Affective Interference: An Explanation for Negative Attention Biases in Dysphoria?

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Affective Interference: An Explanation for Negative Attention Biases in Dysphoria?"

Transcription

1 Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 26, No. 1, February 2002 ( C 2002), pp Affective Interference: An Explanation for Negative Attention Biases in Dysphoria? Greg J. Siegle, 1,4 Rick E. Ingram, 2,3 and Georg E. Matt 3 Research suggests that individuals with features of depression pay excessive attention to negative information. Yet, it is unclear what aspects of negative information are attended to by these individuals. Different answers to this question suggest different roles for attention in the onset and maintenance of depressive states. This study investigated aspects of emotional information to which college students with and without features of depression attend. Research participants completed an affective lexical decision task and an affective valence identification task. Dysphoric individuals were slow to identify the emotional valence of positive information and nonemotional aspects of negative information (the lexicality of negative words), but were not slow to identify the emotional valence of negative words. An affective-interference hypothesis is advanced to explain these results. Dysphoric individuals are proposed to attend to the emotional content of negative information at the expense of attending to other aspects of the information. Results are related to theories of ruminative coping with depression. KEY WORDS: depression; information processing; attention; rumination. Attention to emotional information is central to many theories of the onset and maintenance of depression (e.g., A. T. Beck, 1967, 1974; Ingram, 1984; Ingram, Miranda, & Segal, 1998). Support for these theories is found in literature suggesting that dysphoric and depressed individuals disproportionately attend to and remember negative information (Blaney, 1986; Matt, Vazquez, & Campbell, 1992; Matthews & Harley, 1996; Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996). Yet, it is unclear to which aspects of negative information dysphoric and depressed individuals attend. It is also unclear whether biased attention to negative information occurs in the early 1 University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine and VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System, Pittsburgh, PA. 2 Southern Methodist University. 3 San Diego State University, San Diego, California. 4 Correspondence should be directed to Greg Siegle, PhD, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, 3811 O Hara St., Pittsburgh, PA 15213; gsiegle+@ pitt.edu /02/ /0 C 2002 Plenum Publishing Corporation

2 74 Siegle, Ingram, and Matt stages of attention, having to do with initial perceptions of information, or in late stages of attention, involving retrieval of associations from memory (Macleod & Mathews, 1991). Clearly specifying these aspects of the relationship between mood and attention to emotional information may lead to a better understanding of the role of information processing in affective psychopathology. This paper will focus specifically on attention biases in individuals who are dysphoric (have sad mood states thought to underlie depression) rather than individuals diagnosed with clinical depression to avoid confounding features associated with clinical diagnoses (Persons, 1986). Theories of emotional information processing from cognitive and physiological psychology may help to resolve ambiguities regarding the roles of cognitive and emotional information processing in dysphoric individuals. They suggest that emotional aspects of information (e.g., whether it is positive or negative; e.g., the notion that birthday is positive) and semantic (conceptual, nonemotional) aspects of the same information (e.g., the notion that birthday is the day on which one is born) can be processed in parallel by different highly interconnected physiological (LeDoux, 1996; Tucker & Derryberry, 1992) and cognitive (Bower, 1981; Ingram, 1984) systems. Attention can be differentially allocated to these aspects of information (Kitayama, 1990; Matthews & Harley, 1996). If emotional and nonemotional aspects of information are processed in parallel, then information-processing biases could occur as a function of attention to affective aspects of information, nonaffective aspects of information, or feedback between systems responsible for representing affective and nonaffective aspects of information. Depending on how much feedback occurs between the systems, attending to the affect associated with information could interfere with normal semantic associations (e.g., Siegle, 1999). This phenomenon will be referred to as affective interference. Affective interference might involve responding to the word death by immediately recognizing that the stimulus is negative and then thinking of other negative things unrelated to death such as one s low self-image. Attention to the emotional and nonemotional aspects of information can be examined separately by observing how quickly individuals respond to questions that direct their attention to relevant features of a stimulus. Reaction times have long been assumed to reflect the amount of effort an individual pays to information. Thus, longer reaction times are traditionally associated with paying less attention to a task (Massaro, 1988). By comparing individuals reaction times to questions regarding emotional and nonemotional aspects of a stimulus, relative attentional allocation to each of these components can be measured. Two reaction time tasks can thus be used to determine whether attending to emotional aspects of information impairs attention to nonemotional aspects of information. A valence identification task requires participants to identify the emotion associated with positive, negative, and neutral information. It thus directs participants attention towards the emotional aspects of stimuli. Participants reaction times are presumably related to how much attention they pay to emotional aspects of information. In contrast, an affective lexical decision task requires participants to judge whether a string of letters spells a word that may be positive, negative, or

3 Affective Interference in Dysphoria 75 neutral. This task directs participants attention towards nonemotional aspects of information. If dysphoric individuals attend to, and make associations with, emotional aspects of negative information at the expense of attending to its nonemotional aspects, they would be expected to respond especially quickly to negative information on a valence identification task. Because of affective interference they would be expected to react especially slowly to negative information on a lexical decision task. Feedback between structures responsible for processing affective and nonaffective features could exaggerate these biases. For example, a dysphoric individual who attends to negative aspects of information may respond to the typically positive stimulus birthday by associating it with the (semantic) belief I am getting old. This thought may trigger the person s recognition of negativity. The dysphoric individual may thus be especially slow to identify positive information as positive but, by the same token, be very quick to identify negative information as negative. Put another way, research has long suggested that the more meaningful information is to an individual, the more deeply it is processed (e.g., Craik & Lockhart, 1972). If dysphoric individuals are especially well-practiced at identifying negativity or find negative aspects of information to be particularly meaningful, they may process emotional aspects more deeply than semantic aspects. Such a strategy could allow affective aspects of information to interfere with perception of other (e.g., semantic) aspects of that information. Although the combination of these tasks is fairly novel, both tasks have been examined in the past. Valence identification tasks have been used with anxious individuals (Hill & Kemp-Wheeler, 1989; Mathews & Milroy, 1994) and with threat words with depressed populations (Hill & Kemp-Wheeler, 1989) but not with depressed or dysphoric individuals using positive, negative, and neutral words as stimuli. In contrast, a number of researchers have employed affective lexical decision tasks with nondysphoric, dysphoric, and depressed individuals. Most of these researchers predicted that depressed and dysphoric individuals would respond more quickly to negative words than to nonnegative words on this task (e.g., Challis & Krane, 1988; Macleod, Mathews, & Tata, 1986; Matthews & Southall, 1991; Ruiz Caballero & Bermudez Moreno, 1992). This hypothesis was not confirmed for any of these studies. Moreover, when the studies are looked at together, as summarized in Table I, a conclusion consistent with the notion of affective interference emerges (see Siegle, 1996, for a complete review of these data). A literature search revealed seven lexical decision task studies of nondepressed individuals and four studies involving depressed individuals, in which priming was not employed, 5 and in which reaction times to negative and neutral words were reported. No studies observed a significant difference in nondepressed individuals reactions to negative and neutral words. In contrast, all studies of depressed individuals suggested that they were slightly slower to respond to negative than to neutral words (meta-analytic CI = ms). 5 Priming studies were not included because evidence suggests that priming may statistically interact with affective recognition (Conway & Bekerian, 1987; Matthews & Southall, 1991).

