: relevant object, reflecting whether behaviour was binned as a function of the current target location or the current distractor location, prior location, reflecting whether the relevant object appeared at the location previously held by a target or distractor, and prior reward, reflecting whether highmagnitude or low-magnitude reward was received in the preceding trial (note that trials where neither target nor salient distractor location was repeated were excluded from this analysis). For those subjects who completed the 1.5 hour version of the task the median number of correct trials in the smallest cell of this analysis was 16 trials (13 for 1 hour version). A main effect of relevant object (F(1,77) = 44.68, p,10 29, g p 2 = 0.367) in part reflects the presence of the salient distractor: when the target was the relevant item displays did not contain a salient distractor and response was accordingly faster. An interaction between relevant object and prior location (F(1,77) = 33.94, p,10 27, g p 2 = 0.306) reflects a speeding when the target reappeared at the target location and slowing when it appeared at the distractor location, but a slowing when the distractor appeared at the target location and speeding when it reappeared at the distractor location. Finally, a critical three-way interaction (F(1,94) = 8.00, p = 0.006, g p 2 = 0.094) indicates that this 2-way pattern varied as a function of reward magnitude in the preceding trial (prior reward6prior location: F(1,94) = 1.01, p = 0.319, g p 2 = 0.013; all other Fs,1). Equivalent analysis of accuracy garnered no significant results (reward: F(1,77) = 1.21, p = 0.274, g p 2 = 0.016; prior location: F(1,77) = 2.01, p = 0.161, g p 2 = 0.025). Independent analysis of RT from Experiment 4 garnered exactly the same pattern of statistical results. The median number of correct trials in the smallest cell of this analysis was 8. Analysis of this 17-person dataset revealed a main effect of relevant object (F(1,16) = 10.14, p = 0.006, g p 2 = 0.388), an interaction between relevant object and prior location (F(1,16) = 7.13, p = 0.017, g p 2 = 0.308), and a critical 3-way interaction (F(1,16) = 4.59, p = 0.048, g p 2 = 0.223) but no other effects (prior location: F(1,16) = 1.55, p = 0.231, g p 2 = 0.088; reward6prior location: F(1,16) = 2.65, p 0.122, g p 2 = 0.142; reward6relevant object: F(1,16) = 3.10, p = 0.097, g p 2 = 0.162; reward: F,1). Again, equivalent analysis of accuracy garnered no significant results PLOS ONE 4 July 2014 Volume 9 Issue 7 e103372

