Positional and temporal clustering in serial order memory

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Positional and temporal clustering in serial order memory"

Transcription

1 Positional and temporal clustering in serial order memory Alec Solway Princeton University Bennet B. Murdock University of Toronto Michael J. Kahana University of Pennsylvania The well-known finding that errors in serial recall tend to be clustered around the position of the target item has bolstered positional-coding theories of serial order memory. Here we show that this well-known effect is confounded with the tendency for the item recalled prior to an error to come from the correct list position. When the standard analysis of positional clustering is conditioned on the positional-accuracy of the previously recalled item, one can see very strong effects of temporal contiguity, with errors being distributed near the position of the item following the prior recall. We show that a simple associative chaining model with asymmetric neighboring, remote associations, and a primacy gradient can account for both the standard positional clustering effect and the contiguity effect demonstrated here. Using the same parameters, the model produces reasonable serial position curves and captures the change in these curves across study-test trials as well as the gains and losses of item and order information across trials. Keywords: Serial recall, serial order, association, clustering. Associative chaining and positional coding consistute the two classic models of serial order memory. Although associative chaining was the implicit theory in Ebbinghaus (1885) seminal studies of serial learning, early scholars also recognized that people can remember the positions of list items, and that this positional information was often used to aid the learning process (Ladd & Woodworth, 1911). During the 195 s and 196 s a great deal of research was focused on identifying the functional stimulus in serial learning: the prior item, as predicted by chaining theory, or the item s position, as predicted by positional theory. This work was generally inconclusive, with various experiments lending partial support to one or the other account (see Young, 1968, for a review). In recent decades, as the emphasis shifted from the study of multitrial serial learning to the study of immediate serial recall, theorists have largely rejected the possibility that chaining plays an important role in serial-order memory (e.g., Botvinick & Plaut, 26; Henson, Norris, Page, & Baddeley, 1996; Farrell & Lewandowsky, 22). Instead, most modern accounts of serial order memory emphasize the importance of positional information as the major retrieval cue. One of the major sources of evidence for the positional coding view is the phenomenon of positional clustering (also The authors acknowledge support from National Institutes of Health grant MH55687 and from the Dana Foundation. Correspondence concerning this article may be addressed to either Michael J. Kahana (kahana@psych.upenn.edu) or Alec Solway (asolway@princeton.edu) known as positional gradients, and the locality constraint). Whereas most items that are recalled appear in their correct order, list items that are recalled out of order tend to occur in neighboring positions more often than in distant positions (e.g. Estes, 1972; Lee & Estes, 1977; Nairne, 1992). This phenomenon would be expected if the retrieval cue is a memorial representation of each item s list position. When the positional cue fails to correctly retrieve the appropriate item it will tend to activate items from neighboring list positions. In addition to positional clustering, other critical evidence for position-based models comes from findings of protrusions (erroneous recall of an item from the same position in a prior list), and confusions among phonologically simlar items occuring in distinct list positions (e.g., Baddeley, 1968). Although some experiments have demonstrated a role for associative processes in serial learning (e.g., Serra & Nairne, 2; Kahana, Mollison, & Addis, 21) the striking effects of positional clustering seen in all serial recall tasks suggests that any contributions of associative chaining to serial order memory are secondary to the more prominent role of positional information. In the present paper we reevaluate the phenomenon of positional clustering by looking at the distribution of errors conditional on the accuracy of the preceding responses. Specifically, we compare cases where the preceding response was a list item recalled in the correct vs. the incorrect list position. We also analyze the effect of temporal clustering in serial recall. Whereas positional clustering is observed when recalled items cluster around their correct output positions, temporal clustering is observed when recalled items cluster

2 2 SOLWAY, MURDOCK & KAHANA Table 1 Summary of the studies that were analyzed Presentation Response Lists Length Trials Kahana& Caplan (22, Exp. 2) Spoken Spoken criterion Kahana et al. (21) Spoken Spoken criterion Spoken Spoken criterion around the position of the just-recalled item. Here one finds that order errors are frequently items from list positions near that of the just recalled item, regardless of that item s serial position. This type of temporal clustering was characterized by Kahana (1996) in free recall, and has subsequently been shown to occur in serial recall by a number of investigators (Bhatarah, Ward, & Tan, 26, 28; Golomb, Peelle, Addis, Kahana, & Wingfield, 28; Klein, Addis, & Kahana, 25). Methods To reevaluate clustering effects in serial recall, we focused on two large studies (Kahana & Caplan, 22, Exp. 2; Kahana et al., 21). Both studies used modern experimental methods, including: common words rather than nonsense syllables, the study-test method rather than anticipation learning, and computer controlled randomization and timing of stimulus presentations. Furthermore, rather than writing their responses into a set of pre-allocated positions, participants in both studies vocally recalled the lists. Table 1 contains a summary of each data set. For details, see Appendix B. In analyzing the patterns of positional and temporal clustering, we focused on the middle list items (4 16 for 19-word lists, and 4 1 for 13-word lists). Edge items were excluded because they cannot appear at all distances and lags. Results Estes (1972) reported that recalls made in the wrong serial position still cluster closely around their correct position. This result is replicated in the top row of Figure 1, which shows that the probability of recalling an item decreases as a function of distance from the correct position. The probability of seeing an item at each distance was conditioned on the number of times it was possible to recall an item at that distance. To make this clustering measure concrete, consider the example list ABSENCE HOLLOW PUPIL RIBBON DARLING RAILWAY WIN- DOW. If a participant recalls ABSENCE HOLLOW PUPIL, then PUPIL is at distance, because it appears in the third position both in the list and in the recall sequence. On the other hand, if a participant recalls ABSENCE HOLLOW RIBBON, then RIBBON is at distance 1 because it appears one position early in the recall sequence compared to the list. Likewise, HOLLOW in the recall sequence ABSENCE PUPIL HOLLOW appears one position late, and is at distance+1. By contrast, the top row of Figure 2 shows the degree of clustering around preceding responses (i.e. lag-crp, Bhatarah et al., 26, 28; Golomb et al., 28; Klein et al., 25). Here, a distance of+1 means the item had the same predecessor both in the recall sequence and in the list. A distance of+2 means there was one item in the list separating the item and its predecessor, and so on. Negative distances correspond to filling in items that were skipped over earlier in the recall sequence. Finally, a distance of means that an item was repeated twice in a row. This did not occur in the data we analyzed. The probability of seeing an item at each lag was further conditioned on the lag s availability. The top row of Figure 2 illustrates the strong contiguity effect demonstrated in other serial recall experiments (Bhatarah et al., 26, 28; Golomb et al., 28; Klein et al., 25). Our joint findings of strong positional and temporal clustering effects may seem surprising given that the two phenomena have been associated with very different theoretical models (i.e., associative chaining and positional coding). However, it is important to recognize a critical confound in this comparison. Specifically, in most serial recall studies the majority of responses appear in their correct output positions. Consider, for instance, the correct recall sequence ABSENCE HOLLOW PUPIL from our example list. If the next response is RIBBON, then its distance from the correct output position is, because RIBBON appears in the fourth list position. Its distance from the preceding response, PUPIL, is+1. Correct responses appearing at the beginning of each recall sequence account for the majority of responses made, and they all fall into the bin of the positional clustering measure and the+1 bin of the temporal clustering measure. To our knowledge, this important confound has not been considered in previous analyses of positional clustering. Fortunately, one can eliminate the confound described above by restricting the clustering analyses to items following the first order error on each trial. For example, in the recall sequence ABSENCE HOLLOW RAILWAY WINDOW, WINDOW is included because RAILWAY was recalled early. Plots of positional and temporal clustering for such items are displayed in the bottom rows of Figures 1 and 2 respectively. Figure 1 shows that the probability of recalling an item no longer decreases as a function of distance from the item s correct position. On the other hand, Figure 2 shows that, although weaker than before, the contiguity effect is preserved. After making their first order error, participants tend to pick up with the item that followed the erroneous recall in the list. Strength-based associative chaining model We next asked whether an associative chaining model that lacks any direct representation of positional information can account for the positional and temporal clustering effects described above. In previous work, associative chaining models (e.g., Lewandowsky & Murdock, 1989) have been successfully applied to several key features of serial learning and se-

