Indirect and subliminal mere exposure effect: implicit aspect of attitude formation. Patrick Chambres Delphine Bonin Karine Grenier

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Indirect and subliminal mere exposure effect: implicit aspect of attitude formation. Patrick Chambres Delphine Bonin Karine Grenier"

Transcription

1 Indirect and subliminal mere exposure effect: implicit aspect of attitude formation Patrick Chambres Delphine Bonin Karine Grenier Université Blaise Pascal Author s note Patrick Chambres, Delphine Bonin and Karine Grenier, Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale de la Cognition, CNRS UPRES-A 6024, Université Blaise Pascal, Clermont-Ferrand 2, FRANCE. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Patrick Chambres, Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale de la Cognition, CNRS UPRES-A 6024, 34 avenue Carnot, Clermont-Ferrand Cedex, France. Electronic mail may be sent to chambres@srvpsy.univ-bpclermont.fr Fax (33) Phone (33)

2 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.2 Abstract The experiments reported in this article fall within the scope of the general question of implicit attitude formation. Two studies investigate the extent to which mere exposure effects emerge in implicit stimulus exposure situations. In both experiments, half of the participants were directly and repeatedly exposed to 2 complex visual patterns, which were identified as standards, and the other half were indirectly exposed to these standards in a distorted form. The exposure duration was either supraliminal or subliminal. The results indicate that, in each implicit exposure situation, a mere exposure effect emerged. In the most implicit condition, where indirect exposure and subliminal presentation were combined, participants still generated positive attitudes toward the standards. In the explicit exposure situation (supraliminal and direct exposure), no significant mere exposure effect occurred. These results are discussed in the context of Bornstein and D Agostino s model (1994). Key reference terms Attitude Mere exposure effect Implicit processing

3 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 3 The 2 experiments presented represent an approach to 3 fundamental questions: Why do people like specific things? Why do they not like others? Why are they sometimes unable to explain the reason why they like or dislike something? The third question was examined here primarily through the use of the concept of attitude. An attitude is generally defined as a psychological tendency that is expressed by evaluating a particular entity with some degree of favor or disfavor (e.g. Allport, 1935; Eagly & Chaiken, 1993; Thurstone, 1931). Among very many studies conducted in the field of attitude, a number of experimental results have suggested that the attitude formation process could occur even when the attitude object has not been processed in an analytical way. For instance, within a classical conditioning procedure, an attitude toward an initially neutral stimulus becomes more positive after repeated pairings with other positive objects or events (e.g., Cacioppo, Marshall-Goodell, Tassinary & Petty, 1992; Krosnick, Betz, Jussim & Lynn, 1992; Staats & Staats, 1958; Zanna, Kiesler & Pilkonis, 1970). An attitude toward an object can also emerge after simple repeated unreinforced exposure to that object (Grush, 1976; Saegert, Swap & Zajonc, 1973; Stang, & O Connell 1974; Stang, 1975; Zajonc, 1968). This phenomenon is the well-known mere exposure effect. As far as the processes of attitude formation is concerned, one question has given rise to much controversy: Do such processes, which do not require a rational analysis of the object's characteristics, nevertheless involve a conscious processing of that object? Many studies have been performed to examine this fundamental question (e.g., Bornstein, 1989; Bornstein, Leone & Galley, 1987; Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980; Mandler, Nakamura & Van Zandt, 1987; Murphy, Monahan & Zajonc, 1995; Seamon, Marsh & Brody, 1984; Wilson, 1979). In most of the experiments, the level of consciousness of the stimulus was manipulated by comparing subliminal and supraliminal presentations.

4 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.4 In the literature on mere exposure, opinions are divided. Authors such as Harrison (1967, 1968) and Berlyne (1970, 1971) have proposed models of exposure effects in which they imply that the relationship between the stimulus exposure frequency and the resulting attitude is mediated by a prior cognitive process: that of stimulus recognition. According to them, awareness of the attitude object is a prerequisite for obtaining a positive attitude toward this object after its simple repeated presentation. Other findings have seriously challenged this dependence relationship between stimulus recognition and attitude elaboration. For example, Moreland and Zajonc (1977) have shown that stimulus recognition is a sufficient, but not a necessary, condition for observing an exposure effect. Similarly, Wilson (1979) found that repeated auditory stimuli were preferred even when their recognition was modified by means of a dichotic-listening procedure. Moreover, Bornstein (1989, 1992) has performed meta-analyses of mere exposure research and found that the magnitude of the exposure effect produced by subliminally-presented stimuli is far larger than the magnitude of the exposure effect produced by briefly-presented stimuli that were clearly recognized by participants. Such a finding suggests that, contrary to Harrison and Berlyne s viewpoint, the awareness of stimulus content could somehow inhibit the obtained effect (Bornstein & D Agostino, 1992). In their perceptual fluency/attributional model, Bornstein & D Agostino (1994) advanced that, when stimuli are presented in an explicit way (i.e. supraliminal presentations), participants are consciously aware of just having seen the stimuli that they have to rate. Thus, they can attribute their affective reaction toward the stimuli to these prior exposures. In such cases, participants are inclined to initiate a correction process in order to review their initial evaluation of the stimuli. Such a strategy leads individuals to change their spontaneous judgment by reducing the level of positive assessment of the stimulus. In contrast, in a subliminal exposure

5 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 5 study, participants do not consciously perceive the repeated stimulus presentations. Their reaction toward the stimulus cannot be attributed to the repeated exposure and no correction process is implemented. The key variable which is responsible for the enhanced mere exposure effect in studies using subliminal material is not the stimulus subliminality per se, but rather the subject s lack of awareness of the relationship between the repeated stimulus exposure and the subsequent attitudes (e.g., Bargh, 1992; Bowers, 1984). In connection with this vexed question of consciousness in the mere exposure effect, an experiment conducted by Gordon & Holyoak (1983) proved to be interesting insofar as some of the results confirmed specific propositions of the Bornstein and D Agostino model while others invalidated other aspects of this model. In their study, a new condition of implicit stimulus exposure was created. The attitude object was not presented directly. Participants were only exposed to various distortions of the standard object presented in a supraliminal manner. They were then asked to rate the standard object and certain of its distorted variants which they had not seen directly. The greater the similarity between the distorted object and the standard, the more favorable its affective rating was. This result is in accordance with the contention that the implicit nature of the stimulus repetition is due not only to the subliminality of the stimulus but also, and more importantly, to stimulus exposure conditions which are sufficiently subtle to make subjects unaware of the relationship between prior presentations and the evaluation of the stimulus. However, one of Gordon and Holyoak s findings sits uneasily with the Bornstein & D Agostino model. Indeed, these authors found that the evaluation of the standard object was the same for both direct and indirect exposure. According to Bornstein & D Agostino, participants who saw the standard object several times should have become aware of the relationship between prior presentations of the object and their positive spontaneous evaluation. Thus, they should have

