DUKE UNIVERSITY. stants. For concurrent VI VI, a and b are both. one (undermatching); for concurrent VI VR, b

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "DUKE UNIVERSITY. stants. For concurrent VI VI, a and b are both. one (undermatching); for concurrent VI VR, b"

Transcription

1 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1983, 40, NUMBER 3 (NOVEMBER) MATCHING, MAXIMIZING, AND HILL-CLIMBING JOHN M. HINSON AND J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY In simple situations, animals consistently choose the better of two alternatives. On concurrent variable-interval variable-interval and variable-interval variable-ratio schedules, they approximately match aggregate choice and reinforcement ratios. The matching law attempts to explain the latter result but does not address the former. Hill-climbing rules such as momentary maximizing can account for both. We show that momentary maximizing constrains molar choice to approximate matching; that molar choice covaries with pigeons' momentary-maximizing estimate; and that the "generalized matching law" follows from almost any hill-climbing rule. Key words: optimal behavior, momentary maximizing, matching law, concurrent schedules, variable interval, variable ratio, key peck, pigeons Early research has shown that a hungry ass regularly allowed to choose between two piles of hay, one large and one small, will consistently pick the large. This result conforms to the matching law (Herrnstein, 1961, 1970), but is not explained by it-consistent choice of the small pile would fit as well. Two other theories do explain this result: overall maximization (e.g., Rachlin, Green, Kagel, & Battalio, 1976; Staddon & Motheral, 1978) and hillclimbing, namely, picking the best alternative at a given time (e.g., Hinson & Staddon, 1983; Shimp, 1969). Overall maximization works because consistent choice of the large pile maximizes food intake. Hill-climbing works because always picking the large pile means always picking the best alternative offered. More recent research has shown that on concurrent variable-interval variable-interval (concurrent VI VI), variable-interval, variable-ratio (concurrent VI VR), and some other reinforcement schedules, pigeons, rats, and people allocate their aggregate responding according to the formula x/y = a[r(x)/r(y)]b, (1) where x and y are the average response rates to the two alternatives, R(x) and R(y) the reinforcement rates obtained, and a and b con- This research was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation to Duke University, J. E. R. Staddon principal investigator. Reprints may be obtained from John M. Hinson, Department of Psychology, Washington State University, Pullman, WashingtoIn stants. For concurrent VI VI, a and b are both approximately unity (i.e., the matching law), although some studies find b values less than one (undermatching); for concurrent VI VR, b is near unity but a may be less than one (biased matching); for concurrent chained schedules, a is unity, but b is generally greater than unity (overmatching: Bacotti, 1977; Baum, 1974; Davison, 198-1; Lea, 1981). Equation 1, the "generalized matching law," is flexible enough to accommodate almost any monotonic empirical relationship between choice and reinforcement ratios, given usual experimental variation. Hence, Equation 1 is significant mainly for the consistent relationship betwee-n parameter values and particular procedures. The standard matching law cannot account4or systematic deviations from unity in the two parameters. Behav,ier-on concurrent schedules has propert-ies,mi addi-tion to aggregate choice ratios: dist-rib-tions of interresponse times (IRTs) to each alternative, -interswitch times (i.e., choice bout lengths), and temporal relationships between -4ie sequence of responses to each alternative. Some of these properties are affected in striking ways by procedural features. For example, if each switch (changeover) starts a timer that prevents the delivery of food for t sec after the switch (changeover delay: COD), then interswitch times increase (e.g., Baum, 1982; Hunter & Davison, 1978). As another example, I-RTs to the VR alternative on concurrent VI VR are shorter than IRTs to the VI. None of these properties of choice are explicable by the matching law. 321

2 322 JOHN M. HINSON and J. E. R. STADDON The limited applicability of matching-law formulations has led in recent years to a search for more comprehensive alternatives that can explain simple matching, power-ratio matching (Equation 1), COD and schedule-type effects, as well as exclusive choice. For example, the effects of COD on interswitch time make perfect sense from the point of view of either overall maximizing or hill-climbing; IRT differences and biased matching on concurrent VI VR follow from a form of hill-climbing (Hinson & Staddon, 1983; Staddon, 1980), and as we have seen, exclusive choice in simple situations follows from almost any optimizing theory. Molar measures, such as the aggregate response and reinforcement rates that enter into Equation 1, are made up of molecular elements: moment-by-moment choices of one alternative or the other. Both molar matching and overall reinforcement-rate maximization when they occur must depend upon rules that relate the animal's instantaneous choice to its past history. Such a rule can be called a strategy or policy without implying conscious deliberation by the animal. When molar principles fail, the obvious place to look for an explanation is the molecular strategies that underlie them. We can probably rule out one possibilitywhat we have elsewhere called the molar comparison strategy (Hinson & Staddon, 1983)- that is, the idea that when animals maximize, they do so by varying choice proportions from day to day, remembering the average reinforcement rate associated with each, and choosing the proportion that gives them the highest payoff rate. Memory limitations mean that animals are much more likely to maximize (and match) by adopting a moment-by-moment strategy that does not demand comparisons over long time periods. Hill-climbing, picking the best option available from moment to moment, is one class of strategies that makes limited demands on memory, yet often achieves molar outcomes close to the theoretical optimum (Minsky, 1961). This paper discusses one such hill-climbing strategy, momentary maximizing (Shimp, 1966). We have previously shown that pigeons conform reasonably well to momentary maximizing on both concurrent VI VI and concurrent VI VR schedules (Hinson 8c Staddon, 1981, 1983). Here we show that when pigeons show momentary maximizing, molar choice proportion approximately matches reinforcement proportion; that molar choice varies with the animal's momentary estimate of payoff probabilities for the two alternatives on these two schedules; and that this relation is not an artifact of our method of measuring the animal's estimate. Hence matching, a molar relation, is probably the outcome of moment-by-moment hill-climbing. We show in the Discussion that a class of hill-climbing strategies of which momentary maximizing is a special case implies molar results conforming to Equation 1. We conclude from these simulations that Equation 1 is implied by almost any hill-climbing rule. METHOD This paper presents additional analyses of the experiment described in Hinson and Staddon (1983). The method is given in full in that paper, so we present only a brief summary here. Subjects Eight male, adult White Carneaux pigeons served. Apparatus All experiments were conducted in a standard aluminum and Plexiglas operant-conditioning chamber with two translucent pecking keys. The experimental contingencies and data recording were carried out by a microcomputer in an adjacent room. Data on the absolute time (to one msec) and identity of each experimental event were later transferred to a PDP I1 minicomputer for analysis. Procedure The pigeons received extensive training on concurrent VI VI and VR VI schedules, with random (Fleshler & Hoffman, 1962) interfood intervals and no changeover delay. The experimental conditions for all animals appear in Table 1. Sessions lasted 1 hour, excluding the time taken by food delivery. RESULTS Concurrent VI VI Figure 1 shows molar matching: the relationship between the mean logarithms of choice ratios and the mean of the logarithms of ob-