5 (reward: F(1,16) = 2.13, p = 0.164, g p 2 = 0.118; reward6prior location: F(1,16) = 2.14, p = 0.163, g p 2 = 0.118; all other Fs,1). Results from analysis of the combined data from Experiments 1 through 4 is illustrated in Figure 2a. Planned follow-up tests were conducted on this 95-person dataset. A 2-way RANOVA revealed a significant interaction between prior reward and prior location when analysis was limited to trials where the target or distractor reappeared at the prior distractor location (Figure 2a large trace; interaction: F(1,94) = 7.590, p = 0.007, g p 2 = 0.075; all other Fs, 1). A corresponding RANOVA limited to trials where the target or distractor reappeared at the prior target location (Figure 2a small trace) revealed an effect of relevant item (F(1,94) = 71.80, p, , g p 2 = 0.433) and an interaction between prior reward and prior location (F(1,94) = 4.74, p = 0.032, g p 2 = 0.048; prior reward: F(1,94) = 2.38, p = 0.126, g p 2 = 0.025). Finally, planned contrasts demonstrated that the effect of reward was reliable when the target reappeared at the target location (Figure 2a small solid trace; t(94) = 2.70, p = 0.008, Cohen s d = 0.277), when the target reappeared at the distractor location (Figure 2a large solid trace; t(94) = 2.02, p = 0.047, Cohen s d = 0.207), when the distractor reappeared at the distractor location (Figure 2a large broken trace; t(94) = 2.39, p = 0.019, Cohen s d = 0.245), but not when the distractor reappeared at the target location (Figure 2a small broken trace; t(94) = 0.70, p = 0.485, Cohen s d = 0.072), or when neither target or distractor location was repeated (Figure 2a very small broken trace; t(94) = 0.27, p = 0.794, Cohen s d = 0.027)., footnote 1.. Consistent with prior findings, the presence of the salient distractor slowed response and decreased accuracy [38,39] (RT absent: 663 ms, present: 680 ms; t(94) = 8.83, p,10 27, Cohen s d = 0.675; Accuracy: absent: 95.8%, present: 95.4; t(94) = 2.33, p = 0.022, Cohen s d = 0.239). The magnitude of reward received in the preceding trial had no raw impact on behaviour (RT highmagnitude reward: 670 ms, low-magnitude reward: 671 ms; t(94) = 0.57, p = 0.573, Cohen s d = 0.059; Accuracy high-magnitude reward: 95.2%, low-magnitude reward: 95.0%; t(94) = 0.85, p = 0.398, Cohen s d = 0.087). The 95-person sample includes participants who completed 450, 900, or 1350 trials. During the editorial process a reviewer suggested equating within-subject performance variability across the sample by limiting analysis to only the first 450 trials completed by each participant. This had no impact on the data pattern: an omnibus RANOVA with factors for relevant object, prior location, and prior reward revealed the same three-way interaction (F(1,94) = 8.20, p = 0.005), the same interaction of prior location and relevant object (F(1,64) = 25.28, p,10 29 ), and the same main effect of relevant object (F(1,64) = 18.46, p,10 25 ), but no additional effects (prior reward6prior location: F(1,94) = 2.90, p = 0.092; all other Fs,1). As noted in the Methods, the analyses detailed above are based on results where target repetition of location was measured in trials where the distractor was absent from the display. The same general pattern of results was observed when this constraint was removed, such that analysis of target repetition was based on all trials. As above, a RANOVA of RT from the 95-person dataset revealed a reliable main effect of relevant object (F(1,94) = 47.74, p,10 210, g p 2 = 0.337), an interaction between relevant object and prior location (F(1,94) = 46.73, p,10 210, g p 2 = 0.332), and a critical three-way interaction (F(1,94) = 5.58, p = 0.020, g p 2 = 0.056; reward: F(1,16) = 2.31, p = 0.132, g p 2 = 0.024; all other Fs,1). We conducted an additional analysis to determine the spatial specificity of the effect of reward on location. To this end we examined behaviour when target or distractor reappeared not at the specific locations previously occupied by target or distractor (as detailed above), but rather at the positions immediately adjacent to these locations. If reward has a distributed spatial impact then analysis of hemifield should garner results similar to those detailed above. In contrast, if reward s effect is spatially constrained, the effect should be larger when analysis is based on specific locations. As is evident in Figure 2b, the pattern illustrated in Figure 2a does not reappear when adjacent locations are considered. A RANOVA analysis of these results with factors for prior reward, prior location, and relevant object revealed a significant interaction between prior location and relevant object (F(1,94) = 12.90, p,0.001; g p 2 = 0.121), apparently driven by a slowing of response when the distractor reappeared close to the prior target location, and a marginal main effect of relevant object (F(1,94) = 3.90, p = 0.051, g p 2 = 0.040; all other Fs,1). Reward had no reliable impact on these results. We conducted a 4-factor RANOVA in order to contrast results from the two patterns illustrated in Figures 2a and 2b. This had factors for analysis type (same location vs. adjacent location), relevant object, prior location, and prior reward, and revealed a significant four-way interaction (F(1,94) = 7.61, p = 0.007, g p 2 = 0.075). The significant three-way interaction observed when target and distractor reappeared at specific locations was thus reliably different than the far-from-significant pattern observed when they reappeared at adjacent locations. Reward s impact on locations appears to be strongly circumscribed in space. Finally, we conducted an exploratory analysis to gain insight into the relationship between reward-priming of location and reward-priming of color. In earlier work with this task we have shown that rewarded target selection will prime subsequent selection of stimuli characterized by the target color. As a result, response is fast and accurate when the target and distractor colors are repeated following high-magnitude reward, but slow and inaccurate when the colors characterizing the target and distractor swap [5,18 19]. The results detailed above additionally demonstrate that high-magnitude reward will prime the spatial location of a target and facilitate suppression of the distractor location. Given that we did not control for this reward-priming of location in our earlier work there is the possibility that reward-priming of color and reward-priming of location interact, with the extreme case being a situation where one of these effects is contingent on the other (as has been suggested of location-priming and featurepriming more generally) [28]. With this in mind we examined the current data as a function of reward history and target color repetition, limiting analysis to trials where the target and salient distractor were presented at locations that had held neither stimulus in the preceding trial. Results from 15 participants were not suited for this analysis because the variant of the experiment completed by these people involved a target that did not change in color (see specific details for Experiment 3 in the Methods section). We accordingly based this analysis on data from the 80 participants who completed a task where the target color was randomly red or green in each trial. For those subjects who completed the 1.5 hour version of the task the median number of correct trials in the smallest cell was 98 trials (64 for 1 hour version, 21 for 1/2 hour version). If reward-priming of color is contingent on reward-priming of location we should find no influence of reward in this analysis. As illustrated in Figure 3, results in fact show an interactive pattern familiar from our earlier work: high-magnitude reward created a performance benefit when the colors were repeated between trials but a cost when the colors swapped (Hickey et al. 2010a). This pattern was reliable in a RANOVA with factors for prior reward and color repetition (repeat colors vs. swap colors), as reflected in a PLOS ONE 5 July 2014 Volume 9 Issue 7 e103372

6 Figure 2. Results from a.) analysis of location repetition, and b.) analysis of reappearance at adjacent location. Error bars here and below reflect within-subject standard error [49]. doi: /journal.pone g002 significant interaction between factors (F(1,79) = 4.56, p = 0.036, g p 2 = 0.055; reward: F(1,79) = 1.14, p = 0.288, g p 2 = 0.014; all other Fs,1). Reward-priming of color thus does not appear contingent on reward-priming of location. An important caveat must be attached to this last analysis. The data from Experiments 1 through 3 has been used in earlier work to test hypotheses regarding the impact of reward on color priming [5,18 19]. In the primary analyses detailed above we approach this data with new hypotheses regarding the impact of reward on location. However, this last examination of the data - testing if reward-priming of color is contingent on reward-priming of location - was clearly motivated by earlier identification of the color effect in this data. This hypothesis is accordingly post hoc, and a core assumption to the use of inferential statistics is not met. Strong conclusions regarding the relationship between rewardpriming of color and location will require further dedicated investigation. Discussion The current results demonstrate that location priming in visual search is enhanced by rewarding outcome. We had participants complete a visual search task in which they selected a target, ignored a salient distractor, and received random-magnitude reward for correct performance. High-magnitude reward in one trial facilitated the return of attention to the target position and inhibited the deployment of attention to the location that had held the salient distractor. As a result, we observed a behavioural benefit following reward when the target or distractor location was repeated, but an exacerbated cost when the target appeared at the former distractor location. This pattern suggests that reward outcome guides the manner in which humans deploy attention through space. Importantly, the priming indexed in the current data does not appear strategic in nature. Target and distractor locations in the PLOS ONE 6 July 2014 Volume 9 Issue 7 e103372