4 76 Siegle, Ingram, and Matt Table I. Negative Reaction Times Neutral Reaction Times (D) for Nondepressed and Depressed Individuals, in Milliseconds for Published Affective Lexical Decision Tasks Study Nondepressed Depressed Bradley, Mogg, and Williams (1994) 5 3 Bray (1988) 10 Challis and Krane (1988) 62 Macleod, Tata, and Mathews (1987) Matthews and Southall (1991) SOA = 1500 ms: 18 SOA = 1500 ms: 76 SOA = 240 ms: 29 SOA = 240 ms: 50 Matthews, Pitcaithly, and Mann (1995) SOA = 1500 ms: 13 SOA = 240 ms: 14 Stip and Lecours (1992) and Stip, Lecours, 8 30 Chertkow, Elie, and O Cannor (1994) Williamson, Harpur, and Hare (1991) 46 Meta-analytically derived mean effect size a M D = 2.07, SD D = 4.21 M D = 17.49, SD D = 7.77 a Weighted average effect sizes were calculated using the inverse of the sampling difference variance for weights (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). This technique gives higher weights to studies with larger sample sizes and lower difference variances. Although this result is based on studies in which a variance was reported, results did not change appreciably when a sampling difference variance was estimated for studies in which none was provided. The two conditions from the Matthews and Southhall and Matthews et al. studies (1991, 1995) were counted as different experiments. Thus, these studies generally show the opposite pattern from what is usually predicted. Depressed individuals react more slowly to negative than to neutral words, whereas nondepressed individuals do not. This finding is consistent with the idea that the affect associated with negative information interferes with the recognition of nonaffective aspects of negative information. The affective lexical decision and valence identification tasks are probably not pure uncorrelated measures of affective and nonaffective processing. Rather, it is predicted that dysphoric individuals will be faster to name the valence of negative than positive or neutral information, but will be slower to name the lexicality of negative words because of feedback between systems involved in affective and semantic determination. To test these hypotheses, the current experiment contrasted the performance of dysphoric and nondysphoric individuals on a lexical decision and valence identification task. By analyzing the tasks separately, it can be determined whether the general patterns of interest are present in the data. By correlating performance on the tasks a determination of whether increasing reaction times on one task are associated with decreasing reaction times on the other can be made. METHOD Research Participants A multiple-gating procedure was used for selection of research participants to ensure that they had a stable mood over a number of weeks. Research participants were selected from a pool of over 3000 students in an introductory psychology course at San Diego State University. Dysphoric individuals were selected for having a Beck

5 Affective Interference in Dysphoria 77 Depression Inventory (BDI; A. T. Beck, 1967) score of over 15 (a score indicating moderate depressive symptomatology). Nondysphoric individuals were selected for having a BDI score under 6 at a mass screening at the beginning of the semester. Participants received class credit for their participation. In total, 138 students participated in the experiment, at least 2 weeks after the mass screening. In the dysphoric group, 30 participants had BDI scores over 16 when they came in for testing (M = 22.8, SD = 4.11) and were used in subsequent analyses. The increased cut-off was adopted to reflect the highest possible level of depressive severity upon testing that preserved an adequate sample size, derived through power analyses. Dysphoric participants included 10 men and 20 women of whom 7 identified as Asian, 14 as Caucasian, 7 as Hispanic, and 1 as having another ethnicity. In the nondysphoric group, 46 of the students who had BDI scores between 1 and 6 at the screening were still in this range at testing (M = 3.00, SD = 1.63) and were thus used in further analyses. They included 21 men and 25 women of whom 1 was identified as African American, 6 as Asian, 22 as Caucasian, 11 as Hispanic, and 1 as having another ethnicity. Distributions of ethnicity were not significantly different between the groups, χ 2 (4) = 2.85, p =.65. Measures The BDI was used to assess depressive symptomatology. The BDI is a 21-item self-report inventory. Individual items are worded so that responses reflect increasing degrees of severity and are given the values of 0 3. The total possible score ranges from 0 to 63. The inventory is frequently used to assess depressive symptoms in college populations. It has acceptable validity and reliability (A. T. Beck, 1967; A. Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). The BDI was chosen rather than its successor, the BDI-II, as the study was begun before the BDI II was published. Stimuli for a lexical decision and valence identification task were displayed on an IBM PC compatible 486 computer with a 14-in. color monitor. Research participants sat approximately 28 in. from the bottom of the stimulus. Stimuli were drawn in lowercase letters approximately 5/8 in. high on the monitor, subtending approximately 1.21 of visual angle. For both computer administered tasks two sets of 10 positive, 10 negative, and 10 neutral words balanced for normed affect, word frequency, and word length were chosen from Siegle s corpus (Siegle, 1994). This corpus was compiled from published studies that employed normed word lists and pilot data collected for this experiment. Examples of positive words include happy and bliss. Examples of negative words include hopeless and ashamed. Examples of neutral words include library and slope. The positivity and negativity of word on the first word list were rated by 530 undergraduates. Analysis showed that the each of the word groups strongly and significantly differed from each other in both positivity and negativity ratings. Dysphoric and nondysphoric undergraduates did not tend to differ in their valence ratings (Williams, Conner, Siegle, Ingram, & Cole, 1998). In that sample negative words were rated farther from neutral words than neutral words were from positive words, on negativity. Likewise, positive words were rated farther from neutral words than neutral words were from negative, on positivity, suggesting

6 78 Siegle, Ingram, and Matt that valences differed in the expected direction and magnitude. 6 For the lexical decision task, 15 nonwords consisting of single letter perturbations of equal numbers of normed positive, negative, and neutral words, were also included (e.g., cousip, mendion ). For the lexical decision task, a row of 11 Xs was present on the screen at the beginning of the each presentation. After 2000 ms, the Xs in the middle of the string were replaced by letters spelling a word or nonword, with the Xs on either side remaining on the screen. After the stimulus duration, the letters were again masked by Xs. The question Is it a word? appeared on the screen. The same procedure was used for the valence identification task except that participants responded to the question What s the Valence? Determination of Stimulus Duration To increase the chances that internal feedback processes would be engaged during stimulus recognition, leading to affective interference, stimuli were slightly obscured by using a short stimulus duration. Just the same, shorter stimulus durations were assumed to add more noise (i.e., random variation) to reaction times because of incurred perceptual difficulties. To balance these concerns, pilot data was collected using a stimulus duration of 150 ms, which revealed relevant effects; this duration was thus chosen for the experiment. As a sensitivity check, two shorter durations (100 ms, 50 ms) were also included to make sure that there would be some duration short enough to allow cognitive effort sufficient to reveal biases. These conditions were only to be analyzed as a way of understanding whether stimulus duration contributed to null effects, if they were obtained. Procedure Potential research participants were invited by phone to participate in an experiment involving the perception of words. After obtaining informed consent, directions for the lexical decision task and valence identification tasks were shown to research participants on the computer. Research participants were then shown the directions again for the first task that they would complete. Participants were given a practice session consisting of three trials that were repeated until they were correctly performed. After querying for any questions, participants completed the first task. The identical procedure was used for the second task. In both tasks each stimulus (10 positive words, 10 neutral words, 10 negative words, 15 nonwords) was presented at stimulus durations of 50, 100, and 150 ms for a total of 135 trials. 6 The selection of words for the tasks was done using a computer program (Siegle, 1994) designed to create word lists of an arbitrary length balanced for normed affect, word frequency, and word length. To determine whether word valence covaried with other characteristics of the chosen stimulus words, two analyses were performed. A 3 (valence: positive, negative, neutral) 2 (word list: first, second) analysis of variance (ANOVA) with word frequency revealed no statistically significant main effects or interactions (F < 1.7 for all tests). Similarly, a 3 (valence: positive, negative, neutral) 2 (word list: first, second) ANOVA with word length revealed no statistically significant main effects or interactions (F < 1.5 for all tests). The list generating program and word sets that were used are available from the first author upon request.