3 Conditional response probability.6.6 CLUSTERING IN SERIAL RECALL 3 A B C D E F Distance from correct position Figure 1. Probability of recalling an item as a function of distance from its correct position. Negative values correspond to recalling an item early, while positive values correspond to recalling an item late. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals computed using the method of Loftus & Masson (1994). Panels in the top row were computed based on all recalls, while those in the bottom row were computed based only on recalls following the first order error. Panels are based on the first trial of the following studies: A and D. Kahana & Caplan (22, Exp. 2). B and E. Kahana et al. (21, 13-word lists). C and F. Kahana et al. (21, 19-word lists) rial recall data, but not to the positional clustering effects that have been a critical source of evidence for positional coding theories. To address this question, we developed a reduced form model incorporating the two classic assumptions of associative chaining theory: 1) that the strength of association between items is an (exponentially) decreasing function of their distance, and 2) that forward associations (i.e., those between earlier and later items in the chain) are encoded more strongly than backward associations (Ebbinghaus, 1885/1913; Kahana & Caplan, 22; Raskin & Cook, 1937). Consistent with other models, we assume a primacy gradient in item encoding strength to simulate participants tendency to allocate greater attention to early list items (Brown, Preece, & Hulme, 2; Henson, 1998; Jensen, 1962; Lewandowsky & Murdock, 1989; Page & Norris, 1998). At test, the model simulates recall probabilistically according to a Luce choice rule (Luce, 1959), and can choose to stop at any position. As in other strengthbased recall models, we use matrices to store the strengths of inter-item associations (e.g., Kimball, Smith, & Kahana, 27; Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 198; Sirotin, Kimball, & Kahana, 25); we do not explicitly model item representations themselves. In addition to asking whether the associative chaining model can account for the conditional positional and temporal clustering effects described above, we sought to examine whether the model could simultaneously account for two other critical aspects of serial recall and learning data: specifically, the multi-trial serial position curves (Ward, 1937) and the gains and losses of item and order information across trials Addis and Kahana (24). In most modeling studies, different experiments from the literature are used to illustrate different empirical phenomena. As such, separate model parameters are estimated for each experiment and one cannot be sure whether it is the model s mechanisms or its free parameters that are doing the work of fitting the empirical regularities. In order to assess whether the model can account for each of the findings using a single set of parameter values, we fit data from a single experiment (Kahana & Caplan, 22, Exp. 2). Pariticipants in this experiment learned the lists to a criterion of one perfect recall (see AppendixB), and as such the number of trials to criterion varied from list to list and from participant to participant. To analyze these lists together, we focused on the first three trials of each list. Lists that were learned in fewer than three trials were excluded. Summary of Parameters As an illustrative example of the model s dynamics, consider the list in Figure 3. During study, each item is bound to all of the preceding items. The strength of associations between neighboring items has a Gaussian distribution with

4 4 SOLWAY, MURDOCK & KAHANA Conditional response probability.6.6 A B C D E F Distance from preceding item Figure 2. Probability of recalling an item as a function of distance from preceding recall. Negative values correspond to recalling an earlier item from the list, while positive values correspond to recalling a later item from the list. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals computed using the method of Loftus & Masson (1994). Panels in the top row were computed based on all recalls, while those in the bottom row were computed based only on recalls following the first order error. Panels are based on the first trial of the studies listed in Figure 1. $START$ ABSENCE HOLLOW PUPIL Figure 3. An illustrative example of the chaining model. an exponentially decaying mean (the primacy gradient; c s controls the gain, d s the decay rate, and min as the lower bound) and with varianceσ as. The primacy gradient allows the model to mimic the way in which participants learn lists: items from the beginning of the list are remembered on the first trial, and items from later in the list are progressively added on subsequent trials (Slamecka, 1964). The strength of associations between non-adjacent items decays exponentially with distance. The rate of the decay is Gaussian with parametersµ bs andσ bs (note the increasingly lighter shade of gray between non-adjacent items in Figure 3). Remote associations allow the model to skip ahead and later return to omitted items. Without remote associations, the only type of errors the model would be able to produce are errors of omission. Strengths of backward associations are weighed by the parameter w b. In addition to being bound to one another, list items are bound to an additional non-list item whose serial position precedes the first. During test, this start marker serves as the initial retrieval cue. Retrieval is probabilistic and follows a Luce choice rule with softmax parameterγ. Whenγ=1, the probability of retrieving an item is proportional to the strength of the forward associations between the retrieval cue and the item. Asγapproaches, only the the item with the strongest association can be retrieved. If a list item is successfully retrieved, it then serves as the retrieval cue for the subsequent position. Recall may also terminate at any list position if a competing non-list item is selected (the strength of this item is controlled by the parameter stop and is independent of the retrieval cue). Table 2 provides a summary of the model s parameters along with their corresponding best fit values. A formal description of the model can be found in Appendix A. Modeling positional and temporal clustering Figures 4 and 5 show that the chaining model captures the major qualitative features of the data on both temporal and positional clustering, computed in the standard way (top rows), and conditional on the prior recall being in the wrong position (bottom rows). The parameter values used in the simulation (Table 2) have a simple and direct mapping to these results. The mean rate with which the strength of remote associations is reduced

5 CLUSTERING IN SERIAL RECALL 5 Conditional response probability.6.6 A B C Data Model D E Distance from correct position F Figure 4. Probabilities of recalling an item at various distances from its correct position. Negative values correspond to recalling an item early, while positive values correspond to recalling an item late. The solid curve was computed from Kahana & Caplan (22, Exp. 2) and the dotted curve was computed from the simulation. Model parameters for the simulation are given in Table 2. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals computed using the method of Loftus & Masson (1994). The three columns correspond to the first three trials of the empirical and simulated data. All plots are aggregated across mid-list items (4-16 of a 19 word list). A-C. Based on all items. D-F. Based only on items following the first order error in the recall sequence. Table 2 Model parameters and their corresponding values Parameter Value c s.945 d s.318 min as. σ as.645 µ bs 7. σ bs 9.93 w b.134 stop.1 γ as a function of distance (µ bs ) is high throughout the list. On average, the strength between an item and another item more than two positions away is very low (<.1). Combined with strong forward asymmetry (w b ), these values explain the large contiguity effect typical of serial recall. The model slightly overpredicts the number of items recalled early (Figure 4) for this same reason. Modeling serial position curves over trials Serial position curves for the data are shown in Figure 6A. Values for these curves were computed using relative order scoring (i.e. a recalled item is considered correct if the previous recall was the item s immediate predecessor in the list). This scoring method is well suited to the experimental paradigm that we are modeling. Rather than being forced to make a response at each serial position (e.g. saying pass for serial positions that cannot be recalled), participants were free to recall only the words that came to mind. Using relative order scoring allows items from later serial positions to be correctly recalled even if items from earlier serial positions were omitted (a common occurrence for long lists using spoken recall). As shown in Figure 6B, the chaining model captures the extended primacy effect and the change in both the level and the shape of the serial position curve across trials. The primacy effect is a result of: 1) the inherent interdependencies that exist between items, and 2) the gradient in encoding strengths. On the first trial, mid-list items exhibit low levels of recall because the stop item acts as a formidable contender. This effect is progressively reduced in later trials as inter-item associations are reinforced. Learning is a result of the variability in encoding strength (e.g.σ as ) coupled with a