3 MATCHING, MAXIMIZING, AND HILL-CLIMBING 323 Table 1 Conditions and Number of Sessions for Each Bird Concurrent VI VI Bird # Condition C096 C0123 C0104 Sessions 1 VI60 VI 60 VI60 VI60 VI60 VI VI180VI60 VI180VI60 VI180VI VI60 VI 180 VI60 VI 180 VI60 VI VI60 VI60 VI60 VI60 VI60 VI60 30 CD129 CD117 CD148 1 VI 180 VI 60 VI 180 VI VI60 VI 180 VI60 VI 180 VI60 VI VI 60 VI 60 VI 60 VI 60 VI 60 VI VI 240 VI 60 VI 240 VI 60 VI 240 VI VI 90 VI 180 VI 90 VI 180 VI 90 VI VI180VI30 VI180VI30 15 Concurrent VR VI Bird # Bird # CR129 Sessions CR117 Sessions 1 VR 30 VI VR 30 VI VR 60 VI VR 60 VI VR 15 VI VR 15 VI CR101 CR196 1 VR 15 VI VR 60 VI VR 60 VI VR 60 VI VR 30 VI VR 60 VI tained reinforcement ratios of the last five sessions of each condition for each animal. Vertical bars for each point within the graph show the range of response ratios. Matching in this situation is within the normal range: Slopes of the fitted regression lines, shown for each animal, are generally less than one and the intercepts are close to zero. There are two ways to demonstrate a relationship between matching and momentary maximizing, neither perfectly satisfactory. The first is to show that when animals conform well to the momentary maximizing rule, choice proportions are close to matching. (When animals maximize poorly, their choice proportions may or may not conform to matching, depending on what causes the poor performance. Consequently, molar choice proportion on days when maximizing is poor is less informative than choice performance on days when maximizing is good.) The second way to show a relationship between matching and momentary maximizing is to show that molar choice proportion reliably covaries with the animal's estimate of the momentary maximizing switching line. We will discuss each of these methods in turn. Previous papers (Hinson & Staddon, 1981, 1983; Staddon, Hinson, & Kram, 1981) have shown that the relevant variables for momentary maximizing are t1 and t2, the times since the last responses to alternatives one and two. Momentary maximizing is defined by the switching line, tl = x2t2/xl, where X, is the scheduled VI reinforcement rate for alternative i. The animal is performing perfectly when all instances of response one lie between the switching line and the t1 axis in t1 / t2 space (the clock space) and all instances of response two lie between the line and the t2 axis. Table 2 Procedure for obtaining the momentary maximizing estimate m. Reinforcement Probability p(ritl) > P(R Q p(r tj) < p(r t2) 1 ZPIp-P21=a ZIP1-P21=b Response 2 ZIp1-p2l=c IYpL-p2lu=d m=(a+d;)/(a+b+c+d)

4 324 JOHN M. HINSON and J. E. R. STADDON 0 H Li Uf) z 00t C,) ELI CD129 CD ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~1-2 Y=. 62 X+T. 03 Y=. 57 X-. 17 CDI C C = '7# Y_il- CZ V= -a I Y.I. RC; C Y=.69 X+.11 Y=.73 X LOG OBTAINED RFT RATIO Fig. 1. Log-log coordinate space for ratios of obtained response and reinforcement rates for each bird in concurrent VI VI. Each square is the average of the last five sessions in each condition. The vertical bars indicate the range of ratios within each average. Best-fitting regression lines are drawn through the data points. The regression equation is supplied for each panel. The ordinate represents the4logarithm of responses to Schedule 1 divided by responses to Schedule 2. The abscissa represents the logarithm of reinforcers obtained from Schedule 1 divided by the reinforcers obtained from Schedule 2. To.provide a numerical estimate of the quality of momentary maximizing, we devised a quantity, m, analogous to "total percentage correct," which is defined in Table 2 (see Hinson & Staddon, 1983). For each correct choice, the difference in reinforcement.probability for the two alternatives -is positive, that is, those entries in cells a and d, whereas for each incorrect choice the difference in reinforcement probability for the two alternatives is negative, that is, those entries in cells b and c. The sums of the absolute probability difference for correct choices are then expressed as the proportion of the total amount of absolute probability difference for both correct and incorrect responses. Because m is a proportion, m is equal to one for perfect momentary maxi Li <. 75 H H 50 CD.25 w z H N HX H x ' CD ,s CD'I 48 C-., C S 0 +.a s.50 CDI1t7.1-..l DEVIATION -FO 0 +MH FROM MATCHING Fig. 2. Plots of m versus q, the deviation from matching, for all birds on concurrent VI V! across all conditions. The points in each graph correspond to tthe individual sessions from the last half of each condition for CD129, CD117, and CD148. The last fifteen sessions of each condition are shown for C096, C@123, and C0104. The abscissa is the log deviation from-matching. The ordinate is the corresponding value of m I C s CO 1 23.:t : mizing, zero for momentary minimizing (consistent choice of the lower-probability ailternative), and.5 for random choice. Figure 2 shows the relationship betwee -m, our maximizing estimate, and a variakmle caat summarizes deviation of the choice prfportnio from perfect matching. If, by convention, we take response x as the majority (pmore frequently reinforced) response, then a quanbtity q defined as q = 1n.{(x/y)/[R(x)/R(yt],) (2) should equal 0 for perfect matching, < 0 for undermatching, and > 0 for overmatching. Figure 2 is a scatter plot of m (ordinate) vs. q (abscissa) for the latter part of each condition for all animals exposed to concurrent VI VI. For Birds C096, C0123, and C0104, the 15 sessions of each condi- plots are for the last tion. For Birds CD129, CD117, and CD148,

5 MATCHING, MAXIMIZING, AND HILL-CLIMBING 325 the plots are for the last half of each condition. Each animal, with the possible exception of C096, shows an inverted U-shaped scatter: at high m values, q is slightly less than zero (slight undermatching); at lower m values, the outcome is variable, showing both over- and undermatching. Although these plots do not prove that good momentary maximizing forces the animal to approximate molar matching, they do illustrate that increases in the value of m result in decreases in the variability in choice proportion. A second way to test for a relationship between maximizing and matching is through the hypothesis that molar choice proportion is directly determined by momentary maximizing: If molar choice proportion always follows momentary maximizing, then variations in choice proportion must reflect variations in the animal's estimate of the switching line. A test of this idea requires a way to estimate the animal's switching line that does not artificially force a relationship between the maximizing estimate and choice proportion. One method is illustrated in Figure 3, which shows a sequence of responses to Alternatives 1 and 2. Consider any minority response, such as the one labeled "A" in the figure. If the animal is consistently following a momentary maximizing rule, then the quantity t2/t1 must be greater than the slope of his switching line, that is, greater than Xl'/X2', where X1' and X2' are the animal's estimates of X1 and X2, the scheduled reinforcement rates for the two alternatives. Hence, t2/tl for every minority response can provide us with an estimate of the animal's switching line. Because the switching condition requires t2/tl to be greater than the slope of the switching line, our estimate will be biased; but since we are interested in correlations between changes in estimate and changes in molar choice proportion, this bias in absolute value does not matter. RESPONSE 1 RESPONSE 2 I 11I A I I II-4-f t2 1 TIME.-----> Fig. 3. Hypothetical event diagrams for two responses showing the technique for generating the cumulative time estimate of the switching line. I The simplest way to show changes in the switching-line estimate over time is a cumulative plot of the quantity t2/t1. Thus, for the first minority response of the session, a point is plotted at coordinates t, and t2; for the next minority response, the new t1 and t2 are plotted with the previous 'point as origin, and so on. These closely spaced points make up a cumulative curve whose t2 axis is real time (because every minority response is recorded, so that cumulative t2 time is total time) and whose slope is directly related to the animal's estimate of the switching line. This cumulative curve can then be compared with cumulative choice proportion, where each point represents the total number of Responses 1 and 2 to that point. We expect that for animals trained on concurrent VI VI, these two curves will parallel one another, whereas animals trained on other procedures (concurrent VI VR, for example) will show no such parallelism. Figure 4 shows two comparisons (complete 1-hour sessions) between cumulative switchingline estimate (upper panels) and cumulative choice proportion (lower panels) for Birds C096 and C0123. Hash marks on the lower curves mark 2-min intervals. "-1" and "+1" signify sessions just before and just after a tran- Fig. 4. Comparison of curves of cumulative times since each response with curves of cumulative numbers of each response. (Upper) Cumulative times for the last session of concurrent VI 180 VI 60 (-1) and first session of concurrent VI 60 VI 180 (+1) for (left) Bird C096 and (right) Bird C0123. (Lower) Cumulative responses for the same sessions in the upper graph. Each time axis represents an hour. Downward deflections on the cumulative response curves mark 2-min intervals.