7 Affective Interference in Dysphoria 79 The lexical decision task was conducted in the following manner. The research participant was asked to place his or her right palm below the keyboard with the index finger poised above the space bar, equidistant from the N and M keys. Each word and nonword was presented to the participant as described previously, at each stimulus duration. For each stimulus the research participant pushed a buttons for Yes or No, labels for which were placed over the N and M keys on the keyboard. The research participant s reaction time and response was recorded on the computer for each stimulus. The valence identification task was conducted in the following manner. The research participant was asked to place his or her right palm below the keyboard with the index finger poised above the space bar, equidistant from the Z, X, and C keys. Each word was presented at each stimulus duration to the participant as described previously. In response to each word the research participant pushed buttons labeled +,, or N (standing for Positive, Negative, or Neutral, respectively), which were placed over the Z, X, and C keys. Different keys were used for the different tasks so that all keys could be labeled at the beginning of the experiment. After the completion of both tasks, the BDI was administered on a computer, and participants were debriefed. The order in which tasks were completed, the order in which buttons for responses on the keyboard were labeled for the tasks, and the assignment of which word list was used for which task were counterbalanced using a strategy that ensured a balanced sampling of all possible combinations as the number of participants increased. The order of words within each task, and the order in which a given word was shown in a given stimulus duration were assigned pseudorandomly by computer (i.e., random to the extent possible using a computer-based random number generator) for each participant. RESULTS Mean reaction times to each valence and stimulus duration on each task were calculated. Reaction times below 150 ms were discarded as outliers because previous results suggest that reaction times in this range indicate that a response was made without regard for the stimulus (Matthews & Southall, 1991). Similarly, responses over 5000 ms were discarded as outliers under the assumption that such long response latencies indicated inattention to the trial. This relatively long cut-off was adopted because the magnitude of valid delays in reaction times because of interference effects was unclear a priori. Reaction times to stimuli that were incorrectly identified on the lexical decision task, and reaction times to stimuli whose valence was identified as incongruent with the normed valence on the valence identification task were not removed from the computation of means. Reaction times on the valence identification task were not significantly skewed, skew = 0.40, SE skew = Although reaction times on the lexical decision task were more skewed, skew = 1.90, SE skew = 0.277, no normalization transformation was applied to allow comparisons between effects on the tasks and with other relevant literature. One nondysphoric participant s valence identification task data were corrupted and therefore not included in analyses or

8 80 Siegle, Ingram, and Matt Table II. Mean Reaction Times (in Milliseconds) for the 150-ms Condition for NonDysphoric (N lexical decision = 46, N valence identification = 45) and Dysphoric (N = 30) Individuals Nondysphoric Dysphoric Task Valence M SD M SD Lexical decision Positive Negative Neutral Nonword Valence identification Positive Negative Neutral the table. The performance of dysphoric and nondysphoric individuals on the lexical decision and valence identification tasks are shown in Table II. Planned contrasts were performed to test the following hypotheses: (1) that dysphoric people would respond faster to negative words than to nonnegative words on the valence identification task, (2) that dysphoric people would respond slower to negative than to nonnegative words on the lexical decision task, and (3) that differences in reaction times to negative and nonnegative words would be greater for dysphoric than for nondysphoric people. Based on pilot studies, the 150-ms condition was hypothesized to be fast enough to necessitate cognitive effort in identifying its valence, but slow enough to be reliably recognized. The 150-ms condition was therefore used for all contrasts. Familywise alpha was controlled at.05 using a Bonferroni adjustment for each family of contrasts. Contrasts for each task were analyzed as representing different families of tests. Exact p values are reported to allow interpretation of significance with the use of the applied or other significance thresholds. On the lexical decision task although dysphoric individuals reacted approximately 30 ms slower to negative than to neutral words, this difference was not statistically significant, t(29) = 1.6, p =.12. Nondysphoric individuals reacted approximately 40 ms faster to negative than to neutral stimuli. The difference between the dysphoric and nondysphoric participants differences in means (D) was 70 ms, which was significant, F(1, 74) = 7.37, p =.008, η 2 =.09, supporting the affective interference hypothesis. Differences were smaller but in the same direction when reactions to positive words were compared to negative words, F(1, 74) = 1.99, p =.162, η 2 =.03. On the valence identification task, dysphoric individuals reaction times were 200 ms faster to negative than to neutral stimuli, t(29) = 5.6, p <.001. Because nondysphoric individuals also reacted 180 ms faster to negative than to neutral stimuli, the 20-ms difference in biases between dysphoric and nondysphoric individuals was not significant. The greatest difference between the reactions times of dysphoric and nondysphoric individuals was with regard to positive stimuli on the valence identification task. Dysphoric individuals were significantly slower to react to positive than to negative words, D = 166 ms, in comparison to nondysphoric individuals, D = 20 ms, F(1, 73) = 6.45, p =.013, η 2 =.08, supporting the notion that depressed individuals attend more quickly to negative than to positive information.

9 Affective Interference in Dysphoria 81 Sensitivity Analyses Omnibus ANOVAs To be sure that results of relevant planned contrasts were not qualified by unexpected interactions omnibus group (dysphoric, nondysphoric) valence (positive, negative, neutral) multivariate split-plot ANOVAs, controlled using a Bonferroni correction at each level, were performed on the 150-ms stimulus duration condition. On the lexical decision task, a significant Valence Group interaction, F(2, 72) = 3.65, p <.05, η 2 =.09, was observed. Simple effects analysis (Maxwell & Delaney, 1990) yielded no significant effect of valence within the depressed group, F(2, 28) = 1.25, p >.05, η 2 =.082, or within the control group, F(2, 43) = 2.75, p >.05, η 2 =.11, suggesting the interaction was motivated by the previously reported complex contrast. On the valence identification task, a significant Valence Group interaction, F(2, 72) = 3.39, p <.05, η 2 =.09, was observed. There was a significant effect of valence within the depressed group, F(2, 28) = 15.04, p <.001, η 2 =.52, which was driven by the previously reported faster responses to negative than to positive or neutral words. Similarly, in the control group the effect of valence was also significant, F(2, 43) = 9.23, p <.001, η 2 =.30, and was driven by faster responses to positive and negative words than to neutral words. Robustness of Results to Gender and Stimulus Duration To examine robustness of the results to the extra noise introduced by decreasing stimulus duration, a 2 (dysphoric, nondysphoric) 3 (positive, negative, neutral valence) 3 (50, 100, 150 ms stimulus duration) mixed ANOVA on reaction time was performed for each task. No significant interactions with stimulus duration were observed for either task. The analysis revealed significant main effects of stimulus duration on the lexical decision task, F(2, 73) = 103.8, p <.001, and valence identification task, F(2, 72) = 84.1, p <.001, explained by longer reaction times in the shorter stimulus duration condition. On the valence identification task, a significant Valence Group interaction was also observed, F(2, 72) = 4.6, p <.01. Simple effects analyses, controlled using a Bonferroni correction at each level, revealed that the planned contrasts, reported previously, largely accounted for the Dysphoria Valence interaction. When gender was included in the same ANOVAs as a factor, no significant main effects or interactions with gender were observed. The only significant effects were those present when gender was not included. Thus, the reported effects do not appear to be qualified by gender differences or differences in stimulus duration. Robustness to Outliers The following strategy was adopted to be sure that the current results were robust to outliers. Relevant effects were determined to be captured by a significant Dysphoria Task Valence interaction that accounted for approximately 11.5% of the variation in reaction times, Hotelling s F(2, 69) = 4.48, p <.05. The Dysphoria Task Valence interaction was examined using a number of data aggregation