7 Figure 3. Analysis of color repetition in trials where neither target nor distractor location was repeated. doi: /journal.pone g003 experimental design were random. This feature of the design would have become apparent to participants after a handful of experimental trials and meant that there was no motivation for them to establish a top-down, strategic attentional set for any PLOS ONE 7 July 2014 Volume 9 Issue 7 e103372

8 particular location in space. We believe that the results rather reflect low-level plasticity in visual representation. Recent models of visual learning suggest that such plasticity may occur when a.) attention is applied to a stimulus, and b.) there is concurrent release of a diffuse neuromodulatory signal in visual cortex signalling the receipt of unexpected reward [40 41]. When participants in the current study attended the target and were rewarded for doing so, the resulting reward-elicited neuromodulatory signal may have automatically reinforced the cognitive act of enhancing processing at the target location and inhibiting processing at the location of the salient distractor. A developing literature supports the notion that this kind of plasticity can occur in the absence of volition, strategy, or even awareness. For example, imaging results have shown that rewardassociated stimuli will evoke increased activity in visual cortex even when participants are unaware that a stimulus was presented [42]. Participants will learn about stimuli paired with reward when these stimuli are rendered nonconscious through continuous flash suppression [43] or gaze-contingent crowding [44], and rewardassociated stimuli will preferentially break through such procedures to reach awareness. Consistent with the idea that plasticity may in part rely on selective attention, recent results have demonstrated that factors impacting attentional selection - like perceptual grouping - also have clear effects on perceptual learning [45]. Our interpretation of the results is evocative of instrumental learning accounts of overt behaviour. Instrumental learning is traditionally characterized by an observable change in external action, as when an animal is gradually trained to press a lever by rewarding behaviour that brings it closer to this goal state. However, accumulating research suggests that the tenets of instrumental learning may also be important to our understanding of the activation of covert cognitive mechanisms [4]. By this, the action of such mechanisms is reinforced by good outcome, increasing the likelihood that they be deployed under similar circumstances in the future. In the context of the current data, we believe that rewarding outcome acted to prime both mechanisms that enhance the representation of stimuli at a specific location and those that suppress the representation of stimuli at nontarget locations [35 36]. This priming has a carryover impact on performance in the next trial such that spatial selection became biased toward stimuli at the former target location and away from stimuli at the former distractor location. In the current results both positive and negative priming effects were spatially specific, emerging only when the target and distractor stimuli appear at the discrete locations that had contained one of these stimuli in the preceding trial (see Figure 2). This is in contrast to a prior study of location priming in search from Kumada and Humphreys [31], where positive priming References 1. Posner MI (1980) Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32: Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neursoci 3: Awh E, Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J (2012) Top-Down versus bottom-up attentional control: A failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends Cogn Sci 16: Chelazzi L, Perlato A, Santandrea E, Della Libera C (2013) Rewards teach visual selective attention. Vision Res 85: Hickey C, Chelazzi L, Theeuwes J (2010) Reward changes salience in human vision via the anterior cingulate. J Neurosci 30: Anderson BA, Laurent PA, Yantis S (2011) Value-Driven attentional capture. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108: Kristjánsson A, Sigurjónsdóttir O, Driver J (2010) Fortune and reversals of fortune in visual search: Reward contingencies for pop-out targets affect search efficiency and target repetition effects. Percept Psychophys 72: Hickey C, van Zoest W (2012) Reward creates oculomotor salience. Curr Biol 22: R effects were found to have the same specificity observed in the current data, but negative priming effects were of much the same magnitude regardless of whether the target appeared at the specific location that formerly held the distractor or somewhere in the same visual hemifield. This incongruity between studies may stem from a small change in experimental design. In the paradigm used by Kumada and Humphreys [31] the target and salient distractor could be presented at only four possible locations, two on each side of the display, and when the distractor was present in the display it was always in the hemifield contralateral to the target. This was not the case in our design, where the target and salient distractor locations were unconstrained. This meant that the stimuli could appear in the same hemfield, and even in adjacent positions, likely creating the need for a more spatially-specific application of attention to resolve target information. If the attentional mechanisms responsible for target enhancement and distractor suppression acted with tighter focus it is reasonable that their residual effects are also more spatially constrained. Prior analysis of the current data has shown a.) that reward will speed target response when the colors characterizing the target and salient distractor are repeated between trials, but b.) that reward will slow response when these colors swap [5]. In the results section above we detail an exploratory analysis suggesting that this reward-priming of color is independent of the rewardpriming of location that is the primary topic of the current paper (see Figure 3). This suggests that reward-priming of location is not contingent on reward-priming of color (as has been suggested of location priming and feature priming more generally) [28,46]. However, our expectation is that these effects ultimately reflect action of attentional mechanisms that will commonly be activated under the same circumstances and that they should accordingly covary to a large degree. We have suggested elsewhere that reward-priming of color might reflect a low-level mechanism with evolutionary origins [5,9]. According to this idea, reward signals encoded in mesolimbic dopamine act to bias perception and attention towards objects that have acted as valid reward cues in the past [47 48]. The current results suggest that this general function is created through the action of at least two mechanisms, one working on the visual features that characterize relevant and irrelevant stimuli, the other acting on the contextual location of such stimuli. Because both objects and locations that have proven beneficial in the past are likely to prove beneficial in the future these reward-priming mechanisms could provide very real evolutionary utility. Author Contributions Conceived and designed the experiments: CH LC JT. Performed the experiments: CH. Analyzed the data: CH. Wrote the paper: CH. 9. Serences JT (2008) Value-Based modulations in human visual cortex. Neuron 60: Della Libera C, Chelazzi L (2009) Learning to attend and to ignore is a matter of gains and losses. Psychol Sci 20: Arsenault JT, Nelissen K, Jarraya B, Vanduffel W (2013) Dopaminergic reward signals selectively decrease fmri activity in primate visual cortex. Neuron 77: Buschschulte A, Boehler C, Strumpf H, Stoppel C, Heinze HJ, et al (2014) Reward- and attention-related biasing of sensory selection in visual cortex. J Cog Neurosci 26: Anderson BA, Laurent PA, Yantis S (2011) Learned value magnifies saliencebased attentional capture. Plos One 6: e Anderson BA, Laurent PA, Yantis S (2012) Generalization of value-based attentional priority. Vis Cogn 20: Della Libera C, Chelazzi L (2006) Visual selective attention and the effects of monetary rewards. Psychol Sci 17: PLOS ONE 8 July 2014 Volume 9 Issue 7 e103372