6 326 JOHN M. HINSON and J. E. R. STADDON sition from concurrent VI 180 VI 60 to VI 60 VI 180. It is obvious that the four pairs of curves are all highly similar, and this result is typical of all the comparisons we have made. To quantify this result we correlated the slope, minute by minute, of the cumulative switching-line estimate curve with the slope of the comparable cumulative choice-proportion curve for every animal and every session of concurrent VI VI. As a control, we include the same procedure for animals on concurrent VI VR. As shown earlier, time since a VR response is irrelevant to the switching rule for concurrent VR VI. Therefore, any correlation between the t2/tl ratio and the ratio of choices to the alternatives would likely be simply a property of the IRT distributions and not due to momentary maximizing. The grand mean correlations, for the six pigeons exposed to concurrent VI VI and the four exposed to concurrent VI VR, are shown in Table 3: The correlations for concurrent VI VI are high and positive, those for concurrent VI VR much lower; there is almost no overlap between the two sets. The data in Figure 4 and Table 3 are consistent with the hypothesis that choice proportion on concurrent VI VI is driven by the animal's estimate of the switching line. They do not prove it, for two reasons: the hypothesis is a causal one and we can do no more than exhibit correlations, and many choice data that do not conform to momentary maximizing are bound to show some correlation between our t2/t1 measure and choice proportion. For example, if animals make a choice every At, with probabilities p and I-p, then if p varies in indefinite fashion, the ratio t2/tl will tend to vary in a similar fashion. Thus, the alternative hypothesis that switching-line estimate is driven by some stochastic process that affects Table 3 Mean correlation between choice proportion and switching estimate for all birds across all condtions. VI VI VR VI C CR C CR C CR CR CD CDI17.69 CD mean.63 mean.31 choice proportion can never be conclusively refuted with our data. Nevertheless, although we cannot rule out all alternative hypotheses, we can disconfirm particular hypotheses. For example, assuming that animals choose at random, then a positive correlation (of the sort given in Table 3) between switching-line estimate and cumulative choice proportion is expected. Suppose, however, that we displace the distribution of Response 1 interresponse times by a constant amount At (i.e., a "shifted-distribution" analysis of the type discussed at length in Hinson & Staddon, 1983). If the two choice distributions are independent, shifting each element of either distribution by a constant amount cannot have a consistent effect on the correlation between switching-line estimate and cumulative choice proportion. On the other hand, if the correlation depends upon momentary maximizing, a time shift will degrade momentary maximizing and, thereby, the correlation should be progressively reduced by increasing shifts. Our pigeons showed the latter result, as illustrated in Figure 5. The figure plots the value of the correlation coefficient, r, against shift in.2-sec increments, from -1 to +1 sec. The figure shows three curves: the leftmost curve is for random data in 3:1 proportion, and the other two curves are for typical sessions from two pigeons on concurrent VI 60 VI 180. Both pigeons show a smoothly unimodal correlation function, with the peak at zero shift for one animal, slightly to the left for the other. The z 0 0 RANDOM SHIFTED IRT DISTRIBUTIONS.7.72~8.88 C096 C ' ' AMOUNT OF SHIFT (SEC) Fig. 5. Display of IRT distributions shifted positively and negatively in time, with respect to one another. (Far left) Simulated data for random choice time with matching on concurrent VI 180 VI 60. (Middle) Last session of concurrent VI 180 VI 60 for Bird C096. (Far right) Last session of concurrent VI 180 VI 60 for Bird C0123. The abscissa indicates the amount and direction (positive or negative) of the time shift. The ordinate indicates the correlation coefficient (r) between choice proportion and switching estimate for each time shift.

7 MATCHING, MAXIMIZING, AND HILL-CLIMBING 327 random data vary over a narrower range and show no clear peak, although, as expected, the correlations are positive-although smaller than for the pigeon data. These two data curves are typical of many sessions we have examined. Thus, the similarity between cumulative switching line and cumulative choice proportion in our concurrent VI VI data does not seem to be an artifact. Concurrent VR VI Figure 6 shows molar matching for the concurrent VR VI animals, in the same form as Figure 1: log response ratios vs. log obtained reinforcement ratios. Although we examined only three schedule values, matching is within the normal range; the coefficients of regression lines, which again appear in each graph, show slopes less than one, but intercepts that are generally greater than zero. The switching line for concurrent VR VI is a horizontal line parallel to the axis for the VR response (Hinson & Staddon, 1983; Staddon, Hinson, & Kram, 1981). Nevertheless, it 0 H z a co -2 CR101 CR 1 96 Y=.76 X..21 Y=.75 X -'-.27 CR1 17 CR129 Y=. 52 X -.33 Y= 70 X LOG OBTAINED RFT RATIO Fig. 6. Log-log coordinate space for ratios of obtained response and reinforcement rates for each bird in concurrent VR VI. Each square is the average of the last five sessions in each condition. The vertical bars indicate the range of ratios within each average. Best-fitting regression lines are drawn through the data points. The regression equation is supplied for each panel. The ordinate represents the logarithm of VR schedule responses divided by VI schedule responses. The abscissa represents the logarithm of reinforcers obtained from the VR schedule divided by the reinforcers obtained from the VI schedule. is possible to compute m, our momentary maximizing estimate, in the same way as for concurrent VI VI. Figure 7 shows a scatter plot of m plotted vs. q, an estimate of matching, for all sessions for each bird exposed to concurrent VR VI (this figure parallels Figure 2). The results are similar to those shown in Figure 2: When the animal is maximizing well, molar performance is constrained close to matching; otherwise, there is variation on both sides of matching. Because the switching line for concurrent VR VI is parallel to the VR axis, there is no estimate analogous to the switching-line estimate we used for concurrent VI VI that will permit us to show a correlation between choice proportion and switching line. Part of the problem is that momentary maximizing on concurrent VR VI depends on choice rate, whereas on concurrent VI VI, the expected choice proportion is independent of choice rate. For example, if an animal responds very slowly on concurrent VR VI, at the time of a chqice the probability of reinforcement for a VI response will nearly always be higher than on the VR; hence, almost all choices will be to the VI schedule. Conversely, if choice rate is high, the VR will nearly always be the better prospect and nearly all choices should be to the CR CR1 17 t 'I E 75 X 75X.so.so LU~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ z -CR196 CR129 I Ca S ~~~.7S e DEVIATION FROM MATCHING Fig. 7. Plots of m versus q, the deviation from matching, for all birds on concurrent VR VI across all conditions. The points in each graph correspond to the individual sessions from the last half of each condition. The abscissa is the log deviation from matching. The ordinate is the corresponding value of m.