10 82 Siegle, Ingram, and Matt methods suggested by Ratcliff (1993). These included examination of a range of temporal thresholds for outlier removal other than the 5000-ms threshold used in other analyses. Examined values ranged from 2000 ms to no rejection. Similarly, measures of central tendency for reaction times that have been shown to correct for violations of normality in individual participants data including medians and harmonic means were explored. In each case the Dysphoria Task Valence interaction was significant, and the effect size was comparable to that for the analyses reported above. Results were also qualitatively similar when incorrect responses were removed from the computation of means. In fact, effect sizes for all contrasts increased under this restriction; in most cases signal detection rates were high for both dysphoric and nondysphoric individuals on both tasks. Mixed ANOVAs entering counterbalanced conditions (button order, task order, word list), stimulus duration, and dysphoria as independent variables yielded no main effects or interactions with the counterbalanced variables on reaction times in separate analyses for each task. Reliability of Reaction Times To examine whether apparent differential response latencies were tempered by low reliability of reaction times, the internal consistency of reaction times for each valence, for each word set, on both tasks was calculated. In the reported sample, Cronbach s alpha was >.7 for positive and negative words on each word set, on both tasks, and for neutral words on the valence identification task. Alpha was.68 for one word set, and.61 for the other, for neutral words on the lexical decision task. Does Attending to Emotional Features Disrupt Attention to Aspects of Stimuli? Results presented thus far suggest that dysphoria may be associated with attention to emotional aspects of stimuli, at the expense of attention to nonemotional aspects of stimuli. If true, a large portion of variation in reaction times on the lexical decision task should be accounted for by reaction times on the valence identification task, at least for dysphoric individuals. A first analysis of this relationship involved examining correlations between relevant effects on the tasks. Differences in reaction times to negative and neutral words on the lexical decision task were significantly correlated with differences in reaction times to positive and negative words on the valence identification task, r =.23, p <.05. To investigate this relationship in a more sophisticated fashion, hierarchical multiple regressions were performed on the differences in reaction times to each of the valences. Variables representing differences in reaction times to negative and neutral words, positive and neutral words, and positive and negative words on the valence identification task were entered on the first step of the regression. Dysphoria, scored dichotomously as in previous analyses, was entered on the second step. Terms representing the interaction of dysphoria and valence identification biases were entered on the third step. This technique allowed examination of the unique contribution of valence-mediated reaction-time biases above and beyond dysphoria, as well as the role of dysphoria in moderating affective interference.

11 Affective Interference in Dysphoria 83 Biases on the valence identification task accounted for 5.7% of the variation in the difference in lexical decision reaction times to negative and neutral words, which was marginally significant, F(2, 72) = 2.19, p =.12. Dysphoria accounted for an additional 6.3% of the variation, F(1, 71) = 5.09, p <.05. Interactions of dysphoria and valence identification biases were not statististically significant, R 2 =.016, F(2, 69) =.64, p >.05. In contrast, 10.1% of the variation in the difference in reaction times to negative and positive words on the lexical decision task was accounted for by biases on the valence-identification task, F(2, 72) = 4.04, p <.05. Quicker reaction times to negative versus neutral words on the valence identification task were associated with slower reaction times to negative versus positive words on the lexical decision task. Neither dysphoria nor interactions of dysphoria with valence identification biases explained significantly more variation, R 2 =.04, p >.05. This behavior is consistent with the notion that interference on the lexical decision task is due to preoccuption with negative aspects of the stimuli. DISCUSSION The main findings from this experiment suggest that compared to nondysphoric participants, dysphoric research participants were slower to name the valence of positive than of negative or neutral words, and were slightly, though significantly, slower to identify the lexicality of negative than of neutral words. Being quick to name the negativity of words was associated with being slow to name their lexicality. In general, these findings are consistent with the idea the idea of affective interference, that is, that dysphoric individuals attend to the emotional aspects of stimuli at the expense of attending to the nonemotional aspects of stimuli. Additionally, in support of the idea of affective interference, responding slowly to the lexicality of positive, compared to negative words, was associated with quick identification of the emotionality of negative words. Together, these findings suggest that depressed individuals may more easily recognize emotional qualities of negative than other information, and that such biases could be associated with difficulty responding to any other aspects of negative information. An explanation for the obtained data that is consistent with the affective interference hypothesis suggests dysphoric individuals often ruminate or think repeatedly about information that is salient to their depression. Nolen-Hoeksema has suggested that individuals possessing a ruminative cognitive style tend to focus on their symptoms of depression at the expense of other tasks they are given to do (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1993; Nolen-Hoeksema, Parker, & Larson, 1994). Using a broader definition of rumination (e.g., Ingram s notion that rumination involves feedback between the mental representations of nonemotional and emotional associations with information [Ingram, 1984]) ruminating individuals might be hypothesized to reveal biases on tasks assessing both emotional and nonemotional information processing, when they are dysphoric. Specifically, ruminative coping might lead dysphoric individuals to quickly recognize and ruminate upon negative information, yielding to quick judgments of that information as negative. In contrast, positive information would provide the least possible grounds for either quick recognition or

12 84 Siegle, Ingram, and Matt ruminative association of the information with relevant memories, leading to quick recognition of its lexicality but slow recognition of its emotional features. Appeals to rumination might also predict that people who ruminate on their own dysphoria (or negative information that is relevant to them) would be most delayed in identifying negative information not specifically relevant to their dysphoria; when presented with negative information that is not personally relevant, a dysphoric individual might think about negative information that is personally relevant (e.g., a loss event), and thus respond slowly to the presented nonpersonally relevant negative information. This hypothesis may explain why the difference in response times to negative and neutral words was larger for dysphoric individuals than for nondysphoric individuals on the lexical decision task. It also may suggest a need to assess the personal relevance of experimental stimuli. Yet, relationships between differential lexical decision reaction times to negative and neutral words and valence identification biases were weak and not replicated under different types of analysis. If failure to detect this effect is not due to low power, it could reflect the presence of two groups of dysphoric individuals. One group was slow to respond to negative words on the lexical decision task; these individuals may also have been slow to respond to negative words on the valence-identification task. Such individuals might be said to have a generalized bias away from negative information, perhaps as a result of heightened perceptual defense to negative information (Powell & Helmsley, 1984). A second group of dysphoric individuals were primarily slow to respond to positive words on the valence identification task, a phenomenon that was very weakly associated with being slower to respond to negative than to neutral words on the lexical decision task. This group of individuals might experience affective interference. An alternate explanation for discrepancies between performance on the tasks is that the lexical decision and valence identification tasks might require different levels of processing resources. In this case, the lexical decision task stimuli might not have been processed to the same extent as the valence identification stimuli and thus, biases were not as large. To examine the extent to which each task involved recruitment of processing resources, Siegle, Granholm, Ingram, and Matt (2000) measured pupil dilation, a correlate of cognitive load (e.g., Beatty, 1982), while depressed and never-depressed individuals completed the valence identification and lexical decision tasks. Patterns of pupil dilation on both tasks were similar in shape (i.e., same factor structure) and magnitude (similar peak levels of dilation) suggesting that the tasks required similar processing resources. A final potential explanation for the differential effect sizes concerns variability in the reliability of reaction time estimates on the tasks. Internal consistency was slightly lower for neutral words on the lexical decision task in comparison to neutral words on the valence identification task, and in comparison to positive and negative words on both tasks. As the primary comparison on the lexical decision task was between reaction times to negative and neutral words, the reliability of this difference could have appeared small because of decreased consistency in response times to neutral words. Several limitations of the current study must be acknowledged. Foremost, the idea of affective interference suggests a difference in the way dysphoric individuals will respond to negative stimuli that are and are not personally relevant; the study