9 16. Della Libera C, Perlato A, Chelazzi L (2011) Dissociable effects of reward on attentional learning: From passive associations to active monitoring. Plos One 6: e Hickey C, van Zoest W (2013) Reward-Associated stimuli capture the eyes in spite of strategic attentional set. Vis Cogn 92: Hickey C, Chelazzi L, Theeuwes J (2010) Reward guides vision when it s your thing: Trait reward-seeking in reward-mediated visual priming. Plos One 5: e Hickey C, Chelazzi L, Theeuwes J (2011) Reward has a residual impact on target selection in isual search, but not on the suppression of distractors. Vis Cogn 19: Lee J, Shomstein S (2013) The differential effects of reward on space- and objectbased attentional allocation. J Neurosci 33: Qi S, Zeng Q, Ding C, Li H (2013) Neural correlates of reward-driven attentional capture in visual search. Brain Res1532: Raymond JE, O Brien JL (2009) Selective visual attention and motivation: The consequences of value learning in an attentional blink task. Psycholog Sci 20: Rothkirch M, Ostendorf F, Sax AL, Sterzer P (2013) The influence of motivational salience on saccade latencies. Exp Brain Res 224: Rutherford HJ, O Brien JL, Raymond JE (2010) Value associations of irrelevant stimuli modify rapid visual orienting. Psychonomic Bull Rev 17: Shomstein S, Johnson J (2013) Shaping attention with reward: Effects of reward on space- and object-based selection. Psycholog Sci 24: Wang L, Yu H, Zhou X (2013) Interaction between value and perceptual salience in value-driven attentional capture. J Vis 13: Rabbitt P, Cumming G, Vyas S (1979) Modulation of selective attention by sequential effects in visual search tasks. Q J Exp Psychol 31: Treisman A (1992) Perceiving and re-perceiving objects. Am Psychol 47: Maljkovic V, Nakayama K (1996) Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position. Percept Psychophys 58: Kristjánsson A, Vuilleumier P, Schwartz S, Macaluso E, Driver J (2007) Neural basis for priming of pop-out during visual search revealed with fmri. Cereb Cortex 17: Kumada T, Humphreys GW (2002) Cross-Dimensional interference and crosstrial inhibition. Percept Psychophys 64: Geyer T, Müller HJ, Krummenacher J (2007) Cross-Trial priming of element positions in visual pop-out search is dependent on stimulus arrangement. J Exp Psycolog Human Percept Perform 33: Chun MM, Jiang Y (1998) Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cogn Psychol 36: Camara E, Manohar S, Husain M (2013) Past rewards capture spatial attention and action choices. Exp Brain Res 230: Chelazzi L, Miller EK, Duncan J, Desimone R (1993) A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex. Nature 363: Hickey C, Di Lollo V, McDonald JJ (2009) Electrophysiological indices of target and distractor processing in visual search. J Cogn Neurosci 21: Hickey C, van Zoest W, Theeuwes J (2010) The time course of exogenous and endogenous control of covert attention. Exp Brain Res 201: Theeuwes J (1992) Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Percept Psychophys 51: Hickey C, McDonald JJ, Theeuwes J (2006) Electrophysiological evidence of the capture of visual attention. J Cogn Neurosci 18: Roelfsema PR, van Ooyen A (2005) Attention-Gated reinforcement learning of internal representations for classification. Neural Comput 17: Roelfsema PR, van Ooyen A, Watanabe T (2010) Perceptual learning rules based on reinforcers and attention. Trends Cogn Sci 14: Pessiglione M, Petrovic P, Daunizeau J, Palminteri S, Dolan RJ, et al. (2008) Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain. Neuron 59: Seitz AR, Kim D, Watanabe T (2009) Rewards evoke learning of unconsciously processed visual stimuli in adult humans. Neuron 61: Atas A, Faivre N, Timmermans B, Cleeremans A, Kouider S (2013) Nonconscious learning from crowded sequences. Psycholog Sci 25: Mastropasqua T, Turatto M (2013) Perceptual grouping enhances visual plasticity. Plos One 8: e Park J, Kanwisher N (1994) Negative priming for spatial locations: Identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. J Exp Psycolog Human Percept Perform 20: Berridge KC, Robinson TE (1998) What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res Rev 28: Ikemoto S, Panksepp J (1999) The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: A unifying interpretation with special reference to rewardseeking. Brain Res Rev, 31(1), Cousineau D (2005) Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson s method. Tut Quant Methods Psychol 1: PLOS ONE 9 July 2014 Volume 9 Issue 7 e103372