6 6 SOLWAY, MURDOCK & KAHANA Conditional response probability.6.6 A B C Data Model D E Distance from preceding item F Figure 5. Probabilities of recalling an item at various distances from the preceding recall. Negative values correspond to recalling an earlier item from the list, while positive values correspond to recalling a later item from the list. The solid curve was computed from Kahana & Caplan (22, Experiment 2) and the dotted curve was computed from the simulation. Model parameters for the simulation are given in Table 2. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals computed using the method of Loftus & Masson (1994). The three columns correspond to the first three trials of the empirical and simulated data. All plots are aggregated across mid-list items (4-16 of a 19 word list). A-C. Based on all items. D-F. Based only on items following the first order error in the recall sequence. Recall probability 1.6 A Serial position Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 B Figure 6. Simulating the change in the serial position curve across study-test trials. A. Serial position curves for Trials 1-3 of a 19 word list (Kahana & Caplan, 22, Experiment 2). Trials 1-3 are indicated by solid, dashed and dotted lines respectively. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals computed using the method of Loftus & Masson (1994). B. Simulated values obtained using the chaining model. Model parameters are given in Table 2.

7 CLUSTERING IN SERIAL RECALL 7 Figure 7. State diagram representing the possible gains and losses of item and order information between consecutive trials. The three states correspond to not recalling an item at all, recalling an item out of order, and recalling an item in its correct order. Items transition between states in response to changes in item and order information. It is also possible that no change takes place, in which case an item would stay in the same state (not shown). closed-loop learning rule (see Appendix A). Modeling Gains and Losses of Item and Order (GLIO) If a list item is recalled at test, we say that corresponding item information is present, regardless of the recall s output position. If the recall follows the prior list item, then order information is present as well. On each trial, an item may have no corresponding information (if it was omitted), only item information (if it was recalled out of order), or both item and order information. Figure 7 illustrates the possible changes in information between two consecutive trials. Each circle represents an item s state at trial k-1, and following one of the arrows leads to the state at trial k. All items start in state None. It is also possible that there is no change in information, in which case the item stays in the same state. Such self-referencing transitions are not shown in Figure 7, but are valid. For example, if an item is recalled on the first trial in an incorrect output position (using relative order scoring), it follows transition +I and ends up in state Item. Likewise, if an item is recalled in it s correct position, it follows transition +IO and ends up in state Item&Order. If item information, order information, or both are lost in a later trial, the item transitions back to one of its previous states. In all, there are six possible transitions of interest: +IO Gaining item and order information +I Gaining item information, incorrect order information the parametersµ bs andσ bs, which control the strength of re- associations, and the parameter γ, which controls the +O Gaining order information, maintaining item informationmote -IO Losing item and order information frequency with which weakly associated items are selected -I Losing item information, did not have order information for recall. The low value of w b in our simulation, combined -O Losing order information, maintaining item information Figure 8A shows the probability of gaining item and order information together (+IO) at each serial position of the first three trials. The data show a large primacy effect and small Table 3 Probabilities of gaining and losing item and order information Trial Beh. Data Simulation +IO I O IO I O recency effect on Trial 1 (this is equivalent to the Trial 1 serial position curve in Figure 6A). In later trials, a primacy effect is no longer apparent 1 and instead there is an increase for groups of items from progressively later in the list. The model is able to capture the pattern of combined item and order gains of beginning and mid-list items (cf. Figure 8B). As before, the primacy items are learned first because of the interdependent nature of inter-item associations and the gradient in encoding strength. Further strength increments on later trials allow the mid-list items to be learned. The probability of gaining item and order information separately falls below fifteen percent at almost all serial positions. We summarize these results in Table 3, aggregating across all positions. Here we see a trade-off occur over trials, with some items gained out of order in early trials and supplemented with order information in later trials. As shown in Table 3, the model is able to capture the low level of separate item and order gains compared to the higher level of combined gains. The proportion of combined gains relative to separate gains is controlled by the parameter w b, which determines the likelihood of backward transitions, 1 We did not condition on the availability of particular transitions at each serial position and trial, since it would make it appear as if more items are gained in later trials than in earlier ones. For a full discussion, see Addis and Kahana (24).

8 8 SOLWAY, MURDOCK & KAHANA A B Recall probability 1 1 Serial position Trial Figure 8. Simulating combined gains of item and order information across serial positions and trials. A. The probabilities of gaining item and order information together at each serial position on Trials 1-3 of a 19 word list (Kahana & Caplan, 22, Experiment 2). B. Simulated values obtained using the chaining model. Model parameters are given in Table 2. with the high values ofµ bs andσ bs, and the high value ofγ together make combined gains more frequent. The probability of losing information of any type is below five percent at all serial positions, and so we again aggregate the data across all positions and summarize the results in Table 3. Once an item is placed in its correct position, that position is seldom lost. In the model, this corresponds to an item accumulating enough associative strength from its neighbor. The same parameter values that make combined item and order gains most frequent also ensure that there is almost no chance of retrieving a remote item once the associations between nearest neighbors are sufficiently strong. Table 3 shows that item information by itself is also seldom lost and is instead supplemented with order information on a later trial. In the model, the probability of recalling all items is increased on every trial as inter-item associations are reinforced. General Discussion In recalling sequences of items, people frequently remember items out of their correct order. When making these order errors, people show a strong tendency to cluster these incorrectly recalled items near their proper list positions. This positional clustering effect has been well documented in many recall paradigms, and has been used to support the view that people store the position of each list item and then later use that positional information to guide recall. We have shown that an associative chaining model can exhibit significant positional clustering (see Figure 4A-C), providing a good fit to the levels observed in the data. Because the model lacks a positional coding mechanism, its ability to exhibit appropriate levels of positional clustering suggests that the measures of positional and temporal clustering may be confounded. Conditioning these analyses on an incorrect prior recall allowed us to separately examine the effects of positional and temporal clustering. We found that although attenuated, the asymmetrical temporal contiguity effect typically seen in serial recall persists following order errors, whereas the positional clustering effect collapses. After making an error, participants were most likely to recall the item on the list following the just recalled item and not the item that appeared in the next position. Because chaining theory assumes that each recall acts as the retrieval cue for the next position, our model is able to capture these conditional clustering effects (see Figures 4D-F and 5D-F). Although we have not attempted to simulate conditional clustering effects with a positional coding model, it is worth considering how such a model might account for these results. Consider for example a model in which items are bound to a slowly varying list context (e.g., Brown et al., 2; Burgess & Hitch, 1999, 26). At test, the list context is reset to its state at the start of the list as a cue for the first item. Context is then played back to the network, successively cueing the retreiveal of each subsequent item. Because the context signal is temporally autocorrelated, contexts associated with neighboring items will be similar, thereby producing positional gradients. However, unless recall of an item retrieves its own previously associated context, the model will incorrectly predict that the most likely response following an error is the item that belongs in the serial position of the current context cue rather than the serial position of the item that followed the recall error. As our conditional analysis of recall errors reveals, the responses tend to cluster around the position following the just recalled item. The present findings point to a non-positional contiguitybased associative mechanism as the primary factor underlying positional clustering effects in serial recall and in serial learning. We note, however, that if positional cues are re-