13 Affective Interference in Dysphoria 85 did not assess the personal relevance of presented stimuli. Similarly, obtained results could have been a function of the word sets that were chosen, for example, positive and negative words could be more semantically related than chosen neutral words, leading to confounding of word sets with observed biases. Additionally, the power to detect a strong interaction of dysphoria with valence-identification on lexical decision reaction times may be low because no explicit effort was made to obtain a group of ruminating individuals with low BDI scores. Thus, the weak interaction in performed regressions may not reflect a true absence of an Information Processing Bias Dysphoria Interaction. Differences between task demands (e.g., the lexical decision task required a binary choice, and the valence identification task required choosing between three alternatives) could also have decreased power to detect relationships between measurements of emotional and semantic identification. The lack of observable gender differences may have been due to low power; there were relatively few dysphoric males in the sample (10/30). Other research suggests that potential correlates of affective interference, such as rumination, are subject to gender differences (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema, Larson, & Grayson, 1999). A final limitation involves the interpretation of BDI scores as representing dysphoria. Because other aspects of psychological distress (e.g., anxiety) were not controlled for, it is possible that elevations on the BDI represented diffuse psychological distress rather than pure dysphoria. These limitations aside, the present results suggest some possibly important directions for future research. Because the extent to which evidence for affective interference in dysphoric undergraduates can be used to inform an understanding of clinical depression is unclear, exploring the generalizability of the current findings to clinical depression may also be useful. Previous research has shown that many cognitive aspects of dysphoria mirror those found in clinically depressed samples. Evidence for affective interference in depressed samples might provide a great deal of insight into the role of cognition in depression. To further assess the role of affective interference in emotional information processing, experiments that explicitly identify stimuli that are and that are not personally relevant for dysphoric individuals may be useful. Additionally, it may be useful to investigate whether there is physiological support for the idea that mental structures involved in the identification of the affective content of a stimulus could interfere with those involved in the identification of the nonemotional content of a stimulus. Based on the performance of computational neural network models of relevant brain circuits on simulated lexical decision and valence identification tasks (Siegle, 1999; Siegle & Ingram, 1997), it is expected that individuals who are slow to react to negative information on the lexical decision task may have excessive activation of brain structures responsible for the identification of positive and negative aspects of information both before and after their reaction to the presented stimulus. Another useful future direction will be to examine correspondences between affective interference and other types of information processing biases commonly observed in depressed individuals. Affective interference specifically regards changes in processing because of unusual patterns of attentional allocation. Research has shown that both depression and dysphoria were characterized by better memory for negative than for positive information (e.g., Matt, et al., 1992). Moreover, theorists

14 86 Siegle, Ingram, and Matt have suggested that memory and late elaborative processes may be more representative of information processing disruptions in depression than in the attention biases (e.g., MacLeod & Mathews, 1991). Yet, comparisons of attention and memory biased in dysphoric or depressed individuals have rarely been published. To conclude, these results suggest that dysphoria is more than an emotional state. For some individuals, dysphoria is associated with changing the way individuals perceive information such that they focus on its emotional aspects. For some dysphoric individuals, the state is characterized by not attending to semantic or nonemotional aspects of that information. Rumination may involve both attending to emotional aspects of information and ignoring its nonemotional aspects. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Thanks to Monica Barback who provided valuable feedback on earlier versions of this manuscript, and Mark Shibley, Maureen Flaherty, Sean Gyll, Danielle Grant, and Ivan Nepomunceno who aided in the study s conceptualization and data collection. This research was supported in part by NIMH Grants MH16804 and MH55762, and the Veterans Administration. REFERENCES Beatty, J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin, 91, Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Hoeber. Beck, A. T. (1974). The development of depression. In R. J. Friedman & M. M. Katz (Eds.), The psychology of depression. New York: Winston-Wiley. Beck, A., Steer, R., & Garbin, M. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, Blaney, P. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, Bower, G. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Williams, R. (1994). Implicit and explicit memory for emotional information in non-clinical subjects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(1), Bray, D. E. (1988). The effects of hedonic manipulations on the perceptual processing of linguistic material. Dissertation Abstracts International, 49, Challis, B. H., & Krane, R. V. (1988). Mood induction and the priming of semantic memory in a lexical decision task: Asymmetric effects of elation and depression. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26, Conway, M. A., & Bekerian, D. A. (1987). Situational knowledge and emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1, Craik, F., & Lockhart, R. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11, Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Hill, A. B., & Kemp-Wheeler, S. M. (1989). The influence of anxiety on lexical and affective decision time for emotional words. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, Ingram, R. (1984). Towards an information processing analysis of depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., & Segal, Z. V. (1998). Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New York: Guilford. Kitayama, S. (1990). Interaction between affect and cognition in word perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2), LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.

15 Affective Interference in Dysphoria 87 MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. M. (1991). Cognitive-experimental approaches to the emotional disorders. In P. R. Martin (Ed.), Handbook of behavior therapy and psychological science: An integrative approach (Vol. 164, pp ). New York: Pergamon Press. Macleod, C., Mathews, A. M., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), MacLeod, C., Tata, P., & Mathews, A. M. (1987). Perception of emotionally valenced information in depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26(1), Massaro, D. (1988). Experimental psychology: An information processing approach. San Diego, CA: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Mathews, A., & Milroy, R. (1994). Processing of emotional meaning in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 8, Matt, G., Vazquez, C., & Campbell, W. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, Matthews, G., & Harley, T. A. (1996). Connectionist models of emotional distress and attentional bias. Cognition and Emotion, 10, Matthews, G., Pitcaithly, D., & Mann, R. L. E. (1995). Mood, neuroticism, and the encoding of affective words. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, Matthews, G., & Southall, A. (1991). Depression and the processing of emotional stimuli: A study of semantic priming. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15(4), Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (1990). Designing experiments and analyzing data. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion, 7, Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L. E., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(1), Persons, J. B. (1986). The advantages of studying psychological phenomena rather than psychiatric diagnoses. American Psychologist, 41, Powell, M., & Helmsley, D. R. (1984). Depression: A breakdown of perceptual defense? British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114, Ruiz Caballero, J. A., & Bermudez Moreno, J. (1992). Individual differences in depression, induced mood, and perception of emotionally toned words. European Journal of Personality, 6(3), Siegle, G. J. (1994). The Balanced Affective Word List Creation Program. Retrieved from Siegle, G. J. (1996). Rumination on affect: Cause for negative attention biases in depression? Unpublished master s thesis, San Diego State University. Retrieved from Siegle, G. J. (1999). A neural network model of attention biases in depression. In J. Reggia & E. Ruppin (Eds.), Disorders of brain, behavior, and cognition: The neurocomputational perspective (pp ). New York: Elsevier. Siegle, G. J., Granholm, E., Ingram, R. E., & Matt, G. E. (2000). Pupillary response and reaction time measures of sustained processing of negative information in depression. Manuscript submitted for publication. Siegle, G. J., & Ingram, R. E. (1997). Modeling individual differences in negative information processing biases. In G. Matthews (Ed.), Personality and individual differences in psychopathology. Princeton, NJ: Erlbaum. Stip, E., & Lecours, A. R. (1992). Fonctionnement neuropsychologique du deprime. Epreuve de decision lexicale dans la depression majeure. Encephale, 18, Stip, E., Lecours, A. R., Chertkow, H., Elie, R., & O Connor, K. (1994). Influence of affective words on lexical decision task in major depression. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 19(3), Tucker, D. M., & Derryberry, D. (1992). Motivated attention: Anxiety and the frontal executive functions. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 5, Williams, G., Conner, J., Siegle, G. J., Ingram, R., & Cole, D. (1998). Is more negative less positive? Relating dysphoria to emotion ratings. Paper Presented at the meeting of the Western Psychological Association, Albuquerque, NM. Retrieved from Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, Williamson, S., Harpur, T. J., & Hare, R. D. (1991). Abnormal processing of affective words by psychopaths. Psychophysiology, 28(3),