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Reward Guides Attention to Object Categories in Real-World Scenes Clayton Hickey, Daniel Kaiser, and Marius V. Peelen Online First Publication, January 5, 2015.

More information

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? University of Iowa Honors Theses University of Iowa Honors Program Spring 2017 Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? Ann Walsh University of Iowa Follow this and additional works at:

More information

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set In press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set Timothy J. Wright 1, Walter

More information

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search VISUAL COGNITION, 0000, 00 (0), 111 Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search Walter R. Boot Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee,

More information

Learned Reward Association Improves Visual Working Memory

Learned Reward Association Improves Visual Working Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 4, Vol. 4, No., 84 856 4 American Psychological Association 96-53/4/$. DOI:.37/a353 Learned Reward Association Improves Visual Working

More information

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 3, 580 584 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027885 OBSERVATION Templates

More information

On the feature specificity of value-driven attention

On the feature specificity of value-driven attention RESEARCH ARTICLE On the feature specificity of value-driven attention Brian A. Anderson* Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas, United States of America * brian.anderson@tamu.edu Abstract a1111111111

More information

Components of reward-driven attentional capture. Li Z. Sha Yuhong V. Jiang. Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota

Components of reward-driven attentional capture. Li Z. Sha Yuhong V. Jiang. Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota 1 Article in press, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. November 2015 Abstract Components of reward-driven attentional capture Li Z. Sha Yuhong V. Jiang Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota

More information

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (4), 697-702 Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search JAN THEEUWES Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, the Netherlands and ARTHUR F. KRAMER and

More information

Cognitive control and counterproductive oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli

Cognitive control and counterproductive oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli 1 Cognitive control and counterproductive oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli Daniel Pearson, Chris Donkin Sophia C. Tran, Steven B. Most & Mike E. Le Pelley AUTHOR S MANUSCRIPT COPY This is the

More information

Cross-Trial Priming of Element Positions in Visual Pop-Out Search Is Dependent on Stimulus Arrangement

Cross-Trial Priming of Element Positions in Visual Pop-Out Search Is Dependent on Stimulus Arrangement Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2007, Vol. 33, No. 4, 788 797 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.788

More information

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Clayton Hickey 1,2, Vincent Di Lollo 2, and John J. McDonald 2 Abstract & Attentional selection of a target presented among

More information

Feature Integration Theory Revisited: Dissociating Feature Detection and Attentional Guidance in Visual Search

Feature Integration Theory Revisited: Dissociating Feature Detection and Attentional Guidance in Visual Search Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 1, 119 132 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.119 Feature Integration

More information

Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy

Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy TCS-1101; No. of Pages 7 Review Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy Edward Awh 1, Artem V. Belopolsky 2 and Jan Theeuwes 2 1 Department of Psychology, University

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:296 301 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0353-4 BRIEF REPORT Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Risa Sawaki & Steven J. Luck Published online: 20 December 2012 #

More information

Learning to Attend and to Ignore Is a Matter of Gains and Losses Chiara Della Libera and Leonardo Chelazzi

Learning to Attend and to Ignore Is a Matter of Gains and Losses Chiara Della Libera and Leonardo Chelazzi PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Learning to Attend and to Ignore Is a Matter of Gains and Losses Chiara Della Libera and Leonardo Chelazzi Department of Neurological and Vision Sciences, Section

More information

Which way is which? Examining symbolic control of attention with compound arrow cues

Which way is which? Examining symbolic control of attention with compound arrow cues University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 2016 Which way is which? Examining symbolic control

More information

Bottom-Up Guidance in Visual Search for Conjunctions

Bottom-Up Guidance in Visual Search for Conjunctions Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2007, Vol. 33, No. 1, 48 56 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.1.48

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction

More information

Competition in visual working memory for control of search

Competition in visual working memory for control of search VISUAL COGNITION, 2004, 11 6), 689±703 Competition in visual working memory for control of search Paul E. Downing and Chris M. Dodds University of Wales, Bangor, UK Recent perspectives on selective attention