6 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.6 implemented a correction process and rated the object less favorably than the participants who were not conscious of the repeated character of the stimulus presentation. In order to examine in greater detail the contradiction between Gordon and Holyoak s findings (1983) and the Bornstein & D Agostino model (1994), Gordon & Holyoak s experiment was replicated. To preserve the integrity of the previous procedure, the stimulus exposure frequency was also studied. The purpose of this first experiment was (1) to check the occurrence of exposure effects in a supraliminal but indirect exposure situation, (2) to shed light on whether or not the attitudes obtained in implicit exposure conditions are more positive than in totally explicit conditions. In a second experiment, designed to further investigate the limits of exposure effect occurrence in extremely implicit situations, the implicit character of the stimulus exposure was increased. Subliminal and indirect stimulus presentation were combined. The goal was to examine whether more polarized attitudes might occur in such a condition or, on the contrary, whether there is a boundary beyond which the polarization effect is stabilized. Experiment 1 As in Gordon & Holyoak s study, in the first phase of the experiment (exposure phase), the participants were asked to watch stimuli displayed on a computer screen. In the second phase (test phase), the participants had to give an affective rating of a new set of stimuli. Each participant was tested individually in a session lasting about 30 minutes.

7 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 7 Method Participants The participants consisted of 40 volunteer undergraduate students (25 females and 15 males) attending introductory psychology classes at the University Blaise Pascal in Clermont- Ferrand (France). Material and Procedure Material: 88 matrices consisting of one hundred squares (10x10) generated from 2 standard matrices (A and B) were used. Both standards consisted of fifty randomly chosen blue squares and another fifty orange squares. Eighty-eight distortions of the two standards were created by inverting the color of a number of randomly selected squares. In accordance with Gordon & Holyoak s procedure, 68% of the stimuli corresponded to distortions of less than 9% (3, 5 and 6%), 27% of the stimuli were distortions of between 9% and 15% (9, 10, 11, 12 and 14%), and 5% of the stimuli involved distortions of 29%. Exposure phase: Two methods of exposure were employed. In the abstraction condition, half of the participants were exposed to distortions of the 2 standard matrices. They never saw the standards on their own. In the repetition condition, the other half of the participants were exposed only to repeated presentations of standards A and B. In both exposure conditions, half of the participants were exposed 40 times to the standard A or its distortions, and 20 times to the standard B or its distortions. The proportions were reversed for the other participants. The participants were randomly assigned to one of the four experimental conditions generated by the factorial combination of the exposure condition (abstraction or repetition) and

8 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.8 the frequency of exposure (high or low). They were asked to simply look at the colored pictures appearing on the computer screen. Each stimulus was presented for 3 seconds and a blank mask appeared for 2 seconds between every two trials. Test phase: This phase was the same for the four experimental conditions. A set of 10 matrices was presented to the participants: standards A and B, plus 5, 10, 25 and 50% distortions of each. The matrices were presented for 5 seconds, in a random order. The participants then had to rate how much they liked each of these stimuli on a 7-point scale (0: I don t like it at all to 6: I like it very much ) by pressing one of the 7 keyboard keys that corresponded to the seven points of the scale. The next stimulus was presented 2 seconds after the subject s response. The affective ratings were recorded. Results and Discussion The data (see Figure 1) for each exposure condition were processed in separate analyses of variance with three within-participants factors (the 2 frequencies of presentation - low vs high, the 2 standards - A vs B, and the 5 distortion levels of test stimuli - 0, 5, 10, 25 vs 50%). In the abstraction condition, there was a significant main effect of the distortion level of the test matrices, F(4,18) = 4.52, p<.003. The more interesting result concerned the planned comparisons which revealed that the standards were preferred (m = 3.525) in comparison to all the other test matrices (m = 2.756), F(1, 18) = , p<.002. The participants also rated the low-distortion test matrices (5 and 10 %) significantly more positively (m = 3.00) than the highdistortion test stimuli (25 and 50 %; m = 2.51), F(1,18) = 5.166, p =.026. A linear-trend test showed a significant decrease of the affective ratings of the test matrices with the increase of the distortion level from the standard, t (20) = , p =.013. In line with Gordon & Holyoak s

9 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 9 results (1983), the most negative rating was attributed to the 25% distortion (m = 2.3) and this evaluation was significantly different from the rating of the standard (m = 3.525), F (1,18) = , p< The pattern of results in the repetition condition proved to be similar to that observed in the abstraction condition, but neither the main effect of distortion level, nor the linear-trend test reached significance, F(4, 18) = 0.36, ns, and t(20) = , ns, respectively. This lack of significance is partly due to the very high variance observed in the repetition condition, especially in the standards evaluations. A closer inspection of the responses of subjects in the repetition condition revealed an inverted response pattern for four subjects in comparison with the rest of subjects. For these four subjects, the liking ratings of the matrices increased with increases in the distortion level. Thus their mean rating of the two standards (m = 0,875) was smaller than their mean rating of the two 25% distortion matrices (m = 3,125). One account of the behavior of these four subjects is that, perhaps because they felt as if some of the test items were previously displayed, they spontaneously based their attitude toward the stimuli on a feeling of familiarity rather than on stimuli features (Whittlesea, 1993). This may have triggered a correction process leading them to change their judgment by reducing their evaluation of the stimulus. Similar correction strategies, which often lead to contrast effects, have been documented in the literature on social judgment. Specifically, when individuals are made aware of the impact of some primed material on their judgments, they tend to over-correct for such influences (Lombardi, Higgins & Bargh, 1987 ; Martin, 1986; Martin, Seta & Crelia, 1990 ; Ottati & Isbell, 1996; Schwarz & Bless, 1992; Strack, Schwarz, Bless, Kübler & Wänke, 1993). Thus, from the moment that individuals perceive that their judgments are influenced by factors other than the intrinsic features of the object, they try to eliminate this biasing effect.