8 328 JOHN M. HINSON and J. E. R. STADDON VR schedule. For concurrent VI VI, on the other hand, the choice proportion is independent of choice rate-although, given the smaller probability differences at very low choice rates, when both payoff probabilities are close to unity, one might expect a trend towards indifference (i.e., undermatching) as choice rates fall to low levels. Since choice rate and switching-line estimate are confounded for concurrent VR VI, we cannot show that choice proportion on concurrent VR VI is correlated with the animal's estimate of the switching line in the same fashion as concurrent VI VI. However, momentary maximizing dictates that the proportion of VR responses should be positively correlated (and proportion of VI responses negatively correlated) with the absolute choice rate (i.e., VI + VR response rates). This result is illustrated in a previous paper (Hinson & Staddon, 1983, Table 6): Proportion of VI responses was consistently negatively correlated with overall choice rate on concurrent VR VI; as a control condition, the comparable correlations on concurrent VI VI were close to zero. Thus, in general, momentary maximizing for concurrent VR VI results in predictable changes in choice proportion that are different for different types of schedules. DISCUSSION Previous theoretical papers have shown that periodic choice, combined with momentary maximizing, implies a good approximation to molar matching (Shimp, 1969; Staddon, Hinson, & Kram, 1981). The present data show the same pattern in the aperiodic behavior of pigeons on concurrent VI VI and concurrent VR VI schedules: The better the birds follow momentary maximizing, the closer the approximation to molar matching. In addition, we are able to show a close association on concurrent VI VI between the animal's estimate of the momentary maximizing switching line and molar choice proportion: Changes in choice proportion closely match changes in switching-line estimate. The correlation between the two is much closer on concurrent VI VI than on concurrent VI VR or a random simulation, and the correlation is impaired by manipulations that also impair momentary maximizing (the shifted-distribution test). Thus the correlation between momentary maximizing estimate and molar choice proportion does not seem to be an artifact of the way we estimate momentary maximizing. This test cannot be applied on concurrent VR VI because if animals momentary maximize, choice proportion should (and does) vary with overall choice rate. Nevertheless, the feedbacks inherent in ratio schedules typically ensure that such variations in choice proportion do not yield comparable deviations from matching. For example, if choice rate is high, then VI responses occur only when p(r It) is approximately equal to p(r) for the ratio schedule, that is, 1/the average ratio value. Overall, p(rit) for the VI must equal R(x)/x, where x is the VI response rate. Since 1/mean ratio value is equal to R(y)/y, where y is the average ratio response rate, momentary maximizing with a high choice rate implies matching (cf. Staddon, 1980). On the other hand, if choice rate is low, ratio responses will occur rarely, but when they do, it will be because p(r) for the ratio response is equal to or greater than p(r t) for an interval response. If choice rate is low, we cannot be sure that when a ratio response occurs, reinforcement probability will in every case be equal to the average for the VI; sometimes it will be considerably greater. Thus, when choice rate is low, fewer responses may be made to the ratio schedule than implied by matching, that is, a bias in favor of the VI. By these arguments, the deviation from matching on concurrent VR VI should tend to bias in favor of the VR when choice rates are high, but in favor of the VI when choice rates are low-at low levels of food deprivation, for example. There seem to be no relevant data on these predictions. Most of the data on concurrent VR VI schedules involves COD procedures (e.g., Herrnstein & Heyman, 1979). It is important to note that a COD profoundly changes the feedback properties of a schedule. These feedback changes result in concomitant behavioral changes such as greatly increased run lengths to both keys. Unless the theoretical analysis makes qualifying assumptions, it is not possible to predict choice proportion. At present, we have been unable to find a nonarbitrary way to extend our theoretical analysis to COD procedures.

9 MATCHING, MAXIMIZING, AND HILL-CLIMBING 329 These refinements apart, it is clear that momentary maximizing both implies matching (simple or biased) on concurrent VR VI and implies that matching will be relatively unaffected by variations in absolute choice rate and the associated variations in choice proportion. Moreover, the balance of factors pushing bias towards or away from the ratio side is such that under normal conditions, given typical variation in choice rates, we might expect relatively unbiased matching. The inevitable conclusion is that on concurrent VI VI and concurrent VR VI, all roads lead to matching. Although correlation is not the same as causation, every test we have applied yields results consistent with the idea that molar choice proportion on concurrent VI VI is driven by momentary maximizing. Feedbacks intrinsic to the procedure ensure that if the animal momentary maximizes, choice proportion will still- lead to matching on concurrent VR VI even over the normal range of rate variation on these schedules. It should be noted that the current t-heoretical analysis applies to discrete responses; it is therefore directed toward response rather than time allocation matching. However, as the model of momentary maxi-imizing is based on the time since each response, its prediction should be about the same for time-based results. Such an extension of tie model would requi-re arbitrary assuqptbions about behavior in order to -generate predictions. Since we have not worked out the vparticuiars of the timebased case, we prefer not to specilate further about possible connections between response and time-based matc-hing. Other data consistent witlhmomentarymaxmizing as a widespread -hill-limbing strategy have been reviewed eisewhere (e.g., Stadde., 1980; Staddon, Hinsdn, & Kram, 1981), but we should add as particularly conwpeling evidence an exper-iment by Lea (1976) using a concurrent -ratio sciediule with a titration procedure. The ratio value for Alternative 1 was held constant, whereas the ratio on Alternative 2 decreased when Alternative 1 was chosen. Thus, by varying the frequency of Alternative 1 and Alternative 2 choices, the animal could select almost any Alternative 2 ratio. Overa4l maximizing under these conditions implies that Alternative 1 should be chosen;frequently, so as to keep Alternative 2 at a low value. Momentary maximizing implies a pattern of choice such that Alternative 2 should average the same probability of reinforcement as Alternative 1, which is what Lea found. Momentary maximizing is also consistent with the shifts in preference reported by Herrnstein and Vaughan (1980) in concurrentratio experiments in which pairs of different ratio values were alternated between the two choices: Although exclusive responding to either choice is consistent with matching, their animals consistently shifted towards the lower ratio. Momentary maximizing is also consistent with the results of a complex procedure (Herrnstein & Vaughan, 1980) in which pigeons could achieve higher overall payoff rates by allocating responses disproportionately to one alternative; as in Lea's experiment, unconstrained overall maximizing predicts disproportionate allocation, whereas momentary maximizing ipredicts what was found-approximate matching. Other Hill-Clmbing Rules Momentary -maximizing is not the only possible Whll-cinling rule appropriate for concurrent schedles. We do not wish to explore an infinite fidld, tat it may be instructive to conclude with a4brief discussion of one other class of hill-cir-ibing -rules and show that it, -too, leads to matching of one sort or another on concurrent schedules. Consider Eke conficting demands placed on an animal fresyplaced in a choice situation: 'Me needs toexplere each alternative, and he -needs to be sensitive to the consequences of 4iis hoices. A siwip$e switching rule that accomlishes boe& i4ject'ives is t1 + AI/T =t2+ AIT2, (3) -where t, is time since the last,response to alternative i, Ti is time since the last reinforcewient for alternaive i, and A is a constant that -represents the -rela6ive importance to choice of times t and T. Su;ppose that constant A is zero at the beginning of training; the resulting switching line, -t1 = t2, prescribes simple alternation ("least.recent" choice) between the two alternatives-an appropriate exploratory strategy. If the occurrence of food causes some increase in the value of A, then immediately after getting fed for response i, the animal is much more likely to repeat response i, because the