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action

Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action Report Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action Graphical Abstract Authors Jeff Moher, Brian A. Anderson, Joo-Hyun Song Correspondence jeff.moher@williams.edu In Brief Salient

More information

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task Olga Lositsky lositsky@princeton.edu Robert C. Wilson Department of Psychology University

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load Psychonomic Bulletin & Review In press Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load JOSHUA D. COSMAN AND SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Attentional capture by abrupt onsets can be

More information

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso Control of visuo-spatial attention Emiliano Macaluso CB demo Attention Limited processing resources Overwhelming sensory input cannot be fully processed => SELECTIVE PROCESSING Selection via spatial orienting

More information

Effects of Task Relevance and Stimulus-Driven Salience in Feature-Search Mode

Effects of Task Relevance and Stimulus-Driven Salience in Feature-Search Mode Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2004, Vol. 30, No. 6, 1019 1031 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.6.1019

More information

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1 The attentional window configures to object boundaries University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 2 ABSTRACT When

More information

Random reward priming is task-contingent: the robustness of the 1-trial reward priming effect

Random reward priming is task-contingent: the robustness of the 1-trial reward priming effect ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 10 April 2014 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00309 Random reward priming is task-contingent: the robustness of the 1-trial reward priming effect Árni G. Ásgeirsson 1 * and

More information

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting Ahnate Lim (ahnate@hawaii.edu) Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii at Manoa 2530 Dole Street, Honolulu, HI 96822 USA Scott Sinnett (ssinnett@hawaii.edu)

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory Alejandro Lleras, 1 Ronald A. Rensink, 2 and James T. Enns

More information

Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience

Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1465-8 Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience Daniel B. Vatterott 1 & Michael C. Mozer 2 & Shaun P. Vecera

More information

Attentional capture by a perceptually salient non-target facilitates target processing through inhibition and rapid rejection

Attentional capture by a perceptually salient non-target facilitates target processing through inhibition and rapid rejection Journal of Vision (2010) 10(6):5, 1 12 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/6/5 1 Attentional capture by a perceptually salient non-target facilitates target processing through inhibition and rapid

More information

The effect of reward on orienting and reorienting in exogenous cuing

The effect of reward on orienting and reorienting in exogenous cuing Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2014) 14:635 646 DOI 10.3758/s13415-014-0278-7 The effect of reward on orienting and reorienting in exogenous cuing Berno Bucker & Jan Theeuwes Published online: 27 March 2014

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

Cross-trial priming in visual search for singleton conjunction targets: Role of repeated target and distractor features

Cross-trial priming in visual search for singleton conjunction targets: Role of repeated target and distractor features Journal Perception & Psychophysics 2006,?? 68 (?), (5),???-??? 736-749 Cross-trial priming in visual search for singleton conjunction targets: Role of repeated target and distractor features THOMAS GEYER

More information

Emotion. Enhanced Attentional Capture in Trait Anxiety

Emotion. Enhanced Attentional Capture in Trait Anxiety Emotion Enhanced Attentional Capture in Trait Anxiety Jason S. Moser, Mark W. Becker, and Tim P. Moran Online First Publication, November 7, 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0026156 CITATION Moser, J. S., Becker, M.

More information

Jan Theeuwes. Acta Psychologica (in press)

Jan Theeuwes. Acta Psychologica (in press) Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection Jan Theeuwes Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam Acta Psychologica (in press) Address for correspondence: Jan Theeuwes Dept. of Cognitive Psychology Vrije Universiteit

More information

Memory-Based Attention Capture when Multiple Items Are Maintained in Visual Working. Memory. Andrew Hollingworth and Valerie M.

Memory-Based Attention Capture when Multiple Items Are Maintained in Visual Working. Memory. Andrew Hollingworth and Valerie M. 1 Running head: VWM guidance Memory-Based Attention Capture when Multiple Items Are Maintained in Visual Working Memory Andrew Hollingworth and Valerie M. Beck Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences,

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Direct Evidence for Active Suppression of Salient-but-Irrelevant Sensory Inputs

Direct Evidence for Active Suppression of Salient-but-Irrelevant Sensory Inputs 597913PSSXXX10.1177/0956797615597913Gaspelin et al.suppression of Capture research-article015 Research Article Direct Evidence for Active Suppression of Salient-but-Irrelevant Sensory Inputs Psychological

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect VISUAL COGNITION, 2007, 15 (2), 133148 Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect Jay Pratt University of Toronto,

More information

Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention

Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:1064 1070 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0172-z BRIEF REPORT Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention Sophie Forster & Nilli Lavie Published online: 12 October

More information

Priming in visual search: Separating the effects of target repetition, distractor repetition and role-reversal

Priming in visual search: Separating the effects of target repetition, distractor repetition and role-reversal Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Vision Research 48 (2008) 1217 1232 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Priming in visual search: Separating the effects of target repetition, distractor repetition

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection The New Yorker (2/7/5) Selective Attention Perception and awareness are necessarily selective (cell phone while driving): attention gates access to awareness Selective attention is deployed via two modes

More information

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Limits to Iconic Memory 0 Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Ronald A. Rensink Departments of Psychology and Computer Science University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4 Canada Running Head: Limits

More information

Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty

Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty Perception & Psychophysics 2008, 70 (8), 1435-1443 doi: 10.3758/PP.70.8.1435 Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty ZHE CHEN University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand

More information

Early, Involuntary Top-Down Guidance of Attention From Working Memory

Early, Involuntary Top-Down Guidance of Attention From Working Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2005, Vol. 31, No. 2, 248 261 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.248

More information

Neural Mechanisms of Incentive Salience in Naturalistic Human Vision

Neural Mechanisms of Incentive Salience in Naturalistic Human Vision Report Neural Mechanisms of Incentive Salience in Naturalistic Human Vision Highlights d Examples of reward-associated categories disrupt search for targets in natural scenes d d d Pattern analysis in

More information

Please note that this draft may not be identical with the published version.