10 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.10 When the four subjects who made contrasted evaluations were discarded from the analysis, a main effect of the distortion level emerged, F(4,15) = 5,371, p<.001, and, as predicted by the mere exposure phenomenon, the standard matrices (m = 3,47) were preferred to the other matrices (m = 2,6485), F(1,15) = 13,217, p< Insert Figure 1 about here The results accord only in part with Gordon and Holyoak s findings (1983). In the abstraction condition, participants elaborated a more positive attitude toward this central stimulus than toward the distortions themselves. However, in the repetition condition, even if the general pattern of results is in accordance with the mere exposure effect, no reliable preference for the standard was observed. Thus, as Bornstein & D Agostino (1994) suggested, it seems that some participants became aware of the reasons for their positive reaction to this stimulus. In order to avoid this biasing influence, they deliberately implemented a correction process by decreasing the favorability of their attitude toward the stimulus. Observing a global significant mere exposure effect in supraliminal exposition conditions depends on the number of participants who engaged in a correction process when they became aware of the items previously displayed Even though Gordon & Holyoak did not discuss this difference, their study and the present experiment showed that, in both exposure conditions, participants evaluated the 50% distortions more highly than the 25% matrices. Moreover, an additional study in which independent participants were asked to rate the test matrices without being exposed to prior stimulus presentations revealed 2 complementary results: -First, it appeared that the affective rating of the 25% matrices produced in the abstraction condition was significantly less positive (m=2.3) than the affective rating of these same

11 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 11 matrices in the non exposure condition (m=3.0), F(1,28) = 3.969, p<.05. This indicates that participants developed a negative value toward the high-distortion matrices. -Second, there was no difference between the evaluation of the 50% distortions in the abstraction condition (m=2.725) and in the non-exposure condition (m=2.3), F(1,28) = 1.463, ns. All these data support the idea that when people are repeatedly exposed to a specific object, they are led to evaluate this object positively (classical mere exposure effect). When they have to evaluate another object similar enough to be compared with the presented object, one of two possibilities may occur. First, when the level of similarity is high, people assimilate the object for evaluation to the previously presented object and again produce a positive evaluation, albeit to a lesser extent. Second, when the similarity is low, people contrast the two objects and are then led to evaluate the stimulus negatively. When the test stimulus becomes too different from the previously presented object, another judgment strategy is implemented and replaces the usual comparison process. In this case, people process the object as it had never been seen before, leading generally to an intermediate evaluation. It should be noted that in context of this experiment, a 50% distortion stimulus represented a matrix that was totally different from the standard. Thus, the results of this first experiment support Bornstein & D Agostino's point of view: when stimuli are presented in an explicit way, the mere exposure effect is inhibited. Moreover, they also support the idea that when an evaluated object is totally different from the previously presented standard, no comparison seems to occur and no negative exposure effect is observed. A second experiment was designed to help us gain a better understanding of these phenomena. The goal was first to examine the influence of a more implicit mere exposure on object evaluation and, second, to identify the distorsion at which the negative mere exposure effect disappears.

12 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.12 Experiment 2 Three main changes were made compared to Experiment 1: (i) in both exposure conditions, the stimuli were presented subliminally, (ii) in the abstraction condition, the implicit character of the exposure situation was accentuated by presenting the stimuli in an indirect manner, (iii) to examine in greater detail the shape of the linear trend observed in Experiment 1, the test matrices differed from each other but had the same distortion interval (0, 10, 20, 30, 40, and 50%). Moreover, a forced-choice test of awareness (Eriksen, 1980; Murphy, Monahan & Zajonc, 1995; Murphy & Zajonc, 1993) was carried out to check that the 80 ms presentation of the experimental matrices during the first phase of the experiment was indeed below the threshold of conscious awareness. Method Participants Forty undergraduate students (37 females and 3 males) attending Psychology courses at the Blaise Pascal University of Clermont-Ferrand (France) voluntarily participated in the experiment. Procedure

13 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 13 The matrices were presented subliminally for 80 ms. Each trial began with the presentation of a mask displayed for 1500 ms in the center of the screen. This mask consisted of a draughtboard composed of blue and orange squares. A matrix was then presented and remained displayed for 80 ms. Finally, the mask appeared again for 1000 ms. In order to make sure that the participants would continue to concentrate throughout the exposure phase, they were told that in each trial a word would appear very quickly in the center of the draughtboard, so quickly that it would not be properly legible. Next, another word would be displayed but would remain visible for longer and the participants' task was to categorize it as an old word (the word briefly seen before) or as a new word (a different word) by pressing the corresponding button on the keyboard. The participants heard these instructions and then performed 4 practice trials involving items which would not be used in the experimental phase. There then followed 60 experimental trials (40 presentations of a standard - or distorted variants of it - and 20 presentations of the other standard -or distorted variants of it). Forced-choice test of awareness: 36 independent participants were informed that they would be given a series of trials in which pictures would appear briefly. Each picture was preceded and followed by a draughtboard which acted as a mask. As in the experiment itself, each trial began with a 1500 ms presentation of the mask immediately followed by the exposure of the target stimulus. These target stimuli consisted of 20 distortion matrices of standards A and B. For half of the participants, the target stimulus exposure duration was 80 ms (subliminal condition) and for the other half, it was 3000 ms (supraliminal condition). After a 1000 ms exposure to the backward mask, the participants were presented with two test stimuli: a matrix which actually matched the target stimulus presented between previously presented masks on one side of the

14 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.14 screen and a 25% distortion of this matrix on the other side. The participants were then asked to identify the matrix which they had just seen. The rationale underlying the forced-choice test was that if the participants truly cannot detect the subliminally presented stimulus, then their performance in recognizing should not exceed random response levels. Results and Discussion Forced-choice test of awareness - analyses Participants in the subliminal condition (80 ms) scored an average of 9.06 correct out of a possible 20, which is not significantly different from a chance score of 10 (t(17) = 1.39, p>.10). Participants in the supraliminal condition (3000 ms) scored an average of 18.61, which is significantly better than the chance score (t(17) = , p<.0001). This result demonstrates that with a 80 ms stimulus exposure, participants were unable to distinguish between a matrix presented 1 sec earlier and another new matrix whereas they could make this distinction when the stimulus was presented for 3000 ms. Affective rating - analyses A 2 x 2 x 5 Anova was performed for each exposure condition. These analyses revealed no significant main effect of the distortion level of the test matrices (Abstraction: F (4,18) = 1.752, ns; Repetition: F (4,18) = 2.185, p=.06). Nevertheless, in both exposure conditions, planned comparisons indicated a significant preference for the standard matrices (Abstraction, m = 3.125; Repetition, m = 3.0) over all the other matrices (Abstraction, m = 2.645, F (1,18) = 3.747, p=.05; Repetition, m = 2.435, F(1,18) = 7.515, p=.0074). In particular, the standard

15 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 15 matrices were evaluated more positively than the 30% distortion matrices (Abstraction, m = 2.225, F(1,18) = 7.905, p =.006; Repetition, m = 2.3, F(1,18) = 6.921, p =.01) Insert Figure 2 about here As in many other studies (Bornstein, 1989, 1992; Bornstein, Leone & Galley, 1987; Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980; Mandler, Nakamura & van Zandt, 1987; Seamon, Marsh & Brody, 1984), the results obtained in the repetition condition provide an additional demonstration of the occurrence of the mere exposure effect in an implicit stimulus exposure situation. More interestingly, the data (see Figure 2) show that it was in the abstraction condition, i.e., through indirect and subliminal presentation of the attitude object, that the participants elaborated the most positive attitudes toward the standard objects. On the other hand, a negative linear trend between the distortion level of the test matrices and the evaluation of these matrices was observed up to the 30% distortion level. There was then an increase of the affective ratings for the 40 and 50% distortion. The linear trend was marginally significant in the abstraction condition, t (19) = -1.86, p =.07. It did not reach significance in the repetition condition, t(19) = -1,34, p>.05. The most negative evaluations were attributed to the 30% distortion levels. Beyond these levels, participants rated the stimuli more positively, which suggests that the repeated exposure to a standard stimulus did not influence the evaluation of excessively distorted stimuli. General Discussion and Conclusion