9 CLUSTERING IN SERIAL RECALL 9 trieved by items, the distinction between positional and associative models is significantly blurred and both classes of models are likely to be able to explain the positional and temporal clustering effects reported here. References Addis, K. M., & Kahana, M. J. (24). Decomposing serial learning:what is missing from the learning curve? Psychonomic Bulletin& Review, 11, Baddeley, A. D. (1968). Prior recall of newly learned items and the recency effect in free recall. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 22, Bhatarah, P., Ward, G., & Tan, L. (26). Examining the Relationship Between Free Recall and Immediate Serial Recall: The Effect of Concurrent Task Performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(2), Bhatarah, P., Ward, G., & Tan, L. (28). Examining the relationship between free recall and immediate serial recall: The serial nature of recall and the effect of test expectancy. Memory& Cognition, 36(1), Botvinick, M. M., & Plaut, D. C. (26, Apr). Short-term memory for serial order: A recurrent neural network model. Psychological Review, 113(2), Brown, G. D. A., Preece, T., & Hulme, C. (2). Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 17(1), Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 16(3), Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (26). A revised model of short-term memory and long-term learning of verbal sequences. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, Ebbinghaus, H. (1885/1913). On memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University. Estes, W. K. (1972). An associative basis for coding and organization in memory. In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (p ). Washington, D. C.: Winston. Farrell, S., & Lewandowsky, S. (22). An endogenous distributed model of ordering in serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin& Review, 9, Friendly, M., Franklin, P. E., Hoffman, D., & Rubin, D. C. (1982). The Toronto Word Pool: Norms for imagery, concreteness, orthographic variables, and grammatical usage for 1,8 words. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 14, Golomb, J. D., Peelle, J. E., Addis, K. M., Kahana, M. J., & Wingfield, A. (28). Effects of adult aging on utilization of temporal and semantic associations during free and serial recall. Memory & Cognition, 36(5), Henson, R. N. A. (1998). Short-term memory for serial order: The start-end model. Cognitive Psychology, 36, Henson, R. N. A., Norris, D. G., Page, M. P. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Unchained memory: Error patterns rule out chaining models of immediate serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, Jensen, A. R. (1962). An empirical theory of the serial-position effect [Article]. Journal of Psychology, 53(1), Kahana, M. J. (1996). Associative retrieval processes in free recall. Memory&Cognition, 24, Kahana, M. J., & Caplan, J. B. (22). Associative asymmetry in probed recall of serial lists. Memory& Cognition, 3, Kahana, M. J., Mollison, M. V., & Addis, K. M. (21). Positional cues in serial learning: The spin list technique. Positional cues in serial learning: The spin list technique, 38(1), Kimball, D. R., Smith, T. A., & Kahana, M. J. (27). The fsam model of false recall. Psychological Review, 114(4), Klein, K. A., Addis, K. M., & Kahana, M. J. (25). A comparative analysis of serial and free recall. Memory&Cognition, 33, Ladd, G. T., & Woodworth, R. S. (1911). Elements of physiological psychology: A treatise of the activities and nature of the mind from the physical and experimental point of view. New York, NY: Charles Scribner s Sons. Lee, C. L., & Estes, W. K. (1977). Order and position in primary memory for letter strings. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, Lewandowsky, S., & Murdock, B. B. (1989). Memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 96, Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin& Review, 1, Luce, R. D. (1959). Detection and recognition. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (p ). New York: Wiley. Mitchell, M. (1996). An introduction to genetic algorithms. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Nairne, J. S. (1992). Position-based search of associate memory. (Unpublished manuscript) Page, M. P. A., & Norris, D. (1998). The primacy model: A new model of immediate serial recall. Psychological Review, 15, Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (198). SAM: A theory of probabilistic search of associative memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 14, p ). New York: Academic Press. Raskin, E., & Cook, S. W. (1937). The strength and direction of associations formed in the learning of nonsense syllables. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2, Serra, M., & Nairne, J. S. (2, Jul). Part-set cuing of order information: Implications for associative theories of serial order memory. Memory& Cognition, 28(5), Sirotin, Y. B., Kimball, D. R., & Kahana, M. J. (25). Going beyond a single list: Modeling the effects of prior experience on episodic free recall. Psychonomic Bulletin& Review, 12(5), Slamecka, N. J. (1964). An inquiry into the doctrine of remote associations. Psychological Review, 71, Ward, L. B. (1937). Reminiscence and rote learning. Psychological Monographs, 49, 64. Young, R. K. (1968). Serial learning. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory (p ). Englewood Cliffs, N. J: Prentice-Hall. Appendix A Associative Chaining Model In this appendix, we provide a concise description of our strength-based associative chaining model of serial recall and serial learning. Matlab computer code used to run the simulations can be obtained from

10 1 SOLWAY, MURDOCK & KAHANA Study Phase Our chaining model uses two matrices to hold the strengths of associations between items, one for forward associations and one for backward associations. For simplicity we assume that lists words are not semantically related and that there are no associations across lists, and so we set the initial strengths to zero. Similar to the Lewandowsky and Murdock (1989) model, a start marker is used to simulate the way in which participants access the beginning of a list. The start marker acts as list item and is associated with each subsequent item. Following the study of list item i on trial T, we increment the strength of the forward association from the immediately preceding item, i 1, according to the storage equation: F(i 1, i) T = a s [1 F(i 1, i) T 1 ], (1) where F(i 1, i) T 1 is the strength of the association on the previous trial and a s is a random variable (Gaussian with parameters (µ as andσ as ) that controls the change in strength on the current trial. We do not allow the model to unlearn previous strength increments, and so we clamp negative values of a s to zero. We assume a primacy gradient in encoding strength and reduce the mean of a s exponentially across serial positions to an asymptotic minimum of min as : µ as = c s e d s(i 1) + min as, (2) where i indexes the serial position of the item.σ as in Equation 1 and c s, d s, and min as in Equation 2 are model parameters. Equation 1 implements a closed-loop learning rule (e.g. Lewandowsky & Murdock, 1989): the increment in strength is proportional to the amount of association already in memory. The strength of the backward association from item i to item i 1 is computed by scaling the forward increment by w b, a parameter that ranges from to 1: B(i, i 1) T = w b F(i 1, i) T (3) This allows the model to mimic the forward asymmetry that is typical of serial recall (Bhatarah et al., 26, 28; Golomb et al., 28; Klein et al., 25). In addition to forming associations between nearest neighbors, our model also forms remote associations by incrementing the strength between item i and each earlier list item, i x, according to the more general storage equation: F(i x, i) T = a s [1 F(i x, i) T 1 ]e b s x 1 Here, a s is the same as above, and b s is a random variable (Gaussian with parametersµ bs andσ bs ) that determines the strength of remote associations relative to nearest neighbors. The minimum value b s can take is clamped to one.µ bs and σ bs are both model parameters. Note that when x=1, this equation reduces to Equation 1. During list study, a s is sampled once for each item and b s is sampled once for each list. The strength of remote associations is reduced exponentially (4) Table A1 Associative strengths following the first trial study phase of a sample simulation $ A B C D $ A B C D as a function of the lag, x, which ranges from the previously studied item (x=1) to the start marker (x=i). As with nearest neighbor associations, we assume that remote associations are formed in both the forward and backward directions, with the backward strength increment scaled by w b. Test Phase At test, the start marker serves as the first retrieval cue for each list. After the first position, each successfully retrieved item serves as the cue for the next position. Assuming the model recalled item i, the next item, j, is chosen by the Luce choice rule (Luce, 1959): P( j i)= S (i, j) γ k S (i, k) γ + stopγ, (5) where S = F+ B, k ranges over all unrecalled items, and stop sets the probability of retrieval failure. Both stop andγ are model parameters. A Numerical Example Consider the behavior of the model on the first trial of the list A B C D, using the following parameter values: w b =.15, min as =.1, d s =.55, c s =.6,µ bs = 1.2, stop=.2,γ=1. For simplicity, variability of encoding is omitted. All associations are set to zero at the beginning of the simulation. We begin the study phase by applying Equation 2 to determine the increment in associative strength between the start marker and the first item: µ as (1)=.6e.55() +.1=.7, Equation 4 is used to update the strength matrix. Since there is no encoding variability and no prior learning, the increment is exactly.7 in the forward direction and.15 in the backward direction (.7 w b ). The rest of the list is presented in a similar manner, and Table A1 shows the resulting associative strength matrices. Here we combine the forward (F, located in the upper triangle) and backward (B, located in the lower triangle) matrices for ease of presentation. At test, the probability of retrieving item A is computed using Equation 5: P(A $)= =.789