10 330 JOHN M. HINSON and J. E. R. STADDON quantity A/ T7 will be large; as time elapses, this quantity declines and the least-recent rule gains control over behavior-until the next food delivery, when the tendency to repeat response i will again increase. This model does not result in good momentary maximizing as defined earlier (Hinson & Staddon, 1983). The values of m are consistently low for this choice strategy due to perseveration of responding after a reinforcer. We simulated on a computer the effect on molar choice of responding according to Equation 3, with various values of parameter A. Choice time was random throughout. We simulated choice performance, defined by A parameter and absolute choice rate (defined as the ratio of minority VI reinforcement rate to choice rate:.02 is about the midpoint of the normal range), over five different concurrent VI VI schedules, ranging from 1:1 to 1:6 in relative scheduled reinforcement rate. At each value of A and choice rate, regression lines were fitted to the logarithms of choice and reinforcement ratios from 500,000 program cycles. Some of the results are shown in Table 4. In every case, the fit of the regression line was excellent: Equation 1 fitted impeccably all the data generated by this class of hill-climber. Matching was always unbiased, as one might expect: Constant a in Equation 1 was always close to one. And in general the tendency was towards undermatching, b < 1, with b lower at lower A values, when choice approached simple alternation. The point of this simulation is not that Equation 3 fits actual performance on, for example, concurrent VI VI-in our previous report (Hinson & Staddon, 1983) we in fact found no evidence whatsoever for a reinforcement-recency effect of the sort implied by Equations 3 with A = 0-although such effects Table 4 Parameters of matching lines (Equation 1) for switching rule of Equation 3. Choice Rate.1.01 Inter- Inter- Slope cept Slope cept (b) (log a) rp (b) (log a) r2 A (thousands) are seen frequently under other conditions. Our point is that essentially any hill-climbing rule sufficient to prevent exclusive choice on concurrent VI VI and concurrent VR VI must yield data consistent with Equation 1. Hence, the fact of molar power-ratio matching tells us almost nothing about what sort of choice rules are determining performance. The evidence discussed in the rest of this paper, and in our earlier report, suggests that, for the most part, pigeons' choices are determined by momentary maximizing. REFERENCES Bacotti, A. V. Matching under concurrent fixed-ratio variable-interval schedules of food presentation. Journal of the Exeprimental Analysis of Behavior, 1977, 27, Baum, W. M. On two types of deviation from the matching law: Bias and undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 22, Baum, W. M. Choice, changeover, and travel. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1982, 38, Davison, M. Choice between concurrent variable-interval and fixed-ratio schedules: A failure of the generalized matching law. In C. M. Bradshaw, E. Szabadi, & C. F. Lowe (Eds.), Quantification of steady-state operant behaviour. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland, Fleshler, M., & Hoffman, H. S. A progression for generating variable-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5, Herrnstein, R. J. Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1961, 4, Herrnstein, R. J. On the law of effect. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1970, 13, Herrnstein, R. J., and Heyman, G. M. Is matching compatible with reinforcement maximization on concurrent variable interval, variable ratio? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1979, 31, Herrnstein, R. J., & Vaughan, W., Jr. Melioration and behavioral allocation. In J. E. R. Staddon (Ed.), Limits to action: The allocation of individual behavior. New York: Academic Press, Hinson, J. M., & Staddon, J. E. R. Maximizing on interval schedules. In C. M. Bradshaw (Ed.), Recent developments in the quantification of steady-state operant behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland, Hinson, J. M., & Staddon, J. E. R. Hill-climbing by pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1983, 39, Hunter, I. W., & Davison, M. C. Response rate and changeover performance on concurrent variable-in-

11 MATCHING, MAXIMIZING, AND HILL-CLIMBING 331 terval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1978, 29, Lea, S. E. G. Titration of schedule parameters by pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1976, 25, Lea, S. E. G. Correlation and contiguity in foraging behaviour. In P. Harzem & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Advances in analysis of behaviour (Vol. 2). Predictability, correlation, and contiguity. Chichester and New York: Wiley, Minsky, M. Steps toward artificial intelligence. Proceedings of the Institutes of Radio Engineers, 1961, 49, Rachlin, H., Green, L., Kagel, J. H., & Battalio, R. C. Economic demand theory and psychological studies of choice. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 10). New York: Academic Press, Shimp, C. P. Probabilistically reinforced choice behavior in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1966, 9, Shimp, C. P. Optimal behavior in free-operant experiments. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, Staddon, J. E. R. Optimality analyses of operant behavior and their relation to optimal foraging. In J. E. R. Staddon (Ed.), Limits to action: The allocation of individual behavior. New York: Academic Press, Staddon, J. E. R., Hinson, J. M., & Kram, R. Optimal choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1981, 35, Staddon, J. E. R., & Motheral, S. On matching and maximizing in operant choice experiments. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, Received July 30,1982 Final acceptance June 6,1983

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

OPTIMALITY AND CONCURRENT VARIABLE-INTERVAL VARIABLE-RATIO SCHEDULES WILLIAM M. BAUM AND CARLOS F. APARICIO

OPTIMALITY AND CONCURRENT VARIABLE-INTERVAL VARIABLE-RATIO SCHEDULES WILLIAM M. BAUM AND CARLOS F. APARICIO JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1999, 71, 75 89 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) OPTIMALITY AND CONCURRENT VARIABLE-INTERVAL VARIABLE-RATIO SCHEDULES WILLIAM M. BAUM AND CARLOS F. APARICIO UNIVERSITY

More information

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (3), 264-270 Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pullman, Washington 99163 Five pigeons

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules

Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules Animal Learning & Behavior 1977, 5 (1),11-114 Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pul/man, Washington

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 93 102 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND

More information

Key words: stochastic model, ratio schedules, pigeon, Bush-Mosteller, reward, satisficing, equilibrium analysis, effect-ratio invariance

Key words: stochastic model, ratio schedules, pigeon, Bush-Mosteller, reward, satisficing, equilibrium analysis, effect-ratio invariance Behavioural Processes, 15 (1987) 59-92 Elsevier 59 PROBABILISTIC CHOICE: A SIMPLE INVARIANCE J. M. HORNER and J. E. R. STADDON Department of Psychology, Duke University, Durham, NC 27706, U.S.A. (Accepted

More information

Law of effect models and choice between many alternatives

Law of effect models and choice between many alternatives University of Wollongong Research Online Australian Health Services Research Institute Faculty of Business 2013 Law of effect models and choice between many alternatives Michael Alexander Navakatikyan

More information

Time. Time. When the smaller reward is near, it may appear to have a higher value than the larger, more delayed reward. But from a greater distance,