16 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.16 The pattern of results obtained in the present experiments sheds new light on the discussion of the role of stimulus consciousness in the mere exposure effect and more generally in attitude formation. One of these results allows us to resolve the question that confronted Gordon and Holyoak (1983) and Bornstein & D Agostino (1994) concerning the occurrence of the exposure effect in fully explicit stimulus exposure situations. Indeed, in the supraliminal repetition condition (experiment 1), no significant difference was found between the affective ratings of the standard objects and the other test objects. Such a finding clearly support Bornstein & D Agostino s conception according to which stimulus awareness somehow inhibits the exposure effect. On the other hand, and as reported in many earlier studies (e.g., Bornstein, 1989, 1992; Bornstein, Leone & Galley, 1987; Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980; Mandler, Nakamura & Van Zandt, 198; Seamon, Marsh & Brody, 1984), participants placed in the subliminal repetition condition (experiment 2) reported a significant preference for the objects to which they had been subliminally exposed. More interestingly, in the subliminal abstraction condition (experiment 2), in which the participants could not perceive the stimulus in a conscious way and, moreover, were never exposed directly to this stimulus, they generated a more positive attitude toward this stimulus than towards the other distortions. Taken together, these experiments indicate that the exposure effect is so robust a phenomenon that it may be elicited even in extremely implicit exposure situations and, more generally, our results seem to suggest that stimulus consciousness is not required for the elaboration of attitudes. Overall, this research indicates that attitudes can be generated without individuals performing a detailed analysis of the object and without their consciously processing this object. Highly implicit, repeated exposure to an object is sufficient to affect the subsequent attitude. It is

17 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 17 interesting to note that less positive affect ratings are consistently observed the more the object for evaluation differs from the previously and implicitly presented object. Nevertheless, this phenomenon vanishes when the object to be evaluated is no longer comparable to the repeated object. These findings support the idea that when people rate objects, they base their attitudinal response on different mechanisms. When the object is sufficiently similar to a specific object to which they have previously been exposed, a comparative judgment with the latter occurs. When there is not sufficient similarity between the two objects, people formulate more absolute judgments without implementing a comparison process. Clearly more data needs to be collected in order to confirm this phenomenon, and further studies should be carried out to identify the underlying processes. Researchers still have to work hard to provide significant answers to the 3 introductory questions: Why do people like specific things? Why do they not like others? Why are they sometimes unable to explain the reason why they like or dislike something?

18 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.18 References Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A handbook of social psychology (pp ). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. Bargh, J. A. (1992). Does subliminality matter to social Psychology? Awareness of the stimulus versus awareness of its influence. In R. F. Bornstein & T. S. Pittman (Eds.), Perception without awareness: Cognitive, clinical and social perspectives (pp ). NY: Guilford Press. Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity and hedonic value. Perception and Psychophysics, 8, Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and Affect: overview and meta-analysis of research, Psychological Bulletin, 106, Bornstein, R. F. (1992). Subliminal mere exposure effects. In R. F. Bornstein & T. S. Pittman (Eds.), Perception without awareness: Cognitive, clinical and social perspectives (pp ). NY: Guilford Press. Bornstein, R. F., Leone, D. R., & Galley, D. J. (1987). The generalizability of subliminal mere exposure effects: influence of stimuli perceived without awareness on social behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, Bornstein, R. F., & D Agostino, P. R. (1992). Stimulus recognition and the mere exposure effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, Bornstein, R. F., & D Agostino, P. R. (1994). The attribution and discounting of perceptual fluency: preliminary tests of a perceptual fluency/attributional model of the mere exposure effect. Social Cognition, 12, Bowers, K. S. (1984). On being uncounsciously informed and influenced. In K. S. Bowers & D. Meichenbaum (Eds.), The uncounscious reconsidered (pp ). NY: Wiley. Cacioppo, J. T., Marshall-Goodell, B. S., Tassinary, L. G., & Petty, R. E. (1992). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: Classical conditioning is more effective when prior knowledge about the attitude stimulus is low than high. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28,

19 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 19 Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes, Orlando, FL: Hartcourt Brace & Company. Eriksen, C. W. (1980). Discrimination and learning without awareness: a methodological survey and evaluation. Psychological review, 67, Gordon, P. C., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Implicit learning and generalization of the mere exposure effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, Grush, J. E. (1976). Attitude formation and mere exposure phenomena: A nonartifactual explanation of empirical findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, Harrison, A. A. (1967). Response competition and attitude change as a function of repeated stimulus exposures. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan). Harrison, A. A. (1968). Response competition, frequency, expectancy behavior and liking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, Krosnick, J. A., Betz, A. L., Jussim, L. J., & Lynn, A. R. (1992). Subliminal conditioning of attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, Lombardi, W. J., Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). The role of consciousness in priming effects on categorization : assimilation and contrast as a function of awarenessof the priming task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, Mandler, G., Nakamura, Y., & Van Zandt, B. J. (1987). Nonspecific effects of exposure to stimuli that cannot be recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, Martin, L. M. (1986). Set/reset: use and disuse of concepts in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, Martin, L. L., Seta, J. J., & Crelia, R. A. (1990). Assimilation and contrast as a function of people s willingness to expend effort in forming an impression.. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, Moreland, R. L., & Zajonc, R. B. (1977). Is stimulus recognition a necessary condition for the occurrence of exposure effects? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35,

20 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.20 Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, Cognition, and Awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, Murphy, S. T., Monahan, J. L., & Zajonc, R. B. (1995). Additivity of nonconscious affect: combined effects of priming and exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, Ottati, V. C., & Isbell, L. M. (1996). Effects of mood during exposure to target information on subsequently reported judgments: An on-line model of misattribution and correction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, Saegert, S.C., Swap, W.C., & Zajonc, R.B. (1973). Exposure, context and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25, Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1992). Constructing Reality and its alternatives: An inclusion/exclusion model of assimilation and contrast effects in social judgment. In L. L. Martin, & A. Tesser (Eds), The construction of social judgments (pp ). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Seamon, J. G., Marsh, R. L., & Brody, N. (1984). Critical importance of exposure duration for affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Menory, and Cognition, 10, Staats, C. K., & Staats, A. W. (1957). Meaning established by classical conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, Stang, D.J., & O Connell, E. J. (1974). The computer as experimenter in social psychology research. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 6, Stang, D. J. (1975). Effects of mere exposure on learning and affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, Strack, F., Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Kübler, A., & Wänke, M. (1993). Awareness of the influence as a determinant of assimilation versus contrast. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, Thurstone, L. L. (1931). The measurement of attitudes. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 26,