Memory for emotional faces in naturally occurring dysphoria and

Memory for emotional faces in naturally occurring dysphoria and Running Head: Memory for emotional faces Memory for emotional faces in naturally occurring dysphoria and induced negative mood Nathan Ridout*, Aliya Noreen & Jaskaran Johal Clinical & Cognitive Neurosciences,

More information

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times CHAPTER 4 Lars Kuchinke, Melissa L.-H. Võ, Lars Michael, and Arthur M. Jacobs 5 Abstract In a lexical decision experiment emotional valence and emotional

More information

Attention Allocation and Incidental Recognition of Emotional Information in Dysphoria

Attention Allocation and Incidental Recognition of Emotional Information in Dysphoria DOI 10.1007/s10608-010-9305-3 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Attention Allocation and Incidental Recognition of Emotional Information in Dysphoria Alissa J. Ellis Christopher G. Beevers Tony T. Wells Ó Springer Science+Business

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

2/19/2011. Joseph Bardeen, M.A. Northern Illinois University February 18, 2011

2/19/2011. Joseph Bardeen, M.A. Northern Illinois University February 18, 2011 Lifetime prevalence estimates for PTSD are about 8%; however, the majority of Americans experience a traumatic event in their lifetimes (Kessler et al, 1995: NCS) Joseph Bardeen, M.A. Northern Illinois

More information

Michael Armey David M. Fresco. Jon Rottenberg. James J. Gross Ian H. Gotlib. Kent State University. Stanford University. University of South Florida

Michael Armey David M. Fresco. Jon Rottenberg. James J. Gross Ian H. Gotlib. Kent State University. Stanford University. University of South Florida Further psychometric refinement of depressive rumination: Support for the Brooding and Pondering factor solution in a diverse community sample with clinician-assessed psychopathology Michael Armey David

More information

Irish Journal of Psychology, 20 (1):

Irish Journal of Psychology, 20 (1): Provided by the author(s) and University College Dublin Library in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite the published version when available. Title Mood congruent memory bias of individuals

More information

Automatic and Effortful Processing of Self-Statements in Depression

Automatic and Effortful Processing of Self-Statements in Depression Paper III Wang, C. E., Brennen, T., & Holte, A. (2006). Automatic and effortful processing of self-statements in depression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 35, 117-124. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy Vol 35,

More information

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Michael Romano (mrr2@nyu.edu) Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place New York, NY 1000 USA Abstract Traditional

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Depressive implicit associations and adults reports of childhood abuse

BRIEF REPORT. Depressive implicit associations and adults reports of childhood abuse COGNITION AND EMOTION 2011, 25 (2), 328333 BRIEF REPORT Depressive implicit associations and adults reports of childhood abuse Ashley L. Johnson, Jessica S. Benas, and Brandon E. Gibb Binghamton University

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (1), 159-165 Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions SOWON HAHN AND SCOTT D. GRONLUND University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma Using a visual search

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

Original Papers. Memory bias training by means of the emotional short-term memory task

Original Papers. Memory bias training by means of the emotional short-term memory task Original Papers Polish Psychological Bulletin 2015, vol 46(1), 122-126 DOI - 10.1515/ppb-2015-0016 Borysław Paulewicz * Agata Blaut ** Aleksandra Gronostaj *** Memory bias training by means of the emotional

More information

Source memory and the picture superiority effect

Source memory and the picture superiority effect Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2007 Source memory and the picture superiority effect Noelle L. Brown Louisiana State University and Agricultural and

More information

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

A model of the interaction between mood and memory INSTITUTE OF PHYSICS PUBLISHING NETWORK: COMPUTATION IN NEURAL SYSTEMS Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 12 (2001) 89 109 www.iop.org/journals/ne PII: S0954-898X(01)22487-7 A model of the interaction between

More information

Cognitive Bias Modification: Induced Interpretive Biases Affect Memory

Cognitive Bias Modification: Induced Interpretive Biases Affect Memory Trinity University Digital Commons @ Trinity Psychology Faculty Research Psychology Department 2-2011 Cognitive Bias Modification: Induced Interpretive Biases Affect Memory Tanya B. Tran University of

More information

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Uri Hasson (uhasson@princeton.edu) Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 USA Geoffrey P. Goodwin (ggoodwin@princeton.edu)

More information

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1995 The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the

More information

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? University of Iowa Honors Theses University of Iowa Honors Program Spring 2017 Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? Ann Walsh University of Iowa Follow this and additional works at:

More information

BRIEF REPORT. The effects of optimism and pessimism on updating emotional information in working memory. Sara M. Levens 1 and Ian H.

BRIEF REPORT. The effects of optimism and pessimism on updating emotional information in working memory. Sara M. Levens 1 and Ian H. COGNITION AND EMOTION 2012, 26 (2), 341350 BRIEF REPORT The effects of optimism and pessimism on updating emotional information in working memory Sara M. Levens 1 and Ian H. Gotlib 2 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 699-703 Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening GREGORY J. H. COLFLESH University of Illinois, Chicago,

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA COGNITION AND EMOTION 2007, 21 (3), 681688 BRIEF REPORT Vulnerability to depressive symptoms: Clarifying the role of excessive reassurance seeking and perceived social support in an interpersonal model

More information

Effects of Trait Anxiety and Depression on Working Memory Updating

Effects of Trait Anxiety and Depression on Working Memory Updating University of Colorado, Boulder CU Scholar Undergraduate Honors Theses Honors Program Spring 016 Effects of Trait Anxiety and Depression on Working Memory Updating Joy Walters joy.walters@colorado.edu

More information

Depression and Implicit Memory: Understanding Mood Congruent Memory Bias

Depression and Implicit Memory: Understanding Mood Congruent Memory Bias Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 28, No. 3, June 2004 ( C 2004), pp. 387 414 Depression and Implicit Memory: Understanding Mood Congruent Memory Bias Elaine S. Barry, 1,2,3 Mary J. Naus, 1 and Lynn

More information

Running Head: COGNITIVE VULNERABILITY AND ATTACHMENT. Cognitive Vulnerability and Attachment. Nathan L. Williams University of Arkansas

Running Head: COGNITIVE VULNERABILITY AND ATTACHMENT. Cognitive Vulnerability and Attachment. Nathan L. Williams University of Arkansas Running Head: COGNITIVE VULNERABILITY AND ATTACHMENT Cognitive Vulnerability and Attachment Nathan L. Williams University of Arkansas & John H. Riskind George Mason University Williams, N. L. & Riskind,