Time. Time. When the smaller reward is near, it may appear to have a higher value than the larger, more delayed reward. But from a greater distance, Choice Behavior IV Theories of Choice Self-Control Theories of Choice Behavior Several alternatives have been proposed to explain choice behavior: Matching theory Melioration theory Optimization theory

More information

Herrnstein's equation for the rates of responding during concurrent schedules

Herrnstein's equation for the rates of responding during concurrent schedules Animal Learning & Behavior 1983, II (3),275-289 Herrnstein's equation for the rates of responding during concurrent schedules FRANCES K. McSWEENEY, CAM L. MELVILLE, and J. E. WHIPPLE Washington State University,

More information

Chapter 1: Explaining Behavior

Chapter 1: Explaining Behavior Chapter 1: Explaining Behavior GOAL OF SCIENCE is to generate explanations for various puzzling natural phenomenon. - Generate general laws of behavior (psychology) RESEARCH: principle method for acquiring

More information

Operant matching. Sebastian Seung 9.29 Lecture 6: February 24, 2004

Operant matching. Sebastian Seung 9.29 Lecture 6: February 24, 2004 MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.29J, Spring 2004 - Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Instructor: Professor Sebastian Seung Operant matching Sebastian Seung 9.29 Lecture 6: February

More information

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation The description and presentation of results involves a number of topics. These include scales of measurement, descriptive statistics used to summarize

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Overview. Simple Schedules of Reinforcement. Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement. Combined Schedules of Reinforcement BEHP 1016

Overview. Simple Schedules of Reinforcement. Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement. Combined Schedules of Reinforcement BEHP 1016 BEHP 1016 Why People Often Make Bad Choices and What to Do About It: Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement F. Charles Mace, Ph.D., BCBA-D University of Southern Maine with Jose Martinez-Diaz,

More information

k/2. Thus, Re is measured in the same units

k/2. Thus, Re is measured in the same units JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR INCREASING AND SIGNALING BACKGROUND REINFORCEMENT: EFFECT ON THE FOREGROUND RESPONSE-REINFORCER RELATION TERRY W. BELKE AND GENE M. HEYMAN HARVARD UNIVERSITY

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND

ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2008, 89, 157 167 NUMBER 2(MARCH) ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND IDAHO STATE UNIVERSITY

More information

Variation in Herrnsein s r<sub>0</sub> as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate

Variation in Herrnsein s r<sub>0</sub> as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate Illinois Wesleyan University From the SelectedWorks of James Dougan 1985 Variation in Herrnsein s r0 as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate James Dougan, Illinois Wesleyan University

More information

Business Statistics Probability

Business Statistics Probability Business Statistics The following was provided by Dr. Suzanne Delaney, and is a comprehensive review of Business Statistics. The workshop instructor will provide relevant examples during the Skills Assessment

More information

STATISTICS INFORMED DECISIONS USING DATA

STATISTICS INFORMED DECISIONS USING DATA STATISTICS INFORMED DECISIONS USING DATA Fifth Edition Chapter 4 Describing the Relation between Two Variables 4.1 Scatter Diagrams and Correlation Learning Objectives 1. Draw and interpret scatter diagrams

More information

Describing Naturally Occurring Schedules: Analysis of Feedback Functions for Shooting During Basketball Games. A Thesis Presented

Describing Naturally Occurring Schedules: Analysis of Feedback Functions for Shooting During Basketball Games. A Thesis Presented Describing Naturally Occurring Schedules: Analysis of Feedback Functions for Shooting During Basketball Games A Thesis Presented by Nicholas R. Vanselow The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational

More information

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Behavioural Processes 66 (2004) 213 233 Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Raymond C. Pitts, Stacy

More information

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES Correlational Research Correlational Designs Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are

More information

QUANTIFICATION OF DRUG CHOICE WITH THE GENERALIZED MATCHING LAW IN RHESUS MONKEYS MIKHAIL N. KOFFARNUS AND JAMES H. WOODS

QUANTIFICATION OF DRUG CHOICE WITH THE GENERALIZED MATCHING LAW IN RHESUS MONKEYS MIKHAIL N. KOFFARNUS AND JAMES H. WOODS JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2008, 89, 209 224 NUMBER 2(MARCH) QUANTIFICATION OF DRUG CHOICE WITH THE GENERALIZED MATCHING LAW IN RHESUS MONKEYS MIKHAIL N. KOFFARNUS AND JAMES H. WOODS

More information

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* Perception & Psychophysics 1973. Vol. 13. So. 2.356-360 Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* ANGEL VASSILEV+ Institu te of Physiology. Bulgarian Academy of Sciences.

More information

Chapter 1: Exploring Data

Chapter 1: Exploring Data Chapter 1: Exploring Data Key Vocabulary:! individual! variable! frequency table! relative frequency table! distribution! pie chart! bar graph! two-way table! marginal distributions! conditional distributions!

More information

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL'

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1973, 2, 393-43 NUMBER 3 (NOVEMBER) PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' M. C. DAVISON AND W. TEMPLE UNIVERSITY OF AUCKLAND AND

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2005 April ; 31(2): 213 225. Timing in Choice Experiments Jeremie Jozefowiez and Daniel T. Cerutti

More information

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo Business Statistics The following was provided by Dr. Suzanne Delaney, and is a comprehensive review of Business Statistics. The workshop instructor will provide relevant examples during the Skills Assessment

More information

Regression Discontinuity Analysis

Regression Discontinuity Analysis Regression Discontinuity Analysis A researcher wants to determine whether tutoring underachieving middle school students improves their math grades. Another wonders whether providing financial aid to low-income

More information

Understandable Statistics

Understandable Statistics Understandable Statistics correlated to the Advanced Placement Program Course Description for Statistics Prepared for Alabama CC2 6/2003 2003 Understandable Statistics 2003 correlated to the Advanced Placement

More information

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2008 Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional

More information

The Regression-Discontinuity Design

The Regression-Discontinuity Design Page 1 of 10 Home» Design» Quasi-Experimental Design» The Regression-Discontinuity Design The regression-discontinuity design. What a terrible name! In everyday language both parts of the term have connotations

More information

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, & JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR1 1982, 38, 71-85 NUMBER I (JULY) MEMORY FOR RECENT BEHAVIOR IN THE PIGEON STEPHEN P. KRAMER CORRECTIONS DIVISION, DRAPER, UTAH Variations of the symbolic

More information

Lecture 4: Research Approaches

Lecture 4: Research Approaches Lecture 4: Research Approaches Lecture Objectives Theories in research Research design approaches ú Experimental vs. non-experimental ú Cross-sectional and longitudinal ú Descriptive approaches How to

More information

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research TWO-KEY CONCURRENT RESPONDING: CHOICE AND DELAYS OF REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS Presented to The Faculty of the Division of Graduate Studies and Research By George

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1987, 47, 57-72 CHOICE BETWEEN RELIABLE AND UNRELIABLE OUTCOMES: MIXED PERCENTAGE-REINFORCEMENT IN CONCURRENT CHAINS MARCIA L. SPETCH AND ROGER DUNN DALHOUSIE

More information

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 (emphasis on Chapter 12) Online readings: Appendix D, E & F

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 (emphasis on Chapter 12) Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 (emphasis on Chapter 12) Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Plous Chapters 17 & 18 Chapter 17: Social Influences Chapter 18: Group Judgments and Decisions

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

Section 3.2 Least-Squares Regression

Section 3.2 Least-Squares Regression Section 3.2 Least-Squares Regression Linear relationships between two quantitative variables are pretty common and easy to understand. Correlation measures the direction and strength of these relationships.