21 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 21 Wilson, W. R. (1979). Feeling more than we can know: Exposure effects without learning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph, 9, Zanna, M.P., Kiesler, C.A., & Pilkonis, P.A. (1970). Positive and negative attitudinal affect established by classical conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 14,

22 Implicit Aspect of Attitude p.22 3,8 3,6 3,4 Abstraction Répétition 3,2 3 2,8 2,6 2,4 2,2 2 0% 5% 10% 25% 50% Figure 1: Mean Affective Rating for test matrices as a function of the exposure condition and the distortion level in Study 1

23 Chambres, Bonin & Grenier p. 23 3,2 3 Abstraction Répétition 2,8 2,6 2,4 2,2 2 0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% Figure 2: Mean Affective Rating for test matrices as a function of the exposure condition and the distortion level in Study 2

Recognising what you like: Examining the relation between the mere-exposure effect and recognition

Recognising what you like: Examining the relation between the mere-exposure effect and recognition EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 1/16, PrEview article Recognising what you like: Examining the relation between the mere-exposure effect and recognition Ben R. Newell School of Psychology,

More information

doi: /

doi: / doi: 10.1080/15534510.2014.901245 MERE EXPOSURE AND SOCIAL INTERACTION 1 Running Head: MERE EXPOSURE AND SOCIAL INTERACTION How do implicit effects of subliminal mere exposure become explicit? Mediating

More information

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45 (2009) 1117 1122 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Social Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Reports

More information

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 916-920 Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion JOHN PREDEBON University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia Two experiments

More information

Effects of "Mere Exposure" on Learning and Affect

Effects of Mere Exposure on Learning and Affect Journal ot Personality and Social Psychology 1975, Vol. 31, No. 1, 7-12 Effects of "Mere Exposure" on Learning and Affect David J. Stang Queens College of the City University of New York The mediating

More information

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA This article was downloaded by: [Hicks, Joshua A.][Texas A&M University] On: 11 August 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 915031380] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered

More information

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution 1 Robert D. Mather University of Central Oklahoma Charles M. Mather University of Science

More information

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Sarah J. Miles (smiles25@uwo.ca) and John Paul Minda (jpminda@uwo.ca) Department of Psychology The University of Western Ontario London, ON N6A 5C2 Abstract

More information

Mere exposure effect: A consequence of direct and indirect fluency preference links q

Mere exposure effect: A consequence of direct and indirect fluency preference links q Consciousness and Cognition 15 (2006) 323 341 Consciousness and Cognition www.elsevier.com/locate/concog Mere exposure effect: A consequence of direct and indirect fluency preference links q Sylvie Willems

More information

Familiarity and Liking: Direct Tests of the Preference-Feedback Hypothesis 1. University of Leicester

Familiarity and Liking: Direct Tests of the Preference-Feedback Hypothesis 1. University of Leicester Familiarity and Liking: Direct tests 1 Colman, A. M., Best, W. M., & Austen, A. J. (1986). Familiarity and liking: Direct tests of the preferencefeedback hypothesis. Psychological Reports, 58, 931-938.

More information

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance 1 Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Erica Behrens Winona State University Abstract This study examines the effects of self-consciousness

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

The Instrumentality Heuristic

The Instrumentality Heuristic PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Instrumentality Heuristic Why Metacognitive Difficulty Is Desirable During Goal Pursuit Aparna A. Labroo and Sara Kim University of Chicago ABSTRACT The literature

More information

Comparing Direct (Explicit) and Indirect (Implicit) Measures to Study Unconscious Memory

Comparing Direct (Explicit) and Indirect (Implicit) Measures to Study Unconscious Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association~ Inc. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/91/$3.00 1991, Vol. 17, No. 2, 224-233 Comparing Direct (Explicit)

More information

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts Main Goals of this Paper: Professor JeeLoo Liu 1. To present an account of phenomenal

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

The Mere Exposure Effect and Classical Conditioning

The Mere Exposure Effect and Classical Conditioning University of Colorado, Boulder CU Scholar Undergraduate Honors Theses Honors Program Spring 2015 The Mere Exposure Effect and Classical Conditioning Rosalyn Wong University of Colorado Boulder, Rosalyn.Wong@Colorado.EDU

More information

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 KAZUO MORI HIDEKO MORI

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 KAZUO MORI HIDEKO MORI Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, 785-789. Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 EXAMINATION OF THE PASSIVE FACIAL FEEDBACK HYPOTHESIS USING AN IMPLICIT MEASURE: WITH A FURROWED BROW, NEUTRAL OBJECTS

More information

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH Labovitz School of Business & Economics, University of Minnesota Duluth, 11 E. Superior Street, Suite 210, Duluth, MN 55802 Safe and Easy Or Risky and Burdensome? Fluency

More information

The observation that the mere activation

The observation that the mere activation Preparing and Motivating Behavior Outside of Awareness Henk Aarts,* Ruud Custers, Hans Marien Force (N) 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 The observation that the mere activation of the idea of a behavioral act

More information

Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach

Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach PQJE178995 TECHSET COMPOSITION LTD, SALISBURY, U.K. 6/16/2006 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 0000, 00 (0), 1 17 Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection

More information

Archived at the Flinders Academic Commons:

Archived at the Flinders Academic Commons: Archived at the Flinders Academic Commons: http://dspace.flinders.edu.au/dspace/ This is the authors version of an article published in Psychologica Belgica. The original publication is available by subscription

More information

The categorization effect in hedonic contrast: Experts differ from novices

The categorization effect in hedonic contrast: Experts differ from novices Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (1), 179-183 The categorization effect in hedonic contrast: Experts differ from novices LAUREN M. ROTA AND DEBRA A. ZELLNER Montclair State University, Montclair,

More information

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials for Wisneski and Skitka Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction Table of Contents 2 Pilot Studies 2 High Awareness

More information

Black 1 White 5 Black

Black 1 White 5 Black PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Black 1 White 5 Black Hypodescent in Reflexive Categorization of Racially Ambiguous Faces Destiny Peery and Galen V. Bodenhausen Northwestern University ABSTRACT Historically,

More information

Subliminal Emotional Priming and Decision Making in a Simulated Hiring Situation

Subliminal Emotional Priming and Decision Making in a Simulated Hiring Situation I. Skandrani-Marzouki SwissJ. Psychol. 69 &(4) Y. 2010 Marzouki: Verlag Emotion Hans Huber, and Hogrefe DecisionAG, Making Bern Swiss Journal of Psychology, 69 (4), 2010, 213 219 Original Communication

More information

Linking Automatic Evaluation to Mood and Information Processing Style: Consequences for Experienced Affect, Impression Formation, and Stereotyping

Linking Automatic Evaluation to Mood and Information Processing Style: Consequences for Experienced Affect, Impression Formation, and Stereotyping Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 135, No. 1, 70 77 0096-3445/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.70 Linking Automatic Evaluation

More information

Advertisers frequently use celebrities as spokespeople

Advertisers frequently use celebrities as spokespeople Implicit Assimilation and Explicit Contrast: A Set/Reset Model of Response to Celebrity Voice-Overs MARK R. FOREHAND ANDREW PERKINS* An experiment reveals that the relationship between celebrity attitude

More information

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE Åke Hellström Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, S-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden Email: hellst@psychology.su.se Abstract Participants compared successive

More information

UNCORRECTED PROOF. The Malleable Meaning of Subjective Ease Pablo Briñol, 1 Richard E. Petty, 2 and Zakary L. Tormala 3.