More information

Supplementary Materials: Materials and Methods Figures S1-S2 Tables S1-S17 References

Supplementary Materials: Materials and Methods Figures S1-S2 Tables S1-S17 References Supplementary Materials: Materials and Methods Figures S1-S2 Tables S1-S17 References Materials and Methods Simon Task Participants were randomly assigned to one of four versions of the task. Upon return

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psych.colorado.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Brooding and Pondering: Isolating the Active Ingredients of Depressive Rumination with Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Brooding and Pondering: Isolating the Active Ingredients of Depressive Rumination with Confirmatory Factor Analysis Michael Armey David M. Fresco Kent State University Brooding and Pondering: Isolating the Active Ingredients of Depressive Rumination with Confirmatory Factor Analysis Douglas S. Mennin Yale University

More information

A Behavioral Attention Task for Investigating Rumination in Borderline Personality Disorder: Final Report

A Behavioral Attention Task for Investigating Rumination in Borderline Personality Disorder: Final Report Kaleidoscope Volume 11 Article 68 July 2014 A Behavioral Attention Task for Investigating Rumination in Borderline Personality Disorder: Final Report Jacob Folsom Follow this and additional works at: https://uknowledge.uky.edu/kaleidoscope

More information

Supplementary Materials for. Error Related Brain Activity Reveals Self-Centric Motivation: Culture Matters. Shinobu Kitayama and Jiyoung Park

Supplementary Materials for. Error Related Brain Activity Reveals Self-Centric Motivation: Culture Matters. Shinobu Kitayama and Jiyoung Park 1 Supplementary Materials for Error Related Brain Activity Reveals Self-Centric Motivation: Culture Matters Shinobu Kitayama and Jiyoung Park University of Michigan *To whom correspondence should be addressed.

More information

Mood-congruent free recall bias in anxious individuals is not a consequence of response bias

Mood-congruent free recall bias in anxious individuals is not a consequence of response bias Europe PMC Funders Group Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Memory. 2006 May ; 14(4): 393 399. doi:10.1080/09658210500343166. Mood-congruent free recall bias in anxious individuals is

More information

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2009, 16 (1), 57-61 doi:10.3758/pbr.16.1.57 The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words DANIEL SMILEK, GRAYDEN J. F. SOLMAN, PETER MURAWSKI, AND

More information

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (6), 1067-1073 Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources MOSHE NAVEH-BENJAMIN University of Missouri,

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Personality and Individual Differences 53 (1) 13 17 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Attentional

More information

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Nart Bedin Atalay (natalay@selcuk.edu.tr) Selcuk University, Konya, TURKEY Mine Misirlisoy

More information

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040 School of Psychology Important Degree Information: B.Sc./M.A. Honours The general requirements are 480 credits over a period of normally 4 years (and not more than 5 years) or part-time equivalent; the

More information

Negative affect varying in motivational intensity influences scope of memory

Negative affect varying in motivational intensity influences scope of memory Cognition and Emotion ISSN: 0269-9931 (Print) 1464-0600 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcem20 Negative affect varying in motivational intensity influences scope of memory A.

More information

Depressive deficits in forgetting

Depressive deficits in forgetting Trinity University Digital Commons @ Trinity Psychology Faculty Research Psychology Department 11-2003 Depressive deficits in forgetting Paula T. Hertel Trinity University, phertel@trinity.edu M. Gerstle

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Conflict and Bias in Heuristic Judgment Sudeep Bhatia Online First Publication, September 29, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000307

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2014 Vol. 28, No. 6, 1090 1099, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.866936 BRIEF REPORT Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder James E. Sorenson,

More information

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1983, Vol. 51, No. 5, 721-725 Copyright 1983 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation

More information

Behaviour Research and Therapy

Behaviour Research and Therapy Behaviour Research and Therapy 49 (2011) 406e412 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behaviour Research and Therapy journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat Effect of visual perspective on

More information

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: SOCIAL AFFILIATION CUES PRIME HELP-SEEKING INTENTIONS 1 This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: Rubin, M. (2011). Social

More information

NOTICING THE SELF: IMPLICIT ASSESSMENT OF SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION USING WORD RECOGNITION LATENCIES

NOTICING THE SELF: IMPLICIT ASSESSMENT OF SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION USING WORD RECOGNITION LATENCIES NOTICING THE SELF: IMPLICIT ASSESSMENT OF SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION USING WORD RECOGNITION LATENCIES By: Jan Eichstaedt and Paul J. Silvia Eichstaedt, J., & Silvia, P. J. (2003). Noticing the self: Implicit

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

Automaticity of Number Perception

Automaticity of Number Perception Automaticity of Number Perception Jessica M. Choplin (jessica.choplin@vanderbilt.edu) Gordon D. Logan (gordon.logan@vanderbilt.edu) Vanderbilt University Psychology Department 111 21 st Avenue South Nashville,

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 Supporting Information Variances and biases of absolute distributions were larger in the 2-line

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

The Impact of Ruminative Processing on the Experience of Self Referent Intrusive. Memories in Dysphoria. Alishia D. Williams & Michelle L.

The Impact of Ruminative Processing on the Experience of Self Referent Intrusive. Memories in Dysphoria. Alishia D. Williams & Michelle L. 1 The Impact of Ruminative Processing on the Experience of Self Referent Intrusive Memories in Dysphoria Alishia D. Williams & Michelle L. Moulds The University of New South Wales, Sydney Correspondence:

More information

Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not...

Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not... Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not... Taylor, AJ, Bendall, RCA and Thompson, C Title Authors Type URL Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not... Taylor, AJ, Bendall,

More information

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments Perception, 2010, volume 39, pages 514 ^ 532 doi:10.1068/p6501 The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments Alexander Todorov, Valerie Loehr, Nikolaas N Oosterhof Department

More information

Attentional Bias and Mood Persistence as Prospective Predictors of Dysphoria

Attentional Bias and Mood Persistence as Prospective Predictors of Dysphoria Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 27, No. 6, December 2003 ( C 2003), pp. 619 637 Attentional Bias and Mood Persistence as Prospective Predictors of Dysphoria Christopher G. Beevers 1,2,3 and Charles

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Elaborative Retrieval: Do Semantic Mediators Improve Memory? Melissa Lehman and Jeffrey D. Karpicke Online First Publication, March 4,

More information

Negative Life Events, Self-Perceived Competence, and Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults

Negative Life Events, Self-Perceived Competence, and Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults Cogn Ther Res (2007) 31:773 783 DOI 10.1007/s10608-006-9101-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Negative Life Events, Self-Perceived Competence, and Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults Dorothy J. Uhrlass Æ Brandon E. Gibb

More information

Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations

Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations R461B GTG DG Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2003,?? (?),?? Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations LARRY L. JACOBY Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri D. STEPHEN

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load Psychonomic Bulletin & Review In press Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load JOSHUA D. COSMAN AND SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Attentional capture by abrupt onsets can be

More information

Running head: CO-RUMINATION AND AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY

Running head: CO-RUMINATION AND AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY Co-Rumination & AM 1 Running head: CO-RUMINATION AND AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY Co-Rumination and Autobiographical Memory Naomi Garbisch Gustavus Adolphus College Co-Rumination & AM 2 Abstract: Thirty three

More information

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1994, 47A (1) 201-205 Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing Harold Pashler University of

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Adam Hahn 1 Bertram Gawronski 2 Word count: 20,754 excluding acknowledgements, abstract,

More information

Interaction Between Social Categories in the Composite Face Paradigm. Wenfeng Chen and Naixin Ren. Chinese Academy of Sciences. Andrew W.