Please note that this draft may not be identical with the published version. Please note that this draft may not be identical with the published version. Yamaguchi, M., Valji, A., & Wolohan, F. D. A. (in press). Top-down contributions to attention shifting and disengagement: A

More information

The role of priming. in conjunctive visual search

The role of priming. in conjunctive visual search The role of priming in conjunctive visual search Árni Kristjánsson DeLiang Wang 1 and Ken Nakayama 2 Word count: Main text: 3423 Total: 4368 Correspondence: Árni Kristjánsson Vision Sciences Laboratory

More information

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Masters of Science in Psychology By Quan Ying Gao University of Canterbury

More information

Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search

Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search Perception & Psychophysics 2005, 67 (7), 1252-1268 Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search JOY J. GENG Carnegie Mellon University and Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh,

More information

Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology

Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology 1 Running head: FIXED-FEATURE SEARCH Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology Ali Jannati, John M. Gaspar, and John J. McDonald Department

More information

Redundancy gains in pop-out visual search are determined by top-down task set: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

Redundancy gains in pop-out visual search are determined by top-down task set: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence Journal of Vision (2011) 11(14):10, 1 10 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/14/10 1 Redundancy gains in pop-out visual search are determined by top-down task set: Behavioral and electrophysiological

More information

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture Psychophysiology, 53 (2016), 650 662. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright VC 2016 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12615 The role of top-down spatial attention

More information

Object-based attention: Shifting or uncertainty?

Object-based attention: Shifting or uncertainty? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (7), 1743-1755 doi:10.3758/app.72.7.1743 Research Articles Object-based attention: Shifting or uncertainty? Leslie Drummond and Sarah Shomstein George Washington

More information

Stimulus-Driven Attentional Capture and Attentional Control Settings

Stimulus-Driven Attentional Capture and Attentional Control Settings Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1993, Vol. 19, No. 3, 676-681 Copyright 1993 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. (XW6-152VJ.VS3.00 OBSERVATIONS Stimulus-Driven

More information

Distinct, but top-down modulable color and positional priming mechanisms in visual pop-out search

Distinct, but top-down modulable color and positional priming mechanisms in visual pop-out search Psychological Research (2009) 73:167 176 DOI 10.1007/s00426-008-0207-x ORIGINAL ARTICLE Distinct, but top-down modulable color and positional priming mechanisms in visual pop-out search Thomas Geyer Hermann

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Visual Selective Attention and the Effects of Monetary Rewards Chiara Della Libera and Leonardo Chelazzi

Visual Selective Attention and the Effects of Monetary Rewards Chiara Della Libera and Leonardo Chelazzi PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Visual Selective Attention and the Effects of Monetary Rewards Chiara Della Libera and Leonardo Chelazzi Department of Neurological and Vision Sciences, Section of

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Fortune and reversals of fortune in visual search: Reward contingencies for pop-out targets affect search efficiency and target repetition effects

Fortune and reversals of fortune in visual search: Reward contingencies for pop-out targets affect search efficiency and target repetition effects Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (5), 1229-1236 doi:10.3758/app.72.5.1229 Brief Reports Fortune and reversals of fortune in visual search: Reward contingencies for pop-out targets affect

More information

Cognition 117 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage:

Cognition 117 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage: Cognition 117 (2010) 341 347 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article The influence of reward associations on conflict processing

More information

Short-term and long-term attentional biases to frequently encountered target features

Short-term and long-term attentional biases to frequently encountered target features Article in press, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. March 2017 1 Short-term and long-term attentional biases to frequently encountered target features Abstract Li Z. Sha 1 Roger W. Remington 2,3

More information

Neural Mechanisms of Incentive Salience in Naturalistic Human Vision

Neural Mechanisms of Incentive Salience in Naturalistic Human Vision Report Neural Mechanisms of Incentive Salience in Naturalistic Human Vision Highlights d d d d Examples of reward-associated categories disrupt search for targets in natural scenes Pattern analysis in

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

Mechanisms of Habitual Approach: Failure to Suppress Irrelevant Responses Evoked by Previously Reward-Associated Stimuli

Mechanisms of Habitual Approach: Failure to Suppress Irrelevant Responses Evoked by Previously Reward-Associated Stimuli Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 145, No. 6, 796 805 0096-3445/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000169 Mechanisms of Habitual Approach:

More information

Effect of colour pop-out on the recognition of letters in crowding conditions

Effect of colour pop-out on the recognition of letters in crowding conditions Psychological Research (07) 71: 641 645 DOI 10.7/s00426-006-0053-7 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Endel Põder Effect of colour pop-out on the recognition of letters in crowding conditions Received: 2 March 05 / Accepted:

More information

Reduced attentional capture in action video game players

Reduced attentional capture in action video game players Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (3), 667-671 doi:10.3758/app.72.3.667 Reduced attentional capture in action video game players Joseph D. Chisholm University of British Columbia, Vancouver,

More information

Motivation Versus Aversive Processing During Perception

Motivation Versus Aversive Processing During Perception Emotion Motivation Versus Aversive Processing During Perception Srikanth Padmala and Luiz Pessoa Online First Publication, April 7, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036112 CITATION Padmala, S., & Pessoa,

More information

Stressing the mind: The effect of cognitive load and articulatory suppression on attentional guidance from working memory

Stressing the mind: The effect of cognitive load and articulatory suppression on attentional guidance from working memory Perception & Psychophysics 2008, 70 (5), 924-934 doi: 10.3758/PP.70.5.924 Stressing the mind: The effect of cognitive load and articulatory suppression on attentional guidance from working memory DAVID

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Item and Category-Based Attentional Control During Search for Real-World Objects: Can You Find the Pants Among the Pans? Rebecca Nako,

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making Matthew A J Apps 1,2, Laura Grima 2, Sanjay Manohar 2, Masud Husain 1,2 1 Nuffield Department of Clinical Neuroscience,

More information

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Exp Brain Res (2002) 142:349 353 DOI 10.1007/s00221-001-0899-2 RESEARCH ARTICLE Richard A. Abrams Mark B. Law Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Received: 10 May 2000 / Accepted: 4 September

More information

Attentional Capture by Salient Color Singleton Distractors Is Modulated by Top-Down Dimensional Set

Attentional Capture by Salient Color Singleton Distractors Is Modulated by Top-Down Dimensional Set Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 1, 1 16 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.1 Attentional Capture

More information

Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture

Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (1), 65-70 Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture JAN THEEUWES Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Bacon and Egeth (1994) have

More information

Novelty biases attention and gaze in a surprise trial

Novelty biases attention and gaze in a surprise trial Atten Percept Psychophys (2016) 78:69 77 DOI 10.3758/s13414-015-0995-1 Novelty biases attention and gaze in a surprise trial Gernot Horstmann 1,2,3 & Arvid Herwig 1,3 Published online: 21 October 2015

More information

Taming the White Bear: Initial Costs and Eventual Benefits of Distractor Inhibition

Taming the White Bear: Initial Costs and Eventual Benefits of Distractor Inhibition 66564PSSXXX10.1177/095679761566564Cunningham, EgethCosts and Benefits of Distractor Inhibition research-article016 Psychological Science OnlineFirst, published on February 18, 016 as doi:10.1177/095679761566564

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture

Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture Atten Percept Psychophys (2012) 74:1183 1198 DOI 10.3758/s13414-012-0325-9 Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture

More information

Awareness in contextual cuing with extended and concurrent explicit tests

Awareness in contextual cuing with extended and concurrent explicit tests Memory & Cognition 28, 36 (2), 43-415 doi: 1.3758/MC.36.2.43 Awareness in contextual cuing with extended and concurrent explicit tests ANDREA C. SMYTH AND DAVID R. SHANKS University College London, London,

More information

Vision Research. Task-irrelevant stimulus salience affects visual search q. Dominique Lamy *, Loren Zoaris. abstract

Vision Research. Task-irrelevant stimulus salience affects visual search q. Dominique Lamy *, Loren Zoaris. abstract Vision Research 49 (2009) 1472 1480 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Task-irrelevant stimulus salience affects visual search q

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Context-Dependent Control Over Attentional Capture Joshua D. Cosman and Shaun P. Vecera Online First Publication, October 1, 2012. doi:

More information

Attentional Capture in Singleton-Detection and Feature-Search Modes

Attentional Capture in Singleton-Detection and Feature-Search Modes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2003, Vol. 29, No. 5, 1003 1020 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1003

More information

Selection history: How reward modulates selectivity of visual attention

Selection history: How reward modulates selectivity of visual attention Psychon Bull Rev (2018) 25:514 538 DOI 10.3758/s13423-017-1380-y THEORETICAL REVIEW Selection history: How reward modulates selectivity of visual attention Michel Failing 1 & Jan Theeuwes 1 Published online:

More information

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Selective Attention Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form,

More information

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (4), 951-962 doi:10.3758/app.72.4.951 Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

More information

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Eiichi Hoshino 1,2, Ken Mogi 2, 1 Tokyo Institute of Technology, Department of Computational Intelligence and Systems Science. 4259

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

The role of attention in processing configural and shape information in 3D novel objects

The role of attention in processing configural and shape information in 3D novel objects University of Wollongong Research Online Faculty of Health and Behavioural Sciences - Papers (Archive) Faculty of Science, Medicine and Health 2006 The role of attention in processing configural and shape

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1 Current Biology 24, 193 198, January 20, 2014 ª2014 The Authors http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.001 Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects Report Martin

More information

Context homogeneity facilitates both distractor inhibition and target enhancement

Context homogeneity facilitates both distractor inhibition and target enhancement Journal of Vision (2013) 13(3):11, 1 12 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/3/11 1 Context homogeneity facilitates both distractor inhibition and target enhancement Experimental and Biological Psychology,

More information