UNCORRECTED PROOF. The Malleable Meaning of Subjective Ease Pablo Briñol, 1 Richard E. Petty, 2 and Zakary L. Tormala 3. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Malleable Meaning of Subjective Ease Pablo Briñol, 1 Richard E. Petty, 2 and Zakary L. Tormala 3 1 Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain; 2 Ohio State

More information

Effects of Repeated Expressions on Attitude Extremity

Effects of Repeated Expressions on Attitude Extremity Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1992. Vol. 63. No. 1.17-29 Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/92/S3.00 Effects of Repeated Expressions on Attitude Extremity

More information

Distractor Devaluation in a Flanker Task: Object-Specific Effects Without Distractor Recognition Memory

Distractor Devaluation in a Flanker Task: Object-Specific Effects Without Distractor Recognition Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2014, Vol. 40, No. 2, 613 625 2013 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0034130 Distractor Devaluation

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

The Color of Similarity

The Color of Similarity The Color of Similarity Brooke O. Breaux (bfo1493@louisiana.edu) Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, Lafayette, LA 70504 USA Michele I. Feist (feist@louisiana.edu) Institute

More information

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Paul E. Hemeren (paul.hemeren@his.se) University of Skövde, School of Humanities and Informatics

More information

Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture Fragment Completion Paradigm

Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture Fragment Completion Paradigm Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia - Social and Behavioral Scien ce s 82 ( 2013 ) 482 486 World Conference on Psychology and Sociology 2012 Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture

More information

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Ethan D. Montag Rochester Institute of Technology Munsell Color Science Laboratory Chester F. Carlson Center for Imaging Science

More information

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints Jean-Pierre Thibaut (jean-pierre.thibaut@univ-poitiers.fr) University of Poitiers, CeRCA, CNRS UMR 634, 99 avenue du recteur Pineau

More information

Mental Rehearsal Increases Liking for Repeatedly Exposed Stimuli. Teri A. Kirby

Mental Rehearsal Increases Liking for Repeatedly Exposed Stimuli. Teri A. Kirby Mental Rehearsal Increases Liking for Repeatedly Exposed Stimuli Teri A. Kirby A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science University of Washington

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

Self-generated visual imagery alters the mere exposure effect

Self-generated visual imagery alters the mere exposure effect Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006, 13 (6), 1056-1060 Self-generated visual imagery alters the mere exposure effect CATHERINE CRAVER-LEMLEY Elizabethtown CoUege, Elizabethtown, Pennsylvania and ROBERT

More information

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Jennifer A. Kaminski (kaminski.16@osu.edu) Center for Cognitive Science, Ohio State University 10A

More information

LESSON OBJECTIVES LEVEL MEASURE

LESSON OBJECTIVES LEVEL MEASURE DEOMI SYLLABUS/ NOTETAKER 740 O'Malley Rd Revised: 20 November 2000 Patrick AFB, FL 32925 PERCEPTIONS LESSON OBJECTIVES LEVEL MEASURE A. Identify the perception process Knowledge Written B. Explain perceptual

More information

A functional explanation for the effects of visual exposure on preference

A functional explanation for the effects of visual exposure on preference Perception, 2008, volume 37, pages 1510 ^ 1519 doi:10.1068/p6012 A functional explanation for the effects of visual exposure on preference Mark A Changizi Sloan ^ Swartz Center for Theoretical Neurobiology,

More information

Conscious and unconscious thought preceding complex decisions: The influence of taking notes and intelligence.

Conscious and unconscious thought preceding complex decisions: The influence of taking notes and intelligence. Conscious and unconscious thought preceding complex decisions: The influence of taking notes and intelligence. Aline Sevenants (aline.sevenants@ppw.kuleuven.be) Dieter Daniëls (dieter.daniëls@student.kuleuven.be)

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

Examining the Effect of Subliminal Priming on Ambiguous Figure Perception

Examining the Effect of Subliminal Priming on Ambiguous Figure Perception Graduate Faculty Psychology Bulletin Volume 2, No. 2, 2004 Examining the Effect of Subliminal Priming on Ambiguous Figure Perception Misa Tsuruta, M.A. 1 Abstract ~ Figure-ground organization is a kind

More information

Image generation in a letter-classification task

Image generation in a letter-classification task Perception & Psychophysics 1976, Vol. 20 (3),215-219 Image generation in a letter-classification task THOMAS R. HERZOG Grand Valley State Colleges, Allandale, Michigan 49401 Subjects classified briefly

More information

Concurrent processes: The affect-cognition relationship within the context of the "mere exposure" phenomenon

Concurrent processes: The affect-cognition relationship within the context of the mere exposure phenomenon Perception & Psychophysics /993, 54 (lj, 33-42 Concurrent processes: The affect-cognition relationship within the context of the "mere exposure" phenomenon MATTHEW A. LEE, JANET L. SUNDBERG, and IRA H.

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1995 The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the

More information

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 BLL R. BROWN AND THOMAS J. REBBN2 UNlVERSTY OF LOUSVLLE This experiment required Ss to make Markov-generated histoforms that were

More information

Positivity Can Cue Familiarity

Positivity Can Cue Familiarity 10.1177/0146167203262856 PERSONALITY Garcia-Marques AND et al. SOCIAL / POSITIVITY PSYCHOLOGY AND FAMILIARITY BULLETIN Positivity Can Cue Familiarity Teresa Garcia-Marques Instituo Superior de Psicologia

More information

On the Status of Unconscious Memory: Merikle and Reingold (1991) Revisited

On the Status of Unconscious Memory: Merikle and Reingold (1991) Revisited Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 4, 925 934 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.4.925

More information

Mental Ownership: Does Mental Rehearsal Transform Novel Stimuli Into Mental Possessions? Teri A. Kirby. University of Exeter. Anthony G.