More information

The Empirical Claims of Maximization Theory: A Reply to Rachlin and to Kagel, Battalio, and Green

The Empirical Claims of Maximization Theory: A Reply to Rachlin and to Kagel, Battalio, and Green Psychological Review 1983, Vol. 90, No. 4, 385-389 Copyright 1983 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. The Empirical Claims of Maximization Theory: A Reply to Rachlin and to Kagel, Battalio,

More information

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Elizabeth M. Brannon*, Courtney J. Wusthoff #, C.R. Gallistel^, and John Gibbon @ * Center for Cognitive Neuroscience

More information

Still important ideas

Still important ideas Readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 & Appendix D & E (online) Plous Chapters 17 & 18 - Chapter 17: Social Influences - Chapter 18: Group Judgments and Decisions Still important ideas Contrast the measurement

More information

CHAPTER ONE CORRELATION

CHAPTER ONE CORRELATION CHAPTER ONE CORRELATION 1.0 Introduction The first chapter focuses on the nature of statistical data of correlation. The aim of the series of exercises is to ensure the students are able to use SPSS to

More information

WDHS Curriculum Map Probability and Statistics. What is Statistics and how does it relate to you?

WDHS Curriculum Map Probability and Statistics. What is Statistics and how does it relate to you? WDHS Curriculum Map Probability and Statistics Time Interval/ Unit 1: Introduction to Statistics 1.1-1.3 2 weeks S-IC-1: Understand statistics as a process for making inferences about population parameters

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS

3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS 3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS In this chapter, we examine the conceptual foundations of statistics. The goal is to give you an appreciation and conceptual understanding of some basic statistical

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto Session VI, September 20 2017 Learning objectives 1. Get familiar with the basic idea

More information

6. Unusual and Influential Data

6. Unusual and Influential Data Sociology 740 John ox Lecture Notes 6. Unusual and Influential Data Copyright 2014 by John ox Unusual and Influential Data 1 1. Introduction I Linear statistical models make strong assumptions about the

More information

BOOTSTRAPPING CONFIDENCE LEVELS FOR HYPOTHESES ABOUT QUADRATIC (U-SHAPED) REGRESSION MODELS

BOOTSTRAPPING CONFIDENCE LEVELS FOR HYPOTHESES ABOUT QUADRATIC (U-SHAPED) REGRESSION MODELS BOOTSTRAPPING CONFIDENCE LEVELS FOR HYPOTHESES ABOUT QUADRATIC (U-SHAPED) REGRESSION MODELS 12 June 2012 Michael Wood University of Portsmouth Business School SBS Department, Richmond Building Portland

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

The Matching Law. Definitions. Herrnstein (1961) 20/03/15. Erik Arntzen HiOA Mars, 2015

The Matching Law. Definitions. Herrnstein (1961) 20/03/15. Erik Arntzen HiOA Mars, 2015 The Matching Law Erik Arntzen HiOA Mars, 2015 1 Definitions The Matching Law states that responses are allocated to the richest reinforcement schedules. The Matching Law has been shown in both nonhumans

More information

On applying the matching law to between-subject data

On applying the matching law to between-subject data On applying the matching law to between-subject data Pier-Olivier Caron To cite this version: Pier-Olivier Caron. On applying the matching law to between-subject data. Animal Behaviour, Elsevier Masson,

More information

Mantel-Haenszel Procedures for Detecting Differential Item Functioning

Mantel-Haenszel Procedures for Detecting Differential Item Functioning A Comparison of Logistic Regression and Mantel-Haenszel Procedures for Detecting Differential Item Functioning H. Jane Rogers, Teachers College, Columbia University Hariharan Swaminathan, University of

More information

MCAS Equating Research Report: An Investigation of FCIP-1, FCIP-2, and Stocking and. Lord Equating Methods 1,2

MCAS Equating Research Report: An Investigation of FCIP-1, FCIP-2, and Stocking and. Lord Equating Methods 1,2 MCAS Equating Research Report: An Investigation of FCIP-1, FCIP-2, and Stocking and Lord Equating Methods 1,2 Lisa A. Keller, Ronald K. Hambleton, Pauline Parker, Jenna Copella University of Massachusetts

More information

Assessing Agreement Between Methods Of Clinical Measurement

Assessing Agreement Between Methods Of Clinical Measurement University of York Department of Health Sciences Measuring Health and Disease Assessing Agreement Between Methods Of Clinical Measurement Based on Bland JM, Altman DG. (1986). Statistical methods for assessing

More information

Reinforcement Learning : Theory and Practice - Programming Assignment 1

Reinforcement Learning : Theory and Practice - Programming Assignment 1 Reinforcement Learning : Theory and Practice - Programming Assignment 1 August 2016 Background It is well known in Game Theory that the game of Rock, Paper, Scissors has one and only one Nash Equilibrium.

More information

Quantitative Methods in Computing Education Research (A brief overview tips and techniques)

Quantitative Methods in Computing Education Research (A brief overview tips and techniques) Quantitative Methods in Computing Education Research (A brief overview tips and techniques) Dr Judy Sheard Senior Lecturer Co-Director, Computing Education Research Group Monash University judy.sheard@monash.edu

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Chapter 3: Examining Relationships

Chapter 3: Examining Relationships Name Date Per Key Vocabulary: response variable explanatory variable independent variable dependent variable scatterplot positive association negative association linear correlation r-value regression

More information

12/31/2016. PSY 512: Advanced Statistics for Psychological and Behavioral Research 2

12/31/2016. PSY 512: Advanced Statistics for Psychological and Behavioral Research 2 PSY 512: Advanced Statistics for Psychological and Behavioral Research 2 Introduce moderated multiple regression Continuous predictor continuous predictor Continuous predictor categorical predictor Understand

More information

of behavioral contrast. To the extent that an account of contrast is a major concern, therefore, some alternative formulation seems desirable.

of behavioral contrast. To the extent that an account of contrast is a major concern, therefore, some alternative formulation seems desirable. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AN EQUATION FOR BEHAVIORAL CONTRAST BEN A. WILLIAMS AND JOHN T. WIXTED UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SAN DIEGO 1986, 45, 47-62 NUMBER 1 (JANUARY) Pigeons were

More information

Center for Advanced Studies in Measurement and Assessment. CASMA Research Report

Center for Advanced Studies in Measurement and Assessment. CASMA Research Report Center for Advanced Studies in Measurement and Assessment CASMA Research Report Number 39 Evaluation of Comparability of Scores and Passing Decisions for Different Item Pools of Computerized Adaptive Examinations

More information

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a Effects of Food Deprivation on Memory Performance. Jacob. L. Kerewaro Abstract The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)

More information

J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY. The relative inability of the usual differential. to ask whether performance under DRL schedules

J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY. The relative inability of the usual differential. to ask whether performance under DRL schedules JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 27-38 NUMBER I (JANUARY) THE EFFECT OF INFORMATIVE FEEDBACK ON TEMPORAL TRACKING IN THE PIGEON' J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY Pigeons emitted

More information

Examining Relationships Least-squares regression. Sections 2.3

Examining Relationships Least-squares regression. Sections 2.3 Examining Relationships Least-squares regression Sections 2.3 The regression line A regression line describes a one-way linear relationship between variables. An explanatory variable, x, explains variability

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

Instrumental Conditioning I

Instrumental Conditioning I Instrumental Conditioning I Basic Procedures and Processes Instrumental or Operant Conditioning? These terms both refer to learned changes in behavior that occur as a result of the consequences of the

More information

IAPT: Regression. Regression analyses

IAPT: Regression. Regression analyses Regression analyses IAPT: Regression Regression is the rather strange name given to a set of methods for predicting one variable from another. The data shown in Table 1 and come from a student project

More information

Confounding variables in the discriminated Irt procedure.