11 CLUSTERING IN SERIAL RECALL 11 The probability of retrieving each of the remaining items is computed in a similar fashion, and an item is selected by sampling from this distribution. If an item is successfully retrieved (i.e. the stop marker is not sampled), the next recall is selected by sampling from the distribution formed by conditioning the remaining items on the first recall. Recall proceeds in this manner until either the stop marker is selected or all of the items are recalled. In the case of choosing the stop marker, the study-test cycle is repeated (for a maximum of ten trials). If the list is correctly recalled, the strength matrix is reset to zero and simulation of the next list begins. Parameter Estimation We used a genetic algorithm (Mitchell, 1996) to find a set of parameters that minimized the mean root-mean squared deviation (RMSD) for the three aspects of the data described in the main text (the clustering measures, the serial position curves, and the gains and losses of item and order information). We first used a population of 2, random parameter sets for ten generations, and then reduced the size to 1, members for twenty more generations. The parameter values shown in Table 2 provided the best fit, and the simulations referenced in the main text were performed using these values. Appendix B Experiment Details Kahana and Caplan (22), Exp. 2. Sixty participants performed the experiment over the course of five sessions. The first session consisted of one 1-word, one 15-word, and two 19-word practice lists, and was not included in our analysis. The remaining four sessions each consisted of five 19- word lists. Words were drawn from the Toronto Word Pool (Friendly, Franklin, Hoffman, & Rubin, 1982) without replacement and presented aurally at a rate of 1.5 sec. After studying a list, participants were asked to vocally recall the list in the presented order. The cycle of study and test was repeated for each list until it was recalled perfectly. In order to avoid edge artifacts, we focused our analysis of the recall data on list positions 4 16 (see main text). Furthermore, in analyzing serial position curves and the gains and losses of item and order information across trials, we focused our attention on the first three trials of each list. Lists that were learned in fewer than three trials (about 17%) were excluded. Kahana et al. (21). Forty-two participants performed the experiment over the course of four sessions. Each list consisted of 7, 13, or 19 words drawn from the Toronto Word Pool (Friendly et al., 1982) without replacement and presented aurally at a rate of 1 sec. At test, participants were given 1 min. to vocally recall the list in the presented order, and the study and test cycle was was repeated for each list until it was recalled perfectly. The experiment consisted of two conditions. In the constant start condition, participants studied each list in the usual manner, starting at the same list position. In the spin-list condition, participants studied each list starting in a random position on each trial. After studying the last list item, participants would then see the first item, and the presentation would continue until reaching the position immediately prior to the start. The first session of the experiment consisted of six practice lists, one for each combination of list length and starting condition, and was not included in our analysis. Subsequent sessions consisted of three lists of each possible length under one of the two starting conditions (the order of conditions was counterbalanced across participants). Four subjects were excluded because they failed to learn any lists within a predetermined maximum number of trials. Our analysis includes only the data from the constant start condition. Furthermore, in order to avoid edge artifacts, we focused our analysis on positions 4 1 for lists of length 13 and positions 4 16 for lists of length 19. We excluded lists of length 7.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Mem Cognit. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 February 20.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Mem Cognit. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 February 20. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Mem Cognit. 2012 February ; 40(2): 177 190. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0142-8. Positional and temporal clustering in serial order memory

More information

Positional cues in serial learning: The spin-list technique

Positional cues in serial learning: The spin-list technique Memory & Cognition 2, 38 (), 92- doi:.3758/mc.38..92 Positional cues in serial learning: The spin-list technique Michael J. Kahana, Matthew V. Mollison, and Kelly M. Addis University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia,

More information

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

The spacing and lag effect in free recall The spacing and lag effect in free recall Michael J. Kahana, Bradley R. Wellington & Marc W. Howard Center for Complex Systems and Department of Psychology Brandeis University Send correspondence to: Michael

More information

City, University of London Institutional Repository

City, University of London Institutional Repository City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Tan, L.H.T., Ward, G. & Grenfell-Essam, R. (2010). Examining the relationship between free recall and immediate serial

More information

Temporal associative processes revealed by intrusions in paired-associate recall

Temporal associative processes revealed by intrusions in paired-associate recall Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 28, 15 (1), 64-69 doi: 1.3758/PBR5.64 Temporal associative processes revealed by intrusions in paired-associate recall Orin C. Davis Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts

More information

Journal of Memory and Language

Journal of Memory and Language Journal of Memory and Language 64 (2011) 249 255 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Memory and Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jml Contextual variability in free recall

More information

The mnemonic effects of recall on immediate retention

The mnemonic effects of recall on immediate retention Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (1), 191-199 The mnemonic effects of recall on immediate retention JAMES S. NAIRNE AND DAVID A. CEO Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana AND MATTHEW B. REYSEN University

More information

Understanding serial position curves in short-term recognition and recall

Understanding serial position curves in short-term recognition and recall Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 469 483 Journal of Memory and Language www.elsevier.com/locate/jml Understanding serial position curves in short-term recognition and recall Klaus Oberauer University

More information

Recall termination in free recall

Recall termination in free recall Mem Cogn (212) 4:54 55 DOI 1.3758/s13421-11-178-9 Recall termination in free recall Jonathan F. Miller & Christoph T. Weidemann & Michael J. Kahana Published online: 31 January 212 # Psychonomic Society,

More information

Further Evidence for a Negative Recency Effect in Free Recall 1

Further Evidence for a Negative Recency Effect in Free Recall 1 JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 9, 554-560 (1970) Further Evidence for a Negative Recency Effect in Free Recall 1 FERGUS I. M. CRAIK, JOHN M. GARDINER, AND MICHAEL J. WATKINS Birkbeck College,

More information

Negative Effects of Using List Items as Recall Cues 1

Negative Effects of Using List Items as Recall Cues 1 JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 12, 43-50 (1973) Negative Effects of Using List Items as Recall Cues 1 DEWEY RUNDUS 2 The Rockefeller University, New York, New York 10021 It is proposed

More information

Variation in Working Memory Capacity and Temporal Contextual Retrieval From Episodic Memory

Variation in Working Memory Capacity and Temporal Contextual Retrieval From Episodic Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2011, Vol. 37, No. 6, 1532 1539 2011 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0024852 Variation in Working

More information

An Account of Associative Learning in Memory Recall

An Account of Associative Learning in Memory Recall An Account of Associative Learning in Memory Recall Robert Thomson (rob.thomson@knexusresearch.com) 1,3 1 Knexus Research Corporation, National Harbor, MD 20745 USA Aryn A Pyke 2 (apyke@andrew.cmu.edu)

More information

Presentation of cues during recall impairs recall of non-cued items (part-set cuing

Presentation of cues during recall impairs recall of non-cued items (part-set cuing GODBOLE, NAMRATA, M.A. Assessing Temporal Associations in Recall: A New Take on the Strategy Disruption Account of the Part-Set Cuing Effect. (2012) Directed by Dr. Peter F. Delaney. 54 pp. Presentation

More information

Chapter 6: Absolute or relative position?