Mental Ownership: Does Mental Rehearsal Transform Novel Stimuli Into Mental Possessions? Teri A. Kirby. University of Exeter. Anthony G. Mental Ownership: Does Mental Rehearsal Transform Novel Stimuli Into Mental Possessions? Teri A. Kirby University of Exeter Anthony G. Greenwald University of Washington Author Note Teri A. Kirby, Department

More information

Completing a Race IAT increases implicit racial bias

Completing a Race IAT increases implicit racial bias Completing a Race IAT increases implicit racial bias Ian Hussey & Jan De Houwer Ghent University, Belgium The Implicit Association Test has been used in online studies to assess implicit racial attitudes

More information

What Drives Priming Effects in the Affect Misattribution Procedure?

What Drives Priming Effects in the Affect Misattribution Procedure? 50548PSP40110.1177/01461671350548Personality and Social Psychology BulletinGawronski and Ye research-article013 Article What Drives Priming Effects in the Affect Misattribution Procedure? Bertram Gawronski

More information

Effects of Self-Control Resources on the Interplay between Implicit and Explicit Attitude Processes in the Subliminal Mere Exposure Paradigm

Effects of Self-Control Resources on the Interplay between Implicit and Explicit Attitude Processes in the Subliminal Mere Exposure Paradigm International Journal of Psychological Studies; Vol. 6, No. 2; 2014 ISSN 1918-7211 E-ISSN 1918-722X Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education Effects of Self-Control Resources on the Interplay

More information

Product novelty impact on user Kansei

Product novelty impact on user Kansei KEER2014, LINKÖPING JUNE 11-13 2014 INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON KANSEI ENGINEERING AND EMOTION RESEARCH Product novelty impact on user Kansei Marcos Mendoza Vázquez 1, Toshimasa Yamanaka 2 2 1 University

More information

Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis'

Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis' Japanese Psychological Rescarch 1994, Vol.36, No. 1. 41-48 Special lssue: Decision Making Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis' KAZUHISA

More information

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Materials Supplementary Materials Supplementary Figure S1: Data of all 106 subjects in Experiment 1, with each rectangle corresponding to one subject. Data from each of the two identical sub-sessions are shown separately.

More information

An Implicit Assessment of the Effect of Artificial Cheek Raising: When Your Face Smiles, the World Looks Nicer

An Implicit Assessment of the Effect of Artificial Cheek Raising: When Your Face Smiles, the World Looks Nicer To appear in Perceptual and Motor Skills, Vol. 116 (2013) An Implicit Assessment of the Effect of Artificial Cheek Raising: When Your Face Smiles, the World Looks Nicer Hideko MORI Bunka Gakuen Nagano

More information

Cognitive dissonance in children: Justification of effort or contrast?

Cognitive dissonance in children: Justification of effort or contrast? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (3), 673-677 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.3.673 Cognitive dissonance in children: Justification of effort or contrast? JÉRÔME ALESSANDRI AND JEAN-CLAUDE DARCHEVILLE University

More information

Lodge, Jason Micheal (2011) Mere exposure to a distracting stimulus: like it or not? PhD thesis, James Cook University.

Lodge, Jason Micheal (2011) Mere exposure to a distracting stimulus: like it or not? PhD thesis, James Cook University. ResearchOnline@JCU This file is part of the following reference: Lodge, Jason Micheal (2011) Mere exposure to a distracting stimulus: like it or not? PhD thesis, James Cook University. Access to this file

More information

CHANGES IN VISUAL SPATIAL ORGANIZATION: RESPONSE FREQUENCY EQUALIZATION VERSUS ADAPTATION LEVEL

CHANGES IN VISUAL SPATIAL ORGANIZATION: RESPONSE FREQUENCY EQUALIZATION VERSUS ADAPTATION LEVEL Journal of Experimental Psychology 1973, Vol. 98, No. 2, 246-251 CHANGES IN VISUAL SPATIAL ORGANIZATION: RESPONSE FREQUENCY EQUALIZATION VERSUS ADAPTATION LEVEL WILLIAM STEINBERG AND ROBERT SEKULER 2 Northwestern

More information

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration Dr. Chan-Chien Chiu, Associate Professor of Taiwan Hospitality & Tourism College, Taiwan ABSTRACT This

More information

Masked prime stimuli can bias free choices between response alternatives

Masked prime stimuli can bias free choices between response alternatives Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 463-468 Masked prime stimuli can bias free choices between response alternatives FRIEDERIKE SCHLAGHECKEN University of Warwick, Coventry, England and MARTIN

More information

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH Marie Rivenez 1,2, Chris Darwin 1, Anne Guillaume 2 1 Department of Psychology University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK 2 Département Sciences Cognitives Institut

More information

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY http://www.uiowa.edu/~grpproc/crisp/crisp.html Volume 13, No. 16 Submitted: January 31, 2008 First Revision: March 18, 2008 Accepted: June 24, 2008 Published: July

More information

THE EFFECT OF A REMINDER STIMULUS ON THE DECISION STRATEGY ADOPTED IN THE TWO-ALTERNATIVE FORCED-CHOICE PROCEDURE.

THE EFFECT OF A REMINDER STIMULUS ON THE DECISION STRATEGY ADOPTED IN THE TWO-ALTERNATIVE FORCED-CHOICE PROCEDURE. THE EFFECT OF A REMINDER STIMULUS ON THE DECISION STRATEGY ADOPTED IN THE TWO-ALTERNATIVE FORCED-CHOICE PROCEDURE. Michael J. Hautus, Daniel Shepherd, Mei Peng, Rebecca Philips and Veema Lodhia Department

More information

Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation

Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation Hsiang Chen, Rolf T. Wigand and Michael Nilan School of Information Studies, Syracuse University Syracuse, NY 13244 Email: [ hchen04, rwigand, mnilan]@mailbox.syr.edu

More information

Running head: EFFECT OF HIGH ATTRACTIVENESS ON PERCEIVED INTELLIGENCE 1

Running head: EFFECT OF HIGH ATTRACTIVENESS ON PERCEIVED INTELLIGENCE 1 Running head: EFFECT OF HIGH ATTRACTIVENESS ON PERCEIVED INTELLIGENCE 1 The Effect of High Attractiveness on Perceived Intelligence Katlyn Hogue, Cara Mulhall, and Kara Stewart Hanover College PSY 344:

More information

The Name-Ease Effect and Its Dual Impact on Importance Judgments Aparna A. Labroo, 1 Soraya Lambotte, 1 and Yan Zhang 2

The Name-Ease Effect and Its Dual Impact on Importance Judgments Aparna A. Labroo, 1 Soraya Lambotte, 1 and Yan Zhang 2 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Name-Ease Effect and Its Dual Impact on Importance Judgments Aparna A. Labroo, 1 Soraya Lambotte, 1 and Yan Zhang 2 1 University of Chicago and 2 National University

More information

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES Journal of Experimental Vol. 51, No. 4, 1956 Psychology CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES KENNETH H. KURTZ AND CARL I. HOVLAND Under conditions where several concepts are learned concurrently

More information

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Hidehito Honda (hito@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Kimihiko Yamagishi (kimihiko@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Graduate School of Decision Science

More information

2. Which of the following is not an element of McGuire s chain of cognitive responses model? a. Attention b. Comprehension c. Yielding d.