Confounding variables in the discriminated Irt procedure. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1983 Confounding variables in the discriminated Irt procedure. David C. Palmer University of Massachusetts

More information

MEA DISCUSSION PAPERS

MEA DISCUSSION PAPERS Inference Problems under a Special Form of Heteroskedasticity Helmut Farbmacher, Heinrich Kögel 03-2015 MEA DISCUSSION PAPERS mea Amalienstr. 33_D-80799 Munich_Phone+49 89 38602-355_Fax +49 89 38602-390_www.mea.mpisoc.mpg.de

More information

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4.

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4. Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4. Jude F. Mitchell, Kristy A. Sundberg, and John H. Reynolds Systems Neurobiology Lab, The Salk Institute,

More information

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 122 2001 Participating Faculty Professor James Dickson (dickson@stolaf.edu) Professor Dana Gross (grossd@stolaf.edu)

More information

Write your identification number on each paper and cover sheet (the number stated in the upper right hand corner on your exam cover).

Write your identification number on each paper and cover sheet (the number stated in the upper right hand corner on your exam cover). STOCKHOLM UNIVERSITY Department of Economics Course name: Empirical methods 2 Course code: EC2402 Examiner: Per Pettersson-Lidbom Number of credits: 7,5 credits Date of exam: Sunday 21 February 2010 Examination

More information

7 THE OPTIMAL ALLOCATION OF BEHAVIOR

7 THE OPTIMAL ALLOCATION OF BEHAVIOR 7 THE OPTIMAL ALLOCATION OF BEHAVIOR UTILITY AND ADAPTATION TO CONSTRAINT The last chapter looked at feeding as a typical motivational system and examined the effects of variables such as palatability,

More information

BOOTSTRAPPING CONFIDENCE LEVELS FOR HYPOTHESES ABOUT REGRESSION MODELS

BOOTSTRAPPING CONFIDENCE LEVELS FOR HYPOTHESES ABOUT REGRESSION MODELS BOOTSTRAPPING CONFIDENCE LEVELS FOR HYPOTHESES ABOUT REGRESSION MODELS 17 December 2009 Michael Wood University of Portsmouth Business School SBS Department, Richmond Building Portland Street, Portsmouth

More information

NORTH SOUTH UNIVERSITY TUTORIAL 2

NORTH SOUTH UNIVERSITY TUTORIAL 2 NORTH SOUTH UNIVERSITY TUTORIAL 2 AHMED HOSSAIN,PhD Data Management and Analysis AHMED HOSSAIN,PhD - Data Management and Analysis 1 Correlation Analysis INTRODUCTION In correlation analysis, we estimate

More information

Carrying out an Empirical Project

Carrying out an Empirical Project Carrying out an Empirical Project Empirical Analysis & Style Hint Special program: Pre-training 1 Carrying out an Empirical Project 1. Posing a Question 2. Literature Review 3. Data Collection 4. Econometric

More information

On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013)

On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013) On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013) Pier-Olivier Caron To cite this version: Pier-Olivier Caron. On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013):

More information

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo Please note the page numbers listed for the Lind book may vary by a page or two depending on which version of the textbook you have. Readings: Lind 1 11 (with emphasis on chapters 10, 11) Please note chapter

More information

Further Properties of the Priority Rule

Further Properties of the Priority Rule Further Properties of the Priority Rule Michael Strevens Draft of July 2003 Abstract In Strevens (2003), I showed that science s priority system for distributing credit promotes an allocation of labor

More information

Epidemiologic Methods I & II Epidem 201AB Winter & Spring 2002

Epidemiologic Methods I & II Epidem 201AB Winter & Spring 2002 DETAILED COURSE OUTLINE Epidemiologic Methods I & II Epidem 201AB Winter & Spring 2002 Hal Morgenstern, Ph.D. Department of Epidemiology UCLA School of Public Health Page 1 I. THE NATURE OF EPIDEMIOLOGIC

More information

Human Schedule Performance with Hypothetical Monetary Reinforcement

Human Schedule Performance with Hypothetical Monetary Reinforcement EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2001, 2, 225-234 NUMBER 2 (WINTER 2001)225 Human Schedule Performance with Hypothetical Monetary Reinforcement University College London, UK Experiments examined the

More information

Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking

Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking Miki Matsumuro (muro@cog.human.nagoya-u.ac.jp) Kazuhisa Miwa (miwa@is.nagoya-u.ac.jp) Graduate School of Information

More information

Variability as an Operant?

Variability as an Operant? The Behavior Analyst 2012, 35, 243 248 No. 2 (Fall) Variability as an Operant? Per Holth Oslo and Akershus University College Correspondence concerning this commentary should be addressed to Per Holth,

More information

Experimental and Computational Analyses of Strategy Usage in the Time-Left Task

Experimental and Computational Analyses of Strategy Usage in the Time-Left Task Experimental and Computational Analyses of Strategy Usage in the Time-Left Task Ben Meijering (B.Meijering@student.rug.nl) Hedderik van Rijn (D.H.van.Rijn@rug.nl) Department of Artificial Intelligence

More information

Appendix B Statistical Methods

Appendix B Statistical Methods Appendix B Statistical Methods Figure B. Graphing data. (a) The raw data are tallied into a frequency distribution. (b) The same data are portrayed in a bar graph called a histogram. (c) A frequency polygon

More information

Color Cues and Viscosity in. Iterated Prisoner s Dilemma

Color Cues and Viscosity in. Iterated Prisoner s Dilemma Color Cues and Viscosity in Iterated Prisoner s Dilemma Michael Joya University of British Columbia Abstract The presence of color tag cues and a viscous environment have each been shown to foster conditional

More information

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli James S. Adelman (J.S.Adelman@warwick.ac.uk) Neil Stewart (Neil.Stewart@warwick.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, University

More information

Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology

Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology A. In this section we will look the nearly universal conflict that will eventually arise in any species where there is some form of parental

More information

DYNAMIC RESPONSE-BY-RESPONSE MODELS OF MATCHING BEHAVIOR IN RHESUS MONKEYS BRIAN LAU AND PAUL W. GLIMCHER

DYNAMIC RESPONSE-BY-RESPONSE MODELS OF MATCHING BEHAVIOR IN RHESUS MONKEYS BRIAN LAU AND PAUL W. GLIMCHER JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2005, 84, 555 579 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) DYNAMIC RESPONSE-BY-RESPONSE MODELS OF MATCHING BEHAVIOR IN RHESUS MONKEYS BRIAN LAU AND PAUL W. GLIMCHER NEW YORK

More information