Chapter 6: Absolute or relative position? The nature of positional errors Experiment 2 showed that transpositions between groups tend to maintain the same position within a group. Such interpositions were as common as adjacent transpositions.

More information

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1996, 3 (2), 222 226 Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli ROBERT M. NOSOFSKY Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana and THOMAS J. PALMERI Vanderbilt University,

More information

Memory by Temporal Sampling

Memory by Temporal Sampling Memory by Temporal Sampling Caroline Morin Collaborators: Trevor Penney (Dept. Psychology, National University of Singapore) Stephen Lewandowsky (Dept. Psychology, University of Western Australia) Free

More information

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN FREE RECALL: A PROCEDURE FOR DIRECT OBSERVATION TECHNICAL REPORT NO, 149 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN FREE RECALL: A PROCEDURE FOR DIRECT OBSERVATION TECHNICAL REPORT NO, 149 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN FREE RECALL: A PROCEDURE FOR DIRECT OBSERVATION by Dewey Rundus and Richard C, Atkinson TECHNICAL REPORT NO, 149 August 12, 1969 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES Reproduction in Whole or in Part

More information

An Analysis of the Working Memory Capacity Paradox

An Analysis of the Working Memory Capacity Paradox An Analysis of the Working Memory Capacity Paradox Eddy J. Davelaar (e.davelaar@bbk.ac.uk) Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck College London, WC1E 7HX United Kingdom Abstract In the literature

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report THE LOSS OF POSITIONAL CERTAINTY IN LONG-TERM MEMORY. James S. Nairne Purdue University

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report THE LOSS OF POSITIONAL CERTAINTY IN LONG-TERM MEMORY. James S. Nairne Purdue University Research Report THE LOSS OF POSITIONAL CERTAINTY IN LONG-TERM MEMORY James S. Nairne Purdue University Abstract Long-term memory for sequential position was examined following retention intervals that

More information

Serial position effects in semantic memory: Reconstructing the order of verses of hymns

Serial position effects in semantic memory: Reconstructing the order of verses of hymns Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2002, 9 (4), 816-820 Serial position effects in semantic memory: Reconstructing the order of verses of hymns ELIZABETH A. MAYLOR University of Warwick, Coventry, England Serial

More information

Context Retrieval and Context Change in Free Recall: Recalling From Long-Term Memory Drives List Isolation

Context Retrieval and Context Change in Free Recall: Recalling From Long-Term Memory Drives List Isolation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2008, Vol. 34, No. 1, 112 127 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/08/$12.00 DO: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.112

More information

Associative processes in immediate recency

Associative processes in immediate recency Memory & Cognition 27, 35 (7), 17-1711 Associative processes in immediate recency MARC W. HOWARD AND VIJAY VENKATADASS Syracuse University, Syracuse, New York KENNETH A. NORMAN Princeton University, Princeton,

More information

A Power-Law Model of Psychological Memory Strength in Short- and Long-Term Recognition

A Power-Law Model of Psychological Memory Strength in Short- and Long-Term Recognition Research Article A Power-Law Model of Psychological Memory Strength in Short- and Long-Term Recognition Psychological Science 23(6) 625 634 The Author(s) 2012 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav

More information

Phonological Similarity in Serial Recall: Constraints on Theories of Memory. Stephan Lewandowsky. University of Western Australia. and.

Phonological Similarity in Serial Recall: Constraints on Theories of Memory. Stephan Lewandowsky. University of Western Australia. and. 1 Phonological Similarity in Serial Recall: Constraints on Theories of Memory Stephan Lewandowsky University of Western Australia and Simon Farrell University of Bristol Running head: Address correspondence

More information

RECOGNITION EFFECTS OF STUDY ORGANIZATION AND TEST CONTEXT

RECOGNITION EFFECTS OF STUDY ORGANIZATION AND TEST CONTEXT Journal of Experimental Psychology 1973, Vol. 100, No. 1, 73-82 RECOGNITION EFFECTS OF STUDY ORGANIZATION AND TEST CONTEXT LARRY L. JACOBY i AND REGINALD L. HENDRICKS Iowa State University Effects of study

More information

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination Timothy N. Rubin (trubin@uci.edu) Michael D. Lee (mdlee@uci.edu) Charles F. Chubb (cchubb@uci.edu) Department of Cognitive

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

A spacing account of negative recency

A spacing account of negative recency A spacing account of negative recency Kuhn, J. R. University of California, San Diego Lohnas, L.J. New York University Kahana, M.J. University of Pennsylvania The well-known recency effect in immediate

More information

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES by John W. Brelsford, Jr. and Richard C. Atkinson TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 July 21, 1967 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES!, Reproduction

More information

Semantic similarity and immediate serial recall: Is there an effect on all trials?

Semantic similarity and immediate serial recall: Is there an effect on all trials? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (1), 171-177 Semantic similarity and immediate serial recall: Is there an effect on all trials? JEAN SAINT-AUBIN and DENIS OUELLETTE Université de Moncton, Moncton,

More information

Running head: RECENCY IN SERIAL RECALL 1. Response suppression contributes to recency in serial recall. Simon Farrell

Running head: RECENCY IN SERIAL RECALL 1. Response suppression contributes to recency in serial recall. Simon Farrell Running head: RECENCY IN SERIAL RECALL 1 Response suppression contributes to recency in serial recall Simon Farrell University of Bristol and University of Western Australia Stephan Lewandowsky University

More information

Attentional Theory Is a Viable Explanation of the Inverse Base Rate Effect: A Reply to Winman, Wennerholm, and Juslin (2003)

Attentional Theory Is a Viable Explanation of the Inverse Base Rate Effect: A Reply to Winman, Wennerholm, and Juslin (2003) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2003, Vol. 29, No. 6, 1396 1400 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.6.1396

More information

Local temporal distinctiveness does not benefit auditory verbal and spatial serial recall

Local temporal distinctiveness does not benefit auditory verbal and spatial serial recall Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 3?? (3), (?), 458-465???-??? Local temporal distinctiveness does not benefit auditory verbal and spatial serial recall FABRICE B. R. PARMENTIER, SUZANNE KING,

More information

Part set cuing of order information: Implications for associative theories of serial order memory

Part set cuing of order information: Implications for associative theories of serial order memory Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 847-855 Part set cuing of order information: Implications for associative theories of serial order memory MATT SERRA Duke University, Durham, North Carolina and JAMES S.