2. Which of the following is not an element of McGuire s chain of cognitive responses model? a. Attention b. Comprehension c. Yielding d. Chapter 10: Cognitive Processing of Attitudes 1. McGuire s (1969) model of information processing can be best described as which of the following? a. Sequential b. Parallel c. Automatic 2. Which of the

More information

Sensation & Perception The Visual System. Subjectivity of Perception. Sensation vs. Perception 1/9/11

Sensation & Perception The Visual System. Subjectivity of Perception. Sensation vs. Perception 1/9/11 Sensation & Perception The Visual System Subjectivity of Perception We do not perceive the world directly Perception depends on brain stimulation Distinction between sensation and perception Sensation

More information

Impression order effects as a function of the personal relevance of the object of description*

Impression order effects as a function of the personal relevance of the object of description* Memory & Cognition 1974, Vol. 2, No.3, 561-565 Impression order effects as a function of the personal relevance of the object of description* JOHN H. BRINK Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907

More information

The Influence of Nonremembered Affective Associations on Preference

The Influence of Nonremembered Affective Associations on Preference Emotion Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 6, No. 2, 215 223 1528-3542/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.215 The Influence of Nonremembered Affective Associations on

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Positive mood, attribution, and the illusion of familiarity

Positive mood, attribution, and the illusion of familiarity Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44 (2008) 721 728 www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Positive mood, attribution, and the illusion of familiarity Heather M.

More information

Do Attitudes Affect Memory? Tests of the Congeniality Hypothesis

Do Attitudes Affect Memory? Tests of the Congeniality Hypothesis 5 Do Attitudes Affect Memory? Tests of the Congeniality Hypothesis Alice H. Eagly, 1 Patrick Kulesa, Serena Chen, and Shelly Chaiken Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois

More information

The reality.1. Project IT89, Ravens Advanced Progressive Matrices Correlation: r = -.52, N = 76, 99% normal bivariate confidence ellipse

The reality.1. Project IT89, Ravens Advanced Progressive Matrices Correlation: r = -.52, N = 76, 99% normal bivariate confidence ellipse The reality.1 45 35 Project IT89, Ravens Advanced Progressive Matrices Correlation: r = -.52, N = 76, 99% normal bivariate confidence ellipse 25 15 5-5 4 8 12 16 2 24 28 32 RAVEN APM Score Let us examine

More information

Learning Within-Category Attribute Correlations in a One-Attribute Visual Search Classification Paradigm

Learning Within-Category Attribute Correlations in a One-Attribute Visual Search Classification Paradigm Learning Within- Attribute Correlations in a One-Attribute Visual Search Classification Paradigm Gyslain Giguère (giguere.gyslain@courrier.uqam.ca) Université du Québec à Montréal, Département de psychologie,

More information

Attitude I. Attitude A. A positive or negative evaluation of a concept B. Attitudes tend to be based on 1)...values 2)...beliefs 3)...

Attitude I. Attitude A. A positive or negative evaluation of a concept B. Attitudes tend to be based on 1)...values 2)...beliefs 3)... Attitude I. Attitude A. A positive or negative evaluation of a concept B. Attitudes tend to be based on 1)...values 2)...beliefs 3)...affect, how things make us feels 4)...past experience II. Values A.

More information

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction Tracey Miser (miser.4@osu.edu) Department of Psychology, 1835 Neil Avenue Columbus, OH43210 USA Vladimir Sloutsky (sloutsky.1@osu.edu)

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Thinking about product attributes: Investigating the role of unconscious valence processing in attribute framing

Thinking about product attributes: Investigating the role of unconscious valence processing in attribute framing McElroy, T., & Conrad, J. (2009). Thinking about product attributes: Investigating the role of unconscious valence processing in attribute framing. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 12, 157-161. Published

More information

Affective Priming: Valence and Arousal

Affective Priming: Valence and Arousal Affective Priming: Valence and Arousal Dr. Mark Ashton Smith Introduction Affect: Its hypothesized 3-D structure: valence, arousal and control It is widely held that emotion can be defined as a coincidence

More information

First Sight. Jim Carpenter, PhD, ABPP.

First Sight. Jim Carpenter, PhD, ABPP. First Sight Jim Carpenter, PhD, ABPP www.firstsightbook.com A MODEL OF THE MIND AND A THEORY OF PSI HOW MIGHT ESP WORK? WHAT MIGHT IT BE FOR? Things We Think about Psi Unpredictable Anomalous Limited to

More information

Recognition without face identification

Recognition without face identification Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (7), 1610-1619 Recognition without face identification ANNE M. CLEARY Colorado State University, Fort Collins, Colorado AND LAURA E. SPECKER Williams College, Williamstown,

More information

OF NON-FOCAL EXPOSURE

OF NON-FOCAL EXPOSURE 01-vanhuele 14-10-2005 11:00 Pagina 5 AND MARKETING COMMUNICATIONS THE EFFECTIVENESS MARC VANHUELE HEC School of Management, Paris DIDIER COURBET SYLVAIN DENIS FRÉDÉRIC LAVIGNE AMÉLIE BORDE We show that

More information

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Automatic detection - 1 Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1986, 24 (6), 431-434 SIU L. CHOW The University of Wollongong,

More information

SENSATION AND PERCEPTION KEY TERMS

SENSATION AND PERCEPTION KEY TERMS SENSATION AND PERCEPTION KEY TERMS BOTTOM-UP PROCESSING BOTTOM-UP PROCESSING refers to processing sensory information as it is coming in. In other words, if I flash a random picture on the screen, your

More information

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: SOCIAL AFFILIATION CUES PRIME HELP-SEEKING INTENTIONS 1 This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: Rubin, M. (2011). Social

More information

CAHIER DE RECHERCHE n E3

CAHIER DE RECHERCHE n E3 CAHIER DE RECHERCHE n 2010-02 E3 THE MESSAGE FRAMING OF HEALTH COMMUNICATIONS : HOW TO ELICIT HIGHER INTENTION TO GET AN ANNUAL PAP TEST? BALBO LAURIE Unité Mixte de Recherche CNRS / Université Pierre

More information

Attitudes have been viewed by many (e.g., Allport, Resistance Can Be Good or Bad: How Theories of Resistance and Dissonance Affect Attitude Certainty

Attitudes have been viewed by many (e.g., Allport, Resistance Can Be Good or Bad: How Theories of Resistance and Dissonance Affect Attitude Certainty 10.1177/0146167205286110 PERSONALITY Rydell et al. / RESISTANCE AND SOCIAL CAN PSYCHOLOGY BE GOOD OR BULLETIN BAD Resistance Can Be Good or Bad: How Theories of Resistance and Dissonance Affect Attitude

More information