Interaction Between Social Categories in the Composite Face Paradigm. Wenfeng Chen and Naixin Ren. Chinese Academy of Sciences. Andrew W. Interaction Between Social Categories in the Composite Face Paradigm Wenfeng Chen and Naixin Ren Chinese Academy of Sciences Andrew W. Young University of York Chang Hong Liu Bournemouth University Author

More information

INTERPRETIVE BIAS IN ANXIETY: THE SEARCH FOR AN ELUSIVE COGNITIVE EFFECT. Lucia Fung. BSocSc (Hons) of The University of Western Australia

INTERPRETIVE BIAS IN ANXIETY: THE SEARCH FOR AN ELUSIVE COGNITIVE EFFECT. Lucia Fung. BSocSc (Hons) of The University of Western Australia INTERPRETIVE BIAS IN ANXIETY: THE SEARCH FOR AN ELUSIVE COGNITIVE EFFECT Lucia Fung BSocSc (Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy of The University of Western Australia

More information

Image generation in a letter-classification task

Image generation in a letter-classification task Perception & Psychophysics 1976, Vol. 20 (3),215-219 Image generation in a letter-classification task THOMAS R. HERZOG Grand Valley State Colleges, Allandale, Michigan 49401 Subjects classified briefly

More information

Stability and Change of Adolescent. Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study. Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill. Division of Psychology

Stability and Change of Adolescent. Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study. Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill. Division of Psychology Stability and Change of Adolescent Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill Division of Psychology The Australian National University Paper presented to the

More information

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials for Wisneski and Skitka Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction Table of Contents 2 Pilot Studies 2 High Awareness

More information

Looming Maladaptive Style as a Specific Moderator of Risk Factors for Anxiety

Looming Maladaptive Style as a Specific Moderator of Risk Factors for Anxiety Looming Maladaptive Style as a Specific Moderator of Risk Factors for Anxiety Abby D. Adler Introduction Anxiety disorders are the most common mental illness in the United States with a lifetime prevalence

More information

Number of cues influences the cost of remembering to remember

Number of cues influences the cost of remembering to remember Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (1), 149-156 doi: 10.3758/MC.36.1.149 Number of cues influences the cost of remembering to remember ANNA-LISA COHEN New York University, New York, New York and Yeshiva University,

More information

The Interaction of Mood and Rumination in Depression: Effects on Mood Maintenance and Mood-Congruent Autobiographical Memory

The Interaction of Mood and Rumination in Depression: Effects on Mood Maintenance and Mood-Congruent Autobiographical Memory J Rat-Emo Cognitive-Behav Ther (2009) 27:144 159 DOI 10.1007/s10942-009-0096-y ORIGINAL ARTICLE The Interaction of Mood and Rumination in Depression: Effects on Mood Maintenance and Mood-Congruent Autobiographical

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

Aging, Emotion, Attention, and Binding in the Taboo Stroop Task: Data and Theories

Aging, Emotion, Attention, and Binding in the Taboo Stroop Task: Data and Theories Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 12803-12833; doi:10.3390/ijerph121012803 OPEN ACCESS Article International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health ISSN 1660-4601 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph

More information

A Simulation of the Activation- Selection Model of Meaning. Gorfein, D.S. & Brown, V.R.

A Simulation of the Activation- Selection Model of Meaning. Gorfein, D.S. & Brown, V.R. A Simulation of the Activation- Selection Model of Meaning Gorfein, D.S. & Brown, V.R. Abstract The activation-selection model of determining the meaning of an ambiguous word or phrase (Gorfein, 2001)

More information

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy Volume 17, Number 2, Summer 1999 CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

Perceptron Example: Computational model for the Stroop Task. Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio and Brain Engineering, KAIST

Perceptron Example: Computational model for the Stroop Task. Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio and Brain Engineering, KAIST Perceptron Example: Computational model for the Stroop Task Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio and Brain Engineering, KAIST GREEN YELLOW BLUE BLUE YELLOW GREEN BLUE RED The Stroop Task Can Teach Us

More information

Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill

Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 1979, Vol. S, No. 2, 179-187 Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill John B. Shea and

More information

Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures

Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2001, 54A (3), 665 681 Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures Jenny Yiend and Andrew Mathews MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK

More information

Cognition and Depression: Current Status and Future Directions

Cognition and Depression: Current Status and Future Directions I ANRV407-CP06-11 ARI 18 December 2009 17:42 R E V I E W S E C N A D V A N Cognition and Depression: Current Status and Future Directions Ian H. Gotlib 1 and Jutta Joormann 2 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli James S. Adelman (J.S.Adelman@warwick.ac.uk) Neil Stewart (Neil.Stewart@warwick.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, University

More information

How Do Individual Experiences Aggregate to Shape Personality Development? Elliot M. Tucker-Drob

How Do Individual Experiences Aggregate to Shape Personality Development? Elliot M. Tucker-Drob August 3, 2017 How Do Individual Experiences Aggregate to Shape Personality Development? Elliot M. Tucker-Drob Department of Psychology and Population Research Center University of Texas at Austin Acknowledgements:

More information

Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture Fragment Completion Paradigm

Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture Fragment Completion Paradigm Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia - Social and Behavioral Scien ce s 82 ( 2013 ) 482 486 World Conference on Psychology and Sociology 2012 Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture

More information

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC) Neural Computation Overview of Lecture Models of prefrontal cortex function Dr. Sam Gilbert Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience University College London E-mail: sam.gilbert@ucl.ac.uk Prefrontal cortex

More information

The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure?

The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure? The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure? Contributed by Bruno Rossion, May 2013 Comparing the matching of same top face halves when they are aligned vs. misaligned with different bottom

More information

A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions

A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions Neurocomputing 69 (2006) 1322 1326 www.elsevier.com/locate/neucom A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions Nicola De Pisapia, Todd S. Braver Cognitive

More information

Malleability in Implicit Stereotypes and Attitudes. Siri J. Carpenter, American Psychological Association Mahzarin R. Banaji, Yale University

Malleability in Implicit Stereotypes and Attitudes. Siri J. Carpenter, American Psychological Association Mahzarin R. Banaji, Yale University Malleability in Implicit Stereotypes and Attitudes Siri J. Carpenter, American Psychological Association Mahzarin R. Banaji, Yale University Poster presented at the 2nd annual meeting of the Society for

More information

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels A peer-reviewed electronic journal. Copyright is retained by the first or sole author, who grants right of first publication to Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation. Permission is granted to distribute

More information

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition RESEARCH ARTICLE A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition Sameer Sethi 1 *, Dr. Simon Rigoulot 2, Dr. Marc D. Pell 3 1 Faculty of Science, McGill University,

More information

Running Head: TRUST INACCURATE INFORMANTS 1. In the Absence of Conflicting Testimony Young Children Trust Inaccurate Informants

Running Head: TRUST INACCURATE INFORMANTS 1. In the Absence of Conflicting Testimony Young Children Trust Inaccurate Informants Running Head: TRUST INACCURATE INFORMANTS 1 In the Absence of Conflicting Testimony Young Children Trust Inaccurate Informants Kimberly E. Vanderbilt, Gail D. Heyman, and David Liu University of California,

More information

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1060 1066 Brief Report Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information