More information

AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the

AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the U.S. Office of Naval Research 1 Introduction During

More information

Cognitive modeling versus game theory: Why cognition matters

Cognitive modeling versus game theory: Why cognition matters Cognitive modeling versus game theory: Why cognition matters Matthew F. Rutledge-Taylor (mrtaylo2@connect.carleton.ca) Institute of Cognitive Science, Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Drive Ottawa,

More information

Learning to Use Episodic Memory

Learning to Use Episodic Memory Learning to Use Episodic Memory Nicholas A. Gorski (ngorski@umich.edu) John E. Laird (laird@umich.edu) Computer Science & Engineering, University of Michigan 2260 Hayward St., Ann Arbor, MI 48109 USA Abstract

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psych.colorado.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z

Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 10, 522-527 (1971) Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z JAMES F. JUOLA AND R. C. ATKINSON Stanford University, Stanford, California 94305 Two groups

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Proactive interference plays a role in the word-length effect

Proactive interference plays a role in the word-length effect Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (4), 541-545 Proactive interference plays a role in the word-length effect JAMES S. NAIRNE and IAN NEATH Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana and MATT SERRA

More information

Cuing Effects in Short-term Recall. Gerald Tehan. University of Southern Queensland. Michael S. Humphreys. University of Queensland

Cuing Effects in Short-term Recall. Gerald Tehan. University of Southern Queensland. Michael S. Humphreys. University of Queensland Cuing Effects in Short-term Recall Cuing in Short-term Recall 1 Gerald Tehan University of Southern Queensland Michael S. Humphreys University of Queensland Mailing Address: Gerry Tehan Faculty of Sciences

More information

Technical Specifications

Technical Specifications Technical Specifications In order to provide summary information across a set of exercises, all tests must employ some form of scoring models. The most familiar of these scoring models is the one typically

More information

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task Neath, I., & Brown, G. D. A. (2005). Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task. Memory Lab Technical Report 2005-01, Purdue University. Scale Invariance and Primacy

More information

The Feature-Label-Order Effect In Symbolic Learning

The Feature-Label-Order Effect In Symbolic Learning The Feature-Label-Order Effect In Symbolic Learning Michael Ramscar, Daniel Yarlett, Melody Dye, & Nal Kalchbrenner Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Jordan Hall, Stanford, CA 94305. Abstract

More information

End anchoring in short-term order memory. Simon Farrell and Anna Lelièvre. Department of Psychology. University of Bristol

End anchoring in short-term order memory. Simon Farrell and Anna Lelièvre. Department of Psychology. University of Bristol End anchoring in STM Running head: END ANCHORING IN STM End anchoring in short-term order memory Simon Farrell and Anna Lelièvre Department of Psychology University of Bristol Draft of September, 008.

More information

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES Journal of Experimental Vol. 51, No. 4, 1956 Psychology CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES KENNETH H. KURTZ AND CARL I. HOVLAND Under conditions where several concepts are learned concurrently

More information

Mental operations on number symbols by-children*

Mental operations on number symbols by-children* Memory & Cognition 1974, Vol. 2,No. 3, 591-595 Mental operations on number symbols by-children* SUSAN HOFFMAN University offlorida, Gainesville, Florida 32601 TOM TRABASSO Princeton University, Princeton,

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society Title Effects of Presentation Format on Memory for Order Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/64h487x3 Journal Proceedings

More information

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Michael Romano (mrr2@nyu.edu) Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place New York, NY 1000 USA Abstract Traditional

More information

Recency order judgments in short term memory: Replication and extension of Hacker (1980)

Recency order judgments in short term memory: Replication and extension of Hacker (1980) Recency order judgments in short term memory: Replication and extension of Hacker (1980) Inder Singh and Marc W. Howard Center for Memory and Brain Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences Boston

More information

Feature encoding and pattern classifications with sequentially presented Markov stimuli*

Feature encoding and pattern classifications with sequentially presented Markov stimuli* Feature encoding and pattern classifications with sequentially presented Markov stimuli* BLL R. BROWN and CHARLES E. AYLWORTH University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky 008 The major objective of this

More information

Single Parameter Model for Free Recall And the Nature of Partially Filled Working Memory. Abstract:

Single Parameter Model for Free Recall And the Nature of Partially Filled Working Memory. Abstract: Single Parameter Model for Free Recall And the Nature of Partially Filled Working Memory Eugen Tarnow, Ph.D. 8- Radburn Road Fair Lawn, NJ 74 etarnow@avabiz.com (e-mail) Abstract: I present a single parameter

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Individual Differences in Memory Search and Their Relation to Intelligence M. Karl Healey, Patrick Crutchley, and Michael J. Kahana Online First Publication,

More information

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Automatic detection - 1 Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1986, 24 (6), 431-434 SIU L. CHOW The University of Wollongong,

More information

Memory (1) Visual Sensory Store. Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory

Memory (1) Visual Sensory Store. Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory Memory (1) Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory Visual Sensory Store It appears that our visual system is able to hold a great deal of information but that if we do not attend to this information

More information

Psychology 2019 v1.3. IA2 high-level annotated sample response. Student experiment (20%) August Assessment objectives

Psychology 2019 v1.3. IA2 high-level annotated sample response. Student experiment (20%) August Assessment objectives Student experiment (20%) This sample has been compiled by the QCAA to assist and support teachers to match evidence in student responses to the characteristics described in the instrument-specific marking

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Individual Differences in Attention During Category Learning

Individual Differences in Attention During Category Learning Individual Differences in Attention During Category Learning Michael D. Lee (mdlee@uci.edu) Department of Cognitive Sciences, 35 Social Sciences Plaza A University of California, Irvine, CA 92697-5 USA

More information

Free Recall Shows Similar Reactivation Behavior as Recognition & Cued Recall. Abstract:

Free Recall Shows Similar Reactivation Behavior as Recognition & Cued Recall. Abstract: Free Recall Shows Similar Reactivation Behavior as Recognition & Cued Recall Eugen Tarnow, Ph.D. 18-11 Radburn Road Fair Lawn, NJ 741 etarnow@avabiz.com (e-mail) 1646229787 (phone) 128 44 3638 (fax) Abstract:

More information

Remarks on Bayesian Control Charts

Remarks on Bayesian Control Charts Remarks on Bayesian Control Charts Amir Ahmadi-Javid * and Mohsen Ebadi Department of Industrial Engineering, Amirkabir University of Technology, Tehran, Iran * Corresponding author; email address: ahmadi_javid@aut.ac.ir

More information

Interference and the representation of order within associations

Interference and the representation of order within associations THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2011, 64 (7), 1409 1429 Interference and the representation of order within associations Mayank Rehani 1 and Jeremy B. Caplan 2 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

Grouping of list items reflected in the timing of recall: implications for models of serial verbal memory

Grouping of list items reflected in the timing of recall: implications for models of serial verbal memory Journal of Memory and Language 47 (2002) 360 385 Journal of Memory and Language www.academicpress.com Grouping of list items reflected in the timing of recall: implications for models of serial verbal

More information

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Unsworth, Nash] On: 13 January 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 918847458] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

An Ideal Observer Model of Visual Short-Term Memory Predicts Human Capacity Precision Tradeoffs

An Ideal Observer Model of Visual Short-Term Memory Predicts Human Capacity Precision Tradeoffs An Ideal Observer Model of Visual Short-Term Memory Predicts Human Capacity Precision Tradeoffs Chris R. Sims Robert A. Jacobs David C. Knill (csims@cvs.rochester.edu) (robbie@bcs.rochester.edu) (knill@cvs.rochester.edu)

More information

Associative Retrieval Processes in Episodic Memory

Associative Retrieval Processes in Episodic Memory Associative Retrieval Processes in Episodic Memory Michael J. Kahana Department of Psychology University of Pennsylvania Marc W. Howard Department of Psychology Syracuse University Sean M. Polyn Department

More information

Chapter 5 Short-term/Working Memory

Chapter 5 Short-term/Working Memory Chapter 5 Short-term/Working Memory Original Information Processing Model rehearsal transfer Sensory Memory Working Memory Long-term Memory input from the world attention retrieval Characterizing Memories

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy 2.26 Associative Retrieval Processes in Episodic Memory M. J. Kahana, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA M. W. Howard, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY, USA S. M. Polyn, University of Pennsylvania,

More information

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience Working memory 1 What is working memory? Brief, immediate memory for information we are currently processing. Closely related to attention: attending to something is often

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Lec 02: Estimation & Hypothesis Testing in Animal Ecology

Lec 02: Estimation & Hypothesis Testing in Animal Ecology Lec 02: Estimation & Hypothesis Testing in Animal Ecology Parameter Estimation from Samples Samples We typically observe systems incompletely, i.e., we sample according to a designed protocol. We then

More information

Dissimilar Items Benefit From Phonological Similarity in Serial Recall

Dissimilar Items Benefit From Phonological Similarity in Serial Recall Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2003, Vol. 29, No. 5, 838 849 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.5.838

More information

Cerebral Cortex. Edmund T. Rolls. Principles of Operation. Presubiculum. Subiculum F S D. Neocortex. PHG & Perirhinal. CA1 Fornix CA3 S D

Cerebral Cortex. Edmund T. Rolls. Principles of Operation. Presubiculum. Subiculum F S D. Neocortex. PHG & Perirhinal. CA1 Fornix CA3 S D Cerebral Cortex Principles of Operation Edmund T. Rolls F S D Neocortex S D PHG & Perirhinal 2 3 5 pp Ento rhinal DG Subiculum Presubiculum mf CA3 CA1 Fornix Appendix 4 Simulation software for neuronal

More information

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Yingxue Wang, Sandro Romani, Brian Lustig, Anthony Leonardo, Eva Pastalkova Supplementary Materials Supplementary Modeling

More information

Altering the flow of mental time: A test of retrieved-context theory. Sean M. Polyn, James E. Kragel, Joshua D. McCluey. Vanderbilt University

Altering the flow of mental time: A test of retrieved-context theory. Sean M. Polyn, James E. Kragel, Joshua D. McCluey. Vanderbilt University Variable distraction 1 Running head: VARIABLE DISTRACTION Altering the flow of mental time: A test of retrieved-context theory Sean M. Polyn, James E. Kragel, Joshua D. McCluey Vanderbilt University John

More information

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Ethan D. Montag Rochester Institute of Technology Munsell Color Science Laboratory Chester F. Carlson Center for Imaging Science

More information

No Evidence for Feature Overwriting in Visual Working Memory. Zurich c. Technische Universität Dresden. Fetscherstraße Dresden.

No Evidence for Feature Overwriting in Visual Working Memory. Zurich c. Technische Universität Dresden. Fetscherstraße Dresden. 1 This is a preprint of an article whose final and definitive form will be published in Memory (in press). DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2013.794243 Copyright. Memory (Taylor & Francis Group). This article may

More information

MBios 478: Systems Biology and Bayesian Networks, 27 [Dr. Wyrick] Slide #1. Lecture 27: Systems Biology and Bayesian Networks

MBios 478: Systems Biology and Bayesian Networks, 27 [Dr. Wyrick] Slide #1. Lecture 27: Systems Biology and Bayesian Networks MBios 478: Systems Biology and Bayesian Networks, 27 [Dr. Wyrick] Slide #1 Lecture 27: Systems Biology and Bayesian Networks Systems Biology and Regulatory Networks o Definitions o Network motifs o Examples

More information

Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs

Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs Perception is the conscious experience of things and

More information

How should you study for Friday's exam?

How should you study for Friday's exam? How should you study for Friday's exam? re-read textbook? re-read lecture slides? study guide? NO! these are passive. Use active study. Test yourself by Take the practice quizzes in Moodle Create your

More information

Recognizing Scenes by Simulating Implied Social Interaction Networks

Recognizing Scenes by Simulating Implied Social Interaction Networks Recognizing Scenes by Simulating Implied Social Interaction Networks MaryAnne Fields and Craig Lennon Army Research Laboratory, Aberdeen, MD, USA Christian Lebiere and Michael Martin Carnegie Mellon University,

More information

What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of

What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of Memories AIM:Bartlett aimed to investigate the effect of culture on memory. memory ability/ability to recall and have advantageous effects if asked to do a

More information

INTRODUCTION METHODS

INTRODUCTION METHODS INTRODUCTION Deficits in working memory (WM) and attention have been associated with aphasia (Heuer & Hallowell, 2009; Hula & McNeil, 2008; Ivanova & Hallowell, 2011; Murray, 1999; Wright & Shisler, 2005).

More information

CONTEXTUAL ASSOCIATIONS AND MEMORY FOR SERIAL POSITION 1

CONTEXTUAL ASSOCIATIONS AND MEMORY FOR SERIAL POSITION 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology 1973, Vol. 97, No. 2, 220-229 CONTEXTUAL ASSOCIATIONS AND MEMORY FOR SERIAL POSITION 1 DOUGLAS L. HINTZMAN," RICHARD A. BLOCK, AND JEFFERY J. SUMMERS University of Oregon

More information

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge Kimery R. Levering (kleveri1@binghamton.edu) Kenneth J. Kurtz (kkurtz@binghamton.edu) Department of Psychology, Binghamton

More information

A Context Maintenance and Retrieval Model of Organizational Processes in Free Recall

A Context Maintenance and Retrieval Model of Organizational Processes in Free Recall Psychological Review 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 116, No. 1, 129 156 0033-295X/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014420 A Context Maintenance and Retrieval Model of Organizational Processes

More information

Introduction to Long-Term Memory

Introduction to Long-Term Memory Introduction to Long-Term Memory Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/26/2018: Lecture 05-4 Note: This Powerpoint presentation may contain macros that I wrote to help me create

More information

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept Matthew D. Zeigenfuse (mzeigenf@msu.edu) Department of Psychology, Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48823 USA

More information

THEORETICAL AND REVIEW ARTICLES. Time does not cause forgetting in short-term serial recall

THEORETICAL AND REVIEW ARTICLES. Time does not cause forgetting in short-term serial recall Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 771-790 THEORETICAL AND REVIEW ARTICLES Time does not cause forgetting in short-term serial recall STEPHAN LEWANDOWSKY and MATTHEW DUNCAN University of Western

More information

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Anna SORKIN¹, Avi PELED 2, Daphna WEINSHALL¹ 1 Interdisciplinary Center for Neural Computation, Hebrew University of Jerusalem,

More information

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review submission. For Review Only

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review submission. For Review Only Page of 0 0 Long Term Recency Running head: LONG TERM RECENCY IS JUST ORDINARY FORGETTING Long Term Recency Is Nothing More Than Ordinary Forgetting Robert V. Lindsey University of Colorado, Boulder Department

More information

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) 1 Outline Method of Adjustment Method of Limits Method of Constant Stimuli Probit Analysis 2 Method of Constant Stimuli A set of equally spaced levels of the stimulus

More information

Human memory reconsolidation can be explained using the Temporal Context Model

Human memory reconsolidation can be explained using the Temporal Context Model 1 Human memory reconsolidation can be explained using the Temporal Context Model Per B. Sederberg Department of Psychology The Ohio State University Columbus, OH 43210, USA Samuel J. Gershman Department

More information

Modelling serial position curves with temporal distinctiveness

Modelling serial position curves with temporal distinctiveness MEMORY, 2001, 9 (4/5/6), 301 311 Modelling serial position curves with temporal distinctiveness Jeffrey N. Rouder University of Missouri-Columbia, USA Pablo Gomez Northwestern University, USA We offer

More information

An Activation-Based Model of Routine Sequence Errors

An Activation-Based Model of Routine Sequence Errors An Activation-Based Model of Routine Sequence Errors Laura M. Hiatt (laura.hiatt@nrl.navy.mil) J. Gregory Trafton (greg.trafton@nrl.navy.mil) U.S. Naval Research Laboratory Washington, DC 20375 USA Abstract

More information

Retrieval dynamics in self-terminated memory search

Retrieval dynamics in self-terminated memory search THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 Vol. 67, No. 2, 394 416, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.816750 Retrieval dynamics in self-terminated memory search Erika K. Hussey 1, Michael

More information